Abstract
This study aimed to investigate the association of facial proportion and its relation to the golden ratio with the evaluation of facial appearance among Malaysian population. This was a cross-sectional study with 286 randomly selected from Universiti Sains Malaysia (USM) Health Campus students (150 females and 136 males; 100 Malaysian Chinese, 100 Malaysian Malay and 86 Malaysian Indian), with the mean age of 21.54 ± 1.56 (Age range, 18–25). Facial indices obtained from direct facial measurements were used for the classification of facial shape into short, ideal and long. A validated structured questionnaire was used to assess subjects’ evaluation of their own facial appearance. The mean facial indices of Malaysian Indian (MI), Malaysian Chinese (MC) and Malaysian Malay (MM) were 1.59 ± 0.19, 1.57 ± 0.25 and 1.54 ± 0.23 respectively. Only MC showed significant sexual dimorphism in facial index (P = 0.047; P<0.05) but no significant difference was found between races. Out of the 286 subjects, 49 (17.1%) were of ideal facial shape, 156 (54.5%) short and 81 (28.3%) long. The facial evaluation questionnaire showed that MC had the lowest satisfaction with mean score of 2.18 ± 0.97 for overall impression and 2.15 ± 1.04 for facial parts, compared to MM and MI, with mean score of 1.80 ± 0.97 and 1.64 ± 0.74 respectively for overall impression; 1.75 ± 0.95 and 1.70 ± 0.83 respectively for facial parts. In conclusion: 1) Only 17.1% of Malaysian facial proportion conformed to the golden ratio, with majority of the population having short face (54.5%); 2) Facial index did not depend significantly on races; 3) Significant sexual dimorphism was shown among Malaysian Chinese; 4) All three races are generally satisfied with their own facial appearance; 5) No significant association was found between golden ratio and facial evaluation score among Malaysian population.
Introduction
It is commonly said that ‘beauty lies in the eyes of the beholder’. Nonetheless, is esthetic judgment entirely subjective and purely a matter of arbitrary personal preference or could there be some scientific backings that guide and govern our perception towards beauty?
Ancient Greeks have ventured to question the meaning of beauty and believed that the world is beautiful because there is a certain order, harmony, measure and proportion between its elements [1]. For centuries, the Golden Ratio or Golden Proportion has been considered as the perfect or ideal ratio for beauty. First described by the Greek Mathematician Euclid as the extreme and mean ratio [2], whose definition reads ‘A straight line is said to have been cut in extreme and mean ratio when, as the whole line is to the greater segment, so is the greater to the lesser’; it is later discovered to have a numerical value of precisely 1 to 1.61803399 by Filius Bonacci [3,4] and it is not until the 20th century when the term “Phi” and its symbol Φ were coined by Mark Barr in commemoration of the Greek sculptor Phidias [5,6]. In essence, the golden ratio has captured the fascination of intellectuals from diverse fields and disciplines. Interestingly, golden ratio is also prevalent in nature, where it can be seen in the skeletons of animals and humans, in the arrangement of branches along the stems of plants, in the spirals of sea shells and in the wing dimensions and location of eye-like spots on moths. Even more intriguing is that Phi manifests throughout the human form such as the body, the face, the fingers and the teeth [7,8,9]. Since the divine proportion seems to evoke an aesthetically pleasing effect, it might have been hard-wired into our consciousness as a guide towards aesthetic judgment; as proposed by Jefferson in whose paper a universal standard for facial beauty measurement regardless of race, age, sex and other variables is shown [10]. In fact, several studies have also shown that beautiful faces have facial measurements close to the golden ratio [11,12,13,14,15,16,17].
Ricketts used a golden divider to prove that the harmonious faces of beautiful women followed golden proportions [18]. Presently, several studies on facial aesthetics have been carried out where facial analyses are done using lateral cephalograms [19,20,21], photographs [15,22,23,24,25] and by anthropometrical means with the employment of direct measurements [26,27,2] and three dimensional imaging [28,29], among others. More recent articles have also discussed angular and linear analyses of the soft-tissue profile [30,31,32,33,34] and lip morphology [35]. In addition, extensive literature on modern facial anthropometric data among North American white populations [36], North Eastern Nigerian [37], Indian Americans [38], North Maharashtrian [24], Malaysians [26], and Latvian [39] are available in this present day. Some studies have also attempted to classify facial shapes based on Golden Ratio to determine and compare the prevalence of faces conforming to or approaching the golden ratio across different ethnics and between genders [26,27].
There has been evidence supporting the idea that the objective appraisal of facial beauty is indeed possible and that faces which observe certain universal parameters, such as symmetry, the Neoclassical Canon and Golden Ratio, are deemed beautiful across different culture and ethnics [40,41,42]. However, there is currently little evidence of facial index in relation to the golden ratio in Malaysia. In contrast, other studies have suggested that beauty is multifactorial and its subjectivity is founded primarily on genetics [43], culture and environmental factors [44].
Studies have also been conducted to investigate the evaluation towards facial appearance where Japanese [45], Japanese-Brazillian [46], Indian Subcontinent [47] and Thai [48] laypersons were asked to perceive their own facial appearance and rate their satisfaction level, which has shown to vary across different demographic groups. However, up to this present day, there is no evidence to assess the evaluation and satisfaction of one’s own facial appearance and also to determine its association with the facial proportion in relation to the golden ratio among Malaysian population.
Hence, our study aims to provide a facial anthropometric and aesthetic analysis of the three main ethnic groups in Malaysia and to determine the prevalence of ideal faces which conform to the Golden Ratio by obtaining the facial indices and classifying subjects’ faces into different facial shape groups. Additionally, by determining the level of satisfaction of subjects towards their own facial appearance, we are interested to know whether a certain relationship exists between Facial Golden Ratio and subjects’ evaluation towards their own facial beauty, hence establishing that the Golden Ratio does influence facial attractiveness. It is also necessary to assess the profile preferences of Malaysian adults, as similar studies have not been done to date for the Malaysian population. The specific aims of this study are as follows:
To determine the normal anthropometrical measurements of facial height, facial width and facial indices among Malaysian population.
To identify and classify Malaysian population into different facial shapes based on the golden ratio.
To determine the existence of significant differences in facial measurements and indices between genders and three different races in Malaysia.
To study the evaluation of self-facial appearance among Malaysian population.
To determine the existence of significant differences in the evaluation of self-facial appearance between genders and three different races in Malaysia.
To investigate the association of facial proportion and its relation to the golden ratio with the evaluation of facial appearance among Malaysian population.
To determine the perception towards general facial aesthetic and preference of the lip and chin profiles among Malaysian population.
Materials and Methods
All participants provide their written informed consent. This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Hospital Universiti Sains Malaysia (HUSM) [USM/JEPeM/1405203], which complies with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Subjects and Sample Selection
The subjects of this study consisted of a total of 286 subjects, including 100 Malay (50 male 50 female), 100 Malaysian Chinese (50 male 50 female) and 86 Malaysian Indian (36 male 50 female) from 18 to 25 years of age. The sample comprising of students attending Universiti Sains Malaysia originate from all states in different parts of Malaysia and hence is a true representation of the whole Malaysian population aged 18–25 both in regards to the ethnicity ratio and also geographically. Subjects of mixed ethnicity, subjects with craniofacial deformity, and subjects with a history of orthodontic treatment or facial surgery were excluded from this study. Informed consents were obtained from the subjects before the study was carried out.
Anthropometrical Facial Measurements
Anthropometrical landmarks on the face used for facial measurements are shown in Fig 1, with their definitions provided in Table 1 [49,50]. The relevant participant has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish the photograph in Fig 1 (Eyes were covered). Direct measuring technique, considered to be more accurate than indirect measuring technique, was employed and real-time measurement was done on the 286 subjects. The anthropometrical landmarks involved were palpated and located on the face of the subjects and a total of five measurements were taken including total facial height (Tr-Me), upper facial height (Tr-Gb), middle facial height (Gb-Sn) and lower facial height (Sn-Me) as well as width of face (Zy-Zy) using dental sliding vernier caliper (Boley Dental USA) and cephalometric protractor (Orthopli Corporation Philadelphia).
Fig 1. Anthropometrical landmarks and facial measurements.
Tr- Trichion, Gb- Glabella, Sn- Subnasale, Me- Menton, Zy- Zygion.
Table 1. Definition of anthropometrical landmarks.
Anthropometrical Landmarks | Definition |
---|---|
Trichion (Tr) | Anterior hairline at the midline |
Glabella (Gb) | Most prominent point of the forehead on profile |
Subnasale (Sn) | Junction of the inferior portion of the nasal septum and the upper lip |
Menton (Me) | Most inferior soft-tissue point on chin |
Zygion (Zy) | The most laterally positioned point on the zygomatic arches. |
Facial Index and Facial Shape
The ratio of the bizygomatic width (FW) to the total facial height (TFH) of each subject was calculated and recorded as the subject’s facial index (FI). Subjects were categorized into different facial shapes based on the relationship of their facial indices to the Golden Ratio (1.6–1.699 = Normal, < 1.6 = Short, > 1.699 = Long) [26,27].
Facial Evaluation Questionnaire
Subjects were also asked to fill up a questionnaire to assess their perception towards beauty and evaluation of their own facial appearance. The questionnaire consisted of 3 parts.
Section 1 consisted of patients’ demographical data, namely name, sex, age and race.
Section 2 consisted of questions for silhouette study which included 2 series of 8 profiles each. The first series of profile showed changes in relation to the chin while the second series showed changes in relation to the lips. The average profile was No. 4. The chin or lips were protruded or retruded in 1-mm increments from the average profile, and the chin or lips positions were changed parallel to the Frankfort horizontal plane. Therefore, the chin was most protrusive in profile 1 and the most retrusive in profile 8 for the first series. Meanwhile, the lips were most protrusive in profile 8 and the most retrusive in profile 1 for the second series. Black and white silhouette profiles were used in this study to avoid any bias due to preferences in certain skin and hair style or colour. In this section, subjects were required to choose the most and least appealing profile of the lips and chin respectively. Lips and chin profiles [51] were assessed since these two facial elements are considered the most influential features of the face [29,52].
Section 3 consisted of questionnaire to assess the evaluation of the subjects towards their own facial appearance. In this section the subjects were asked to choose from 5-degree of satisfaction (1: satisfied, 2: somewhat satisfied, 3: neither satisfied nor dissatisfied, 4: somewhat dissatisfied, and 5: dissatisfied). Nine questions about the subjects’ overall impression such as the impression of the face and the outline of the face. The satisfaction score for overall impression is the mean of the responses to these nine questions. Fifteen questions about the satisfaction towards their own facial parts such as eyelids, eyes, nose, cheeks, lips, teeth, chin, lower lip, dentition, bite (anterior and posterior) and angle of jaw. The mean of the responses to these fifteen questions is recorded as the satisfaction score for facial parts.
Statistical analysis
The data were analyzed statistically using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0 (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. Released 2013). The measurements and results of the questionnaires were presented as mean with standard deviation (SD). The statistical significance of intergender differences in mean values of the measured parameters and satisfaction scores from the questionnaire was examined using Independent t-test while One-Way ANOVA was done to investigate the existence of statistical significance between three different races. Subjects were then categorized into different facial shapes based on the relationship of their facial indices to the golden ratio (1.6–1.699 = Normal, <1.6 = Short, > 1.699 = Long)[26,27]. Graphical representation of the subjects with different facial shapes and the frequency of the lips and chin profile preferences were obtained from Microsoft excel 2010. Associations between different variables were analyzed using Simple Linear Regression. The confidence level was set at 95% (P < 0.05).
Thirty (30) subjects were randomly selected and re-measured 1 month after the initial measurements. Intra-rater reliability was determined using the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC). Overall, the k value of the raters for each measurement was at least 0.70, indicating good intra-rater reliability. Cronbach's alpha was above 0.800 for all measurements, which indicated a high level of internal consistency.
Results
Intersexes disparities
Table 2, Fig 2a and 2b shows the mean facial measurements between different genders. Also illustrated is a highly significant sexual dimorphism in all three races for LFH (P< 0.001). For MFH, Malaysian Chinese (MC) (P< 0.001), Malaysian Malay (MM) and Malaysian Indian (MI) (P = 0.05) showed significant inter-gender difference. In regard to TFH, Malaysian Malay (P<0.001), Malaysian Chinese (P< 0.001) and Malaysian Indian (P = 0.001) showed significant inter-gender difference. Only MM suggested significant difference between genders for UFH (P< 0.001). For facial index, only MC showed significant inter-gender difference (P<0.05). No statistically significant difference between genders was found in all three races for facial width.
Table 2. Comparison of facial measurements between different genders.
Measurements | Races | Mean (SD) | P-value | 95% CI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | Lower | Upper | |||
Upper facial height (UFH) | MM | 62.98(7.17) | 55.15(8.46) | <0.001*** | 5.716 | 10.934 |
MC | 65.11(7.89) | 62.83(9.32) | 0.190 | -1.146 | 5.706 | |
MI | 58.08(7.76) | 57.26(6.67) | 0.602 | -2.286 | 3.922 | |
Middle facial height (MFH) | MM | 55.37(8.27) | 51.26(5.65) | 0.005* | 1.300 | 6.924 |
MC | 59.83(5.43) | 52.59(9.58) | <0.001*** | 4.147 | 10.331 | |
MI | 57.73(5.53) | 54.47(4.9) | 0.005** | 1.013 | 5.510 | |
Lower facial height (LFH) | MM | 60.70(6.69) | 55.42(5.40) | <0.001*** | 2.877 | 7.700 |
MC | 63.49(7.02) | 57.15(10.44) | <0.001*** | 2.807 | 9.867 | |
MI | 62.44(8.11) | 56.55(6.52) | <0.001*** | 2.750 | 9.033 | |
Total Facial Height (TFH) | MM | 179.05(15.27) | 161.83(14.14) | <0.001*** | 11.386 | 23.070 |
MC | 188.43(14.00) | 172.57(22.52) | <0.001*** | 8.414 | 23.298 | |
MI | 178.25(13.17) | 168.28(13.45) | 0.001** | 4.175 | 16.767 | |
Facial Width (Z-Z) | MM | 114.75(10.10) | 110.12(15.15) | 0.075 | -0.482 | 9.738 |
MC | 117.10(11.48) | 115.19(13.36) | 0.445 | -3.032 | 6.852 | |
MI | 112.67(9.63) | 107.83(13.76) | 0.074 | -0.472 | 10.145 | |
Facial Index (FI) | MM | 1.57(0.19) | 1.50(0.27) | 0.130 | -0.021 | 0.162 |
MC | 1.62(0.18) | 1.52(0.30) | 0.047* | 0.001 | 0.195 | |
MI | 1.59(0.17) | 1.58(0.20) | 0.787 | -0.070 | 0.093 |
(SD: Standard deviation, CI: Confidence interval, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001).
Fig 2. Comparison of facial measurements between, a: different genders and b: different genders in relation to different race.
Interracial disparities
Table 3 and Fig 3 highlights the mean measurements of each racial group. Our findings revealed a highly significant racial difference (P<0.001) for UFH between MC and MM, and also between MC and MI. MFH was statistically significant (P<0.05) between MM and MC. In regard to TFH, statistically significant difference was shown between MM and MC (P<0.001) and between MC and MI (P<0.01). A comparison between MC and MI showed significant racial difference (P<0.01) for Zygion to Zygion. However, no significant racial difference was found in LFH measurement and facial index.
Table 3. Comparison of facial measurements between different ethnics.
Measurement | Mean (SD) | P-Values | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MM | MC | MI | MM Vs MC | MM Vs MI | MC Vs MI | ||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||||
Upper Facial Height (mm) | 59.06 | 8.74 | 63.97 | 8.67 | 57.60 | 7.11 | <0.001*** | 0.689 | <0.001*** |
Mid Facial Height (mm) | 53.32 | 7.35 | 56.21 | 8.56 | 55.84 | 5.39 | 0.016* | 0.058 | 1.000 |
Lower Facial Height (mm) | 58.06 | 6.60 | 60.32 | 9.40 | 59.02 | 7.76 | 0.142 | 1.000 | 0.812 |
Total Facial Height (mm) | 170.44 | 17.01 | 180.50 | 20.29 | 172.46 | 14.15 | <0.001*** | 1.000 | 0.006** |
Zygion To Zygion (mm) | 112.43 | 13.02 | 116.15 | 12.43 | 109.85 | 12.37 | 0.116 | 0.497 | 0.002** |
Facial Index | 1.54 | 0.23 | 1.57 | 0.25 | 1.59 | 0.19 | 0.771 | 0.435 | 1.000 |
(SD: Standard deviation, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001).
Fig 3. Comparison of facial measurements between different ethnics.
Table 4 illustrates interracial difference in facial measurements among male and female subjects. Significant difference was shown between MM and MI (P<0.05) and MC and MI (P<0.001) in UFH, between MM and MC (P<0.01) in MFH as well as between MM and MC (P<0.01) and between MC and MI (P<0.01) in TFH among male subjects. Among female subjects, UFH showed significant difference between MM and MC (P<0.001) and MC and MI (P<0.01). Besides, significant difference was also shown in TFH between MM and MC (P<0.01) as well as ZTZ between MC and MI (P<0.05).
Table 4. Comparison of facial measurements between different races among male and female subjects.
Measurements | P-Value | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | |||||
MM Vs MC | MM Vs MI | MC Vs MI | MM Vs MC | MM Vs MI | MC Vs MI | |
Upper Facial Height (mm) | 0.486 | 0.011* | <0.001*** | <0.001*** | 0.605 | 0.003** |
Mid Facial Height (mm) | 0.003** | 0.319 | 0.456 | 1.000 | 0.071 | 0.544 |
Lower Facial Height (mm) | 0.167 | 0.816 | 1.000 | 0.798 | 1.000 | 1.000 |
Total Facial Height (mm) | 0.004** | 1.000 | 0.004** | 0.006** | 0.188 | 0.644 |
Zygion To Zygion (mm) | 0.796 | 1.000 | 0.167 | 0.223 | 1.000 | 0.030* |
Facial Index | 0.492 | 1.000 | 1.000 | 1.000 | 0.383 | 0.821 |
(*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001).
Classification of facial shapes based on the Golden Ratio
Fig 4 revealed that in Malaysian Malay subjects, 16 (12 male, 4 female) had an ideal face, 55 (25 male, 30 female) had a short face, 29 (13 male, 16 female) had a long face. In Malaysian Chinese subjects, 18 (8 male, 10 female) had an ideal face, 53 (24 male, 29 female) had a short face, 29 (18 male, 11 female) had a long face. In Malaysian Indian subjects, 15 (8 male, 7 female) had an ideal face, 48 (20 male, 28 female) had a short face, 23 (8 male, 15 female) had a long face. Out of 286 subjects, the face shape was ideal in 49 subjects, short in 156 subjects, and long in 81 subjects.
Fig 4. Pie charts showing facial shape classification of subjects according to the Golden Ratio.
Mean facial evaluation score between genders and different races
Table 5 shows the mean evaluation score for both overall impression (OI) and facial parts (FP) in male and female subjects.
Table 5. Comparison of mean facial evaluation score between different genders.
Mean Facial evaluation Score | Mean (SD) | P-value | 95% CI | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | Total | Lower | Upper | ||
Mean OI | 1.79 (0.72) | 1.97 (0.82) | 1.89 (0.77) | 0.056 | -0.356 | 0.005 |
Mean FP | 1.84 (0.70) | 1.91 (0.73) | 1.87 (0.72) | 0.395 | -0.239 | 0.095 |
As shown in Table 6, mean evaluation scores for OI and FP were significantly higher in MC than MM and MI. Significant inter-racial difference was shown for mean OI score between MM and MC and between MC and MI (P<0.001).
Table 6. Comparison of mean facial evaluation score between different races.
Mean Facial Evaluation Score | Mean (SD) | P-Values | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MM | MC | MI | MM Vs MC | MM Vs MI | MC VS MI | ||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||||
Mean OI | 1.80 | 0.86 | 2.18 | 0.76 | 1.64 | 0.58 | 0.001** | 0.456 | <0.001*** |
Mean FP | 1.75 | 0.74 | 2.15 | 0.73 | 1.70 | 0.56 | <0.001*** | 1.000 | <0.001*** |
(**p<0.01, ***p<0.001).
Figs 5 and 6 shows the mean evaluation score for each OI and FP, respectively. Significant differences between races for different variables were observed. No significant difference for mean facial evaluation score was found between different facial shapes, as shown in Table 7.
Fig 5. Mean evaluation score for overall impression in different races.
Fig 6. Mean evaluation score for facial parts in different races.
Table 7. Comparison of mean facial evaluation score between different facial shapes.
P-Value | |||
---|---|---|---|
Ideal Vs Short | Ideal Vs Long | Short Vs Long | |
Mean OI | 1.000 | 1.000 | 1.000 |
Mean FP | 1.000 | 0.698 | 0.723 |
Association of facial evaluation score with various factors (Sex, Race, Facial Index and Facial Shape)
Tables 8 and 9 highlight the association of mean overall impression score and mean facial parts score with various factors. No significant linear relationships are established between facial index with both mean overall impression score and mean facial parts score.
Table 8. Simple linear regression for association between mean overall impression score and facial index, facial shape (with ideal face group as control), sex (with male as control) and race (with Malay as control).
Variables | r2 | b (95% CI) | t (df) | P-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Facial Index | 0.002 | 0.152 (- 0.249, 0.553) | 0.746 (284) | 0.456 |
Facial Shape | 0.003 | |||
Short | - 0.003(- 0.254, 0.248) | -0.021 (283) | 0.983 | |
Long | 0.089 (- 0.188, 0.366) | 0.631 (283) | 0.529 | |
Sex | 0.013 | |||
Female | 0.176 (- 0.005, 0.356) | 1.917 (284) | 0.056 | |
Race | 0.083 | |||
Chinese | 0.377 (0.169, 0.585) | 3.567 (283) | <0.001*** | |
Indian | -0.158 (-0.374, 0.058) | -1.436 (283) | 0.152 |
[***p<0.001]
Table 9. Simple linear regression for association between mean facial parts score and facial index, facial shape (with ideal face group as control), sex (with male as control) and race (with Malay as control).
Variables | R Square | b (95% CI) | t (df) | P-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Facial Index | 0.004 | 0.210 (- 0.158, 0.579) | 1.122 (284) | 0.263 |
Facial Shape | 0.007 | |||
Short | 0.040 (- 0.191, 0.270) | 0.339 (283) | 0.735 | |
Long | 0.155 (- 0.100, 0.410) | 1.196 (283) | 0.233 | |
Sex | 0.003 | |||
Female | 0.072 (-0.095, 0.239) | 0.853 (284) | 0.395 | |
Race | 0.082 | |||
Chinese | 0.403(0.211,0.594) | 4.142 (283) | <0.001*** | |
Indian | -0.053 (-0.252, 0.146) | -0.520 (283) | 0.604 |
[***p<0.001]
Simple Linear Regression revealed no significant linear relationships between facial shape with both mean overall impression score and mean facial parts score among subjects with long face and short face when ideal face is used as the control.
Simple Linear Regression also showed no significant linear relationships between sexes with both mean overall impression and facial parts scores. Interestingly, significant linear relationship was shown between race with both mean overall impression score (P<0.001) and mean facial parts score (P<0.001) only among MC with MM as the control. It is observed that Chinese subjects have a 37.7% higher score compared to the Malay group for mean overall impression score (CI 0.169, 0.585), and about 8.3% of variation in the mean overall impression score can be explained by race. Chinese subjects also scored 40.3% higher than the Malay subjects for mean facial parts score (CI 0.211, 0.594), with approximately 8.2% of variation in the mean facial parts score can be explained by race.
Silhouette Studies: Lips and chin profiles preferences
Fig 7 revealed that highest percentage of subjects selected the average, orthognathic profile 4 as the most appealing for chin position and the most protrusive profile 1 as the least appealing. As shown in Fig 8, profiles 4 and 5 were chosen as the most attractive for lips position, while the most protrusive profile 8 for lips was considered the least attractive by most male and female subjects.
Fig 7. Bar chart showing frequency of chin profile preference of subjects.
Fig 8. Bar chart showing frequency of lips profile preference of subjects.
It is interesting to note that no subjects chose profile 5 as the least appealing chin position, which has a 1mm increment of protrusion from the average profile 4.
Discussion
Facial measurements of different ethnicities and populations across the globe
Extensive data of facial measurements including the total facial height and facial width of different ethnicities and populations globally from various studies are summarized in Table 10.
Table 10. Comparison of facial measurements in present study with studies done across the globe on different populations.
Author | Year | Sample size | Population | Methods | Total Facial Height | Facial Width | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | F | M | F | M | F | ||||
Farkas et al [36] | 2005 | 30 | 30 | North American | Spreading and sliding caliper | 187.5±16.2 | 172.5 ±15.0 | 137.1±8.6 | 129.9±10.6 |
30 | 30 | Azerbajian | 185.1±18.0 | 175.4 ±13.6 | 147.5±10.8 | 138.7±10.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Bulgarian | 184.3±17.4 | 170.5 ±13.6 | 139.5±11.2 | 130.9±8.8 | |||
30 | 30 | Czech | 181.7±15.8 | 182.9 ±16.2 | 134.9±26.6 | 126.4±28.8 | |||
30 | 30 | Croatian | 180.1±21.2 | 172.6 ±17.4 | 140.7±12.0 | 133.2±13.6 | |||
30 | 30 | German | 182.2±22.2 | 170.9 ±14.4 | 133.2±15.0 | 123.4±18.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Greek | 178.7±25.8 | 173.8±13.8 | 128.6±22.8 | 132.2±9.6 | |||
30 | 30 | Hungarian | 181.3±28.4 | 169.4±15.4 | 142.1±10.6 | 131.3±7.0 | |||
30 | 30 | Italian | 186.0±21.2 | 171.4 ±18.4 | 143.2±11.8 | 133.3±8.2 | |||
30 | 30 | Polish | 181.9±16.4 | 172.1 ±17.8 | 142.6±9.4 | 135.5±11.0 | |||
30 | 30 | Portugese | 190.7±14.2 | 177.4 ±19.0 | 125.1±10.8 | 120.4±10.8 | |||
30 | 30 | Russian | 184.4±16.2 | 174.4 ±17.4 | 141.2±8.8 | 132.3±9.6 | |||
30 | 30 | Slovak | 183.7±17.6 | 169.7 ±17.5 | 134.7±11.0 | 125.0 ±11.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Slovenian | 181.3±20.6 | 170.4 ±30.2 | 136.2±11.6 | 129.5±10.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Iranian | 180.3±20.4 | 175.9 ±15.0 | 138.4±11.4 | 131.7±13.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Turkish | 186.5±12.8 | 179.2 ±18.8 | 140.4±16.4 | 134.5±8.6 | |||
30 | 30 | Egyptian | 176.9 ±26.8 | 161.4 ±17.8 | 139.8 ±13.8 | 130.3 ±10.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Indian | 161.3±4.6 | 163.0 ±16.6 | 135.8±8.6 | 124.9±16.9 | |||
30 | 30 | Singapore Chinese | 187.3±14.4 | 176.2 ±16.6 | 144.6±11.2 | 136.2±8.0 | |||
30 | 30 | Vietnamese | 180.9±16.6 | 171.1 ±14.2 | 144.0±8.8 | 134.3±5.8 | |||
30 | 30 | Thai | 185.1±15.4 | 172.8 ±17.4 | 147.1±11.0 | 138.3±12.6 | |||
30 | 30 | Japanese | 191.4±16.6 | 182.8 ±14.4 | 147.2±11.2 | 141.2±11.8 | |||
30 | 30 | Angolan | 182.6±18.2 | 172.4 ±17.8 | 139.8±10.2 | 132.8±8.4 | |||
30 | 30 | Tonga | 161.8±17.0 | - | 133.3±2.6 | ||||
30 | 30 | Zulu | 209.2±20.6 | 179.1 ±19.8 | 138.5±9.2 | 128.4±9.6 | |||
30 | 30 | Afro American | 194.6±21.2 | 180.1 ±15.0 | 138.7±11.2 | 130.5±9.6 | |||
Erika[39] | 2005 | 39 | 38 | Latvian | “ | 187.3 | 177.0 | 133.1 | 122.4 |
Omar et al [38] | 2005 | - | 102 | Indian American | Photo-graphic | - | 169.4 ±13.3 | - | 125.9±10.1 |
Ngeow et al [53] | 2009 | 50 | 50 | Malay | Spreading and sliding caliper | - | - | 132.5±7.0 | 140.1±4.9 |
Ngeow et al [54] | 2009 | 50 | 50 | Malaysian Indian | “ | - | - | 136.3±4.8 | 126.7 ±3.9 |
Raji et al [37] | 2010 | 200 | 143 | North Eastern Nigerian | “ | - | - | 115.1 | 111.3 |
Jeremic et al [55] | 2013 | 360 | 340 | Serbian | Spreading Caliper | - | - | 129.1±8.9 | 120.0±6.4 |
Kumar et al [56] | 2013 | 300 | 300 | Haryanvi Bania | “ | - | - | 130.8±7.3 | 123.5±7.6 |
Milutinovic et al [57] | 2014 | - | 83 | Caucasian | Photographic | - | - | 141.7±18.8 | - |
Packiri-swamy et al [26] | 2012 | 50 | 50 | Malaysian Chinese | Spreading and sliding caliper | 192.1±9.6 | 186.6 ±9.9 | 140.1±7.4 | 135.2±10.8 |
50 | 50 | Malaysian Indian | 182.5±11.0 | 172.7±10.9 | 130.3±8.9 | 124.0±6.6 | |||
50 | 50 | Malay | 189.1±8.4 | 179.2 ±7.8 | 131.3±8.7 | 134.0±10.2 | |||
Present study | 2014 | 50 | 50 | Malaysian Chinese | “ | 188.4±14.0 | 172.6 ±22.5 | 117.1±11.5 | 115.2±13.4 |
36 | 50 | Malaysian Indian | 178.3±13.2 | 168.3 ±13.5 | 112.7±9.6 | 107.8±13.8 | |||
50 | 50 | Malay | 179.1±15.3 | 161.8 ±14.1 | 114.8±10.1 | 110.1±15.2 |
(M: Male, F: Female)
The present study and Packiriswamy et al. [26] both showed similar pattern and trend in interracial and intersexes comparison. Results from both studies showed that MC reported the highest value for both TFH and facial width in male and female subjects. It has been widely maintained that, in comparison to other races, MI males were revealed to have the lowest values for both TFH and facial width while MI females also had the lowest values for facial width. Moreover, both studies also reported that male subjects showed higher values for TFH and facial width compared to female subjects in all 3 Malaysian populations, which are consistent with values of other populations across the world reported by Raji et al. [37], Ngeow et al. [53,54], Omar et al. [38] and Erika et al. [39]. In contrast, Farkas et al. [36] reported that female subjects show higher value of TFH compared to male subjects in Czech and Indian population, while female Greek subjects show higher values for Facial Width compared to male subjects.
TFH of Malaysian Chinese males (188.4mm) showed almost similar values with that of Singapore Chinese males (187.3mm) [36], which could possibly be explained by similar ancestral origin. Similar value of TFH was also shown by Latvian male subjects (187.3mm) [39]. TFH measurements of Malaysian Chinese female (172.6mm) also coincide with the values of North American females (172.5mm) [36]. TFH of Malaysian Indian males (178.3mm) was similar to that of Greek males (178.7mm) [36]. Malaysian Malay females (161.8mm) and Egyptian females (161.4mm) [36] showed almost similar TFH measurements. Facial width of female Malay subjects (114.8mm) was shown to be close to the value of North Eastern Nigerian females (115.1mm) [38].
Craniofacial parameters from our anthropometric studies on Malaysian populations can be used to provide crucial data for anatomical and anthropological research as well as research in forensic medicine. In clinical practice, these data can serve as important guidelines and references among reconstructive and plastic surgeons, maxillofacial surgeons, orthodontists and prosthodontists, particularly in analysis of treatment outcome [55,56,57,58]. For evaluation of variations in craniofacial morphology and also to detect potential pathological abnormalities, standards of anthropometric measurements should be established for Malaysian population.
Interracial differences of facial measurements
Significant difference was shown between MM and MC (P<0.01) for TFH in both sexes. This was in contrast to the result reported by Packiriswamy et al. [26] where significant difference of TFH was reported only among female subjects while male subjects showed significant interracial difference only for facial width and zygion to zygion.
In contrast to our study where no significant difference in TFH and Zygion to Zygion for both genders was shown between MM and MI, Packiriswamy et al. [26] reported significant difference in both TFH and zygion to zygion among female subjects and TFH among male subjects.
Between MC and MI, Packiriswamy et al. [26] reported a significant difference in TFH and zygion to zygion in both genders. However, in our study significant difference in TFH was found only among male subjects and zygion to zygion among female subjects.
Packiriswamy et al. [26] showed no significant difference in facial index in both sexes between MM and MI and also between MC and MI, which is in accordance to the results reported by our study.
Facial shapes according to the Golden Ratio
Similar to studies done by Packiriswamy et al. [26] and Saraswathi et al. [27], the highest number of subjects had short face in our study. However, in contrast to the studies done by Packiriswamy et al. [26] and Saraswathi et al. [27], which reported the least number of subjects in the long face group, our findings showed that subjects with ideal face shape were the least in all three races. It was interesting to note that although Packiriswamy et al. [26] reported that none of the Malay female had long face, our study indicated that the percentage of Malay female with long face was the second highest percentage after Chinese male; while Chinese female recorded the lowest percentage in the long face group. On this point literature is not unanimous; this could be attributed to factors such as high hairline in the measurement involving trichion and many other ethnic variables that should be taken into account.
Facial evaluation score of different populations
As illustrated in Table 11, the mean evaluation score for both overall impression and facial parts of our study were lower than that among Thai laypersons [48], Japanese-Brazilian female laypersons [46] and Japanese laypersons [45], suggesting a higher satisfaction among Malaysian populations; but higher when compared to Indian subcontinent laypersons [47]. It is obvious in this study that although Malaysian population is generally satisfied with their own facial appearance, MC is the least satisfied of the three races.
Table 11. Comparison of facial evaluation score of present study with studies on other populations.
Author | Ethnicity | Sample Size | Range of Satisfaction Score | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Overall Impression | Facial Parts | |||
Leonardo et al [31] | Japanese Brazilian | 19 | 1.5 to 2.9 | 1.4 to 2.8 |
Alam et al [32] | Indian subcontinent | 48 | 1.3 to 2.1 | 1.3 to 1.9 |
Alam et al [30] | Japanese | 96 | 2.95 to 3.5 | 2.55 to 3.55 |
Luppanapornlap et al [33] | Thai | 120 | 1.98 to 3.26 | 1.95 to 3.20 |
Present study | Malaysian Malay | 100 | 1.71 to 1.9 | 1.45 to 2.14 |
Malaysian Chinese | 100 | 2.01 to 2.35 | 1.87 to 2.55 | |
Malaysian Indian | 86 | 1.53 to 1.81 | 1.49 to 2.05 |
Regarding satisfaction for each facial element, the items which most subjects are least satisfied with were “teeth” and “dentition” for both male and female, which was similar to the findings by Luppanapornlarp et al. [48].
Similar to the study done among Thai laypersons [48], no statistically significant difference was noted between male and female for both mean satisfaction score of overall impression (P = 0.056) and facial parts (P = 0.395).
Association of facial proportion and its relation to the Golden Ratio with the evaluation of facial appearance among Malaysian population
Previous studies have been conducted using various applied method with different examined distances and ratios and facial views as well as analysed sample. Generally, the majority of investigations reported a weak correlation between golden proportion and attractiveness [28,59]. Similarly, a study done to investigate the association between the perception of facial beauty and divine proportion found that ratios of 3D facial distances were not related to attractiveness in young, white adults, as assessed by a panel of dental professionals [6]. To our knowledge, in no case has the relationship between golden proportion and perception towards subjects’ own facial appearance via facial satisfaction score evaluation been investigated. The results of the current study show that the examined facial index and facial shape have no relationship with the mean facial evaluation score. As shown in Table 9, one-way ANOVA test shows no significant difference in the mean facial evaluation score between subjects of different facial shapes. In the present study, Simple Linear Regression failed to establish significant association between facial index and mean facial evaluation score. The mean evaluation score where subjects rated their level of satisfaction on a degree of 1 to 5 in our questionnaire serves to objectify the subjectiveness of an individual’s perception towards his or her own facial esthetic. Since results from our study showed no association between facial measurement and the mean evaluation score, which is a reflection of how satisfied or dissatisfied the subjects were of their own face, hence it can be concluded that there is no association of facial proportion and its relation to the golden ratio with the evaluation of facial appearance among Malaysian population. In other words, individuals who find themselves attractive might not necessarily have facial measurements which conform to or approach the golden ratio. This might be attributed to the various psychological factors, different cultural landscapes, social acceptance and expectations, social economic status, ethnic origins, social demographic backgrounds in addition to inherent influences that affect the personal perception or judgments towards the concept of facial esthetics.
Interestingly, Simple Linear Regression shows significant association between race and mean overall impression and facial parts score among Chinese subjects (P<0.001) with Malay as the control, suggesting that perception towards own facial appearance is different across different races.
Silhouette studies: Lips and chin profiles preferences
Previous methods used to assess facial profile attractiveness included profile line drawings [60], facial photographs and imagings [61,62] and silhouettes [63,64,65], which was adopted by our study. The overall trend in our study demonstrated that milder degrees of chin retrusion and protrusion were selected as more attractive compared to profiles with greater degrees of deviation, though the tendency was for chin protrusion to be perceived as less attractive than retrusion. This was supported by Mantzikos et al. [66] done among Japanese population who found that a straight profile was ranked the most attractive while mandibular retrognathic and prognathic profiles had poor rankings. Naini et al. [67] showed that the greater the retrusion or prominence of the chin, the lower the rating of the perceived attractiveness. Similar to a study by Maganzini et al. [68], our study indicated either a retrognathic or a prognathic mandible were found to be the least appealing by both male and female subjects.
According to Polk et al. [64], significantly more African American male and female judges preferred more retruded jaw profiles, which in is accordance to our study. Similarly, Soh et al. [69,70] reported that profiles with protrusive mandibles were perceived to be the least attractive by dental professionals, dental students and laypersons in Singapore. A study done among Japanese adults showed that mandibular retrusion was generally more favoured than mandibular protrusion [71], but the results of this study did not provide confirmation, showing that most subjects selected the most protrusive chin position as the least appealing profile. In another study, Caucasian males also preferred mandibular protrusion more than retrusion [72]. Such discrepancies might be due to cross-cultural differences between different populations.
Similar conclusions were obtained in certain studies in the orthodontic field, where in addition to the lips profile preferences, some facial profile angles were investigated. Similar to our study, Foster [73], Lines et al. [74] and Czarnecki et al. [63] used silhouette profiles with altered lip positions, where it was reported that significantly more males preferred retruded lip profiles compared to females. Other studies done among Mexican American judges [75] and Japanese orthodontists and students [65] also showed preference towards more retruded lip profiles. Our findings suggested that labial protrusion was slightly better tolerated in females compared to males, which was in line with the study by Czarnecki et al. [63].
In Summary, we found, Malaysian population has a total facial height value of 181.95 mm, facial width of 114.84 mm and facial index of 1.59. Only 17.1% of Malaysian facial proportion conformed to the golden ratio, with majority of the population having short face (54.5%). Facial index did not depend significantly on races; significant sexual dimorphism was shown among Malaysian Chinese. All three races are generally satisfied with their own facial appearance. Significant interracial differences in facial evaluation score were shown between Malaysian Chinese with Malaysian Malay and between Malaysian Chinese and Malaysian Indian; no sexual dimorphism was shown. No significant association was found between golden ratio and facial evaluation score among Malaysian population. An average profile of the lips and chin are preferred over more retrusive or protrusive profiles among Malaysian population.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper.
Funding Statement
The authors received no specific funding for this work.
References
- 1. Gaut B and Lopes DM. eds. London: Routledge, 2000, pp. 477–490. 2d Ed (rev.), 2005; pp. 55–101. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Mizumoto Y, Deguchi T Sr, Fong KW 2009. Assessment of facial golden proportions among young Japanese women. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 136:168–174. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2007.07.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Ricketts RM 1982. The biologic significance of the divine proportion and Fibonacci series. Am J Orthod 81: 351–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Livio M. The Golden Ratio: The Story of Phi, The World's Most Astonishing Number. New York: Broadway Books; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Vegter F, Hage JJ 2000. Clinical Anthropometry and Canons of the Face in Historical Perspective. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery 106(5):1090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Rossetti A, De Menezes M, Rosati R, Ferrario VF, Sforza C 2013. The role of the golden proportion in the evaluation of facial esthetics. Angle Orthod 83(5): pp. 801–808. 10.2319/111812-883.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Saraf S, Saraf P 2013. The Golden Proportion: Key To The Secret Of Beauty. Internet J Plast Surg 9 (1). [Google Scholar]
- 8. Preston JD 1993. The golden proportion revisited. J Esthet Dent 5: 247–251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Seghers MJ, Longacre JJ, de Stefano GA 1964. The golden proportion and beauty. Plast Reconstr Surg 34: 382–386. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Jefferson Y 2004. Facial beauty-establishing a universal standard. Int J Orthod Milwaukee 15:9–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Medici Filho E, Martins MV, dos Santos da Silva MA, Castilho JC, de Moraes LC, Gil CT 2007. Divine proportions and facial esthetics after manipulation of frontal photographs. World J Orthod 8:103–8 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Pancherz H, Knapp V, Erbe C, Heiss AM 2010. Divine Proportions in Attractive and nonattractive faces. World J Orthod 11(1):27–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Moss JP, Linney AD, Lowey MN 1995. The use of three- dimensional techniques in facial esthetics. Semin Orthod 1:94–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Ricketts RM 1982. Divine proportion in facial esthetics. Clin Plast. Surg 9:401–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Tripathi AA, Tandon RP, Hantodkar N 2014. Facial Divine Proportions in Attractive North Indian Females: A Photographic Study. World J Dent 4(1):41–46. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Kiekens RM, Kuijpers-Jagtman AM, van 't Hof MA, van 't Hof BE, Maltha JC. 2008. Putative golden proportions as predictors of facial esthetics in adolescents. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 134(4): 480–483. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2006.10.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Schmid K, Marx D, Samal A. Computation of a face attractiveness index based on neoclassical canons, symmetry, and golden ratios 2006. Faculty Publications, Department of Statistics, University of Nebraska. Paper 12. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Ricketts RM 1981. The golden divider. J Clin Orthod 15: 752–759. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Nanda R, Nanda RS 1969. Cephalometric study of the dentofacial complex of North Indians. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 39 (1): 22–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Downs WB 1956. Analysis of the dentofacial profile. Angle Orthod 26: 191–212. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Erbay EF, Canikliog˘lu CM 2002. Soft tissue profile in AnatolianTurkish adults: part II. Comparison of different soft tissue analyses in the evaluation of beauty. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 121:65–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Peerlings RH, Kuijpers-Jagtman AM, Hoesksma JB 1995. A photographic scale to measure facial esthetics. Eur J Orthod 17(2):101–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Jefferson Y 1993. Facial esthetics—presentation of an ideal face. J Gen Orthod 4(1):18–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Sunilkumar LN, Jadhav KS, Nazirkar G, Singh S, Nagmode PS, Ali M 2013. Assessment of Facial Golden Proportions among North Maharashtrian Population. J Int Oral Health 5(3):48–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Stoner MM 1955. A photometric analysis of the facial profile. Am J Orthod 41:453–469. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Packiriswamy V, Kumar P, Rao M 2012. Identification of facial shape by applying golden ratio to the facial measurements: an interracial study in malaysian population. N Am J Med Sci 4(12): 624–9. 10.4103/1947-2714.104312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Saraswathi P 2007. The golden proportion and its application to the human face. European Journal of Anatomy 11, 177–180. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Lazzari Marques PAP, Castro MI, Rodrigues CK, Ricardo M, Eros P 2012. Photometric study of divine proportion and its correlation with facial attractiveness. Dent Press J Orthod 17(2): 124–131. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Thareja V, Shivaprakash G, Shamnur N, Kumar GA 2012. An Appraisal of Indian Profile Attractiveness using Digital Image Morphing. J Ind Orthod Soc 46(1):9–16. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Ferrario VF, Sforza C, Poggio CE, Tartaglia G 1995. Facial morphometry of television actresses compared with normal women. J Oral Maxillofac Surg 53:1008–1014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Naini FB, Donaldson AN, McDonald F, Cobourne MT 2012. Assessing the influence of chin prominence on perceived attractiveness in the orthognathic patient, clinician and layperson. Int J Oral Maxillofac Surg. 41:839–846. 10.1016/j.ijom.2012.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Purmal K, Alam MK, Norzakiah MZ 2013. Cephalometric norms of Malaysian adult Indian. Int Med J 20(2): 192–196. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Alam MK, Basri R, Kathiravan P, Sikder MA, Saifuddin M, Iida J 2012. A soft tissue cephalomeric analysis for Bangladeshi adult using Holdway’s analysis. Int Med J 19(4): 333–336. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Legan H, Burstone CJ 1980. Soft tissue cephalometric analysis for orthognathic surgery.J Oral Surg 38: 744–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Alam MK, Basri R, Purmal P, Sikder MA, Saifuddin M, Iida J 2013. Cephalometric lip morphology in Bangladeshi population. Int Med J 20(2): 201–203. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Farkas LG, Katic MJ, Forrest CR, Alt KW, Bagic I, Baltadjiev G 2005. International anthropometric study of facial morphology in various ethnic groups/races. J Craniofac Surg 16: 615–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Raji JM, Garb SH, Numan AI, Waziri MA, Maina MB 2010. Morphological evaluation of head and face shapes in a North-Eastern Nigerian population. Aust J Basic Appl Sci 4: 3338–41. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Husein OF, Sepehr A, Garg R, Sina-Khadiv M, Gattu S, Waltzman J, et al. 2010. Anthropometric and aesthetic analysis of the Indians American woman's face. J Plast Reconstr Aesthet Surg 63:1825–31. 10.1016/j.bjps.2009.10.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Erika N, Uldis T, Dzintra K 2005. Craniofacial anthropometry in a group of healthy latvian residents. Acta Medica Lituanica 12:47–53. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Bull R, Rumsey N 1988. The social psychology of facial appearance. Springer-Verlag, New York. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Alley TR, Hildebrandt KA 1988. Determinants and consequences of facial aesthetics Alley TR (Ed.), Social and applied aspects of perceiving faces, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale:101–140. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Iliffe A 1960. A study of preferences in feminine beauty. Br J Psychol 51:267–273. [Google Scholar]
- 43. Naini FB, Moss JP, Daljit SG 2006. The enigma of facial beauty: Esthetics, proportions, deformity, and controversy. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 130(3): 277–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Martin JG 1964. Racial ethnocentrism and judgment of beauty. J Soc Psycho 63: 59–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Alam MK, Leonardo SE, Sato Y, Iida J 2006 Comparison of facial appearance awareness between Indian-subcontinents and Japanese laypersons. IADR 84th General Session, Brisbane, Australia.
- 46.Leonardo SE, Alam MK, Sato Y, Iida J 2006 The awareness of own facial appearance and profile in Japanese-Brazilian laypersons. 65th Annual Meeting of The Japanese Orthodontic Society, the 1st Joint Meeting of the Japanese Orthodontic Society and the Korean Association of Orthodontist, SapporoJapan.
- 47.Alam MK, Leonardo SE, Sato Y, Iida J 2006 A study regarding the awareness of facial appearance and their profile of Indian-subcontinent laypersons. 65th Annual Meeting of The Japanese Orthodontic Society, the 1st Joint Meeting of the Japanese Orthodontic Society and the Korean Association of Orthodontist, Sapporo, Japan.
- 48.Luppanapornlarp S, Prompitak N, Boonprathum S, Sato Y, Iida J 2014 Awareness and Satisfaction of Personal Facial Appearance Among Thai Laypersons. IADR General Session and Exhibition, Cape Town, South Africa, June 26, 2014. Available: https://iadr.confex.com/iadr/14iags/webprogram/Paper190252.html
- 49. Moore-Jansen PH, Ousley SD, Jantz RL 1994. Data collection procedures for forensic skeletal material Report No.: 48. Knoxville, TN: Department of Anthropology, the University of Tennessee. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Zimbler MS, Ham J 2005. Aesthetic facial analysis Cummings Otolaryngology Head & Neck Surgery. 4th ed Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Inc: 513–528. [Google Scholar]
- 51. Naini FB 2011. Facial aesthetics: concepts and clinical diagnosis. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Lew KKK, Ho KK, Keng SB, Ho KH 1992. Soft-tissue cephalometric norms in Chinese adults with aesthetic facial profiles. J Oral Maxillofa Surg 50: 1184–1189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Ngeow WC, Aljunid ST 2009. Craniofacial anthropometric norms of Malays. Singapore Med J 50: 525–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Ngeow WC, Aljunid ST 2009. Craniofacial anthropometric norms of Malaysian Indians. Indian J Dent Res 20: 313–19. 10.4103/0970-9290.57372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Jeremić D, Sanja K, Vulović M, Sazdanović M, Sazdanović P, Jovanović B, et al. 2013. Anthropometric Study of the Facial Index in the Population of Central Serbia. Arch Biol Sci Belgrade 65 (3): 1163–1168. [Google Scholar]
- 56. Kumar M, Muzzafar Lone M 2013. The Study of Facial Index among Haryanvi Adults. Int J Sci Res 2(9):51–53. [Google Scholar]
- 57. Milutinovic J, Zelic K, Nedeljkovic N 2014. Evaluation of Facial Beauty Using Anthropometric Proportions. Sci World J Article ID 428250, 8 pages. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Bashour M 2006. An objective system for measuring facial attractiveness. Plast Reconstr Surg 118(3):757–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Vegter F, Joris Hage J 2000. Clinical Anthropometry and Canons of the Face in Historical Perspective. Plast Reconst Surg 106(5): 1090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Downs WB 1956. Analysis of the dentofacial profile. Angle Orthod 26: 191–212. [Google Scholar]
- 61. Nanda R, Nanda RS 1969. Cephalometric study of the dentofacial complex of North Indians. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 39 (1): 22–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Peerlings RH, Kuijpers-Jagtman AM, Hoesksma JB 1995. A photographic scale to measure facial esthetics. Eur J Orthod 17(2):101–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Czarnecki ST, Nanda RS, Currier GF 1993. Perceptions of a balanced facial profile. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orhop 104:180–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Polk MS Jr, Farman AG, Yancey JA, Gholston LR, Johnson BE 1995. Soft tissue profile: a survey of African-American preference. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 108: 90–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Ioi H, Nakata S, Nakasima A, Counts AL 2005. Anteroposterior lip positions of the most-favored Japanese facial profiles. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 128: 206–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Mantzikos T 1998. Esthetic soft tissue profile preferences among the Japanese population. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 114:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Naini FB, Donaldson AN, McDonald F, Cobourne MT 2012. Assessing the influence of chin prominence on perceived attractiveness in the orthognathic patient, clinician and layperson. Int J Oral Maxillofac Surg 41:839–846. 10.1016/j.ijom.2012.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Maganzini AL, Tseng JYK, Epstein JZ 2000. Perception of facial esthetics by native Chinese participants by using manipulated digital imagery techniques. Angle Orthod 70:393–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Soh J, Chew MT, Wong HB 2005. A comparative assessment of the perception of Chinese facial profile esthetics. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 127:692–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Soh J, Chew MT, Wong HB 2005. Professional assessment of facial profile attractiveness. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 128: 201–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Kuroda S, Sugahara T, Takabatake S 2009. Influence of anteroposterior mandibular positions on facial attractiveness in Japanese adults. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 135(4): 73–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. de Almeida MD, Farias ACR, Bittencourt MAV 2010. Influence of mandibular sagittal position on facial esthetics. Dent Press J Orthod 15(2): 87–96. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Foster EJ 1973. Profile preferences among diversified groups. Angle Orthod 43:34–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Lines PA, Lines CA 1978. Profilemetrics and facial esthetics. Am J Orthod 73:648–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Mejia-Maidl M, Evans CA, Viana G, Anderson NK, Giddon DB 2005. Preferences for facial profiles between Mexican Americans and Caucasians. Angle Orthod 75:763–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper.