Abstract
Low socioeconomic status (SES) children perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from higher SES backgrounds, but the developmental relationship between intelligence and SES has not been adequately investigated. Here, we use latent growth curve (LGC) models to assess associations between SES and individual differences in the intelligence starting point (intercept) and in the rate and direction of change in scores (slope and quadratic term) from infancy through adolescence in 14,853 children from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), assessed 9 times on IQ between the ages of 2 and 16 years. SES was significantly associated with intelligence growth factors: higher SES was related both to a higher starting point in infancy and to greater gains in intelligence over time. Specifically, children from low SES families scored on average 6 IQ points lower at age 2 than children from high SES backgrounds; by age 16, this difference had almost tripled. Although these key results did not vary across girls and boys, we observed gender differences in the development of intelligence in early childhood. Overall, SES was shown to be associated with individual differences in intercepts as well as slopes of intelligence. However, this finding does not warrant causal interpretations of the relationship between SES and the development of intelligence.
Keywords: Intelligence, IQ, Socioeconomic status, Latent growth, Gender
Highlights
-
•
IQ growth trajectories were modeled in British children from age 2 to 16 years.
-
•
Children's socioeconomic background (SES) was associated with IQ growth.
-
•
High and low SES children differed by 6 IQ points at age 2.
-
•
By age 16, this IQ difference between high and low SES children had tripled.
1. Introduction
Individual differences in intelligence influence developmental trajectories across the lifespan, affecting socioeconomic, psychological, and health outcomes (Deary, 2012). Differences in intelligence have been shown to be highly stable from early adolescence to late adulthood (Deary, Pattie, & Starr, 2013), but are more variable in infancy and childhood, with some children showing substantial gains in intelligence and others considerable losses between infancy and adolescence (Bayley, 1955, Feinstein, 2003, Tucker-Drob and Briley, 2014). These variations in the development of intelligence are likely to be associated with children's family socioeconomic status (SES; e.g. Dyume et al., 1999, Heckman, 2006, Tucker-Drob et al., 2011). Children from disadvantaged family backgrounds score on average lower on intelligence tests than their high SES peers (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002, Schoon et al., 2012, Strenze, 2007), and their performance has been suggested to worsen over time, even if they did relatively well in early assessments (Feinstein, 2003). Conversely, high SES children are thought to gain in intelligence over time, even if they initially had a lower test score (Feinstein, 2003). However, research to date on the impact of SES on developmental change in intelligence is inconclusive for two reasons.
First, it has been suggested that the previously reported association between SES and children's IQ development results from regression to the mean, because children with either extremely high or low scores in early IQ tests are less likely to score as extremely in later tests, independent of their family background (Jerrim and Vignoles, 2011, Saunders, 2012). Regression to the mean occurs when children are grouped according to their scores at one measurement occasion and then the groups' development is analyzed across subsequent assessments. This statistical artifact can be avoided by applying latent growth curve (LGC) models to non-selected samples, because LGC analyzes longitudinal data at the level of individuals rather than groups.
Second, most previous studies included intelligence assessments at relatively few ages and at short age intervals in early life (Feinstein, 2003, Spinath et al., 2003, von Stumm, 2012; see Tucker-Drob & Briley, 2014, for a review). No study to date has modeled change in intelligence in a representative sample from infancy through late adolescence, using multiple assessments of intelligence over time that allow for identifying individual differences in developmental trajectories. Overcoming these limitations, in the present study we fitted LGC models to intelligence data from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), whose participants were assessed 9 times on intelligence from age 2 to 16 years. We then tested the extent to which SES, as a time-invariant covariate, accounted for individual differences in slopes of change in intelligence from age 2 to 16 years, as well as individual differences in the starting points (intercepts) at the age of 2 years.
2. Methods
2.1. Sample
The Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) recruited families of twins born in England and Wales in 1994, 1995, and 1996 (Haworth, Davis, & Plomin, 2013). Since then, the sample has remained representative of the UK population (Kovas, Haworth, Dale, & Plomin, 2007). We excluded twins who suffered from severe medical problems currently or at birth (e.g., post-natal surgery); whose mothers reported severe medical problems during pregnancy; whose first language was not English; and who had been assessed less than twice on intelligence between the ages of 2 and 16 years. The final analysis sample comprised of 14,853 twins; that is, 7426 complete pairs, including 2564 monozygotic pairs and 4862 dizygotic twin pairs, of which 2375 were of opposite sex. Overall, the sample included 7768 girls and 7085 boys.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Socioeconomic status (SES)
Parental education and occupation (mother's and father's highest educational qualification and job status) were recorded at the first contact with the families, when twins were 18 months old, and again when they were 7 years old. Family income was assessed when the twins were 9 years old. A composite of parental education and occupation at twins' age of 18 months correlated at .77 with a composite of parental education and occupation at twins' age of 7 years, which in turn correlated at .57 with family income at twins' age of 9 years, suggesting that SES was relatively stable over time in TEDS (Hanscombe et al., 2012). Data from the assessment at 18 months were used in cases where information at age 7 was missing; for all others, records of parental education and occupation at age 7 were employed. Summary SES composites were created as a unit-weighted sum of the education, occupation, and income after mapping to a standard normal distribution with the rank-based van der Waerden transformation (Hanscombe et al., 2012).
2.2.2. Intelligence
The twins were assessed at 2, 3, 4, 7, 9, 10, 12, 14 and 16 years on intelligence, using parent-administered tests and ratings of ability at ages 2, 3, and 4, and a mixture of web-based, telephone-based, and parent-administered tests at later ages. At each testing age, twins completed at least two state-of-the-art ability tests. The tests have been described in detail elsewhere (Hanscombe et al., 2012) and are only briefly reviewed here.
2.2.2.1. Measures at ages 2, 3, and 4
Parent-administered tests and parent-reported observations were used to assess verbal and nonverbal cognitive abilities. These measures have been validated against standard tests administered by trained testers (Oliver et al., 2002, Saudino et al., 1998). Specifically, nonverbal cognitive performance was assessed using age-appropriate versions of the Parent Report of Children's Abilities (PARCA; Oliver et al., 2002, Saudino et al., 1998), while verbal ability measures included vocabulary and grammar as assessed by parent reports for the CDI–III, an extension of the short form of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Words and Sentences (Fenson et al., 2000).
2.2.2.2. Measures at age 7
Children were tested on verbal and nonverbal abilities by telephone (Petrill, Rempell, Oliver, & Plomin, 2002). Prior to the telephone call, parents were sent a booklet of test items along with testing instructions. The booklet contained two tests of verbal tests: the Similarities subtest and the Vocabulary subtest from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III-UK; Wechsler, 1992), and the Picture Completion subtest from the WISC-III-UK and Conceptual Grouping from the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (McCarthy, 1972).
2.2.2.3. Measures at age 9
Participants were mailed a test booklet with two verbal and two nonverbal tests to be administered under the supervision of the parent, who had received a corresponding instruction booklet. The verbal tests comprised vocabulary and general knowledge tests adapted from the multiple-choice version of the WISC-III-UK (Wechsler, 1992). The nonverbal tests included a Puzzle test adapted from the Figure Classification subtest of the Cognitive Abilities Test 3 (CAT3; Smith, Fernandes, & Strand, 2001) and a Shapes test also adapted from the CAT3 Figure Analogies subtest (Davis, Arden, & Plomin, 2008).
2.2.2.4. Measures at age 10
Testing was web-based, and children completed two verbal and two non-verbal tests using their home computers (Haworth et al., 2007). Tests were drawn from the WISC-III-PI, including Multiple Choice Information (General Knowledge), Vocabulary Multiple Choice, and Picture Completion (Wechsler, 1992), and from Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1996).
2.2.2.5. Measures at age 12
Testing was web-based and conducted at home computers, using age-matched versions of the tests previously used at age 10, including again two verbal and two non-verbal ability tests (Kaplan et al., 1998, Raven et al., 1996, Wechsler, 1992).
2.2.2.6. Measures at age 14
Twins completed two web-based tests at their home computers, including the WISC-III-PI Vocabulary Multiple Choice for 14-year olds (Kaplan et al., 1998), and Raven's Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., 1996).
2.2.2.7. Measures at age 16
Twins completed web-based adaptations of Raven's Standard and Advanced Progressive and the Mill-Hill Vocabulary Scale using their home computers (Raven et al., 1998, Raven et al., 1996).
3. Analysis
3.1. Latent growth curve models
In a first step, first principal factors were extracted at each age from the administered intelligence tests. Regression factor scores were transformed into standardized IQ scores with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 (Hanscombe et al., 2012). The comparability of IQ scores is here theoretically inferred, because intelligence was assessed at each measurement occasion with multiple, well validated tests that should have assessed identical constructs, yielding invariant common variance factors of intelligence, even if different tests were administered at different times and ages. Previously, multiple first factors extracted from cognitive test batteries were shown to be invariant in adults (Johnson et al., 2004, Johnson et al., 2008), although the invariance of such factors in children or over the course of time has not been established.
In a second step, latent growth curve (LGC) models were fitted to two subsamples that each consisted of one twin randomly selected from a pair (sample 1 with N = 7440, and sample 2 with N = 7413). This method enabled a replication of the LGC model across the two samples, and it also ensured that model fit statistics were not erroneously inflated because of the dependence of observations (i.e. relatedness of twins). LGC factors are extracted from repeatedly observed intelligence factor scores and describe a sample's average starting point, typically referred to as intercept, and systematic changes that occur over time, which are typically known as slope (McArdle, 2009). LGC factors were modeled to freely correlate. To determine the number of LGC factors that best represented the data, the fit (i.e. χ2(df)) of a LGC model with two growth parameters (intercept and slope) was compared to the fit of a model with three growth parameters (intercept, slope and quadratic term). That is, the slope represents linear changes, while the quadratic growth parameter assesses systematic non-linear accelerations or decelerations of the growth trend (i.e. systematic curvilinear change not accounted for by the slope). At each age, loadings on the intercept were fixed to 1, and those on the slope were defined as 0, 1, 2, 5, 7, 8, 10, 12, and 14, representing time periods in years between each assessment point, ranging in real time from 2 to 16 years. With that, the intercept was defined where the slope had a zero loading (i.e. at age 2). Loadings on the quadratic term were the square of the slope loadings. Both 2- and 3-factor LGC models were specified as multi-group models to test for measurement invariance across the two random twin subsamples. Here, the fit of an ‘unrestricted’ baseline multi-group LGC model was compared to a restricted model that held means, intercepts and residuals equal across groups.1
Next, multi-group LGC models were fitted separately to samples of boys and girls investigate if LGC factors differed across gender. Accordingly, the fit of a baseline model, which was ‘unrestricted’ besides pre-defined factor loadings1, was compared to the fit of a ‘restricted’ model that held intercepts, means and residuals equal across boys and girls. Finally, SES was added to the LGC model and specified as a time-invariant covariate of the growth parameters in order to investigate the extent to which LGC factors differed as a function of SES.
All models were fitted using full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) assuming data missing at random and no biases of the results (Arbuckle, 1996). Several fit indices evaluated the LGC models' fit, including the model χ2 test, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), and the RMSEA with Confidence Intervals of 95% (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
4. Results
4.1. Correlations
Across ages, intelligence scores were positively inter-correlated in a simplex pattern, with stronger associations between more proximate assessments, in line with previous studies of different samples (e.g., Bartels, Rietveld, van Baal, & Boomsma, 2002; Table 1). SES correlated positively with intelligence at all ages, and increasingly so, as the children grew older, which is also in line with previous research (e.g. Tucker-Drob et al., 2011).
Table 1.
N | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | IQ at 2 | 4730 | – | ||||||||
2 | IQ at 3 | 4522 | .66 | – | |||||||
3 | IQ at 4 | 5725 | .57 | .70 | – | ||||||
4 | IQ at 7 | 4620 | .23 | .31 | .31 | – | |||||
5 | IQ at 9 | 3059 | .26 | .35 | .33 | .41 | – | ||||
6 | IQ at 10 | 2475 | .23 | .31 | .27 | .40 | .57 | – | |||
7 | IQ at 12 | 3981 | .18 | .27 | .29 | .44 | .56 | .63 | – | ||
8 | IQ at 14 | 2599 | .21 | .26 | .24 | .40 | .46 | .51 | .63 | – | |
9 | IQ at 16 | 2224 | .21 | .26 | .22 | .42 | .45 | .50 | .58 | .64 | – |
10 | SES | 6884 | .10 | .17 | .17 | .32 | .30 | .26 | .30 | .36 | .35 |
Note. Correlations were computed after pairwise deletion in a subsample of one randomly selected twin per pair.
The correlations of intelligence scores over time in TEDS ranged from .21 to .70 with an average value of .40, which may appear low compared to other research (e.g. Deary et al., 2013). However, intelligence is more variable in childhood than in later life (e.g. Bayley, 1955); also, intelligence assessment methods in TEDS varied considerably across time (see Discussion for details).
4.2. Latent growth curve models
A two-factor latent growth curve model fitted the data worse (sample 1 with N = 7440: χ2 (40) = 1090.35; sample 2 with N = 7413: χ2 (40) = 1141.62) than the three-factor model in both twin subsamples (sample 1: χ2 (36) = 553.67; sample 2: χ2 (36) = 602.31). In multi-group models across two samples of one twin randomly selected from a pair, LGC model χ2 values did not differ significantly. Thus, individual differences in intelligence from age 2 to 16 years were here best explained by an intercept (average starting point), and factors of linear (slope) and non-linear (quadratic term) change. All three growth factors accounted for significant variance, suggesting individual differences in intercept and in linear and non-linear change.
In the multi-group models for gender, the fit of unrestricted models differed significantly from the fit of models that held intercepts, means and residuals equal (p < .001; Table 2). In other words, boys and girls differed in their developmental trajectories of intelligence: girls started with an advantage of about 5 IQ points at the age of 2 years compared to boys. However due to different values for slope and quadratic term in boys and girls (Table 2), the gender difference in IQ development had mostly disappeared by the age of 16 years (Fig. 1). Specifically, the slope was negative in girls, while the quadratic term was positive. As a result, girls had on average a higher IQ starting point but showed decline thereafter, which was somewhat absorbed by the quadratic term. By comparison, boys' slope was positive and the quadratic negative: thus, they improved from their low average IQ starting point but the negative quadratic term dampened and even reversed the IQ growth trend over time.
Table 2.
Boys 1 | Girls 1 | Boys 2 | Girls 2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N | 3549 | 3891 | 3536 | 3877 |
χ2(36) | 361.27 | 298.94 | 402.45 | 285.94 |
Intercept | 97.17 | 103.13 | 97.33 | 102.93 |
SEI | 0.27 | 0.26 | 0.27 | 0.25 |
VarianceI | 163.52 | 162.21 | 164.25 | 159.42 |
CI (95%)I | 96.64 to 97.70 | 102.63 to 103.63 | 96.80 to 97.86 | 102.43 to 103.42 |
Slope | 0.83 | − 0.79 | 0.80 | − 0.80 |
SES | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.08 |
VarianceS | 7.44 | 7.55 | 8.06 | 7.25 |
CI (95%)S | 0.66 to 0.99 | − 0.94 to − 0.64 | 0.64 to 0.97 | − 0.95 to − 0.64 |
Quadratic | − 0.06 | 0.03 | − 0.05 | 0.03 |
SEQ | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.01 |
VarianceQ | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.02 |
CI (95%)Q | − 0.07 to − 0.04 | 0.02 to 0.05 | − 0.06 to − 0.04 | 0.02 to 0.04 |
Note. Fit indices for the multi-group model in sample 1 were: CFI = .947; TLI .947; and RMSEA = .047 (CI 95% .044 to .050). Fit indices for the multi-group model in sample 2 were: CFI = .944; TLI = .944; and RMSEA = .048 (CI 95% .045 to .051). SE refers to Standard Error; CI 95% refers to Confidence Interval of 95%; the subscripts I, S, and Q refer to intercept, slope and quadratic term respectively.
4.3. Associations between SES and latent growth curve factors
SES was a significant predictor of all three latent growth factors in boys and girls across two subsamples of twins (p < .001 in all cases, Table 3). The χ2 of a model that held means, intercepts, and residuals equal across groups did not differ significantly from the χ2 associated with a model that also constrained the SES regression parameters to be equal across groups (p > .05 in all cases). Thus, the association between SES and intelligence latent growth factors did not vary as a function of gender or twin subsample.
Table 3.
Bi | SEi | βi | Bs | SEs | βs | Bq | SEq | βq | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boys 1 | 2.19 | .39 | .12 | 0.98 | .12 | .26 | − 0.05 | .01 | − .21 |
Girls 1 | 2.73 | .37 | .15 | 0.81 | .11 | .21 | − 0.03 | .01 | − .16 |
Boys 2 | 2.05 | .39 | .11 | 0.95 | .12 | .24 | − 0.04 | .01 | − .19 |
Girls 2 | 2.70 | .37 | .15 | 0.75 | .11 | .20 | − 0.03 | .01 | − .15 |
Note. B refers to the unstandardized regression estimate; SE is the Standard Error; and β is the standardized regression coefficient. Subscripts denote the latent growth factor that values refer to (i = intercept; s = slope; q = quadratic term). All regression coefficients are significant at p < .001.
SES was positively associated with the intercept for intelligence with coefficients ranging from .11 to .15, suggesting that children from more advantaged SES backgrounds had higher intelligence scores in infancy. SES was also positively related to the slope with coefficients ranging from .20 to .26, which indicates that children from more advantaged SES backgrounds also experienced greater linear gains from age 2 to 16 years. Associations between SES and the quadratic term were negative, with coefficients ranging from − .21 to − .15, which in combination with the observed gender differences in latent growth factors resulted in differently shaped growth curves. In boys, the negative association of SES with the negative quadratic term implied a greater steepness of the growth curves as SES increased. Conversely in girls, the negative association between SES and a positive quadratic term resulted in flatter growth curves with higher SES. Fig. 2 illustrates the relationship between SES and latent growth in intelligence for boys and girls from low (<− 1 SD), medium (±1 SD) and high (> 1 SD) SES families. Both boys and girls from low SES backgrounds scored on average about 6 IQ points lower at age 2 than children from high SES family backgrounds. By age 16, this discrepancy had multiplied: low SES boys scored on average 15 IQ points less than high SES boys, and in girls this difference amounted to approximately 17 IQ points.
5. Discussion
Our results suggest that family socioeconomic status (SES) impacts children's development of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Children of the highest and lowest SES backgrounds were on average separated by 6 IQ points at the age of 2 years. By the age of 16, the IQ gap had almost tripled (Fig. 2). Thus, children from more disadvantaged families not only did worse than their peers in early intelligence tests but their intelligence handicap amplified over time, suggesting a long-term agglomeration of SES influences on cognitive development. We want to emphasize here that these SES influences comprise not only environmental variance but also their association with children's cognitive growth is likely to be partially mediated by genetic factors (Trzaskowski et al., 2014). However, the current study design only allows for speculating about the mechanisms that potentially underlie the association between SES and IQ growth. It is plausible that children from higher SES families experience greater opportunities for and support in cognitive engagement and learning than children from more disadvantaged homes (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). Differences in the availability of learning opportunities, support and resources are thought to accentuate individual differences in cognitive ability (Hayes, 1962, von Stumm, 2012). That said, the precise mechanisms underlying the association between SES and intelligence growth curves are yet to be identified.
We also observed significant gender differences in the IQ starting point and in the growth curves for cognitive development, with girls outperforming boys at the ages of 2, 3, and 4 years. However in later childhood and adolescence, gender differences in cognitive growth diminished and by the age of 16 years, the differences had disappeared. Our findings concur with reports about gender differences in cognitive abilities and in brain anatomy (e.g. Ganjavi et al., 2011, Haier et al., 2005). However to our knowledge, no comparable longitudinal data are available that would allow replicating the pattern of gender differences in cognitive development that we observed here. Furthermore, we found no differences in the association between SES and cognitive development across boys and girls.
5.1. Strengths and limitations
Our study has many notable strengths, including a large sample of twins representative of the UK population (Kovas et al., 2007) with intelligence assessed 9 times between the ages of 2 and 16 years. The main weakness for longitudinal comparisons is that different measures of and assessment methods for intelligence were used as different ages, thus confounding age and methodological differences. For the initial three assessment waves at the twins' ages of 2, 3, and 4 years, tests were parent-administered, but at later ages, children completed phone-based and web-based IQ tests without much parental involvement. These measurement differences may have resulted in our lower than expected correlations between intelligence scores across twins' ages. That said, the correlations observed here are only marginally lower compared to estimates from other samples at comparable ages (Bayley, 1955, Bartels et al., 2002, Lobo and Galloway, 2013). A second limitation is our treatment of family SES as a time-invariant covariate in the analyses, although SES indicators did vary over time in our sample (Hanscombe et al., 2012). Notwithstanding, the stability of SES was here greater than its degree of change with correlations of three SES measurements exceeding .5 over a period of 7 years, suggesting that treating SES as time-invariant covariate was appropriate. A third limitation is that, although our sample consisted of twins, we did not conduct genetic analyses, primarily because SES is a between-family variable. As such, it is not amenable to genetic analysis using the twin method, which relies on within family differences.
6. Conclusions
This study showed that children from lower SES backgrounds tend to perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from more privileged homes as early as at the age of 2 years. Furthermore, SES accentuated these differences throughout childhood and adolescence: the 6-point IQ difference in infancy between children from low and high SES homes almost tripled by the time the children were 16 years old. Our findings confirm changes in intelligence throughout early life and suggest a meaningful relationship between IQ growth and socioeconomic factors.
Acknowledgments
We thank the TEDS twins and their parents who have contributed to the study since the twins were infants. TEDS is supported by the UK Medical Research Council [G0901245; and previously G0500079], with additional support from the US National Institutes of Health [HD044454; HD059215]. RP is supported by a Medical Research Council Research Professorship award [G19/2] and a European Research Council Advanced Investigator award [295366].
Footnotes
Configural measurement invariance is given in LGC multi-group models, because factor loadings are pre-defined to describe intercept (loadings of 1), slope (loadings from 0 to 14) and quadratic term (loadings from 0 to 196).
References
- Arbuckle J.L. Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In: Marcoulides G.A., Schumacker R.E., editors. Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1996. pp. 243–277. [Google Scholar]
- Bartels M., Rietveld M.J., van Baal G.C., Boomsma D.I. Genetic and environmental influences on the development of intelligence. Behavior Genetics. 2002;32:237–249. doi: 10.1023/a:1019772628912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayley N. On the growth of intelligence. American Psychologist. 1955;10:805–818. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley R.H., Corwyn R.F. Socioeconomic status & child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis O.S.P., Arden R., Plomin R. g in middle childhood: Moderate genetic and shared environmental influence using diverse measures of general cognitive ability at 7, 9 and 10 years in a large population sample of twins. Intelligence. 2008;36(1):68–80. [Google Scholar]
- Deary I.J. Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology. 2012;63:453–482. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deary I.J., Pattie A., Starr J.M. The stability of intelligence from age 11 to age 90 years: The Lothian birth cohort of 1921. Psychological Science. 2013;12:2361–2368. doi: 10.1177/0956797613486487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dyume M., Dumaret A.M., Tomkiewicz S. How can we boost IQs of ‘dull children’? A late adoption study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. 1999;96:8790–88794. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.15.8790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinstein L. London School of Economics, Centre for Economic Performance; 2003. Very early cognitive evidence. Centre piece (24–30) [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L., Pethick S., Renda C., Cox J.L., Dale P.S., Reznick S. Short-form version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2000;21:95–116. [Google Scholar]
- Ganjavi H., Lewis J.D., Bellec P., Macdonald P.A., Waber D.P., Evans A.C. Negative associations between corpus callosum midsagittal area and IQ in a representative sample of healthy children and adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e19698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haier R.J., Jung R.E., Yeo R.A., Head K., Alkire M.T. The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: Sex matters. NeuroImage. 2005;25:320–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanscombe K.B., Trzaskowski M., Haworth C.M.A., Davis O.S.P., Dale P.S., Plomin R. Socioeconomic status (SES) and children's intelligence (IQ): In a UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(2) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haworth C.M.A., Davis O.S.P., Plomin R. Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A genetically sensitive investigation of cognitive and behavioural development from childhood to young adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2013;16(01):117–125. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haworth C.M.A., Harlaar N., Kovas Y., Davis O.S.P., Oliver B.R., Hayiou-Tomas M.E. Internet cognitive testing of large samples needed in genetic research. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2007;10:554–563. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.4.554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes K.J. Genes, drives, and intellect. Psychological Reports. 1962;10:299–342. [Google Scholar]
- Heckman J.J. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. 2006;312:1900–1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1128898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L., Bentler P.M. Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Jerrim J., Vignoles A. DoQSS working paper, London, UK. 2011. Social mobility: Regression to the mean and the cognitive development of high ability children from disadvantaged homes. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W., Bouchard T.J., Jr., Krueger R.F., McGue M., Gottesman I.I. Just one g: Consistent results from three test batteries. Intelligence. 2004;34:95–107. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W., te Nijenhuis J., Bouchard T.J., Jr. Still just one g: Consistent results from five test batteries. Intelligence. 2008;36:81–95. [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan E., Fein D., Kramer J., Delis D., Morris R. The Psychological Corporation; New York: 1998. WISC-III as a Process Instrument (WISC-III-PI) [Google Scholar]
- Kovas Y., Haworth C.M.A., Dale P.S., Plomin R. The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2007;72:1–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2007.00439.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobo M.A., Galloway J.C. Assessment and stability of early learning abilities in preterm and full-term infants across the first two years of life. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2013;34(5):1721–1730. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McArdle J.J. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:577–605. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy D. The Psychological Corporation; New York: 1972. McCarthy scales of children's abilities. [Google Scholar]
- Oliver B., Dale P.S., Saudino K., Petrill S.A., Pike A., Plomin R. The validity of parent-based assessment of non-verbal cognitive abilities of three-year olds. Early Child Developmental Care. 2002;172:337–348. [Google Scholar]
- Petrill S.A., Rempell J., Oliver B., Plomin R. Testing cognitive abilities by telephone in a sample of 6- to 8-year-olds. Intelligence. 2002;30:353–360. [Google Scholar]
- Raven J.C., Court J.H., Raven J. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 1996. Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. [Google Scholar]
- Raven J.C., Court J.H., Raven J. Oxford Psychologists Press; Oxford, UK: 1998. Manual for Raven's advanced progressive matrices. [Google Scholar]
- Saudino K.J., Dale P.S., Oliver B., Petrill S.A., Richardson V., Rutter M. The validity of parent-based assessment of the cognitive abilities of 2-year-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1998;16:349–363. [Google Scholar]
- Saunders P. Civitas research report. 2012. Social mobility delusions. [Google Scholar]
- Schoon I., Jones E., Cheng H., Maughan B. Family hardship, family instability, and cognitive development. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2012;66:716–722. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.121228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith P., Fernandes C., Strand S. nferNELSON; Windsor, UK: 2001. Cognitive Abilities Test 3 (CAT3) [Google Scholar]
- Spinath F.M., Ronald A., Harlaar N., Price T.S., Plomin R. Phenotypic g early in life: On the etiology of general cognitive ability in a large population sample of twin children aged 2 to 4. Intelligence. 2003;31:195–210. [Google Scholar]
- Strenze T. Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence. 2007;35:401–426. [Google Scholar]
- Trzaskowski M., Harlaar N., Arden R., Krapohl E., Rimfeld K., McMillan A. Genetic influence on family socioeconomic status and children's intelligence. Intelligence. 2014;42:83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker-Drob E.M., Briley D.A. Continuity of genetic and environmental influences on cognition across the life span: A meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2014;140:949–979. doi: 10.1037/a0035893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker-Drob E.M., Rhemtulla M., Harden K.P., Turkheimer E., Fask D. Emergence of a gene-by-socioeconomic status interaction on infant mental ability between 10 months to 2 years. Psychological Science. 2011;22:125–133. doi: 10.1177/0956797610392926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Stumm S. You are what you eat? Meal type, socio-economic status and cognitive ability in childhood. Intelligence. 2012;40:576–583. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. The Psychological Corporation; London, UK: 1992. Wechsler intelligence scale for children — Third edition UK (WISC-IIIUK) manual. [Google Scholar]