Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Nov 16;10(11):e0142391. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142391

Differential Neural Processing during Motor Imagery of Daily Activities in Chronic Low Back Pain Patients

Andrea Vrana 1,2,*, Sabina Hotz-Boendermaker 1, Philipp Stämpfli 3,4, Jürgen Hänggi 5, Erich Seifritz 3, B Kim Humphreys 1, Michael L Meier 1,6
Editor: Gerwin Schalk7
PMCID: PMC4646462  PMID: 26569602

Abstract

Chronic low back pain (chronic LBP) is both debilitating for patients but also a major burden on the health care system. Previous studies reported various maladaptive structural and functional changes among chronic LBP patients on spine- and supraspinal levels including behavioral alterations. However, evidence for cortical reorganization in the sensorimotor system of chronic LBP patients is scarce. Motor Imagery (MI) is suitable for investigating the cortical sensorimotor network as it serves as a proxy for motor execution. Our aim was to investigate differential MI-driven cortical processing in chronic LBP compared to healthy controls (HC) by means of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Twenty-nine subjects (15 chronic LBP patients, 14 HC) were included in the current study. MI stimuli consisted of randomly presented video clips showing every-day activities involving different whole-body movements as well as walking on even ground and walking downstairs and upstairs. Guided by the video clips, subjects had to perform MI of these activities, subsequently rating the vividness of their MI performance. Brain activity analysis revealed that chronic LBP patients exhibited significantly reduced activity compared to HC subjects in MI-related brain regions, namely the left supplementary motor area and right superior temporal sulcus. Furthermore, psycho-physiological-interaction analysis yielded significantly enhanced functional connectivity (FC) between various MI-associated brain regions in chronic LBP patients indicating diffuse and non-specific changes in FC. Current results demonstrate initial findings about differences in MI-driven cortical processing in chronic LBP pointing towards reorganization processes in the sensorimotor network.

Introduction

Low back pain (LBP) is a major health problem with a lifetime prevalence of 85% [1]. While the majority of acute LBP patients recover within weeks, a small minority becomes chronic (pain lasts > 6 months). Chronification is accompanied by psychosocial interferences and causes enormous health care expenditure [2]. A wealth of studies have investigated possible etiologies and consequences of chronic LBP by 1) focusing on ‘end organ dysfunction’, suggesting structural and biomechanical abnormalities at the spinal level, as well as functional impairments [3,4,5,6], 2) describing psychosocial and behavioral variables, such as fear avoidance beliefs that strongly predict the transition from acute to chronic LBP [7,8,9,10,11], for review see Linton [12] and by 3) reporting malfunctional neuroplastic changes on the supraspinal level [13,14,15], for review see Wand et al. [16]. Spinal dysfunction and persistent pain may result in altered sensorimotor integration and may subsequently lead to maladaptive cortical changes in motor control [17]. Indeed, reorganization within primary motor cortex (M1) was revealed by showing a discrete loss of trunk representation [14,18] and reduced anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs) were reported in chronic LBP patients [19]. However, evidence on cortical sensorimotor reorganization in LBP patients investigated by neuroimaging is sparse. This is especially true for functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures, as it is challenging to investigate brain activity related to whole-body movements in an MR scanner due to the fact that fMRI data quality is strongly sensitive to subject motion. Therefore, the present investigation used motor imagery (MI) of activities of daily living as a proxy for investigating cortical sensorimotor processing of motor execution (ME). In accordance to Jeannerod’s [20] ‘simulation or resonance theory of action’ hypothesis, MI, action observation and simple understanding of motor actions correspond to a subliminal activation of the sensorimotor system [21,22]. The MI-network comprises the premotor cortex (including the supplemental motor area [SMA]), the superior and inferior parietal lobule (SPL, IPL), the insula, prefrontal regions as well as subcortical structures such as the basal ganglia, and the thalamus [23,24,25,26]. MI has been studied extensively in healthy subjects [25,27,28,29], especially in motor learning and is used to improve performance in sports [30,31]. Moreover, it may play an important role in neuro-rehabilitation [32,33,34], however evidence for cortical functioning of MI in chronic LBP patients is lacking.

Therefore, the aim of the current study was to investigate the functioning of the MI-network in chronic LBP patients using a visually guided MI paradigm by means of fMRI. Due to the paucity of studies related to MI in chronic LBP patients our hypothesis was confined to expecting differential MI-related activity and functional connectivity (FC) in chronic LBP patients compared to healthy controls (HC), indicating potential maladaptive alterations in the sensorimotor network. A better understanding of sensorimotor reorganization processes in chronic LBP patients might help to broaden the basis for a better understanding of motor control impairments and to develop novel approaches for therapeutic MI-guided interventions.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-three subjects participated in this study. Twenty-nine subjects, fifteen chronic LBP patients (mean age 39.7 years; SD 13.5 years; 4 women) and fourteen HC (mean age 33.6 years; SD 12.6 years; 9 women) were included in the final analysis. Four subjects (two HC and two chronic LBP patients) had to be excluded due to excessive head movements (>2.5 mm). Subjects were recruited by online advertisement and word-of-mouth recommendation. Groups were age- and gender-matched (Mann-Whitney U-test for age and Fisher-Yates-test for gender, p>0.05). One patient was left-handed, all others were right-handed according to the Edinburgh Inventory for the assessment and analysis of handedness [35]. The fMRI-images of the left-handed subject were flipped to account for possible differential activations related to handedness. Inclusion criteria for patients were non-specific chronic LBP (neither traumatic nor inflammatory in origin) with a duration of longer than six months. The Pain Detect Questionnaire [36] was used in order to assure that patients fulfilled the inclusion criteria (e.g. no radiculopathy). Exclusion criteria for the HC were acute and/or recurrent back pain within the last six months and past chronic pain episodes. No participants had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorder. All subjects provided written informed consent for the participation in the experiment that was part of a study approved by the Ethics Committee of the Canton of Zurich (KEK-ZH 2012-0029) and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Subjects were financially compensated for their participation.

Study design

Behavioral assessments

Preceding the fMRI experiment, all participants completed the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) which measures state and trait anxiety levels [37]. To assess LBP complaints, patients were asked about location and duration of their back pain and they filled in the Pain-Detect questionnaire [36] which collects data on pain quality and measures pain intensity on an 11-point Numeric Rating Scale (NRS). In addition, patients filled in the Bournemouth questionnaire [38] for the assessment of psychosocial factors and the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia (TSK) questionnaire [8].

Experimental protocol

The stimuli presented during the fMRI measurements consisted of 4s video clips showing whole-body activities of daily living (“Activities”) as well as walking (“Walking”). Activities of daily living were shoveling soil, lifting a flowerpot and vacuum cleaning under a coffee table, whereas the walking videos were comprised of walking on even ground as well as walking up and down on stairs (Fig 1). The video clips were recorded from a third-person perspective which corresponded to the representation of the movement as if the subject was a spectator and another person performed the action [11,39]. The task consisted of visually guided MI as it has been shown that MI under visual guidance (i.e. simultaneous action observation) elicits stronger cortical activations within the ME network than conventional MI [40,41,42,43]. While watching the video clips, both HC and chronic LBP patients were instructed to imagine themselves performing the activities without actually executing them. Preceding the scanning session both groups performed visually guided MI of each activity once. The chronic LBP patients additionally rated the painfulness of those activities on a visual analogue scale (VAS) which was anchored with the endpoints ‘no pain’ (0) and ‘worst pain ever’ (10). During the scanning session video clips were presented by using MR compatible goggles (Resonance Technology Inc., Northridge, USA) that were connected to a notebook computer running the Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Davis, CA). The fMRI session consisted of 30 trials. Each of the six video clips was presented five times in pseudo-randomized order (no more than two identical consecutive trials). After each video clip, subjects rated the vividness of MI performance of the specific activity on a VAS which was anchored with the endpoints ‘very bad’ (0) and ‘very good’ (10). The ratings were carried out by using an MR compatible trackball (Current Designs Inc., Philadelphia, USA) that moved a cursor on the displayed VAS scale. The rating task was followed by an inter-stimulus interval (ISI: black screen with a fixation cross) of a duration jittered between 6.5 and 8.5 s that was used as baseline.

Fig 1. The six presented video clips.

Fig 1

A) Activities of daily living (“Activities”) and B) Walking activities (“Walking”).

Neuroimaging data acquisition

All measurements were performed on a 3-T whole-body MRI system (Philips Achieva, Best, the Netherlands) equipped with a 32-element receive head coil and MultiTransmit parallel RF transmission. Each imaging session consisted of a survey scan, a B1 calibration scan (for MultiTransmit), a SENSE reference scan and a high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical scan followed by the fMRI acquisition.

fMRI data were acquired with a whole brain gradient-echo echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (365 dynamic scans) consisting of 37 slices in axial direction with the following parameters: field of view = 240 x 240 mm2, matrix = 96 x 96 pixels, slice thickness = 2.8 mm, voxel size 2.5mm x 2.5mm no slice gap, repetition time (TR) = 2100 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, SENSE factor = 2.5, flip angle 80°, interleaved slice acquisition. The number of transversal oriented slices was maximized with respect to the selected repetition time and gradient performance. The resulting field of view in slice direction did not allow an inclusion of the entire cerebellum. Anatomical data were obtained with a 3D T1-weighted turbo field echo scan consisting of 145 slices in sagittal orientation with the following parameters: field of view = 230 x 226 mm2, slice thickness = 1.2 mm, acquisition matrix = 208 x 203 pixels, TR = 6.8 ms, TE = 3.1 ms, flip angle = 9°, number of signal averages = 1.

Statistical neuroimaging analysis

Image preprocessing

SPM8 (release v4010, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) software package running on MATLAB R2010b (Mathworks, Natick, MA) was used for fMRI data preprocessing and statistical voxel-by-voxel analysis. EPI volumes of each subject were corrected for differences in head motion, spatially normalized according to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space and finally smoothed with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. To control for head movement effects, individual movement parameters (translations in x, y and z-direction, as well as rotations around x, y, and z axis) were implemented in the 1st level model as regressors of no interest. Excessive head motion was defined as a dislocation of more than once in the in-plane voxel resolution (>2.5mm). For removing the low frequency noise a high-pass filter with a cut-off of 128s was used. Trials were modeled as a boxcar response function and convolved with the standard canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) as implemented in SPM8.

Event-related brain activity analysis

For the 1st level analysis the general linear model (GLM) was fitted for each subject by a design matrix composed of the onsets and duration (4 s) of the “Activities” and “Walking” video clips. For each subject, parameter estimates (beta) and contrast images (cons) were computed. The resulting images were analyzed using a random-effects model to allow for population inferences [44]. First, whole-brain activations pooled across conditions (”Activities” and “Walking”) were extracted for each group. Subsequently, between group analyses using two-sample T-tests were computed for the contrasts “Activities > baseline” and “Walking > baseline”. The variance between groups was assumed to be unequal. Error covariance components were estimated using restricted maximum likelihood as implemented in SPM8. To control for false positive results a cluster-based false discovery rate (FDR) correction based on the Gaussian Random Field Theory [45] with a FDR of q <0.05 was used.

Event-related functional connectivity analysis

The main advantage of psycho-physiological-interactions (PPI) analysis is that it assesses covariance between regions across time and therefore provides a test of task effects on connectivity. Furthermore, as stated by Gerchen et al., PPI analysis “allows extracting additional information about the brain, which might not be apparent in regional brain activation alone” [46]. The generalized form of the context-dependent PPI approach (gPPI) increases the flexibility of the statistical modelling and improves single-subject model-fit, thereby increasing the sensitivity to true positive findings and a reduction in false positives [47]. For each subject, the deconvolved time course was averaged across 6mm-spheres of MI associated brain regions. Peaks of the MI seed regions were taken from a meta-analysis of MI network activations reported by Hétu and colleagues [23]: left supplementary motor area (SMA), bilateral precentral gyrus (M1), left inferior parietal lobule (IPL), bilateral superior parietal lobule (SPL), bilateral supramarginal gyrus (SMG), bilateral putamen, bilateral middle frontal gyrus (MFG), bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), bilateral insula and left thalamus (Fig 2). In addition, the superior temporal region (superior temporal gyrus and sulcus [STG/STS]) was also included as a seed region because this brain region is consistently activated in action observation and imitation [48]. Further, to control for possible enhanced overall brain connectivity of the chronic LBP patients, the primary auditory cortex was used as an additional MI-unrelated seed region. Whole-brain functional connectivity analysis was performed using each of the eleven seed regions as well as the additional seed of the primary auditory cortex which was taken from the WFU-pickatlas, Brodmann Area 17, bilaterally. Subsequently, separate psychological terms (”Activities” and “Walking” video clips), physiological regressors (time courses of seed regions) and their PPI interaction terms, as well as the movement parameters, were included in the PPI model. The resulting PPI connectivity estimates were then taken into a two-sample T-test to investigate group differences. Identified clusters were considered to be significant when falling below a cluster-corrected FDR of q < 0.05.

Fig 2. An overview of the motor imagery network based on the meta-analyzes of Hétu and colleagues (2013).

Fig 2

MNI-peak coordinates are surrounded by a 6mm-sphere. Views: sagital and axial. 1 Inferior Frontal Gyrus (IFG), bilateral; 2 Inferior Parietal Lobule (IPL), left; 3 Insula, bilateral; 4 Middle Frontal Gyrus (MFG), bilateral; 5 Precentral Gyrus (M1), bilateral; 6 Putamen, bilateral; 7 Supplementary Motor Area (SMA), left; 8 Superior Parietal Lobule (SPL), bilateral; 9 Supramarginal Gyrus (SMG), bilateral; 10 Superior Temporal Sulcus (STS/STG), bilateral; 11 Thalamus, left.

Behavioral analyses

SPSS Statistics version 19 software (http://www-01.ibm.com/software/analytics/spss/) was used for the statistical analyzes of the behavioral data. Questionnaires and rating data were verified for normal distribution using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Based on normal distribution, two-sample T-tests were performed for comparisons of the STAI questionnaire between groups. To test for differences in the rating data a repeated measurement ANOVA with within-subject factor “type of movement” and between-subject factor “group” was conducted. Additionally, to exclude interrelationships Spearman correlations were computed between the different questionnaires as well as between ratings. The significance level was set at p<0.05.

Results

Behavioral results

Questionnaires

Behavioral data was normally distributed. The State and Trait scales did not reveal significant differences between the two groups (State: HC mean=42.4, SD=3.2; chronic LBP mean=44.5, SD=5.3; p>0.202 and T=-1.314; Trait: HC mean=42.8, SD=3.0; chronic LBP mean=43.3, SD=6.9; p>0.807 and T=-0.247). In the chronic LBP group, the Pain Detect Questionnaire showed an overall mean score of 11.2 (SD 6.6) and indicated a mean pain intensity of 4.8 (SD 1.9, range 1.0-7.0). Furthermore, the TSK questionnaire yielded a mean of 37.9 (SD 5.9) and the Bournemouth Questionnaire a mean of 22.1 (SD 11.6).

Ratings

Mean ratings of vividness in the HC group were 6.1 (SD 2.5) for the “Activities” and 8.0 (SD 1.6) for the “Walking” videos. The chronic LBP group showed a mean rating of 5.7 (SD 2.4) for the “Activities” and 7.7 (SD 2.1) for the “Walking” videos. The repeated measure ANOVA for the ratings of the MI performance revealed a main effect of condition (“Activities” vs. “Walking”, F=10.153, p<0.005), whereas no interaction effect (type of movement*group) was found (F=0.019, p>0.8). The pain ratings of the chronic LBP group revealed a significantly greater mean of 5.9 (SD 2.5) for the “Activities” compared to a mean of 1.6 (SD 1.7) for the “Walking” videos revealed by a paired T-test (T=7.079, p<0.001).

Correlations

Spearman correlations were conducted for the patient-specific questionnaires (Bournemouth, TSK, pain intensity) and the ratings. No significant relationships were found in any of those correlations.

fMRI results

Event-related activity

The pooled (”Activities” and “Walking”) whole-brain activity in both groups revealed activations in frontal, temporal and parietal cortices as well as in the occipital lobe (FDR-corrected with q <0.05). In the HC group, frontal lobe activations were detected in the middle frontal gyrus, which correspond to the SMA, temporal lobe activations were located in the STG and middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and parietal lobe activations were observed in the IPL and SPL (precuneus). Activations in the occipital lobe were located in the cuneus and lingual gyrus. Subcortically, activity in the thalamus was observed (Table 1, Fig 3A). The chronic LBP group exhibited frontal lobe activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), the temporal lobe activations were only observed in the MTG and parietal lobe activity was detected in SPL and IPL. Occipital activations were located in the lingual gyrus. Thalamus activity could also be observed (Table 2, Fig 3B).

Table 1. Brain Activity of the healthy controls (HC) group, peak activations.

Cluster maxima of brain activity of the video clips of activities of daily living (“Activities”) and walking activities (“Walking”) together. All results are false discovery rate corrected (FDR, q<0.05). MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, STG = superior temporal gyrus, MTG = middle temporal gyrus, MFG = middle frontal gyrus, OL = occipital lobe, SPL = superior parietal lobule, IPL = inferior parietal lobule, PreCG = precentral gyrus, DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

1 HC Group, “Activities” and “Walking” > baseline, N = 14
Cluster size q(FDR) Z MNI Peak coordinates Brain region
5248 <0.001 6.52 60 -44 16 Right STG
6.07 50 -68 14 Right MTG
3.63 20 -32 2 Right Thalamus (Pulvinar)
11359 <0.001 6.27 -46 -68 14 Left MTG
5.51 -10 -80 -2 Left OL (lingual gyrus)
4.95 -8 -96 26 Left OL (cuneus)
4.90 -10 -64 62 Left SPL (precuneus)
4.54 12 -46 58 Right SPL (precuneus)
4.21 -60 -42 24 Left IPL
3.51 12 -22 68 Right PreCG
157 <0.01 5.02 42 -42 -18 Right fusiform gyrus
4.45 36 -22 -14 Right hippocampus
133 <0.02 4.08 -10 12 52 Left MFG (SMA)
Fig 3. Pooled (“Activities” and “Walking” together) task-related activity per group.

Fig 3

A) Healthy controls (HC) and B) chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) patients (q<0.05, false discovery rate corrected, FDR).

Table 2. Brain activity of the chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) group, peak activations.

Cluster maxima of brain activity of the video clips of activities of daily living (“Activities”) and walking activities (“Walking”) together. All results are false discovery rate corrected (FDR, q<0.05). MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, STG = superior temporal gyrus, MTG = middle temporal gyrus, MFG = middle frontal gyrus, OL = occipital lobe, SPL = superior parietal lobule, IPL = inferior parietal lobule, PreCG = precentral gyrus, DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

2 Chronic LBP Group, “Activities” and “Walking” > baseline, N = 15
Cluster size q(FDR) Z MNI Peak coordinates Brain region
33236 <0.001 6.96 -14 -82 -4 Left OL (lingual gyrus)
6.41 -20 -46 -14 Left fusiform gyrus
6.27 46 -72 14 Right MTG
6.03 18 -32 0 Right Thalamus
5.39 22 -44 -16 Right fusiform gyrus
5.12 34 -12 -22 Right Hippocampus
5.01 0 -68 58 Left SPL (precuneus)
4.97 16 -70 30 Right IPL (precuneus)
247 <0.01 4.97 -52 34 20 Left DLPFC

Direct comparison between groups (two-sample T-tests) on the whole-brain level yielded significantly stronger activity in the right STG of the HC group for both contrasts „Activities > baseline“ (q<0.001 FDR-corrected; Z=5.54; MNI peak coordinates 60 -44 16; cluster size 267) and „Walking > baseline“ (q<0.05, FDR-corrected; Z=4.21; MNI peak coordinates 68 -44 12; cluster size 66;). Furthermore, the contrast “Walking > baseline” revealed also enhanced activity in the left SMA (q<0.05, FDR-corrected; Z=3.93 MNI peak coordinates -18 6 70; cluster size 64) in the HC group.

Functional connectivity

The majority of seed-regions demonstrated significantly higher FC within the MI network in chronic LBP patients compared to the HC group for the contrasts “Activities > baseline” and “Walking > baseline”, with few exceptions (see Tables 36 for details, FDR-corrected with q<0.05). In the HC group compared to the chronic LBP patients only the thalamus revealed enhanced FC with the MTG in the contrast “Activities > baseline”. The control analysis with the auditory cortex as a seed region revealed no significantly enhanced FC of the chronic LBP patients in the “Activities>baseline” condition. The contrast “Walking>baseline” yielded small clusters located in the MFG and STG/STS (n=3; extent threshold in voxel: 105, 85 and 49; FDR-corrected with q<0.05).

Table 3. Functional connectivity cluster maxima of the chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) group vs. healthy controls (HC), contrast activities of daily living (“Activities”).

All results are false discovery rate corrected (FDR, q<0.05). MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, STG = superior temporal gyrus, ITG = inferior temporal gyrus, MTG = middle temporal gyrus, MFG = middle frontal gyrus, SFG = superior frontal gyrus, SMG = supramarginal gyrus, OL = occipital lobe, SPL = superior parietal lobule, IPL = inferior parietal lobule, PreCG = precentral gyrus, PCL = paracentral lobule, SMG = supramarginal gyrus.

3 Chronic LBP > HC, “Activities” > baseline, N = 29
seed cluster size q(FDR) Z MNI Peak coordinates Brain region
IPL 74 <0.001 4.58 20 -2 64 Right MFG (SMA)
IFG 159 <0.001 4.48 -16 -78 54 Left SPL
75 <0.02 4.17 -10 2 74 Left SFG (SMA)
210 <0.001 3.92 8 -52 68 Right SPL (precuneus)
Insula No significant results
M1 83 <0.007 4.97 58 -58 -16 Right ITG
92 <0.006 4.07 8 -50 56 Right SPL (precuneus)
160 <0.001 3.84 32 -70 46 Right SPL
MFG 58 <0.04 4.71 32 -76 52 Right SPL
66 <0.04 4.14 -48 -18 48 Left PCG (S1)
Putamen 158 <0.001 4.11 2 -38 66 Right PCL
SMA 204 <0.001 4.43 18 -74 26 Right IPL (precuneus)
155 <0.001 4.34 -16 -72 24 Left IPL (precuneus)
53 <0.04 4.06 -54 22 34 Left MFG
47 <0.04 3.93 -10 -4 14 Left Thalamus
94 <0.003 3.90 -58 -54 20 Left STG
SPL 85 <0.007 4.22 -42 -72 36 Left IPL (precuneus)
73 <0.02 4.04 -26 2 62 Left MFG
231 <0.001 3.97 -58 -54 18 Left STG
94 <0.006 3.87 64 -38 -6 Right MTG
60 <0.03 3.70 22 -72 54 Right SPL
STG 210 <0.001 4.02 -10 4 74 Left SFG (SMA)
54 <0.04 3.88 14 6 74 Right SFG (SMA)
78 <0.02 3.84 16 -60 60 Right SPL
SMG 888 <0.001 4.31 -42 -34 56 Left PCG (S1)
348 <0.001 4.09 42 -26 44 Right PCG (S1)
259 <0.001 4.01 -16 -6 68 Left SFG (SMA)
Thalamus No significant results
Table 6. Functional connectivity cluster maxima of the healthy controls (HC) vs. chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) group, contrast walking activities (“Walking”).

All results are false discovery rate corrected (FDR, q<0.05). MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute

6 HC > chronic LBP, “Walking” > baseline, N = 29
seed cluster size q(FDR) Z MNI Peak coordinates Brain region
All No significant results
Table 4. Functional connectivity cluster maxima of the chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) group vs. healthy controls (HC), contrast walking activities (“Walking”).

All results are false discovery rate corrected (FDR, q<0.05). MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute, STG = superior temporal gyrus, ITG = inferior temporal gyrus, MTG = middle temporal gyrus, MFG = middle frontal gyrus, SFG = superior frontal gyrus, SMG = supramarginal gyrus, OL = occipital lobe, SPL = superior parietal lobule, IPL = inferior parietal lobule, PreCG = precentral gyrus, PCL = paracentral lobule, SMG = supramarginal gyrus

4 Chronic LBP > HC, “Walking” > baseline, N = 29
seed cluster size q(FDR) Z MNI Peak coordinates Brain region
IPL No significant results
IFG 159 <0.001 4.48 16 10 62 Right SFG (SMA)
Insula 95 <0.02 4.33 14 -48 54 Right SPL (precuneus)
M1 124 <0.02 3.84 -6 -2 68 Left SFG (SMA)
MFG No significant results
Putamen 124 <0.02 3.95 -62 -28 20 Left pars opercularis (S2)
56 <0.05 3.55 62 -18 12 Right pars opercularis (S2)
SMA 92 <0.006 4.41 -56 -50 20 Left STG
SPL 92 <0.006 4.34 -6 -36 72 Left PCL
58 <0.03 4.21 -66 -38 8 Left STG
56 <0.03 4.08 -48 -18 48 Left PCG (S1)
80 <0.008 4.08 68 -24 34 Right SMG (S2)
140 <0.002 3.95 10 6 70 Right SFG (SMA)
STG 103 <0.003 4.33 -62 -14 -6 Left MTG
83 <0.007 4.09 -10 8 68 Left SFG (SMA)
174 <0.001 4.03 -12 -28 68 Left PCL
114 <0.003 3.89 22 -30 70 Right PCG (S1)
SMG No significant results
Thalamus 218 <0.001 4.55 14 -34 72 Right PCG (S1)
Table 5. Functional connectivity cluster maxima of the healthy controls (HC) vs. chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) group, contrast activities of daily living (“Activities”).

All results are false discovery rate corrected (FDR, q<0.05). MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute

5 HC > chronic LBP, “Activities” > baseline, N = 29
seed cluster size q(FDR) Z MNI Peak coordinates Brain region
Thalamus 65 <0.04 3.94 42 -66 12 Right OL (right MTG)
All other No significant results

Discussion

The present fMRI-study aimed at investigating the functioning of the MI network in chronic LBP patients. Visually-guided MI of daily activities was used in order to disentangle expected differential neural sensorimotor processing among HC and chronic LBP patients. As a novel finding the MI-driven activity yielded reduced brain activation within the SMA and STG/STS, while the connectivity analysis indicated significantly enhanced FC within the MI-network in chronic LBP compared to the HC group.

Event-related activity

The simulation theory of Jeannerod [20] postulates that MI involves similar brain areas as ME and even sole action observation. Based on this assumption MI was used as a proxy for ME of daily activities in order to investigate the functioning of sensorimotor networks in chronic LBP patients. The results of the event related activity analysis of both groups are in keeping with known MI associated brain activity involving a fronto-parietal network and subcortical structures such as the thalamus [23,24,48]. However, M1 activity could not be demonstrated, supporting the rather ambiguous role of M1 in MI [23]. Due to the visual-guidance of the MI task, extensive activations were also observed in visual and visuo-motor areas in both groups.

Regarding the behavioral measures, both groups rated the vividness of MI performance equally indicating that chronic LBP patients retained their ability to perform mental movements. In contrast, on the neural level the hypothesis of maladaptive alterations in the sensorimotor processing was confirmed by demonstrating significant differences in the functioning of the MI-network in chronic LBP patients regarding brain activity as well as connectivity patterns. Importantly, pain ratings of the chronic LBP group indicated that patients experienced only the MI-performance of the “Activities” as painful [49] while the MI of the “Walking” condition was not painful.

With respect to the brain activations the between group (two-sample T-tests) comparisons on the whole-brain level revealed reduced activity within two brain regions in chronic LBP patients, namely the left SMA and right STG/STS.

While robust MI-driven SMA activity was observed in HC subjects, it was completely absent in chronic LBP patients even at an uncorrected threshold of p<0.001. The SMA represents an inherent part of the MI-network [23,24,48] and is linked to adequate motor planning and voluntary motor control [50,51,52], for review see [53]. With respect to the stimuli i.e. whole-body activities as presented in the current study, there is also considerable evidence for the involvement of SMA in postural control especially in anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs) and related body balance control [54,55]. In particular, SMA was activated when pressure stimuli were applied to the lumbar spine indicating fine scaled motor preparation/trunk stabilization mechanisms in the absence of an intended or actual performance, verifying the crucial role of this region in anticipatory postural control [56]. In chronic LBP patients, Jacobs and colleagues revealed delayed APAs suggesting a decrease in postural control and stability [19]. Furthermore, significantly improved APAs resulted from the application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulations (rTMS) over the SMA in a neurologic patient group with considerable APA delays [55]. Together, these findings suggest a direct involvement of the SMA in trunk movement coordination. Therefore, the demonstrated maladaptive functioning of the SMA revealed by MI might be based on progressive dysfunction of motor circuits and thus provides a mechanism to explain reported impairments in postural control in chronic LBP patients.

However, it remains to be explored to which extent clinically relevant impairments of postural control in chronic LBP patients [57] might be linked with maladaptive brain mechanisms.

Extensive STG/STS activation was observed as a consequence of the visual guidance (e.g. action observation) of the MI-task. Interestingly, when contrasted against baseline STG/STS activity during “Activities” and “Walking” was significantly reduced in chronic LBP patients compared to HC. The STG/STS responds to images of human bodies and their orientations [58,59] and is known to play an important role in the understanding and interpreting of human movements as well as in matching sensory inputs with internal movement representations [60,61,62,63]. Correspondingly, MI and ME require a high amount of sensory input processing in order to provide a real-time representation of the body (e.g. body-scheme) [64,65]. In order to perform MI and ME appropriately, the matching of the internal representation of a person’s body with an intended movement is realized by comparing the predicted sensorimotor consequences of the action and the actual sensorimotor feedback [66]. In chronic LBP patients, altered sensory input characterized e.g. by reduced two-point discrimination and altered body-scheme has been previously reported [67,68,69,70,71]. Furthermore, it has been revealed that chronic LBP patients showed a reduced ability to discriminate weights in possibly hurtful trunk movements (e.g. lifting objects) in human point-light biological motion displays [72,73]. As their general ability in visual discrimination tasks regarding biological motion in point-light displays remained unharmed [73], their constraints seemed to be limited only to trunk movements. The impairment of sensory discrimination and subsequently restrained interpretation of trunk-related visual movement tasks might be linked with reduced STG/STS activation in the present visually-guided MI task.

Event-related functional connectivity (FC)

Activity and connectivity analyses are the two main applications of fMRI. As both analyses are based on the measure of changes in hemodynamic responses one could expect a certain overlap of brain patterns for the same task. However, FC changes can occur even if a particular brain area does not appear to be activated throughout a task and vice versa providing additional information about neural functioning [74,75]. In the current investigation we used the gPPI method to reveal FC changes [47] which yielded major differences between the chronic LBP and HC group in the MI-network. Overall, the chronic LBP patients exhibited significantly enhanced MI-driven FC compared to the HC group throughout the MI network. To further control for possibly enhanced whole-brain connectivity within the chronic LBP group we performed an additional control analysis using the primary auditory cortex as a MI-unrelated seed region. The analysis revealed no enhanced FC of the chronic LBP group in the “Activities>baseline” condition. This observation confirms largely MI-related changes in FC whereas an overall enhanced brain connectivity of chronic LBP patients is less probable. The enhanced FC of chronic LBP patients in the MI network can be interpreted in different ways and these explanations are not mutually exclusive. Enhanced FC in the default mode network as well as in the executive attention network might be associated with hyper-excitability as previously demonstrated in fibromyalgia patients, a pathology associated with ongoing diffuse chronic pain state [76]. In addition, the FC strength among these networks correlated with spontaneous pain ratings. With respect to chronic LBP, diffuse and non-specifically enhanced FC across the MI network during a pain free MI-task might indicate a common neural maladaptive process. There is evidence about lower pain thresholds and enhanced functional activity in particular regions involved in pain processing in chronic pain states as well as during acute noxious stimulations [15,18,77]. Furthermore, dysfunctional central pain modulation and central sensitization characterized by e.g. hyper-excitability have been also reported in chronic LBP patients [78,79] and may also have an impact on FC between different brain regions.

Supporting this, studies using electroencephalography (EEG) have shown that the sensorimotor cortical activation, as reflected by EEG alpha rhythms, is modulated during the expectancy of somatosensory stimuli and that patients might exaggerate the anticipatory activation of sensorimotor cortex to negligible noxious stimuli [80,81,82]. Thus, chronic pain might be maintained through hypervigilance toward noxious stimuli due to abnormal attentional cortical-thalamic systems [83].

An alternative explanation for the observed enhanced FC in chronic LBP patients might be based on the high neural demand of a MI task. It has been shown that healthy subjects accommodated to a more demanding task by enhancing FC within the MI-network [28]. Taking into account the non-significant group differences of the ratings about the vividness of the MI-performance, this would imply that chronic LBP patients retained the ability to perform complex MI and ME tasks, however they required more cortical recruitment depicted by enhanced FC in order to perform a task.

Limitations

Regarding shortfalls of the present study there are mainly methodological issues to mention. We considered the ISI as a baseline although there is no inherent baseline associated with the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal [84]. We assumed that the baseline represented something akin to a zero-activity condition which was then compared with the activity during the MI-tasks. Next, the canonical hemodynamic response function that was used in the deconvolution step of the gPPI was assumed to be constant across voxels and subjects [85]. Until now, the choice of model for the deconvolution has not been investigated, despite different hemodynamic response functions being used in activation studies [47,86]. Thus, future studies should investigate the effect of variable hemodynamic response functions on PPI, especially focusing on differential hemodynamic responses in patient populations due to possible changes in neurovascular coupling. Furthermore, current fMRI group level statistics are based on an averaged anatomical template. As there is a high degree of variability in inter-subject anatomy and functional localization, the use of spatial alignment and smoothing kernels increases the overlap among subjects but makes it difficult for identifying distinct activity in small brain regions. To reduce inter-subject anatomical variation e.g. in the SMA, specific registration techniques such as DARTEL or HAMMER are recommended [87]. Further, visual MI requires self-visualization of a movement from a first- (internal) or third-person (external) perspective [39]. As the subjects were instructed to imagine themselves performing the activities both perspectives end up in a self-visualization of the subject. However, the observation of a “third person” might have additionally influenced brain activity but this was assumed to be equal in both groups. Finally, the current findings have to be carefully interpreted. To our knowledge this study presents initial findings regarding neural correlates of MI in chronic LBP patients. However, a causal relationship between the current findings of alterations in MI-related brain mechanisms and the disease pattern of chronic LBP patients cannot be demonstrated and remains therefore further investigation.

Conclusion

The current investigation provides first evidence for obvious differences between chronic LBP patients and HC subjects regarding MI-driven activity and FC. First, reduced activity in the SMA suggests dysfunctional mechanisms regarding motor planning, feed-forward monitoring and related APAs, whereas reduced STG/STS activity indicates deficits in the integration of sensory inputs. Second, the non-specifically enhanced FC within the MI network of chronic LBP patients might indicate pain-driven maladaptive alterations in the sensorimotor network in terms of hyperexcitability or an enlarged need for neural resources. These findings may broaden the basis for the understanding of sensorimotor reorganization processes in chronic LBP patients and might ultimately help developing novel approaches for therapeutic MI-guided interventions.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from (1) Foundation for the Education of Chiropractors in Switzerland and (2) Balgrist Foundation. In addition, we thank Valentine Marcar (PhD) for English proof-reading and all volunteers for their participation in the current study.

Data Availability

All fMRI-data files are available at the Dryad Digital Repository platform (doi:10.5061/dryad.2h0q3).

Funding Statement

This work was supported by grants from (1) Foundation for the Education of Chiropractors in Switzerland and (2) Balgrist Foundation (salary payments). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Andersson GB (1997) Low back pain. Journal of rehabilitation research and development 34: ix–x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Andersson GB (1999) Epidemiological features of chronic low-back pain. Lancet 354: 581–585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Robinson JP AA (2009) Low back pain. In: Mayer EA, Bushnell MC, editors; Functional pain syndromes: presentation and pathophysiology: p. 23-53.
  • 4. D'Hooge R, Hodges P, Tsao H, Hall L, Macdonald D, Danneels L (2013) Altered trunk muscle coordination during rapid trunk flexion in people in remission of recurrent low back pain. Journal of electromyography and kinesiology: official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology 23: 173–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Hodges PW (2001) Changes in motor planning of feedforward postural responses of the trunk muscles in low back pain. Experimental brain research 141: 261–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Cholewicki J, McGill SM (1996) Mechanical stability of the in vivo lumbar spine: implications for injury and chronic low back pain. Clinical biomechanics 11: 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Verbunt JA, Seelen HA, Vlaeyen JW, van de Heijden GJ, Heuts PH, Pons K, et al. (2003) Disuse and deconditioning in chronic low back pain: concepts and hypotheses on contributing mechanisms. European journal of pain 7: 9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Leeuw M, Goossens ME, Linton SJ, Crombez G, Boersma K, Vlaeyen JW (2007) The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence. Journal of behavioral medicine 30: 77–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Wertli MM, Rasmussen-Barr E, Weiser S, Bachmann LM, Brunner F (2014) The role of fear avoidance beliefs as a prognostic factor for outcome in patients with nonspecific low back pain: a systematic review. The spine journal: official journal of the North American Spine Society 14: 816–836 e814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Boersma K, Linton SJ (2005) Screening to identify patients at risk: profiles of psychological risk factors for early intervention. The Clinical journal of pain 21: 38–43; discussion 69-72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Meier ML, Stampfli P, Vrana A, Humphreys BK, Seifritz E, Hotz-Boendermaker S (2015) Fear avoidance beliefs in back pain-free subjects are reflected by amygdala-cingulate responses. Frontiers in human neuroscience 9: 424 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Linton SJ (2000) A review of psychological risk factors in back and neck pain. Spine 25: 1148–1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Hashmi JA, Baliki MN, Huang L, Baria AT, Torbey S, Hermann KM, et al. (2013) Shape shifting pain: chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. Brain: a journal of neurology 136: 2751–2768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Tsao H, Galea MP, Hodges PW (2008) Reorganization of the motor cortex is associated with postural control deficits in recurrent low back pain. Brain: a journal of neurology 131: 2161–2171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Flor H, Braun C, Elbert T, Birbaumer N (1997) Extensive reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in chronic back pain patients. Neuroscience letters 224: 5–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Wand BM, Parkitny L, O'Connell NE, Luomajoki H, McAuley JH, Thacker M, et al. (2011) Cortical changes in chronic low back pain: current state of the art and implications for clinical practice. Manual therapy 16: 15–20. 10.1016/j.math.2010.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Haavik H, Murphy B (2012) The role of spinal manipulation in addressing disordered sensorimotor integration and altered motor control. Journal of electromyography and kinesiology: official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology 22: 768–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Tsao H, Danneels LA, Hodges PW (2011) ISSLS prize winner: Smudging the motor brain in young adults with recurrent low back pain. Spine 36: 1721–1727. 10.1097/BRS.0b013e31821c4267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Jacobs JV, Henry SM, Nagle KJ (2009) People with chronic low back pain exhibit decreased variability in the timing of their anticipatory postural adjustments. Behavioral neuroscience 123: 455–458. 10.1037/a0014479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Jeannerod M (2001) Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage 14: S103–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Jeannerod M, Frak V (1999) Mental imaging of motor activity in humans. Current opinion in neurobiology 9: 735–739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Kosslyn SM, Behrmann M, Jeannerod M (1995) The cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. Neuropsychologia 33: 1335–1344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Hetu S, Gregoire M, Saimpont A, Coll MP, Eugene F, Michon PE, et al. (2013) The neural network of motor imagery: an ALE meta-analysis. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 37: 930–949. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Lotze M, Halsband U (2006) Motor imagery. Journal of physiology, Paris 99: 386–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Lorey B, Naumann T, Pilgramm S, Petermann C, Bischoff M, Zentgraf K, et al. (2013) How equivalent are the action execution, imagery, and observation of intransitive movements? Revisiting the concept of somatotopy during action simulation. Brain and cognition 81: 139–150. 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Gatti R, Tettamanti A, Gough PM, Riboldi E, Marinoni L, Buccino G (2013) Action observation versus motor imagery in learning a complex motor task: a short review of literature and a kinematics study. Neuroscience letters 540: 37–42. 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.11.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Hanakawa T, Dimyan MA, Hallett M (2008) Motor planning, imagery, and execution in the distributed motor network: a time-course study with functional MRI. Cerebral cortex 18: 2775–2788. 10.1093/cercor/bhn036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Szameitat AJ, McNamara A, Shen S, Sterr A (2012) Neural activation and functional connectivity during motor imagery of bimanual everyday actions. PloS one 7: e38506 10.1371/journal.pone.0038506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Solodkin A, Hlustik P, Chen EE, Small SL (2004) Fine modulation in network activation during motor execution and motor imagery. Cerebral cortex 14: 1246–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Short SE, Tenute A, Feltz DL (2005) Imagery use in sport: mediational effects for efficacy. Journal of sports sciences 23: 951–960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Murphy SM (1994) Imagery interventions in sport. Medicine and science in sports and exercise 26: 486–494. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Moseley GL (2004) Graded motor imagery is effective for long-standing complex regional pain syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. Pain 108: 192–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J (2001) Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation 82: 1133–1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Wei G, Luo J (2010) Sport expert's motor imagery: functional imaging of professional motor skills and simple motor skills. Brain research 1341: 52–62. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Freynhagen R, Baron R, Gockel U, Tolle TR (2006) painDETECT: a new screening questionnaire to identify neuropathic components in patients with back pain. Current medical research and opinion 22: 1911–1920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Spielberger CD (1971) Notes and comments trait-state anxiety and motor behavior. Journal of motor behavior 3: 265–279. 10.1080/00222895.1971.10734907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Bolton JE, Humphreys BK (2002) The Bournemouth Questionnaire: a short-form comprehensive outcome measure. II. Psychometric properties in neck pain patients. Journal of manipulative and physiological therapeutics 25: 141–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Guillot A, Collet C, Nguyen VA, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J (2009) Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: an fMRI study. Human brain mapping 30: 2157–2172. 10.1002/hbm.20658 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Villiger M, Estevez N, Hepp-Reymond MC, Kiper D, Kollias SS, Eng K, et al. (2013) Enhanced activation of motor execution networks using action observation combined with imagination of lower limb movements. PloS one 8: e72403 10.1371/journal.pone.0072403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Nedelko V, Hassa T, Hamzei F, Schoenfeld MA, Dettmers C (2012) Action imagery combined with action observation activates more corticomotor regions than action observation alone. Journal of neurologic physical therapy: JNPT 36: 182–188. 10.1097/NPT.0b013e318272cad1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Conson M, Sara M, Pistoia F, Trojano L (2009) Action observation improves motor imagery: specific interactions between simulative processes. Experimental brain research 199: 71–81. 10.1007/s00221-009-1974-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Vogt S, Rienzo FD, Collet C, Collins A, Guillot A (2013) Multiple roles of motor imagery during action observation. Frontiers in human neuroscience 7: 807 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Price CJ, Buchel C, Worsley KJ (1999) Multisubject fMRI studies and conjunction analyses. NeuroImage 10: 385–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Chumbley JR, Friston KJ (2009) False discovery rate revisited: FDR and topological inference using Gaussian random fields. NeuroImage 44: 62–70. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Gerchen MF, Bernal-Casas D, Kirsch P (2014) Analyzing task-dependent brain network changes by whole-brain psychophysiological interactions: a comparison to conventional analysis. Human brain mapping 35: 5071–5082. 10.1002/hbm.22532 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. McLaren DG, Ries ML, Xu G, Johnson SC (2012) A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. NeuroImage 61: 1277–1286. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Caspers S, Zilles K, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB (2010) ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage 50: 1148–1167. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Moseley GL, Zalucki N, Birklein F, Marinus J, van Hilten JJ, Luomajoki H (2008) Thinking about movement hurts: the effect of motor imagery on pain and swelling in people with chronic arm pain. Arthritis and rheumatism 59: 623–631. 10.1002/art.23580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Michelon P, Vettel JM, Zacks JM (2006) Lateral somatotopic organization during imagined and prepared movements. Journal of neurophysiology 95: 811–822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Iseki K, Hanakawa T (2010) [The functional significance of the basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop in gait control in humans: a neuroimaging approach]. Brain and nerve = Shinkei kenkyu no shinpo 62: 1157–1164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Iseki K, Hanakawa T, Shinozaki J, Nankaku M, Fukuyama H (2008) Neural mechanisms involved in mental imagery and observation of gait. NeuroImage 41: 1021–1031. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Nachev P, Kennard C, Husain M (2008) Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature reviews Neuroscience 9: 856–869. 10.1038/nrn2478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Mihara M, Miyai I, Hatakenaka M, Kubota K, Sakoda S (2008) Role of the prefrontal cortex in human balance control. NeuroImage 43: 329–336. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Jacobs JV, Lou JS, Kraakevik JA, Horak FB (2009) The supplementary motor area contributes to the timing of the anticipatory postural adjustment during step initiation in participants with and without Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience 164: 877–885. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Meier ML, Hotz-Boendermaker S, Boendermaker B, Luechinger R, Humphreys BK (2014) Neural responses of posterior to anterior movement on lumbar vertebrae: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of manipulative and physiological therapeutics 37: 32–41. 10.1016/j.jmpt.2013.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. van Dieen JH, Selen LP, Cholewicki J (2003) Trunk muscle activation in low-back pain patients, an analysis of the literature. Journal of electromyography and kinesiology: official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology 13: 333–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Barraclough NE, Xiao D, Oram MW, Perrett DI (2006) The sensitivity of primate STS neurons to walking sequences and to the degree of articulation in static images. Progress in brain research 154: 135–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Han Z, Bi Y, Chen J, Chen Q, He Y, Caramazza A (2013) Distinct regions of right temporal cortex are associated with biological and human-agent motion: functional magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological evidence. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33: 15442–15453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Boussaoud D, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R (1990) Pathways for motion analysis: cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque. The Journal of comparative neurology 296: 462–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Hietanen JK, Perrett DI (1996) Motion sensitive cells in the macaque superior temporal polysensory area: response discrimination between self-generated and externally generated pattern motion. Behavioural brain research 76: 155–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Cross ES, Hamilton AF, Kraemer DJ, Kelley WM, Grafton ST (2009) Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network. The European journal of neuroscience 30: 1383–1392. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06941.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Herrington JD, Nymberg C, Schultz RT (2011) Biological motion task performance predicts superior temporal sulcus activity. Brain and cognition 77: 372–381. 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Coslett HB, Saffran EM, Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledge of the human body: a distinct semantic domain. Neurology 59: 357–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Shenton JT, Schwoebel J, Coslett HB (2004) Mental motor imagery and the body schema: evidence for proprioceptive dominance. Neuroscience letters 370: 19–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Kessler K, Biermann-Ruben K, Jonas M, Siebner HR, Baumer T, Munchau A, et al. (2006) Investigating the human mirror neuron system by means of cortical synchronization during the imitation of biological movements. NeuroImage 33: 227–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Wand BM, Keeves J, Bourgoin C, George PJ, Smith AJ, O'Connell NE, et al. (2013) Mislocalization of sensory information in people with chronic low back pain: a preliminary investigation. The Clinical journal of pain 29: 737–743. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e318274b320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Brumagne S, Cordo P, Lysens R, Verschueren S, Swinnen S (2000) The role of paraspinal muscle spindles in lumbosacral position sense in individuals with and without low back pain. Spine 25: 989–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Brumagne S, Janssens L, Janssens E, Goddyn L (2008) Altered postural control in anticipation of postural instability in persons with recurrent low back pain. Gait & posture 28: 657–662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Moseley GL (2008) I can't find it! Distorted body image and tactile dysfunction in patients with chronic back pain. Pain 140: 239–243. 10.1016/j.pain.2008.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Luomajoki H, Moseley GL (2011) Tactile acuity and lumbopelvic motor control in patients with back pain and healthy controls. British journal of sports medicine 45: 437–440. 10.1136/bjsm.2009.060731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. de Lussanet MH, Behrendt F, Puta C, Schulte TL, Lappe M, Weiss T, et al. (2013) Impaired visual perception of hurtful actions in patients with chronic low back pain. Human movement science 32: 938–953. 10.1016/j.humov.2013.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. de Lussanet MH, Behrendt F, Puta C, Weiss T, Lappe M, Schulte TL, et al. (2012) A body-part-specific impairment in the visual recognition of actions in chronic pain patients. Pain 153: 1459–1466. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD (2009) Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophrenia bulletin 35: 509–527. 10.1093/schbul/sbn176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Poline JB, Kohn PD, Holt JL, Egan MF, Weinberger DR, et al. (2001) Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. The American journal of psychiatry 158: 1809–1817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Napadow V, LaCount L, Park K, As-Sanie S, Clauw DJ, Harris RE (2010) Intrinsic brain connectivity in fibromyalgia is associated with chronic pain intensity. Arthritis and rheumatism 62: 2545–2555. 10.1002/art.27497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Giesecke T, Gracely RH, Grant MA, Nachemson A, Petzke F, Williams DA, et al. (2004) Evidence of augmented central pain processing in idiopathic chronic low back pain. Arthritis and rheumatism 50: 613–623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Lidbeck J (2002) Central hyperexcitability in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a conceptual breakthrough with multiple clinical implications. Pain research & management: the journal of the Canadian Pain Society = journal de la societe canadienne pour le traitement de la douleur 7: 81–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Roussel NA, Nijs J, Meeus M, Mylius V, Fayt C, Oostendorp R (2013) Central sensitization and altered central pain processing in chronic low back pain: fact or myth? The Clinical journal of pain 29: 625–638. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31826f9a71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Del Percio C, Le Pera D, Arendt-Nielsen L, Babiloni C, Brancucci A, Chen AC, et al. (2006) Distraction affects frontal alpha rhythms related to expectancy of pain: an EEG study. NeuroImage 31: 1268–1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Babiloni C, Brancucci A, Capotosto P, Arendt-Nielsen L, Chen AC, Rossini PM (2005) Expectancy of pain is influenced by motor preparation: a high-resolution EEG study of cortical alpha rhythms. Behavioral neuroscience 119: 503–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Babiloni C, Brancucci A, Babiloni F, Capotosto P, Carducci F, Cincotti F, Buttiglione M, et al. (2003) Anticipatory cortical responses during the expectancy of a predictable painful stimulation. A high-resolution electroencephalography study. The European journal of neuroscience 18: 1692–1700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Babiloni C, Capotosto P, Brancucci A, Del Percio C, Petrini L, et al. (2008) Cortical alpha rhythms are related to the anticipation of sensorimotor interaction between painful stimuli and movements: a high-resolution EEG study. The journal of pain: official journal of the American Pain Society 9: 902–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Stark CE, Squire LR (2001) When zero is not zero: the problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98: 12760–12766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Gitelman DR, Penny WD, Ashburner J, Friston KJ (2003) Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution. NeuroImage 19: 200–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ (1996) Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 16: 4207–4221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Tahmasebi AM, Abolmaesumi P, Zheng ZZ, Munhall KG, Johnsrude IS (2009) Reducing inter-subject anatomical variation: effect of normalization method on sensitivity of functional magnetic resonance imaging data analysis in auditory cortex and the superior temporal region. NeuroImage 47: 1522–1531. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All fMRI-data files are available at the Dryad Digital Repository platform (doi:10.5061/dryad.2h0q3).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES