Abstract
Background and Aims
Adolescents with substance use disorders (SUD) exhibit hyposensitivity to pleasant internally generated (interoceptive) stimuli and hypersensitivity to external rewarding stimuli. It is unclear whether similar patterns exist for aversive interoceptive stimuli. We compared activation in the insular cortex and other brain regions during the anticipation and experience of aversive stimuli between adolescents with SUD and those without.
Design and participants
Cross-sectional experimental study with two groups: Adolescents (ages 15–17) with an alcohol or marijuana SUD (n=18) and healthy comparison subjects (CON, n=15). Participants were recruited by distributing flyers at local high schools.
Setting
Keck Imaging Center, University of California San Diego, USA.
Measurements
Behavioral and neural responses to a continuous performance task with inspiratory breathing load recorded during an fMRI session. Questionnaires assessed lifetime drug use, anxiety, sensation seeking, impulsivity, affect, and bodily awareness. Visual analogue scales assessed drug craving and breathing load responses.
Findings
Across subjects, experience of breathing load elicited greater bilateral anterior and posterior insula (AI and PI, respectively) activation than anticipation (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05). SUD exhibited greater left AI and bilateral PI activation during breathing load than anticipation, compared with CON (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05). In contrast, CON showed greater activation during anticipation than breathing load in left PI, compared with SUD (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05).
Conclusions
Adolescents with alcohol and marijuana substance use disorders may be hypersensitive to aversive interoceptive stimuli.
Introduction
Adolescence is an important time for exploring and risk-taking, which can involve experimentation with drugs. Among U.S. 12th graders, 68% have consumed alcohol and 46% have tried marijuana (1). Given that substance use during adolescence increases risk for adulthood addiction (2–5), investigating potential risk markers for addiction may help develop treatment targets and early interventions aimed at changing processing of visceral bodily sensations, for example by mindfulness meditation (6).
Interoception (7,8), the processing of internal bodily sensations, is thought to moderate impaired decision processes that may drive addiction (9–13). Adolescents may use substances to either amplify or dampen the influence of bodily sensations on decision-making. For example, an adolescent feeling nervous about attending a party may drink alcohol to reduce bodily sensations of anxiety. One characteristic of addiction is an impaired interoceptive system, causing inaccurate registration or correction of bodily sensations. Interoception is important for homeostasis (7,8), driving approach or avoidance of stimuli and allocating resources to regain perceived equilibrium (14). One’s predicted versus actual internal state (i.e., body prediction error; (11)) may motivate the individual to take substances to feel better or to avoid withdrawal (9).
Insular cortex is a critical neural substrate for interoception and addiction (13,15–17). Individuals with nicotine, marijuana, and cocaine use disorders show increased insular reactivity to drug-related cues (18–20). Research demonstrates that: (a) alcohol-dependent teenagers exhibit insular hyperactivity to alcohol cues (21); and (b) insular alcohol cue response predicts increased drinking and alcohol-related problems in college students (22). In contrast, studies involving non-drug cues reveal that lower insula activation is linked to adolescent alcohol/nicotine substance use disorders (SUD) within the context of decision-making tasks and/or non-drug rewards (23–25). Taken together, individuals with SUD appear to exhibit insular hypersensitivity to substance-related stimuli, but hyposensitivity in other contexts.
Interoception research indicates that hedonic aspects of drinking moderate left insula white matter volume and frequency of binge drinking in adolescents (26). Moreover, cigarette-smoking teenagers show an exposure-dependent decrease in right insular cortical thickness (27). Adults with SUD exhibit anterior insula (AI) attenuation during anticipation and experience of a soft brush stroke to the palm/forearm as a pleasant interoceptive manipulation (28). In contrast, adolescents with SUD exhibit AI hyperactivity during soft touch, suggesting that neural patterns of interoceptive responsiveness might differ in early versus chronic stages of SUD (29). Although hedonic aspects of interoception have been investigated with cued reward paradigms, less is known about links between aversive interoception and SUD in adolescents. Investigating aversive stimuli is crucial, considering discomfort experienced by addicted users during withdrawal, which might trigger future use.
An inspiratory breathing load paradigm (30–32) was used to investigate aversive interoception in adults with SUD and showed lower insular and/or anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activation during breathing load than controls (30,31), suggesting that the aversive interoceptive system is hyporesponsive. Given that adolescents exhibit exaggerated brain activation for pleasant stimuli (decreased insula activation for adults (28) versus increased activation for adolescents; (29)), adolescents with SUD may show a hyper-reactive interoceptive response to aversive stimuli. In support of this assertion, adolescents at high risk for alcohol problems are more sensitive to negative stimuli and more motivated to drink alcohol in negative emotion-arousing situations than low-risk adolescents (33).
Another aspect of aversive stimuli is anticipation of negative events, where predictability can lessen psychological impact. Inability to anticipate an aversive stimulus could reflect an inhibitory control deficit linked to SUD development (34). Studies show that adolescents who: (a) fail to anticipate aversive events are more likely to be alcohol and nicotine dependent (34); and (b) do not take advantage of the predictability of aversive stimuli (considered poor modulators) have more SUDs than good modulators (35). Lack of cognitive control in adolescent SUD can be explained by a less mature cognitive control system and a hyperactive reward system (36–38). This dual-process model (39) suggests that top-down cognitive control limitations result in reduced ability to regulate bottom-up urges.
To examine whether SUD adolescents possess a dysfunctional aversive interoceptive system, the present investigation used fMRI to examine insula activation in adolescents with current alcohol or marijuana SUD. It was hypothesized that: (a) SUD adolescents would show greater insular activation during the experience of breathing load than healthy controls and (b) SUD would exhibit greater ACC and prefrontal cortex activation than controls, since insula integrates activity from ACC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC; (7,8)), both of which play a role in aversive interoceptive processing in healthy individuals (40) and adults with SUD (30).
Methods
Participants
The University of California San Diego Human Research Protections Program approved this study. Written informed consent was provided by one parent or legal guardian of the adolescent participant, who provided assent. Adolescents (ages 15–17) were recruited by distributing flyers at local high schools. Adolescents were phone screened to rule out: lifetime Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (41) Axis I psychiatric disorder independent of SUD; current use of psychoactive medications; history of major neurological or medical disorder, head trauma with loss of consciousness >5m, learning disability, serious physical health problems, complicated or premature birth, fMRI contraindications (e.g., irremovable metal), left handedness, non-correctable vision or hearing problems, and prenatal alcohol/drug exposure. Participants were compensated $180 for their participation.
Fifteen SUD (9M, 6F) and eighteen healthy controls (CON; 11M, 7F) participated in this study. SUD were defined by: (a) current endorsement of 2+ DSM-5 (41) SUD criteria for either alcohol or marijuana and (b) alcohol or marijuana use within the last three months. The primary diagnosis for 27% of SUD was alcohol use disorder; the other 73% endorsed marijuana use disorder. Number of SUD criteria endorsed ranged from 2–8 (M=3.4; SD=2.0). CON had very limited lifetime alcohol/marijuana uses and no other illicit drug use (see Table 1). Although most research has focused on the effects of stimulants on the insula, alcohol and marijuana users are included in this study, as methamphetamine use is not as prevalent among 12th graders (1). Our sample was considered sufficient to detect a medium effect size, using a similar study with an experimental task in SUD and CON adolescents as an estimate (29). Participants completed a clinical assessment session, which included self-report questionnaires, and abstained from substance use at least 72 h prior to their fMRI session. Abstinence was confirmed by self-report, urine toxicology and breathalyzer screens.
Table 1.
Subject characteristics by group.
CON n = 18 |
SUD n = 15 |
df | t/X2 | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CHARACTERISTICS | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||
Age | 16.50 | 0.62 | 16.60 | 0.63 | 31 | −0.92 | 0.65 |
Education | 10.67 | 0.59 | 10.67 | 0.90 | 31 | 0.00 | 1.00 |
BSMSS | 48.5a | 12.12 | 51.18b | 11.14 | 29 | −0.64 | 0.53 |
Estimated Verbal IQ | 111.22 | 13.98 | 110.40 | 16.58 | 31 | 0.16 | 0.88 |
YSR Internalizing T-score | 45.00 | 10.36 | 53.27 | 10.90 | 31 | −2.23 | 0.03 |
YSR Externalizing T-score | 44.94 | 10.22 | 58.33 | 9.20 | 31 | −3.92 | 0.00 |
DEMOGRAPHICS | N | % | N | % | |||
Female | 7 | 38.90 | 5 | 33.3 | 1 | 0.11 | 0.74 |
Caucasian | 15 | 83.33 | 12 | 80 | 1 | 0.06 | 0.81 |
Hispanic | 3 | 16.70 | 3 | 20 | 1 | 0.06 | 0.81 |
QUESTIONNAIRES | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||
Barratt Impulsivity Scale | 58.00c | 12.02 | 60.64b | 9.14 | 28 | −0.67 | 0.51 |
Sensation Seeking Scale | 14.72 | 3.79 | 25.14b | 6.05 | 30 | −5.97 | 0.00 |
Body Awareness Questionnaire | 81.17 | 20.22 | 79.13 | 14.19 | 31 | 0.33 | 0.75 |
STAI-T Trait | 33.44 | 9.82 | 39.27 | 11.44 | 31 | −1.57 | 0.13 |
PANAS positive | 27.67 | 7.97 | 27.67 | 6.04 | 31 | 0.00 | 1.00 |
PANAS negative | 12.50 | 3.99 | 13.73 | 4.74 | 31 | −0.81 | 0.42 |
% DRUG USE | N | % | N | % | |||
% Used alcohol in lifetime | 6 | 33.3 | 15 | 100 | 1 | 15.71 | 0.00 |
% Used alcohol in past week | 1 | 5.6 | 10 | 66.7 | 1 | 13.75 | 0.00 |
% Used marijuana in lifetime | 2 | 11.1 | 15 | 100 | 1 | 25.88 | 0.00 |
% Used marijuana in past week | 0 | 0 | 10 | 66.7 | 1 | 17.22 | 0.00 |
% Used other drugs in lifetime | 0 | 0 | 11 | 73.3 | 1 | 19.8 | 0.00 |
% Used other drugs in past week | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0.0 | .d | .d | .d |
LIFETIME DRUG USE | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | U | z | p |
Lifetime alcohol use | 2.44 | 5.26 | 108.33 | 105.37 | 0.00 | −0.52 | 0.00 |
Lifetime marijuana use | 0.28 | 0.96 | 351.87 | 284.49 | 4.50 | −4.84 | 0.00 |
Lifetime other drug use | 0 | 0.00 | 45.20 | 75.02 | 36.00 | −4.27 | 0.00 |
n=17;
n=14;
n=16;
no statistics because ‘other drugs used in past week’ is a constant.
CON = Control group; SUD = Substance Use Disorder group; BSMSS = Barratt Simplified Measure of Social Status; YSR = Youth Self Report; STAI-T = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; SD = Standard Deviation; U=Mann-Whitney U test result.
Clinical Interview Session
The Semi-Structured Assessment for Drug Dependence and Alcoholism (SSADDA (42)) was used to assess lifetime DSM-5 substance use history and SUD diagnoses, which were confirmed by a psychiatrist (M.P.P.) and psychologist (S.F.T.). The Timeline Followback (TLFB; (43)), a retrospective calendar-based measure, was used to assess substance use for the past 30 days.
Questionnaires were administered to assess characteristics known to correlate with SUD, including the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS-V; (44)), Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; (45)), and Youth Self Report (YSR; (46)). Barratt Simplified Measure of Social Status (BSMSS; (47)) was completed using parental responses to measure socioeconomic status (SES). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; (48)) was used to measure affect, and trait anxiety with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; (49)). Verbal IQ was measured with the Wide Range Achievement Test 4 (WRAT4; (50)). The Body Awareness Questionnaire (BAQ; (51)) measured attentiveness to non-emotive bodily processes. Substance craving was measured with a 10-cm visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from ‘no craving’ to ‘most craving ever experienced’.
FMRI Session: Aversive Inspiratory Breathing Load Paradigm
This continuous performance task (CPT) requires participants to anticipate and experience an aversive interoceptive stimulus (see Figure 1). Subjects wore a nose clip and breathed through a mouthpiece with a non-rebreathing valve (2600 series, Hans Rudolph). Breathing equipment was attached to the scanner head coil eliminating the need for subjects to contract mouth muscles. A hose connected the mouthpiece to the inspiratory resistance load of 40 cmH2O/L/sec, which consisted of a sintered bronze disk in a Plexiglas tube. This disk partially limits the airflow through the breathing tube, producing the resistance load. Padding between the coil and headphones was used to keep the subjects’ heads snug in the coil to minimize movement.
Figure 1.
Inspiratory breathing load.
Prior to scanning, participants were given task instructions, experienced 60s segments of various breathing loads (no load, 10, 20, and 40 cmH2O/L/sec), and then completed VAS ratings of breathing load on a 10cm scale, from ‘not at all’ to ‘extremely’ on the 16 dimensions (see Table 2; 32,52). These loads were used prior to the scan to investigate whether increased load altered VAS and also to familiarize participants with equipment. After the scan, participants were asked to complete VAS ratings for breathing load experienced during the CPT.
Table 2.
Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) scores for breathing load.
CON N = 18 |
SUD N = 15 |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
VAS item | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | df | t | p |
Pleasant | 2.16 | 1.72 | 1.16 | 1.13 | 30 | 2.01 | 0.05 |
Unpleasant | 5.54 | 2.97 | 5.86 | 3.01 | 30 | −0.29 | 0.77 |
Intense | 3.62 | 3.11 | 4.36 | 2.62 | 30 | −0.71 | 0.48 |
Tingling Sensations | 1.93 | 2.83 | 1.44 | 1.98 | 30 | 0.56 | 0.58 |
Fear of Losing Control | 1.21 | 1.93 | 1.51 | 2.18 | 30 | −0.42 | 0.68 |
Faintness | 1.53 | 2.32 | 0.99 | 1.71 | 30 | 0.73 | 0.47 |
Fear of Dying | 0.37 | 1.10 | 0.54 | 1.83 | 30 | −0.33 | 0.75 |
Unreality | 0.94 | 1.43 | 0.67 | 1.88 | 30 | 0.47 | 0.64 |
Hot/Cold Flushes | 0.66 | 1.35 | 0.24 | 0.46 | 22 | 1.21 | 0.24 |
Trembling | 0.42 | 1.12 | 0.91 | 1.82 | 30 | −0.93 | 0.36 |
Choking | 0.46 | 1.13 | 0.89 | 2.08 | 30 | −0.75 | 0.46 |
Fear of Going Crazy | 0.41 | 1.12 | 0.49 | 1.56 | 30 | −0.18 | 0.86 |
Abdominal Distress | 0.75 | 1.82 | 0.71 | 1.68 | 30 | 0.07 | 0.95 |
Chest Pain | 0.61 | 1.35 | 0.81 | 1.88 | 30 | −0.36 | 0.73 |
Palpitations | 0.48 | 1.22 | 0.71 | 1.78 | 30 | 0.42 | 0.68 |
Sweating | 0.36 | 1.16 | 0.11 | 0.23 | 30 | 0.80 | 0.43 |
Dizziness | 1.16 | 1.89 | 1.19 | 1.83 | 30 | −0.05 | 0.96 |
VAS = Visual Analogue Scale; CON = Control group; SUD = Substance Use Disorder group; SD = Standard Deviation; The variable Hot/Cold Flushes had unequal variances.
Participants performed the CPT with intermittent inspiratory breathing load during fMRI recording. Subjects were asked to respond quickly and accurately to the orientation (left or right) of the arrow with the button box. Briefly, background color served as a breathing load cue. Blue indicated no breathing load, and a yellow background indicated a 1 in 4 (25%) chance of experiencing the load (see Figure 1). Subjects experienced three conditions: (1) baseline: task performed with a blue background; (2) anticipation: a yellow background indicated a 25% chance of a breathing load; and (3) breathing load: participant experiences 40s of resistive loaded breathing. Both response accuracy and reaction time (RT) were obtained. CO2 levels of respiration were recorded via nasal cannula at a rate of 40 Hz. Event-related fMRI data collection parameters are explained in detail elsewhere (31).
FMRI Data Analysis
Single-Subject Image Analysis Pathway
fMRI data were preprocessed with the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software package (Cox, 1996). GE slices were reconstructed into AFNI BRIK format. A temporal region containing the largest span with fewest voxel-wise outliers was used as a 3d registration baseline. Six motion parameters (dx, dy, dz, roll, pitch, yaw) were obtained across the time series. Data were visually checked to ensure that any repetitions with movement were removed. Motion parameters were used as regressors to adjust EPI intensity changes due to motion artifacts. The functional EPI underwent automatic co-registration to the high-resolution anatomical image and was visually inspected to confirm successful alignment. New outliers were generated based on whether a given time point greatly exceeded the mean number of voxel outliers for the time series. Deconvolution was performed to determine breathing load task decision phase activations. Six movement regressors, a baseline/linear drift regressor, and two decision making regressors (anticipation, breathing load) were convolved with a modified hemodynamic response function (53–55). The baseline task condition served as the baseline for this analysis. A Gaussian Spatial Filter (4mm FWHM) was used to blur data to account for anatomical differences. Automated Talairach transformations were applied to anatomical images and EPIs were subsequently transformed into Talairach space. Percent signal change (PSC) was determined by dividing the signal for each regressor of interest (anticipation, breathing load) by the baseline regressor and multiplying by 100. Five participants were excluded because of excessive movement during fMRI (n=2), anatomical abnormalities (n=1) or fMRI acquisition errors (n=2), leaving a total of 33 participants.
Group Level Analyses
For each voxel, a linear mixed effects (LME) model was computed in R (56), using Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MLE) to examine group differences in brain activation. Group (CON, SUD) and condition (anticipation, breathing load) were modeled as fixed factors, and subject was a random factor. The dependent measure was PSC. A threshold adjustment method based on Monte-Carlo simulations was used to guard against identifying false positive areas of activation. Based on AFNI AlphaSim, a voxel-wise p<.01 was associated with a 768 µL volume threshold (whole-brain mask; 12 contiguous voxels) for a clusterwise probability of p<.05 (two-sided). To examine a priori predictions, masks for bilateral insula and ACC, defined by the Talairach atlas (57), were employed with a minimum cluster volume of 384 µL (6 contiguous voxels) for p<.05 (two-sided) corrected for multiple comparisons. Anticipation and breathing load activation were both initially compared to baseline activity. The group main effect is the difference between CON and SUD across all conditions. The condition main effect examines anticipation versus breathing load. The group by condition interaction examines between-group differences in anticipation and breathing load, and within-group differences between anticipation and breathing load.
Brain Behavior Relationships
PSC (for brain regions with a significant group by condition interaction) were correlated within-group with lifetime drug use, drug craving, STAI-T, BAQ, PANAS, BIS, SSS-V, YSR and VAS scores. These 9 variables were correlated with 6 brain regions using SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY), resulting in 51 comparisons. No Bonferroni corrections were applied since correlations were considered exploratory.
Behavioral Data Analysis
SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY) was used to analyze RT and accuracy using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) to examine within-subject differences as a function of condition (with baseline subtracted) and between-subject group differences. Pre- and post-VAS breathing ratings were examined using independent samples t-tests between groups. CO2 levels during anticipation and breathing load (with baseline subtracted) and between-subject group differences were examined using a repeated measures ANOVA.
Results
Subject Characteristics
Demographics
Groups did not differ significantly in age, gender, education, verbal IQ, or ethnicity. SUD endorsed higher sensation seeking, internalizing, and externalizing scores than CON but groups did not differ on impulsivity (see Table 1). SUD reported greater lifetime substance use than CON (see Table 1). Pre-scan VAS ratings showed that, across participants, responses were significantly affected by breathing load for pleasantness (F(2, 64)=5.8, p<.01), unpleasantness (F(1.62, 51.84)=5.01, p<.02), and intensity (F(2,64)=4.36,p<.02). SUD had more lifetime cigarette use (M=164.43, SE=73.76) than CON (M=0.17, SE=0.17; t(13), p=.04). Multiple regressions predicting brain activation during breathing load within SUD using alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use as predictors found no significant effect of cigarette use on BOLD response. For post-scan VAS ratings, SUD rated breathing load as less pleasant (M=1.16, SD=1.13) than CON (M=2.16, SD=1.72; t(30), p=.05; see Table 2).
Behavioral Responses
RT was similar for anticipation (M=645.98, SE=21.84) and breathing load (M=630.63, SE=29.00) for all participants (F(1,24)=.81, p=.38) and did not differ between groups for both anticipation (MCON=645.62, SECON=32.92; MSUD=646.47, SESUD=27.35) and breathing load (MCON=627.06, SECON=44.58; MSUD=634.02, SESUD=31.29), F(1,24)=1.84, p=.19. No significant interaction between group and condition emerged (F(1,24)=.05, p=.82). Groups were equally accurate (>97% correct on average) (F(1,25)=.003, p=.97). There was a trend for both groups to be less accurate during anticipation (98.9%) than breathing load (99.4%; F(1,25)=4.24, p=.05). There was no group by condition interaction effect (F(1,25)=.20, p=.67).
CO2 Levels
CO2 output did not differ between groups (F(1,24)=.29, p=.64); however CO2 was lower during breathing load than anticipation across participants (F(1,24)=19.29, p<.01). No group by condition interaction effect emerged (F(1,24)=.23, p=.64), replicating prior work using this breathing paradigm (30,31,58). CO2 was not significantly correlated with breathing load PSC in any brain region from group LME analyses.
BOLD Response Contrast
Condition and Group Main Effects
Across subjects, the ROI analysis showed that loaded breathing elicited greater bilateral AI and PI activation than anticipation (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05; see Figure 2). Table 3 indicates results from both the ROI and whole brain analyses that loaded breathing induced a large change in BOLD response affecting several areas of the brain (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05). Furthermore, the whole brain analysis showed that CON exhibited greater precentral gyrus activation than SUD across conditions (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05; see Table 3). There was no group main effect for the insula or ACC.
Figure 2.
The main task effect showed greater bilateral insula activation during breathing load than anticipation across participants (F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05). Error bars reflect ± 1 standard error.
Table 3.
Task effect: fMRI results for main effect of condition and main effect of group.
Main effect of Condition | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mask | Voxels | Volume (uL) | x | y | z | L / R | BA | Center of mass | Result |
Whole brain | 602 | 38528 | −47 | −7 | 26 | L | Precentral Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 585 | 37440 | 49 | −8 | 26 | R | Precentral Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 417 | 26688 | −10 | −70 | −17 | L | Declive | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 363 | 23232 | 1 | −17 | 58 | R | Medial Frontal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 312 | 19968 | 29 | −42 | 13 | R | Caudate | Ant>Plug | |
rROI | 118 | 7552 | 39 | 0 | 12 | R | Anterior & Posterior Insula | Plug>Ant | |
rROI | 89 | 5696 | −37 | 2 | 9 | L | Anterior & Posterior Insula | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 85 | 5440 | −1 | −14 | −26 | L | Brain | Ant>Plug | |
Whole brain | 76 | 4864 | −41 | −25 | −9 | L | Parahippocampal Gyrus | Ant>Plug | |
Whole brain | 49 | 3136 | −26 | −46 | 9 | L | Parahippocampal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 32 | 2048 | 17 | 5 | −2 | R | Lentiform Nucleus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 27 | 1728 | −5 | −50 | 58 | L | Precuneus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 19 | 1216 | −34 | 37 | 36 | L | Middle Frontal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 19 | 1216 | −9 | −12 | 43 | L | Paracentral Lobule | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 17 | 1088 | −7 | 61 | −2 | L | Superior Frontal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 16 | 1024 | 48 | −48 | 21 | R | Superior Temporal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 14 | 896 | −21 | 0 | 38 | L | Cingulate Gyrus | Ant>Plug | |
Whole brain | 13 | 832 | −56 | −45 | −12 | L | Middle Temporal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 12 | 768 | −31 | 52 | −3 | L | Middle Frontal Gyrus | Plug>Ant | |
Whole brain | 12 | 768 | −2 | −95 | 2 | L | Cuneus | Ant>Plug | |
Whole brain | 12 | 768 | −13 | −17 | 9 | L | Thalamus | Plug>Ant | |
Main Effect of Group | |||||||||
Whole brain | 12 | 768 | −27 | −5 | 33 | L | Precentral Gyrus | CON>SUD |
Plug = breathing load; Ant = Anticipation; CON = Control group; SUD = Substance Use Disorder group; L = left hemisphere; R = right hemisphere; rROI = Restricted ROI based on hypothesis; Talairach coordinates reflect center of mass. Regions reflect significant clusters of at least 12 contiguous voxels (whole brain; 768µL) or 6 contiguous voxels (rROI; 384µL) meeting the F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05 threshold corrected for multiple comparisons via AlphaSim.
Group by Condition Interaction
The whole brain analysis showed that groups differed by condition in several regions. Generally, CON showed greater activation during anticipation than SUD, whereas during breathing load, SUD showed greater activation than CON (see Table 4). During anticipation, CON exhibited greater right PI activation than SUD. During breathing load, SUD showed greater activation than CON in right PI, left parahippocampal gyrus, and left superior temporal gyrus. Moreover, the ROI analysis showed that CON exhibited greater left PI activation during anticipation than SUD.
Table 4.
fMRI results for interaction effect of group by condition.
Mask | Voxels | Volume (uL) |
x | y | z | L/R | Center of mass | BA | Ant | Plug |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Whole brain | 36 | 2304 | 45 | −7 | 17 | R | Posterior Insula | BA 13 | CON>SUD | SUD>CON |
Whole brain | 26 | 1664 | −33 | 43 | 25 | L | Middle Frontal Gyrus | BA 10 | CON=SUD | SUD>CON |
Whole brain | 22 | 1408 | −24 | −21 | −22 | L | Parahippocampal Gyrus | BA 35 | CON>SUD | CON=SUD |
Whole brain | 20 | 1280 | −30 | 5 | −27 | L | Uncus | BA 38 | CON=SUD | SUD>CON |
Whole brain | 15 | 960 | −59 | −36 | −11 | L | Middle Temporal Gyrus | BA 21 | CON=SUD | SUD>CON |
Whole brain | 14 | 896 | −31 | 24 | 10 | L | Anterior Insula | BA 13 | CON=SUD | SUD>CON |
Whole brain | 13 | 832 | −45 | 4 | −7 | L | Superior Temporal Gyrus | BA 38 | CON>SUD | CON=SUD |
Whole brain | 13 | 832 | 41 | 25 | 10 | R | Inferior Frontal Gyrus | BA 13 | CON=SUD | SUD>CON |
rROI | 8 | 512 | −45 | 4 | −6 | L | Posterior Insula | BA 38 | CON>SUD | CON=SUD |
L = left hemisphere; R = right hemisphere; Ant = Anticipation; Plug = breathing load; CON = Control group; SUD = Substance Use Disorder group; rROI = Restricted ROI based on hypothesis; Talairach coordinates reflect center of mass; BA = Brodmann Area. Regions reflect significant clusters of at least 12 contiguous voxels (whole brain; 768µL) or 6 contiguous voxels (rROI; 384µL) meeting the F(1,31)=4.16, p<.05 threshold corrected for multiple comparisons via AlphaSim.
Within-group differences indicate that SUD showed greater activation during breathing load than anticipation in bilateral PI, left AI, left middle frontal gyrus, and right inferior frontal gyrus (see Figures 3–4). CON showed the opposite pattern, with greater activation during anticipation than breathing load in left PI (see Figure 3).
Figure 3.
The group by condition interaction showed: (1) between-group differences, wherein adolescents with substance use disorder (SUD) exhibited greater right posterior insula and left anterior insula activation than healthy controls (CON) during breathing load; and (2) within-group differences, wherein SUD exhibited consistent modulation in bilateral posterior insula and left anterior insula as a function of anticipation and breathing load conditions, but CON did not. Error bars reflect ± 1 standard error. * p=<.05; **p<.01.
Figure 4.
The group by condition interaction showed: (1) between-group differences, wherein adolescents with substance use disorder (SUD) exhibited greater left middle frontal gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus than healthy controls (CON) during breathing load; and (2) within-group differences, wherein SUD exhibited consistent modulation in these regions as a function of anticipation and breathing load conditions, but CON did not. Error bars reflect ± 1 standard error. * p=<.05; **p<.01.
Brain Behavior Relationships
To address greater brain activation during breathing load in SUD compared to (a) anticipation in SUD and (b) breathing load in CON, self-report and drug use measures were correlated within brain activation within SUD during breathing load. SUD with lower scores on the BAQ showed greater left middle frontal gyrus activation (r=−.62, p=.02; Figure 5a) and right PI activation (r=−.58,p=.02; Figure 5b) during breathing load.
Figure 5.
Brain activation during breathing load in adolescents with substance use disorder (SUD). SUD exhibited a negative relationship between (a) left middle frontal gyrus (r=−.62, p=.02) and (b) right posterior insula (r=−.58, p=.02) and Body Awareness Questionnaire (BAQ) scores. Brain activation during breathing load for SUD shows a positive relationship between (c) left middle frontal gyrus and visual analogue scale (VAS) intensity of the breathing load (r=.52, p=.05). SUD exhibited a negative relationship between (d) right inferior frontal gyrus and sensation seeking (SSS; r=−.56, p=.04).
Discussion
This investigation examined aversive interoceptive processing in adolescents with SUD and yielded three main results. First, consistent with our prediction, SUD showed greater insula activation during breathing load than CON. Second, SUD showed greater breathing-load activation than CON in left middle frontal gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus. SUD, but not CON, exhibited consistent modulation in these frontal regions as a function of condition. Third, SUD rated the breathing load as less pleasant than CON. Taken together, findings suggest that the insula of SUD adolescents is hypersensitive to aversive stimuli.
Our results are consistent with heightened sensitivity exhibited by adolescent SUD during pleasant stimuli processing (29). Not only do adolescents generally show greater striatal sensitivity to valenced stimuli than adults (59), but adolescents with SUD demonstrate greater responses than CON to aversive stimuli in the present study. Thus, SUD adolescents may not be able to accurately predict physiological bodily changes. Differences seen between anticipation and breathing load in SUD might reflect a bodily prediction error. This is illustrated in the PI difference between anticipation and breathing load for SUD (see Figure 3). SUD show a diminished anticipatory response in conjunction with an exaggerated response during breathing load. Perhaps this discrepancy illustrates an inability to make use of the predictability of the aversive event, evident in prior research (34,35). Addiction may represent a chronic imbalance of a homeostatic condition of the body, leading to maladaptive regulation of the internal state through substances (11). Negative reinforcement mechanisms of drug seeking to avoid aversive consequences of withdrawal has been thought to be crucial in addiction development (60). However, we did not find a relationship between drug craving and insula activation in SUD. Previous research shows that SUD exhibit increased insular cue reactivity to drug-related stimuli, thought to be linked to craving (18–20). Craving in cigarette-smoking adolescents is negatively correlated with cortical thickness of right ventral AI (27). Perhaps craving measurements should be completed while the participant is still in the scanner, during the CPT. Insula integrates information from ACC and dlPFC to facilitate goal representation and motivation (17). The present study suggests that this integration may be disrupted in SUD, potentially resulting in craving, thereby overruling non-drug taking goals.
Insular cortex is a crucial component of a neural salience network involved in switching between the default mode network (involved with craving and withdrawal symptoms) and the executive control network (involved in decision-making) (61–63). Increased insula activation in SUD might be related to the inability to disengage from the default mode network, and consequently the inability to switch to the executive control network for top-down control.
Contrary to our prediction, ACC did not show differential activation between groups. ACC activation has been found using the same aversive breathing task in SUD and CON adults, and is thought to monitor and inhibit reactions to aversive stimuli (30,31,40). Perhaps ACC plays a different role in behavioral control for adolescents than for adults. For example, when making risky choices, adults exhibited greater ACC activation than adolescents (64). Moreover, adults showed greater ACC activation than both adolescents and young adults when anticipating pleasant interoceptive stimuli (65). However, youth at high risk for SUD show greater ACC, prefrontal and AI activation than CON in response to risky decisions involving high potential for negative outcomes (66). Thus, it is unclear what role ACC plays in adolescents.
This study has several limitations. First, our sample size was modest and limited to adolescents who endorsed alcohol or marijuana SUD. Future research should recruit larger samples to investigate potential neural differences between substances. A larger sample could also investigate differences between gender, since functional and structural imaging studies have showed differential patterns for heavy drinking adolescents, with females generally showing more abnormalities than males (24,67–69). Second, this cross-sectional study cannot determine whether the dysfunctional interoceptive system reflects a pre-existing condition or a consequence of neurotoxic effects of substance use. Future studies could investigate this directionality similarly to research showing that children with mothers who smoked displayed more impulsivity than children of nonsmokers on a delayed discounting task (70). If adolescents at risk of developing SUD show altered interoceptive function compared to those without family history of SUD, this would support that it is a pre-existing condition.
Despite these limitations, this investigation shows altered interoceptive functioning in adolescents with alcohol/marijuana SUD indicated by: (a) increased insular activation during breathing load, and (b) modulation in insular and frontal regions as a function of anticipation and breathing load compared to CON. Our findings suggest that adolescents with SUD are hypersensitive to aversive stimuli, which may lead to drug-seeking behavior.
Acknowledgments
Data collection and analysis for the present study were funded by NIDA Grant 5P20DA027843. This work was also supported by NIAAA Grant T32 AA013525.
Footnotes
All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, Miech RA. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2013. 2014 May 30;:1–659. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Anthony JC, Petronis KR. Early-onset drug use and risk of later drug problems. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1995 Nov;40(1):9–15. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(95)01194-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Grant BF, Stinson FS, Harford TC. Age at onset of alcohol use and DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: a 12-year follow-up. J Subst Abuse. 2001;13(4):493–504. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(01)00096-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bonomo YA, Bowes G, Coffey C, Carlin JB, Patton GC. Addiction. 12. Vol. 99. Blackwell Science Ltd; 2004. Dec, Teenage drinking and the onset of alcohol dependence: a cohort study over seven years; pp. 1520–1528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Grant JD, Scherrer JF, Lynskey MT, Lyons MJ, Eisen SA, Tsuang MT, et al. Psychol Med. 1. Vol. 36. Cambridge University Press; 2006. Jan, Adolescent alcohol use is a risk factor for adult alcohol and drug dependence: evidence from a twin design; pp. 109–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Farb NAS, Segal ZV, Anderson AK. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 1. Vol. 8. Oxford University Press; 2013. Jan, Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention; pp. 15–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002 Aug;3(8):655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Craig ADB. How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009 Jan;10(1):59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Naqvi NH, Bechara A. Brain Struct Funct. 5–6. Vol. 214. Springer-Verlag; 2010. Jun, The insula and drug addiction: an interoceptive view of pleasure, urges, and decision-making; pp. 435–450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Paulus MP. Neural basis of reward and craving--a homeostatic point of view. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2007;9(4):379–387. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2007.9.4/mpaulus. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Paulus MP, Tapert SF, Schulteis G. The role of interoception and alliesthesia in addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009 Nov;94(1):1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Verdejo-Garcia A, Clark L, Dunn BD. The role of interoception in addiction: a critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012 Sep;36(8):1857–1869. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Paulus MP, Stewart JL. Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2014 Jan;76 Pt B:342–350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Craig AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003 Aug;13(4):500–505. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. Science. 5811. Vol. 315. American Association for the Advancement of Science; 2007. Jan 26, Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking; pp. 531–534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Naqvi NH, Bechara A. The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends Neurosci. 2009 Jan;32(1):56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Naqvi NH, Gaznick N, Tranel D, Bechara A. The insula: a critical neural substrate for craving and drug seeking under conflict and risk. In: Kingstone A, Miller MB, editors. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1. Vol. 1316. 2014. May, pp. 53–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Goudriaan AE, de Ruiter MB, van den Brink W, Oosterlaan J, Veltman DJ. Addict Biol. 4. Vol. 15. Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2010. Oct, Brain activation patterns associated with cue reactivity and craving in abstinent problem gamblers, heavy smokers and healthy controls: an fMRI study; pp. 491–503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Filbey FM, Schacht JP, Myers US, Chavez RS, Hutchison KE. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 31. Vol. 106. National Acad Sciences; 2009. Aug 4, Marijuana craving in the brain; pp. 13016–13021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bonson KR, Grant SJ, Contoreggi CS, Links JM, Metcalfe J, Weyl HL, et al. Neural systems and cue-induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002 Mar;26(3):376–386. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00371-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Tapert SF, Brown GG, Baratta MV, Brown SA. fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addict Behav. 2004 Jan;29(1):33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dager AD, Anderson BM, Rosen R, Khadka S, Sawyer B, Jiantonio-Kelly RE, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) response to alcohol pictures predicts subsequent transition to heavy drinking in college students. Addiction. 2014 Apr;109(4):585–595. doi: 10.1111/add.12437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Crowley TJ, Dalwani MS, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Du YP, Lejuez CW, Raymond KM, et al. Risky decisions and their consequences: neural processing by boys with Antisocial Substance Disorder. In: Zhan W, editor. PLoS ONE. 9. Vol. 5. Public Library of Science; 2010. p. e12835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Caldwell LC, Schweinsburg AD, Nagel BJ, Barlett VC, Brown SA, Tapert SF. Gender and adolescent alcohol use disorders on BOLD (blood oxygen level dependent) response to spatial working memory. Alcohol Alcohol. 2005 May;40(3):194–200. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agh134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rubinstein ML, Luks TL, Dryden WY, Rait MA, Simpson GV. Nicotine Tob Res. 8. Vol. 13. Oxford University Press; 2011. Aug, Adolescent smokers show decreased brain responses to pleasurable food images compared with nonsmokers; pp. 751–755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Chung T, Clark DB. Insula white matter volume linked to binge drinking frequency through enhancement motives in treated adolescents. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014 Jul;38(7):1932–1940. doi: 10.1111/acer.12461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Morales AM, Ghahremani D, Kohno M, Hellemann GS, London ED. Neuropsychopharmacology. 8. Vol. 39. Nature Publishing Group; 2014. Jul, Cigarette exposure, dependence, and craving are related to insula thickness in young adult smokers; pp. 1816–1822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.May AC, Stewart JL, Migliorini R, Tapert SF, Paulus MP. Methamphetamine dependent individuals show attenuated brain response to pleasant interoceptive stimuli. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013 Aug;131(3):238–246. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.05.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Migliorini R, Stewart JL, May AC, Tapert SF, Paulus MP. What do you feel? Adolescent drug and alcohol users show altered brain response to pleasant interoceptive stimuli. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013 Dec 1;133(2):661–668. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stewart JL, May AC, Poppa T, Davenport PW, Tapert SF, Paulus MP. You are the danger: attenuated insula response in methamphetamine users during aversive interoceptive decision-making. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014 Sep 1;142:110–119. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Stewart JL, Juavinett AL, May AC, Davenport PW, Paulus MP. Do you feel alright? Attenuated neural processing of aversive interoceptive stimuli in current stimulant users. Psychophysiology. 2014 Sep 2;:n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Davenport PW, Vovk A. Cortical and subcortical central neural pathways in respiratory sensations. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2009 May 30;167(1):72–86. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2008.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Randall DM, Cox WM. Experimental mood inductions in persons at high and low risk for alcohol problems. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2001 Feb;27(1):183–187. doi: 10.1081/ada-100103126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Taylor J, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, Lykken DT, McGue M. Individual differences in electrodermal responsivity to predictable aversive stimuli and substance dependence. Psychophysiology. 1999 Mar;36(2):193–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Taylor J. Psychophysiology. 6. Vol. 41. Blackwell Publishing; 2004. Nov, Electrodermal reactivity and its association to substance use disorders; pp. 982–989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang G-J, Swanson JM. Mol Psychiatry. 6. Vol. 9. Nature Publishing Group; 2004. Jun 1, Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications; pp. 557–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RCME, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007 Feb;86(2):263–283. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Casey BJ, Jones RM. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: implications for substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010 Dec;49(12):1189–1201. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.017. quiz1285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. A time of change: behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cogn. 2010 Feb;72(1):124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Paulus MP, Flagan T, Simmons AN, Gillis K, Kotturi S, Thom N, et al. Subjecting elite athletes to inspiratory breathing load reveals behavioral and neural signatures of optimal performers in extreme environments. In: Lucia A, editor. PLoS ONE. 1. Vol. 7. Public Library of Science; 2012. p. e29394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Association AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pierucci-Lagha A, Gelernter J, Feinn R, Cubells JF, Pearson D, Pollastri A, et al. Diagnostic reliability of the Semi-structured Assessment for Drug Dependence and Alcoholism (SSADDA) Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005 Dec 12;80(3):303–312. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sobell LC, Maisto SA, Sobell MB, Cooper AM. Reliability of alcohol abusers' self-reports of drinking behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1979;17(2):157–160. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(79)90025-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Zuckerman M. Item revisions in the Sensation Seeking Scale Form V (SSS-V) Pers Individ Dif. 1996 Apr;20(4):515. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995 Nov;51(6):768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School–Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont. Research Center for Children; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Barratt W. The Barratt simplified measure of social status (BSMSS) Indiana State University; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988 Jun;54(6):1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists' Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wilkinson GS, Robertson GJ. Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT4) Psychological Assessment Resources. 2006 [Google Scholar]
- 51.Shields SA, Mallory ME, Simon A. The Body Awareness Questionnaire: Reliability and Validity. 4. Vol. 53. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1989. pp. 802–815. http://dxdoiorg/101207/s15327752jpa5304_16. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Chan P-YS, Davenport PW. Respiratory-related evoked potential measures of respiratory sensory gating. J Appl Physiol. 2008 Oct;105(4):1106–1113. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.90722.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ. Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci. 1996 Jul 1;16(13):4207–4221. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04207.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cohen M. Parametric Analysis of fMRI Data Using Linear Systems Methods. Neuroimage. 1997 Aug;6(2):93–103. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Friston KJ, Frith CD, Turner R, Frackowiak RSJ. Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI. Neuroimage. 1995 Jun;2(2):157–165. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pinheiro J, Bates D, Saikat D, Sarkar D, Team RDC. the R Development Core Team: nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-111. 2013 [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, et al. Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000 Jul;10(3):120–131. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Stewart JL, Parnass JM, May AC, Davenport PW, Paulus MP. Front Syst Neurosci. Vol. 7. Frontiers; 2013. Altered frontocingulate activation during aversive interoceptive processing in young adults transitioning to problem stimulant use; p. 89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Galván A, McGlennen KM. J Cogn Neurosci. 2. 6 ed. Vol. 25. 55 Hayward Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1315 USA: MIT Press; 2013. Feb, Enhanced striatal sensitivity to aversive reinforcement in adolescents versus adults; pp. 284–296. journals-info@mit.edu. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997 Oct 3;278(5335):52–58. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lerman C, Gu H, Loughead J, Ruparel K, Yang Y, Stein EA. JAMA Psychiatry. 5. Vol. 71. American Medical Association; 2014. May, Large-scale brain network coupling predicts acute nicotine abstinence effects on craving and cognitive function; pp. 523–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sutherland MT, McHugh MJ, Pariyadath V, Stein EA. Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: Lessons learned and a road ahead. Neuroimage. 2012 Oct 1;62(4):2281–2295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Weiland BJ, Sabbineni A, Calhoun VD, Welsh RC, Hutchison KE. Reduced executive and default network functional connectivity in cigarette smokers. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015 Mar;36(3):872–882. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Eshel N, Nelson EE, Blair RJ, Pine DS, Ernst M. Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia. 2007 Mar 25;45(6):1270–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.May AC, Stewart JL, Tapert SF, Paulus MP. Front Behav Neurosci. Vol. 8. Frontiers; 2014. The effect of age on neural processing of pleasant soft touch stimuli; p. 52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Hulvershorn LA, Hummer TA, Fukunaga R, Leibenluft E, Finn P, Cyders MA, et al. Neural activation during risky decision-making in youth at high risk for substance use disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2015 May 21; doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Medina KL, McQueeny T, Nagel BJ, Hanson KL, Yang TT, Tapert SF. Addict Biol. 4. Vol. 14. Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2009. Sep, Prefrontal cortex morphometry in abstinent adolescent marijuana users: subtle gender effects; pp. 457–468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Schweinsburg BC, Alhassoon OM, Taylor MJ, Gonzalez R, Videen JS, Brown GG, et al. Effects of alcoholism and gender on brain metabolism. Am J Psychiatry. 2003 Jun;160(6):1180–1183. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Squeglia LM, Sorg SF, Schweinsburg AD, Wetherill RR, Pulido C, Tapert SF. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 3. Vol. 220. Springer-Verlag; 2012. Apr, Binge drinking differentially affects adolescent male and female brain morphometry; pp. 529–539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Reynolds B, Leraas K, Collins C, Melanko S. Delay discounting by the children of smokers and nonsmokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009 Jan 1;99(1–3):350–353. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]