Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2015 Oct 12;112(44):13687–13692. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517629112

Bursts of beta oscillation differentiate postperformance activity in the striatum and motor cortex of monkeys performing movement tasks

Joseph Feingold 1,1, Daniel J Gibson 1,1, Brian DePasquale 1, Ann M Graybiel 1,2
PMCID: PMC4640760  PMID: 26460033

Significance

Studies of beta oscillations, highlighted clinically because of exaggerated beta-band activity in Parkinson’s disease, have focused on measures averaged over multiple trials, and readers outside the field could have the misperception that beta oscillations persist over seconds. We show that brief bursts of oscillation are responsible for virtually all beta-band activity in healthy monkeys. The state called “beta synchronization” consists of numerous brief (∼40–120 ms) bouts of oscillation within a nonoscillatory background, and these bouts can also occur during periods of “desynchronization.” Thus, postmovement “synchronization” of beta represents a transient increase in oscillation probability, with different time courses in the motor cortex and striatum. We suggest that the timing and duration of these bursts are critical parameters for network function.

Keywords: basal ganglia, local field potentials, beta band, sequential movement, synchronization

Abstract

Studies of neural oscillations in the beta band (13–30 Hz) have demonstrated modulations in beta-band power associated with sensory and motor events on time scales of 1 s or more, and have shown that these are exaggerated in Parkinson’s disease. However, even early reports of beta activity noted extremely fleeting episodes of beta-band oscillation lasting <150 ms. Because the interpretation of possible functions for beta-band oscillations depends strongly on the time scale over which they occur, and because of these oscillations’ potential importance in Parkinson’s disease and related disorders, we analyzed in detail the distributions of duration and power for beta-band activity in a large dataset recorded in the striatum and motor-premotor cortex of macaque monkeys performing reaching tasks. Both regions exhibited typical beta-band suppression during movement and postmovement rebounds of up to 3 s as viewed in data averaged across trials, but single-trial analysis showed that most beta oscillations occurred in brief bursts, commonly 90–115 ms long. In the motor cortex, the burst probabilities peaked following the last movement, but in the striatum, the burst probabilities peaked at task end, after reward, and continued through the postperformance period. Thus, what appear to be extended periods of postperformance beta-band synchronization reflect primarily the modulated densities of short bursts of synchrony occurring in region-specific and task-time-specific patterns. We suggest that these short-time-scale events likely underlie the functions of most beta-band activity, so that prolongation of these beta episodes, as observed in Parkinson’s disease, could produce deleterious network-level signaling.


Oscillations of brain activity in the beta band (13–30 Hz) have been implicated in sensorimotor control and integration (15) and are pathologically synchronized and exaggerated in Parkinson's disease (611). Although reports have published examples of very brief (<150 ms) bursts of beta-band oscillation (1215), the analysis of beta-band activity has focused primarily on data averaged over trials, which show variations in average beta-band power occurring on a time scale of seconds.

To address the apparent discrepancy in time scales between the single-trial results and the trial-averaged results, we analyzed the relationship of brief beta bursts as viewed at a single-trial level to the substantially slower variations in trial-averaged power that are conventionally referred to as periods of “synchronization” or “desynchronization” of beta-band activity. We examined beta-band activity recorded in two regions of prime clinical interest, the motor-premotor regions of the neocortex and the striatum, in macaque monkeys performing well-learned movement sequences. Our findings suggest that these regions exhibit different peak times of synchronization of beta bursting during the classic postmovement period, comprising differentially timed alignments of beta bursts within a desynchronized background of beta activity. Such brief, one to several cycle alignments of beta oscillation are suitable for realignment of circuit activity according to contextual demands. We suggest that the prolongation of beta bursts known to occur in Parkinson’s disease could blunt or abolish such flexibility in the neural control of behavior.

Results

We recorded local field potentials (LFPs) from multiple microelectrodes placed in the primary motor and premotor cortex and the caudate nucleus (CN) and putamen of two female monkeys (Fig. 1B and Fig. S1). Visual cues instructed the monkey to perform either a single joystick movement to a peripheral target and back to the center position (1M1T; Fig. 1A), or a series of three such movements (3M3T; Fig. 1C). Each movement had to be preceded by holding the joystick in the center for a short (0.6–1.2 s) or long (1.4–2.0 s) period that the monkey had to estimate by itself.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Overview of tasks and power spectra. (A and C) Task timelines. (B) Monkey’s position during the task. (D and E) Average LFP spectrograms calculated over all correct trials of 1M1T (D) and 3M3T (E) tasks across two monkeys for each region, using a 1-s moving window. Power is shown in decibels relative to a 1/f1.5 curve fitted to each channel. White vertical lines mark the boundaries between panels aligned to different task events. Colored vertical lines mark task events: empty targets on (E, black); colored cue targets on (C, black); first movement (1, red); second movement (2, green); third movement (3, blue); reward delivery (R, purple); ends of movements or reward delivery (unlabeled, black).

Fig. S1.

Fig. S1.

Grid maps illustrating electrode placements in the mediolateral (ML) versus anteroposterior (AP) plane as viewed from above, for two implants in monkey HH (A) and in monkey JB (B). Each circle with a number inside designates one electrode. The fill colors show how electrodes were grouped for analysis, as indicated in the color legends. Those with no fill color were not used owing to defects in placement and/or signal quality. CN electrodes were divided into an anterior group (CNa) and a posterior group (CNp) at AP 19.5 for monkey HH, but not for monkey JB.

As viewed in grand average spectral power over all electrodes in both monkeys (Fig. 1 D and E), beta power was clearly task-modulated. In each region, beta power fell sharply when the empty cues were presented to signal that a trial could be initiated, and it remained low in both regions throughout the execution of the task. Beta power reached its highest level in the motor-premotor cortex abruptly after cessation of the last movement. In the striatum, beta power also rose postmovement, but peak power was not reached until later in the intertrial interval (ITI), after reward delivery. Similar results were obtained using the method of local average referencing (14) in two sessions (Fig. S2), indicating that volume conduction contributed little to the results. Differences between subregions of the striatum were less marked than the difference between the striatum and the motor cortex, although local-average-referenced analyses indicated that the moderate increase in striatal beta power at movement cessation is due largely to activity in the putamen, where beta power after movement cessation may be nearly equal to that during the ITI (Figs. S3 and S4). An analysis of coherence in two sessions (Fig. S5) indicated that the regions recorded from were functionally connected in the beta band during the ITI. In one session (HH092807), coherence was stronger immediately after movement cessation, possibly owing to the fact that the motor-premotor cortical electrodes were located more posteriorly in this implant.

Fig. S2.

Fig. S2.

Examples of spectrograms generated with monopolar and local-average-referenced signals for one session from each monkey (session HH092807 from implant HH0407 and session JB082305 from implant JB1104; see grid maps in Fig. S1). Although there was considerable variation between the monopolar and local-average-referenced spectrograms on individual electrodes (Fig. S4), the results shown here of averaging over all of the electrodes within each region were remarkably similar. As would be expected based on the rule of thumb that volume-conducted signals tend to be greatest at low frequencies, here the greatest differences between the monopolar and local-average-referenced signals manifest predominantly below 10 Hz.

Fig. S3.

Fig. S3.

Spectrograms for subregions of striatum. (A) From monopolar signals, averaged over all sessions for each monkey. In monkey HH, the CN was divided into an anterior region spanning AP 20–26 and a posterior region spanning AP 14–19 (Fig. S1). In monkey JB, spectrograms were averaged over all channels in the CN, spanning AP 14.6–27.6. For the average over the two monkeys, data from anterior CN and posterior CN recorded in monkey HH were averaged first, and the results were averaged with CN data recorded in monkey JB. (B) From local-average-referenced signals, averaged within a single session from each monkey.

Fig. S4.

Fig. S4.

Monopolar and local-average-referenced signals for three adjacent individual electrodes from each region in one session (HH092807). (A) Anterior CN. Spectrograms of monopolar signals were similar on all three channels, whereas the local-average-referenced signals show that the oscillatory activity on electrode 29 was common to all electrodes in the region, likely owing in large part to volume conduction from sources outside the region. This observation suggests that oscillatory activity in the striatum may be present or absent depending on the exact location, to a precision of ≤1 mm, a spatial scale comparable with known anatomic features (41). The average of the local-average-referenced signals shown in Fig. S2 closely resembles the average of the monopolar signals at frequencies above 10 Hz, suggesting that the monopolar recordings may have in effect performed a physical averaging of the current sources over a larger volume of tissue, as theoretically might be expected. Note the wider color scale than the scales used in average spectrograms. (B) Putamen. There appears to be a local beta-band current source near electrode 56 (the middle electrode), but very little beta-band activity near the neighboring electrodes 55 and 57 located 1 mm to each side. The beta-band activity confined to electrode 56 bears a striking resemblance to the activity at electrode 70 in the motor cortex; however, further investigation is needed to determine whether this constitutes evidence of functional connectivity. (C) Motor cortex. Local average referencing generally may make less difference in the neocortex than in the striatum, given that the electrodes were displaced from each other roughly tangentially to the cortical surface, whereas synaptic currents flow predominantly radially.

Fig. S5.

Fig. S5.

Time-frequency plots showing coherence between motor cortex and striatum for the two sessions for which local-average-referenced signals were computed. CN electrodes were limited to the anterior region (anterior to AP19.5) of the CN, and were analyzed separately from those in the putamen. Coherence was computed for each electrode pair comprising one motor cortical electrode and one striatal electrode.

Changes in Beta-Band Power in Single Trials Occur Much Faster Than Those Detectable in Averages over Trials.

There was a striking contrast between the time courses of beta power fluctuations visible in trial-averaged and single-trial analyses (Fig. 2). Whether averaged with a 1-s analysis window that produced strong temporal smoothing (Fig. 2 A and D) or plotted with minimal smoothing (Fig. 2 B and E), the time courses of increased beta span seconds. In the single trial traces (Fig. 2 C and F), instead of such long periods of increased beta power, there were many short-duration episodes of increased power that become denser during the periods that appeared as synchronized beta activity in the trial-averaged plots. The lack of consistency from trial to trial cannot be accounted for by variations in task demands, because all of the trials shown had the same spatial and temporal patterns of visuomotor requirements. Such inconsistency was typical of the entire dataset, indicating that the trial-averaged analyses blurred brief, often nonsynchronous episodes of beta-band activity. Thus, synchronized beta activity actually comprised many bursts, only some of which were synchronized over successive trials.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Examples of LFP activity recorded on individual channels from motor cortex (A–C) and striatum (D–F) in one session from 2 s before until 2 s after the end of the third (final) movement. (A and D) Spectrograms averaged over 27 correct 3M3T trials with the long-long-long hold sequence and the up/left-down-left spatial sequence, with power coded as in Fig. 1. Purple lines denote the beta band. The color scale in A applies to D as well. (B and E) Time course of average power in the 13–30 Hz pass band (black lines) with 95% confidence limits (gray shading). (C and F) Smoothed power traces for each of the 27 trials. The gray horizontal lines below each trace represent zero power. Blue, purple, and black dots mark the onset of third movement, reward delivery, and end of movements/reward delivery, respectively.

Beta-Band Power Is More Variable in LFPs Than in Random Surrogate Data.

As a control to determine whether the trial-to-trial variability was different from that produced by random signals, we constructed randomized surrogate signals that were spectrally matched to each channel of actual data for 3M3T blocks (SI Materials and Methods). The resulting signals had a temporal structure equivalent to random noise (compare Fig. 2 and Fig. S6). We then calculated the coefficient of variation (CV) of beta power across the entire 3M3T block for both the original signals and the randomized signals (Fig. 3). The CVs for randomized signals fell almost entirely into just one bin; those for the actual data were all higher than any value obtained from any randomized channel, and covered a range of values (Table 1). A paired-sample t test showed the difference to be significant (P < 5 × 10−69).

Fig. S6.

Fig. S6.

Same analyses as in Fig. 2, but for phase-randomized control signals. (A and D) Spectrograms averaged over 27 trials, with power coded as in Fig. 1. (B and E) Time course of average power in the 13–30 Hz pass band (black lines) with 95% confidence limits (gray shading). (C and F) Smoothed power traces for each of the 27 trials.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Distributions of the CV of beta-band power across all channels in all sessions of both monkeys (blue), and across their phase-randomized counterparts (red). Owing to the extreme disparity in the shapes of the two distributions, a different vertical scale is used for each one.

Table 1.

CV for each channel and its matching randomized signal (n = 197)

Data type Minimum Maximum Mean SD
Actual data 0.520 0.785 0.583 0.0395
Randomized data 0.512 0.513 0.512 0.000297

Beta Bursts Come in All Shapes and Sizes.

We marked bursts in each channel whenever power in the beta band met or exceeded a threshold of 3 times the median power for that channel (Fig. 4). All surrounding samples for which power was at least 1.5 times the median were considered part of the burst. We recorded the duration, maximum power, and mean power of each identified burst. Because maximum power and mean power were highly correlated (Pearson’s rho = 0.93), we pursued analyses of mean power and duration only. As shown in Fig. 5, although peaks of the distributions of duration and power were located in the same bin for the randomized and actual data (90–113 ms duration, 2.4–2.55 times the median power; see also “Burst Marking and Burst Parameter Distributions” in SI Materials and Methods), the tails of the distributions were radically different. The burst parameters clustered much more tightly around the peak in the randomized data than in the actual data. We verified that it was reasonable to aggregate bursts across monkeys, regions, tasks, and task periods by plotting each combination separately (Fig. 5C and Fig. S7). The contour lines indicate that the apparent difference in tail size was due mainly to the different sizes of the datasets.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Procedure for measuring beta bursts illustrated for LFP recorded in the motor cortex in a single trial. Events are shown as in Fig. 1. (A, Upper) Raw LFP (blue) and beta bandpass-filtered LFP (green) superimposed. (Lower) Beta-band power together with red horizontal lines at the two thresholds used (1.5 times and 3 times the median power, respectively). Shading indicates individual burst periods. (B) Same data as in A on an expanded time scale.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Joint distribution of duration and power values across all marked beta bursts in the dataset. Contour lines show the boundaries of the tails containing one-half (black), one-quarter (gray), one-eighth (red), and one-sixteenth (blue) of the counts. Straight black lines show least squares fits. (A) Phase-randomized control channels. (B) Actual (nonrandomized) LFPs. (C) Same data as in B, but plotted separately for monkey and brain region.

Fig. S7.

Fig. S7.

Further breakdown of the data shown in Fig. 5 into each combination of monkey, task, brain region, and task period.

The Proportions of High Values of Burst Power and Duration Depend on Task Time.

During the cue period, the distributions of burst parameters in real and randomized data appeared similar (Fig. 6 A and C and Figs. S8 and S9), but extremely high values were more common in the real data (Fig. 6 B and D). Such extreme values were particularly common during the postmovement and ITI periods (Fig. 6 EL), so that the peaks of the real distributions fell below the peaks of the randomized distributions. Strikingly, the extreme values for motor cortex and striatum effectively traded places between the postmovement period and the ITI (Fig. S8); the largest tails in the motor cortex occurred postmovement, whereas in the striatum they were maximum during the ITI.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Histograms showing the distributions of duration and power of bursts with peaks detected during the cue period (A–D), postmovement period (E–H), and ITI (I–L). Data were aggregated across both tasks and both monkeys for the motor cortex (blue) and striatum (green). Plots are paired, with the peaks of the distributions on a fine scale on the left and the logarithms of the distributions on a coarse scale on the right. The first two columns show durations, and the last two show power averaged over the whole burst. Results aggregated across all task periods are also shown for the phase-randomized counterparts of the signals from motor cortex (red) and striatum (gold).

Fig. S8.

Fig. S8.

Results from Fig. 6, replotted to facilitate comparison of beta bursts between the postmovement period and the ITI.

Fig. S9.

Fig. S9.

Same analyses as in Fig. 6, but broken down by task and by session. Each session is shown as a single curve. Sessions recorded from the same implant share the same color, as indicated in the legend. The first two characters of each implant ID designate the monkey.

Modulations of Trial-Averaged Beta Power Primarily Reflect Modulations of Burst Density.

The motor cortex and the striatum exhibited the same reversal between postmovement and ITI in burst density that they showed for burst power and burst duration (Fig. 7); cortical burst density was higher during postmovement, and striatal burst density was higher during ITI. Most of the variations in power were expressed by variations in burst density. The one notable exception was the sharp peak in motor cortex power after termination of the final movement, which was absent or much reduced in the burst density traces. To account for this feature of the trial-averaged power, a change in other burst parameters must be invoked, for example, a brief increase in burst power or a decrease in variability of timing of the first burst after movement termination.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Burst density (A) and beta-band power (13–30 Hz; B) averaged over trials for two timing patterns in each task. Breaks in traces mark the boundaries between panels that were aligned to different task events. Event markers are colored as in Fig. 1. Shading shows ±2 SEM.

Discussion

Our findings demonstrate that beta-band events in LFPs occur predominantly in brief bursts both in the motor-premotor cortex and in the striatum of monkeys performing self-timed movement tasks, and show that the probability of bursting in both regions is heightened after completion of the ongoing task. The brevity of many of the beta bursts is striking, comprising only one to several cycles of beta. Even during task periods in which beta bursts were most likely to occur, the chance of seeing a burst never exceeded 55% (Fig. 7 and Table S1). Thus, beta synchronization and desynchronization are quantum-mechanics-like probabilistic states, reflecting the probability of much shorter bursts of oscillation rather than the strength of sustained oscillations. The task-selective modulation of these probabilistic states suggests a functional import for the oscillations that must be realized during their short lifetimes.

Table S1.

Maximum burst densities by task type

Region 3M3T 1M1T
Long-long-long Short-short-short Long Short
Motor cortex 0.4344 0.5470 0.3458 0.3234
Striatum 0.4438 0.4658 0.3736 0.379

Notably, we found that the timing of the episodes of posttask beta bursting were largely different in the two regions. In the motor-premotor cortex, peak beta-burst probabilities occurred in the immediate postmovement period, corresponding to the time classically referred to as postmovement resynchronization. In the striatum, although there were some sites, especially in the putamen, at which beta bursts were prominent in the postmovement period, for the most part the maximum beta bursting occurred later, during the ITI that followed reward and task end. Thus, postperformance beta oscillations in the motor cortical region appear to be related to movement completion, as indicated in classic studies, but striatal beta oscillations on average mark task end twice as strongly as movement completion. This late striatal postperformance beta-band activity accords with the occurrence of brief spike activity peaks recorded in the striatum and prefrontal cortex of both monkeys and rodents after task completion (1620).

Our work emphasizes the fast dynamics of beta activity. Early studies in humans and nonhuman primates prominently featured such brief bursts of beta oscillations (1214), but the majority of later studies have concentrated on trial-averaged views of beta modulation, obscuring the very fast dynamics visible in individual trials. The terms “event-related synchronization,” “movement-related desynchronization,” and “resynchronization” tend to foster the misapprehension that beta oscillations are persistently present or suppressed, when in fact beta power in between bursts in any given trial is low during much of the synchronization, and beta power during bursts can still be relatively high during desynchronization. A recent study that recognized the importance of a fast time scale is that of Cagnan et al. (21), who examined the correlation between beta amplitude and the angle of beta-band phase-locking between the human subthalamic nucleus and globus pallidus during phase-locking events as brief as 50 ms. Similarly brief bursts of gamma oscillation have been found in rodent studies of phase-amplitude coupling (2224) and movement initiation (25).

Here we focused on the postperformance period, following up on studies of the rodent basal ganglia in which accentuated postperformance beta bursting was documented (15, 26). Numerous studies in nonhuman primates have demonstrated significant task-related effects in the low beta band (4, 5, 2729), but except for the elegant studies of Tan and colleagues (30, 31), oscillation during the postmovement period has not been analyzed in detail. Brief episodes of heightened spike activity at task end have been documented in both nonhuman primates and rodents (1620, 32). It has been suggested that the end-signaling could be part of a mechanism to tag completion of successful sequences of behavior, providing action boundaries to facilitate the expression of such action sequences (16, 17). In the ventromedial striatum, such task-end spike and beta-burst activities were found to develop with behavioral learning (26), further suggesting that the LFP beta-burst activity could reflect local neuronal activity patterns and plasticity.

Maintenance of the status quo (33), error evaluation and motor adaptation (30, 31), and task difficulty (12, 13) are among the interpretations given to bouts of heightened beta-band activity in normal subjects. Here we suggest that beta bursts could have different functions in different regions of the brain. For the motor cortex and striatum of monkeys, the critical context that defined the end of the task differed in a way that seemed to match the functional specializations of the two regions: the motor cortical areas for the execution of the movements, and the striatum for modulation of task performance based on cost-benefit analysis through reinforcement-based learning. By extension, in many regions of the central nervous system, a substantial fraction of the beta bursts that follow task completion could be related to modifying (in error trials) or to maintaining (in correct trials) the strengths of the connections involved in task performance. The details of timing and selectivity for error or correct performance could vary from region to region.

Potential Implications for Beta Activity in Parkinson’s Disease.

Beta-band oscillations are best known as antimovement neural signals, exaggerated in patients with Parkinson’s disease, and are the targets of therapeutic interventions to minimize their power. We note several features of this pathological beta rhythm compared with characteristics of healthy beta rhythm that we describe here. First, we found that on a trial-by-trial basis, the timing of bursts of beta power was highly variable in normal subjects repetitively performing a given task. In comparison, in Parkinson’s patients off medication, Little et al. (34) reported that patients with the most severe motor symptoms had extremely low variation in power in the beta band, in some cases as low as those for our randomized signals. Second, we found that beta bursts were mainly brief, but Parkinson’s patients off medication have been shown to exhibit an abnormally large proportion of “long” episodes (>100 ms) of beta-band phase-locking at phases that promote higher beta power (21). Abnormal prolongation of beta oscillation could be due in part to an abnormal thalamocortico-basal ganglia network resonance of the kind found by Moran et al. (35) in 6-hydroxydopamine model parkinsonian rats. In contrast, the durations of the beta bursts that we describe accord with the durations of beta oscillations evoked in healthy human subjects by the experimental delivery of a single transcranial magnetic stimulation pulse to parietal cortex, circa one or two cycles of oscillation occurring over widespread cortical regions (36).

Based on these findings, we suggest that the brevity of beta bursts could be critical to normal beta-band function, and that the pathological prolongation of beta bursts could, in particular, be critical to the parkinsonian state. If postperformance beta bursts represent brief messages regarding the retrospective evaluation of task performance, and potentially affecting the next task performance, then a pathologically prolonged postperformance beta episode could lead to repetition of the same message many times, with a range of possible consequences. The exaggerated beta-band oscillations might simply crowd out the transmission of other messages. Because the durations of normal beta bursts, on the order of 50–150 ms, comport with time windows relevant to neuroplasticity and other features of neurotransmission, the bursts also could lead neural circuits to fall into fixed states, resulting in freezing or perseveration, states that are, like beta activity, decreased by dopamine replacement therapy (37). Thus, the indiscriminately high postperformance beta-band activity in Parkinson’s patients could limit future choices for action by directly interfering with the normal adaptive mechanisms facilitated by postperformance beta bursts.

Materials and Methods

All methods were approved by the Committee on Animal Care at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Recordings were made in two female rhesus macaque monkeys (designated HH and JB) fitted with a titanium head post and a Delrin recording chamber in which electrodes were implanted for chronic recording, Recording sites were analyzed postmortem by histological analysis, as described in detail by Feingold et al. (38).

The monkeys, on a food- or water-restricted diet, were trained to manipulate a joystick with the right hand in a series of center-out-center movements, the details of which were specified in each trial by an array of visual cues. On correct completion of the instructed movements, the monkeys received liquefied food reward. Trials of a particular task type were grouped together in continuous blocks, with a short block of 1M1T at the beginning, followed by a long block of 3M3T, and then another short block of 1M1T. Monkeys were overtrained extensively on all variations of the task before recording began. We analyzed six sessions from monkey HH and seven sessions from monkey JB.

Further details are available in SI Materials and Methods and Table S2.

Table S2.

Numbers of trials and electrodes analyzed for each session

Trial type Number of electrodes
3M3T 1M1T
Implant Session Correct Error Correct Error CN Putamen Motor cortex
HH1006 HH020207 638 280 509 112 11 8 7
HH032207 669 176 510 26 13 10 0
HH0407 HH081707 862 156 510 62 7 4 6
HH090607 864 191 509 23 13 8 7
HH092107 864 219 510 45 14 9 7
HH092807 864 157 508 76 15 7 7
JB1104 JB010905 542 461 394 79 8 0 3
JB052705 809 434 1037 224 11 1 2
JB082305 674 333 250 34 14 1 3
JB1005 JB020606 671 680 506 179 0 3 3
JB031706 657 1307 440 146 0 6 3
JB051106 671 579 128 39 0 2 2
JB052606 652 932 511 238 0 0 2

Error trials were not included in any of the analyses described in this study, but are shown in this table as an indication of each monkey’s task performance.

SI Materials and Methods

Behavioral Task and LFP Recording.

The monkey sat inside a sound-attenuating booth in a primate chair with its head fixed, grasping a joystick with its right hand. To start a trial, the monkey had to hold the joystick in the center position. An array of eight unfilled red circular cues (4 degrees visual angle in diameter) was then presented at 9 degrees from a slightly larger center cue (5 degrees diameter). After a random delay of 0.5–1.1 s, a set of colored cues appeared to instruct the monkey as to the movement sequence required. The directions, order, and timing of the movements were indicated by the positions, colors, and shapes of the cues, respectively (Fig. 1).

In the 1M1T task, a single red peripheral target was presented along with the yellow center target, and the monkey had to hold a joystick-controlled cursor on the center target for a long or short duration before moving the cursor to the peripheral target and back again. The monkey had to time the duration of the hold itself, as instructed by the center cue; a filled disk indicated a short (0.6–1.2 s) hold, and an annulus indicated a long (1.4–2.0 s) hold. In the 3M3T task, the requirements for the first movement were the same as in the 1M1T task, but an additional two movements specified by green (second movement) and blue (third movement) peripheral targets were required as well. The required timing of each central-cue hold period between movements was indicated by the shape (solid or annulus) of the peripheral cue that had just been hit (same coding as the center hold cue), and all eight patterns of short and long holds were used. The monkeys were never prompted to move by a “go” cue, and thus had to estimate the proper amount of time to wait before initiating each movement. A subset of all of the possible spatial and timing patterns was chosen for use on a given day, so that the same pattern could be repeated in multiple trials. After the last movement, the monkey had to hold the joystick steady for a variable (1.2–1.8 s) delay for delivery of reward. The screen then went black, and a 3-s long ITI period began, during which the screen remained black and the monkey was not required to do anything. Following detection of a behavioral error, the screen went black, and the 3-s long ITI period began. Sucking and swallowing typically commenced in anticipation of reward delivery and continued throughout the ITI and the following cue period.

Electrodes were implanted in the left hemisphere at locations chosen based on physiological mapping, MRI, and the standard atlas of Paxinos et al. (39). Recording sites in the putamen included the zone of termination of primary motor cortex fibers reported by Kunzle (40), corresponding roughly to interaural 10–20 mm (39). LFPs were recorded with respect to an electrode in contact with saline covering the dura mater. Signals were low-pass filtered at 475 Hz and recorded continuously at a nominal 2-kHz sampling rate using the Cheetah Data Acquisition System (Neuralynx). Offline, LFPs were digitally low-pass filtered at 100 Hz (two passes of four-pole Butterworth in forward and reverse time to yield zero phase shift) and down-sampled to 220 Hz for analysis.

The numbers of correct and error trials and the numbers of electrodes in each region for each session are shown in Table S2. The placements of the electrodes in the mediolateral/anteroposterior plane in each implant are shown in Fig. S1, with the interaural line serving as the anteroposterior zero point. For two sessions (HH098207 and JB082305), “reference-free” signals (14) were computed as a control for volume conduction effects as follows. Spatially limited groups of electrodes were defined within each brain region (Fig. S1), and the signals on all electrodes within each group were averaged separately. Each group's average signal was then subtracted from the signal on each individual electrode to yield a set of local-average-referenced signals.

Task Time Reconstruction from Multiple Alignment Events.

Given that most of the relevant task events were timed by the monkey, task event timing was variable from trial to trial. To illustrate results in relation to all of the task events, we performed each analysis aligned with respect to each of the main task events, and then assembled the data by setting the spacing between consecutive alignment events equal to the median interevent interval for that pair of events, and then splicing together the results from different alignments at the midpoint of each interevent interval.

Spectral Analysis.

All spectra were computed from 1-s data windows using a single taper. The taper function was the first Slepian (i.e., discrete prolate spheroidal) sequence with time half-bandwidth product (also known as “nw”) equal to 1.8. This taper is similar in shape to both the Hanning window and the Hamming window.

1/f1.5 Spectrum Normalization.

A baseline spectrum was computed for each channel in each session by averaging over every 1-s window that could be accommodated by each trial in both 1M1T blocks, averaging over every 1-s window in each trial in the 3M3T block, and finally averaging the 1M1T and 3M3T spectra. An analytic pink noise spectrum of the form

a*f^(−3/2),

where a is a constant and f is frequency, was then fitted for each channel using the formula

log10(a)=mean(log10(pwr./f^(3/2))),

where pwr denotes the power in the actual baseline spectrum. All spectra shown were normalized to the fitted pink noise spectrum.

Screening of Session Average Spectrograms Before Aggregating.

At each level of aggregation of data (i.e., across sessions, across implants, and across monkeys), we verified that the spectrograms to be averaged included features common to the individual datasets, and that the average was representative of those common features. However, this criterion was not necessarily the case across all individual channels that contributed to each session average. We occasionally noticed substantial differences between different channels in the same region and same session. Such variability merits further study.

Bandpass Power.

Beta-band power was computed by filtering the LFP with a 13–30 Hz fourth-order Butterworth bandpass filter in forward time and again in backward time, squaring the value at each sample, and then smoothing with a Hanning window 35 samples wide (i.e., half-width equal to one cycle of 13 Hz).

Burst Marking and Burst Parameter Distributions.

The median beta power value used in our burst marking procedure was computed from the entirety of the 3M3T block when marking bursts in the 3M3T block, and from the combination of both 1M1T blocks when marking bursts in them. This procedure was followed as a computational convenience. To check whether this protocol introduced some bias into the burst parameter measurements, we performed a paired t test on the 1M1T medians against the 3M3T medians, and found no significant difference between them (P = 0.55). Furthermore, the sign of the mean difference between 1M1T and 3M3T median power values (whose magnitude was only 0.04 of the SD) indicated that if there were any trend, it was for 1M1T median power to be smaller than 3M3T median power. We would expect this trend either to be neutral with respect to burst counts or to bias the burst counts toward higher values for 1M1T than for 3M3T, which is the opposite of what we observed.

The fact that the peaks of the distributions moved so little across different task conditions, brain regions, and monkeys (Fig. 6 and Fig. S7) could be related in part to our threshold-based method for defining bursts. The theoretical minimum value for duration is determined by the response of our bandpass filter and the shape of the Hanning window that we used to smooth the power traces. Duration can never be <90 ms, and although the peak of the distribution is not yet reached in the first bin that is entirely beyond that value, it is always reached within three or four bins. Average power can asymptotically approach 1.5 times the median as the burst becomes arbitrarily long, but the average power for a minimal duration burst as determined by the smoothing kernel would be 2.4 times the median, and the distribution of power values does in fact reach its maximum in the 2.4–2.5 bin or in the 2.5–2.6 bin. Based on these considerations, we can conclude that the most common burst duration could be shorter than the shortest duration that we could measure, and likely determined the most common power that we did measure.

Randomization of Signals.

For each channel, the entire 3M3T block was Fourier-transformed, and the magnitude of the transform was computed. The phase at each frequency from zero to the Nyquist frequency was randomly assigned from a uniform distribution ranging from 0 to 2*pi. Complex spectral values above the Nyquist frequency were assigned to be mirror-image conjugate-symmetric to the phase-randomized values below the Nyquist frequency. Finally, the spectrum was inverse–Fourier-transformed back to a time series of the same length as the original. The accuracy of the computation was examined by verifying that none of the values in the final time series had an imaginary component.

Plotting Contour Lines in Fig. 5.

First, the counts in all bins in the 2D array were sorted by decreasing value, and the cumulative sum of the sorted vector was computed. The last bin in which the cumulative sum was less than (1 − p) was identified, where p is the tail size for any given contour line as stated in the figure, and the contour threshold was calculated as the mean of the number of counts in that bin and the number of counts in the next-smaller bin. The contour was then plotted using standard Matlab functions. Note that these contours are not guaranteed to be single closed curves, as can be seen in several of the more sparsely populated plots in Fig. S7.

Burst Density Calculation.

For each channel, a binary time series was constructed at the same sampling rate as the LFP, and the value 1 was assigned at each time point that belonged to a burst, with 0 values at all other points. The burst density for that channel was then defined simply as the average over trials of the binary time series, aligned to a specified task event. The burst density at each time point thus shows the fraction of trials that contained a burst including that time point, and therefore serves as an estimate of the probability of bursting at that time in the task.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. Naotaka Fujii, Theresa Desrochers, Robert Marini, Moshe Abeles, Christian Brown, Mark Histed, and Yasuo Kubota for their comments and advice, along with current and former laboratory members, including Dr. Naomi Hasegawa, Margo Cantor, Patricia Harlan, Christine Keller-McGandy, and Henry Hall, for their help with technical aspects of this study. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grant R01 NS025529), the Office of Naval Research (Grant N00014-07-1-0903), and the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (Grant NBCHC070105).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1517629112/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Neuper C, Wortz M, Pfurtscheller G. ERD/ERS patterns reflecting sensorimotor activation and deactivation. Prog Brain Res. 2006;159:211–222. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59014-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stancak A, Jr, Pfurtscheller G. Event-related desynchronisation of central beta-rhythms during brisk and slow self-paced finger movements of dominant and nondominant hand. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1996;4(3):171–183. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(96)00031-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Oscillations in local field potentials of the primate motor cortex during voluntary movement. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993;90(10):4470–4474. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.10.4470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rubino D, Robbins KA, Hatsopoulos NG. Propagating waves mediate information transfer in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(12):1549–1557. doi: 10.1038/nn1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Reimer J, Hatsopoulos NG. Periodicity and evoked responses in motor cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30(34):11506–11515. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5947-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brown P, et al. Dopamine dependency of oscillations between subthalamic nucleus and pallidum in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci. 2001;21(3):1033–1038. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-03-01033.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cassidy M, et al. Movement-related changes in synchronization in the human basal ganglia. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 6):1235–1246. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Marsden JF, Limousin-Dowsey P, Ashby P, Pollak P, Brown P. Subthalamic nucleus, sensorimotor cortex and muscle interrelationships in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2001;124(Pt 2):378–388. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.2.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Oswal A, Brown P, Litvak V. Synchronized neural oscillations and the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease. Curr Opin Neurol. 2013;26(6):662–670. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Williams D, et al. Dopamine-dependent changes in the functional connectivity between basal ganglia and cerebral cortex in humans. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 7):1558–1569. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hammond C, Bergman H, Brown P. Pathological synchronization in Parkinson’s disease: networks, models and treatments. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(7):357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Murthy VN, Fetz EE. Coherent 25- to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992;89(12):5670–5674. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Murthy VN, Fetz EE. Oscillatory activity in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys: Synchronization of local field potentials and relation to behavior. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76(6):3949–3967. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pfurtscheller G, Stancak A, Jr, Neuper C. Post-movement beta synchronization: A correlate of an idling motor area? Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;98(4):281–293. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(95)00258-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Leventhal DK, et al. Basal ganglia beta oscillations accompany cue utilization. Neuron. 2012;73(3):523–536. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Barnes T, Kubota Y, Hu D, Jin DZ, Graybiel AM. Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature. 2005;437(7062):1158–1161. doi: 10.1038/nature04053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fujii N, Graybiel A. Representation of action sequence boundaries by macaque prefrontal cortical neurons. Science. 2003;301(5637):1246–1249. doi: 10.1126/science.1086872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Thorn CA, Atallah H, Howe M, Graybiel A. Differential dynamics of activity changes in dorsolateral and dorsomedial striatal loops during learning. Neuron. 2010;66(5):781–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Smith KS, Graybiel AM. A dual operator view of habitual behavior reflecting cortical and striatal dynamics. Neuron. 2013;79(2):361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jin X, Costa RM. Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature. 2010;466(7305):457–462. doi: 10.1038/nature09263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cagnan H, Duff EP, Brown P. The relative phases of basal ganglia activities dynamically shape effective connectivity in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2015;138(Pt 6):1667–1678. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.von Nicolai C, et al. Corticostriatal coordination through coherent phase-amplitude coupling. J Neurosci. 2014;34(17):5938–5948. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5007-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fujisawa S, Buzsaki G. A 4-Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron. 2011;72(1):153–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tort ABL, et al. Dynamic cross-frequency couplings of local field potential oscillations in rat striatum and hippocampus during performance of a T-maze task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(51):20517–20522. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810524105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Masimore B, Schmitzer-Torbert NC, Kakalios J, Redish AD. Transient striatal gamma local field potentials signal movement initiation in rats. Neuroreport. 2005;16(18):2021–2024. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200512190-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Howe MW, Atallah HE, McCool A, Gibson DJ, Graybiel AM. Habit learning is associated with major shifts in frequencies of oscillatory activity and synchronized spike firing in striatum. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(40):16801–16806. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113158108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Antzoulatos EG, Miller EK. Increases in functional connectivity between prefrontal cortex and striatum during category learning. Neuron. 2014;83(1):216–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Buschman TJ, Denovellis EL, Diogo C, Bullock D, Miller EK. Synchronous oscillatory neural ensembles for rules in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2012;76(4):838–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Saalmann YB, Pinsk MA, Wang L, Li X, Kastner S. The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science. 2012;337(6095):753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tan H, Jenkinson N, Brown P. Dynamic neural correlates of motor error monitoring and adaptation during trial-to-trial learning. J Neurosci. 2014;34(16):5678–5688. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4739-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tan H, et al. Human subthalamic nucleus in movement error detection and its evaluation during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci. 2014;34(50):16744–16754. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3414-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Desrochers TM, Amemori K, Graybiel AM. Habit learning by naive macaques is marked by response sharpening of striatal neurons representing the cost and outcome of acquired action sequences. Neuron. 2015;87(4):853–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations: signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20(2):156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Little S, Pogosyan A, Kuhn AA, Brown P. Beta band stability over time correlates with parkinsonian rigidity and bradykinesia. Exp Neurol. 2012;236(2):383–388. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.04.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Moran RJ, et al. Alterations in brain connectivity underlying beta oscillations in parkinsonism. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7(8):e1002124. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Johnson JS, Kundu B, Casali AG, Postle BR. Task-dependent changes in cortical excitability and effective connectivity: A combined TMS-EEG study. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107(9):2383–2392. doi: 10.1152/jn.00707.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rutledge RB, et al. Dopaminergic drugs modulate learning rates and perseveration in Parkinson’s patients in a dynamic foraging task. J Neurosci. 2009;29(48):15104–15114. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3524-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Feingold J, et al. A system for recording neural activity chronically and simultaneously from multiple cortical and subcortical regions in nonhuman primates. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107(7):1979–1995. doi: 10.1152/jn.00625.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Paxinos G, Huang X-F, Toga AW. The Rhesus Monkey Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press; San Diego: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Künzle H. Bilateral projections from precentral motor cortex to the putamen and other parts of the basal ganglia: An autoradiographic study in Macaca fascicularis. Brain Res. 1975;88(2):195–209. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90384-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Courtemanche R, Fujii N, Graybiel A. Synchronous, focally modulated β-band oscillations characterize local field potential activity in the striatum of awake behaving monkeys. J Neurosci. 2003;23(37):11741–11752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-37-11741.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES