Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015 Nov;10(6):832–851. doi: 10.1177/1745691615596788

Seven fears and the science of how mobile technologies may be influencing adolescents in the digital age

Madeleine J George 1, Candice L Odgers 1
PMCID: PMC4654691  NIHMSID: NIHMS704598  PMID: 26581738

Abstract

Close to 80% of U.S. adolescents now own a mobile phone and they are using them frequently. Adolescents send, on average, 60 text messages per day from their mobile phones and most adolescents (74%) access the Internet from a mobile device. Many adults are asking how this constant connectivity is influencing adolescents’ development. This paper examines seven commonly voiced fears about the influence of mobile technologies on adolescents’ safety (cyberbullying and online solicitation), social development (peer relationships, parent-child relationships, and identity development), cognitive performance, and sleep. Three sets of findings emerge. First, with some notable exceptions (e.g., sleep disruption and new tools for bullying), the majority of online behaviors and threats to well-being are mirrored in the offline word, such that offline factors predict negative online experiences and effects. Second, the effects of mobile technologies are not uniform in that benefits appear to be conferred for some adolescents (e.g., skill building among shy adolescents) and risk exacerbated among others (e.g., worsening existing mental health problems). Third, experimental and quasi-experimental studies that go beyond a reliance on self-reported information are required to understand how, for whom, and under what conditions mobile technologies influence adolescents still developing social relationships, brains, and bodies.

Keywords: mobile technologies, adolescent development, cyberbullying, parental monitoring, peer relationships, online safety, multi-tasking, cognitive performance, sleep


Adolescents spend much of their day texting, exploring the web, and interacting with their mobile devices (Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010). The vast majority of adolescents are using mobile technologies and are doing so frequently: 78% of adolescents in the United States now own mobile phones and 74% report accessing the Internet via mobile devices (Madden, Lenhart, Duggan, Cortesi, & Gasser, 2013). The question is no longer if adolescents are using mobile technologies, but how, why, and with what effects. With adolescents sending an average of 60 text messages per day from their phones (Lenhart, 2012), and 80% of mobile phone owning adolescents reporting that they sleep with their phone (Lenhart, Ling, Campbell, & Purcell, 2010), many parents, educators, and policy-makers are asking whether mobile technologies are having a negative influence on adolescents. Mobile (or new) technologies are defined throughout as mobile devices that provide almost constant connection to others and the online world, including access to social networking tools, text messaging, and the Internet. 1

Common Fears about the Effects of Mobile Technologies on Adolescent’s Development

Concern among adults over how young people are using their time is not a new phenomenon. Generations of parents, teachers, and other adults have worried about whether exposure to ‘new’ forms of media, including the radio (Heisler, 1948; Longstaff, 1936), comic books (Thrasher, 1949), television (Maccoby, 1951), video games (Egli & Meyers, 1984), and violent media (Anderson et al., 2010; Ferguson & Kilburn, 2010), is “harming our children.” This review evaluates the most recent set of fears about adolescents’ interactions with new technologies. The list of fears we review is not exhaustive; rather, the selections are based on: (1) results from large-scale surveys of parents detailing aspects of new technologies that parents are most concerned about, (2) in-depth interviews with parents of adolescents participating in our own studies of adolescent mobile phone usage, and (3) a review of recent media coverage related to adolescents’ use of new technologies.

Parents responding to large-scale surveys consistently cite online safety as a primary concern (boyd & Hargittai, 2013; Madden, Cortesi, Gasser, Lenhart, & Duggan, 2012). For example, in a recent survey of 1000 parents of children between the ages of 10 and 14, 63% of parents reported being “extremely concerned” that their child may meet a stranger online, and one in three were “extremely concerned” that their child would be a victim of cyber bullying (boyd & Hargittai, 2013). Many parents (69%) also report being worried about their adolescents’ online activities and how their children are managing their reputations online (Madden et al., 2012). In our own interviews with 141 parents of young adolescents (Russell, Odgers, & Wang, submitted), parents also commonly voiced concerns about not being able to keep pace with their, often more the ‘tech savvy’, adolescents and effectively monitor their children’s online behavior and safety.

When identifying commonly expressed fears we also considered how adolescents’ use of new technologies has been framed in the popular press as media coverage can both capture, albeit not always accurately, and influence societal fears. Common themes that emerged included: concerns about cyberbullying and its effects on victims (e.g., see Hoffman’s December, 2010 article ‘As bullies go digital, parents struggle to catch up’ in The New York Times), fears that time spent on devices is interfering with adolescents’ ability to develop effective social and relationship skills (e.g., see Sherry Turkle’s 2011 book ‘Alone Together: Why we Expect More from Technology and Less from Each Other’ and Fowlkes ’ October, 2011 piece ‘Viewpoint: Why social media is destroying our social skills’ in USA Today), concerns that multi-tasking on devices is impairing cognitive performance (e.g., see Conley ’s March, 2011 article ‘Wired for distraction: Kids in social media,’ in Time Magazine), and claims that device usage is causing adolescents to lose sleep (e.g., Holson ’s July, 2014 piece ‘Social media’s vampires: They text by night’, in The New York Times). Although a number of additional ‘fears’ emerged from our examination of media coverage,2 the seven fears selected for this review reflect areas where there was evidence that parents were also concerned about the issue (versus representing only the views of a reporter or a single high-profile story) and where there was sufficient research to conduct a balanced review of the topic.

A Focus on the Adolescent Period

Our review of the potential negative effects of mobile technologies focuses on the adolescent period, broadly defined as between the ages of 12 and 20, for three reasons. First, there is a close congruence between the key features of mobile devices and critical developmental tasks required during adolescence. For example, communication between peers naturally increases in terms of both frequency and intensity during adolescence (Larson & Richards, 1989; Raffaelli & Duckett, 1989). Friendships and communication with peers are viewed as critical venues for the development of life-long social and relationship skills (for more discussion see for example Hartup, 1996; Hartup & Stevens, 1997; or Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995) and mobile technologies facilitate constant connectivity with peers and provide new tools for communication. Second, even though adolescents are not alone in their high usage of mobile devices, they are seen as a potentially vulnerable subgroup given the dramatic social, cognitive, biological, and psychological changes that characterize this period (Giedd, 2012). Third, the current generation of adolescents are unique in that they are “born digital”2; that is, most do not remember a time without access to the Internet and mobile devices (Palfrey & Gasser, 2008). Adolescents are now faced with the challenge of mastering key developmental tasks, including identity formation, building positive friendships, and transitioning to young adulthood, while fully immersed in the digital age. As such, updated theories and data are required to understand how their high engagement with the virtual world is influencing their development.

We begin our review with two of the most commonly voiced fears by parents; that is, concerns about whom adolescents are interacting with online and cyberbullying (fears 1 and 2). We then move to discuss how mobile technologies may be influencing adolescents’ relationships with friends and parents, as well as their own evolving sense of self and identity (fears 3–5). Finally, we review what is known about the potential effects of mobile technologies on adolescents’ cognitive performance (fear 6) and sleep (fear 7). For each fear, we review existing evidence, summarize what is currently known, and identify questions or approaches for future research. Evaluating the effects of new technologies on adolescents is hampered by the fact that the field, albeit evolving rapidly, is still in its infancy. Throughout the paper we emphasize the need for more rigorous study designs that are able to isolate whether documented associations are driven by the use of new technologies per se versus simply reflecting adolescents’ offline preferences and characteristics. When possible, we suggest steps that parents, teachers, and policy-makers can take to promote positive development among adolescents in the digital age.

Fear #1: Parents Worry about Who Adolescents are Interacting with Online and What Type of Information they are Sharing with Others

Adolescents in the United States are spending an unprecedented seven and half hours a day, on average, consuming electronic media, including about an hour and a half spent text messaging, and an additional half hour spent interacting with social media (Rideout et al., 2010). Outside of school, text messaging is the most frequent form of daily communication among adolescents, with a median of 60 messages per day and upwards of 100 messages for frequent users (Lenhart, 2012). Many parents report being concerned about who their adolescents are interacting with online (boyd & Hargittai, 2013). However, research has consistently shown that online versus offline networks look very similar. For example, a three-day diary study of 261 middle and high school students found that communication with strangers was infrequent and that the vast majority of online messages were exchanged between close, offline friends (Gross, 2004). A more recent study focused on adolescents’ interactions on social networking sites found that adolescents (n=250) reported interacting with over 77% of their social network friends in face-to-face settings (Reich, Subrahmanyam, & Espinoza, 2012). The authors concluded that, for the most part, adolescents were using these spaces to “interact with people from their known, offline worlds” (p.365). Finally, in one of the few studies to go beyond self-reported information, an analysis from a four-day sampling of text messages of 171 young adolescents reported that 70% of all text messages were classified as interactions between friends/peers, followed by romantic partners (21%), and parents (3%); with only 1% of text messages being sent to other adults (Underwood, Ehrenreich, More, Solis, & Brinkley, 2015).

Thus, for most adolescents, the most common daily online activities appear to be connecting with existing offline friends and managing social relationships. With that said, the majority of research to date has relied on self-reports of online exchanges and friendship networks. To overcome these methodological limitations, real time, unobtrusive monitoring of mobile phone logs, data sharing, or other forms of virtual communication is required. In addition, the application of more advanced social network analysis models is needed to capture the complexity of adolescents’ online networks and their evolution over time.

Many parents and educators also worry about the content of adolescents’ online exchanges. Past research has focused primarily on who adolescents are communicating with online and how frequently they are exchanging information. Some investigators have asked adolescents to voluntarily share copies of their text messages. However, virtually no studies have conducted unfiltered content analyses of adolescents’ online exchanges (for a review see: Underwood and colleagues (2012, 2014, or 2015). As an important exception, the “Blackberry Project,” was a naturalistic study that analyzed the content of 175 14-year-old adolescents’ text messages over two two-day assessment periods. In-depth coding revealed that the majority of messages sent by young adolescents were positive or neutral in content (Underwood et al., 2015) and only a small fraction of the messages contained sexual (6.7%) or profane language (7%) (Underwood et al, 2012). This study was pioneering in that it provided one of the first attempts to capture the content of online exchanges. Future research that applies these types of approaches to capture adolescents’ often complex and sophisticated online communication patterns is lacking, but sorely needed (for a fuller discussion and important exceptions to this limitation see work by Underwood, Rosen, More, Ehrenreich, and Gentsch (2012), Marwick and Ellison (2012), and Marwick and boyd (2010)).

The majority of text messages sent by younger adolescents appear to be positive or neutral. However, reports of digital data sharing indicate that many young people, particularly older adolescents, are sharing sexually explicit materials online. For example, a survey of 1034 10th graders (ages 15–16) from a large, urban school in Texas found that 20% of students reported sending a nude picture, semi-nude picture, or sexual text-only message, while 30% of students reported receiving such as message (Fleschler Peskin et al., 2013). In another Texas-based study of adolescents aged 14 to 19 years old (n=964), 28% of adolescents reported sending a naked picture of themselves through text or email (Temple et al., 2012). Similarly, among a probability sample of 1,839 students in Los Angeles High Schools, 15% reported sending a sexually explicit message or video (Rice et al., 2012) and in a separate study of 606 students from a private school in the southwestern United States, 20% reported sending a sexually explicit photo of themselves (Strassberg, McKinnon, Sustaita, & Rullo, 2013). Although the majority of these images were sent to romantic partners, the content is easily shared beyond the intended recipient. For example, one in four US adolescents report that they have forwarded a sexually explicit cell phone picture (Strassberg et al., 2013). In contrast to these findings, a survey of 1560 youth Internet users found that only 2.5% of adolescents reported appearing in or creating nude or nearly nude pictures (Mitchell, Finkelhor, Jones, & Wolak, 2012). However, the estimates provided by this study have been criticized due to the reliance on a primarily young sample (25% of the sample was between 10 and 12 years of age) and the use of telephone based interviews when privacy was not guaranteed (see Strassburg et al, 2013 for a fuller discussion).

To summarize, we have learned that: (1) there is a significant degree of overlap between online versus offline friendships; (2) much of the content of these high frequency exchanges among younger adolescents appears to be positive or neutral; (3) however, a significant percentage of older adolescents report participating in “sexting”. Although estimates vary depending on how sexting is defined, the ease at which photos and videos can be created and shared via new technologies is creating some new risks for adolescents in the online world. Future research that goes beyond counting how many and to whom adolescents send messages is needed. More specifically, a more intensive focus on both the content and motivations underlying online exchanges is required to understand what these types of online behaviors may signal for adolescents’ offline relationships and adjustment.

Fear #2: Parents Fear that their Children will be Victims of Cyberbullying or be Solicited by Strangers Online

Parents have always been very concerned about the safety of their children. With the introduction of new technologies, new fears have been introduced (e.g., the idea that strangers will contact and victimize their children online), while other fears have been amplified (e.g., concerns that their children will be more easily harassed and victimized by their peers). Given the constant connectivity and challenges in monitoring online behaviors, it is no surprise that online safety is one of the most frequently cited concerns by parents in large-scale surveys (e.g. boyd & Hargittai, 2013). For example, among a national sample of over 1000 U.S. parents of children between the ages of 10 and14 years old, over 90% of parents expressed some level of concern that their child would be a victim of bullying online, and close to 80% reported being “very” or “extremely” concerned that their child would meet a stranger online (boyd & Hargittai, 2013).

Parents fear cyberbullying because it is more difficult to monitor than traditional bullying, allows perpetrators to remain anonymous, and may enter their child’s life at any time of day or night. Cyberbullying is typically defined as aggression that is intentionally and repeatedly carried out via electronic mediums, such as text messages and social networking sites (Kowalski, Giumetti, Schroeder, & Lattanner, 2014). Cyberbullying is one of the few areas where a substantial amount of data regarding the effects of online interactions on adolescents’ lives now exists. A recent meta-analysis of 131 studies by Kowalski et al. (2014) highlighted the following key findings: (1) most estimates of cyberbullying among adolescents involvement fall between 11% to 48% and estimates vary widely depending on the definition of cyberbullying, the age and characteristics of the study members, and the reporting time frame, (2) there is a substantial degree of overlap between adolescents who bully others offline and those who engage in cyberbullying; similarly, victims of cyberbullying are often victimized offline, and (3) adolescents who experience cyberbullying are at increased risk for a wide range of mental and physical health problems.

Cyberbullying has been a growing source of concern among educators, parents, and the media over the last decade. However, a recent analysis of large-scale longitudinal studies concluded that cyberbullying has a much lower prevalence than traditional bullying, has not increased in the last five to seven years, and has not produced many new bullies or victims (Olweus, 2012). This examination of traditional and cyberbullying involvement among over 440,000 US students converged on the following points. First, 17.6% of students, on average, reported being verbally bullied (offline bullying) between 2007 and 2012, versus 4.5% of youth who reported cyber victimization. Second, 9.6% of youth, on average, reported bullying others offline across this time period, versus 2.8% of youth who reported cyberbullying. The author also presented very similar patterns of findings from a large sample of Norwegian students (n=9,000) drawn from over 41 schools and followed from 2006 to 2010. Findings across both samples illustrate that significantly more youth were involved in traditional versus cyber bullying across this time period. Third, no significant increases in cyberbullying involvement were observed over time despite the increasing accessibility and use of mobile phones during this period. Fourth, an extremely high degree of overlap between cyber and traditional bullying was observed. That is, among reported victims of cyberbullying, close to 90% had experienced traditional forms of bullying. Similarly, among the students who reported cyberbullying others, approximately 90% also reported bullying others offline. That is, only one in ten of reported cyber victims/bullies did not report a history of victimization/bullying offline.

Although cyberbullying may have a lower prevalence than traditional bullying and may not create a large number of new victims, being a victim of cyberbullying is routinely associated with a number of negative outcomes. The majority of victims report negative feelings such as embarrassment, worry, fear, depression, loneliness, or anger after cyberbullying events (Ortega et al., 2012). The severity of both offline and online bullying events has been shown to predict future psychopathology including suicide ideation and self-harm (Hinduja & Patchin, 2010) and findings from a recent meta-analysis indicate that cyberbulling relates more strongly to suicide ideation than traditional bullying (Van Geel, Vedder & Tanilon, 2014). Already marginalized adolescents, such as Lesbian Gay Bisexual Transgender (LGBT) youth, also report significantly more frequent online attacks than heterosexual youth (Finn, 2004). More generally, cyber victims are likely to have significant mental health and social problems (Kowalski & Limber, 2014; Ybarra, Diener-West, & Leaf, 2007; Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004). Thus, cyberbullying may not create many new victims, but may exacerbate problems for already vulnerable adolescents.

In sum, many adolescents at risk for cyberbullying are also at risk for traditional bullying and victimization. As in traditional bullying, many adolescents report not wanting to “tattle” on their peers and report feeling that adults cannot help their situation (Englander, 2013). However, bullying within online contexts appears also to introduce new risks (e.g., potential for anonymity, a large audience, and a digital record) and may be driven by different motivations than traditional bullying. Most victims can identify their bullies as classmates or friends (Juvonen & Gross, 2008), but some cannot definitely name their attackers either because they were strangers or due to the anonymity provided by the virtual setting (Kowalski & Limber, 2007). If cyberbullies remain anonymous, then bullies may never learn the consequences of their actions and victims may experience greater distress and feel less in control of their online activities. A more nuanced picture of the social dynamics of online bullying is required to understand how cyberbullying attacks influence social networks and whether, because of their immersion in the digital world, it may be especially difficult for adolescents to avoid unwanted harassment. Indeed, one of the reasons that adolescents report not wanting to report bullying experiences is a fear that their parents will take away their devices (Englander, 2013).

Given the high degree of overlap between offline and online bullying, the case has been made that cyberbullying should be viewed and treated in the context of traditional bullying (for a discussion see Olweus, 2012). Empirically-based guidelines for the creation of intervention programs targeting both traditional bullying and cyberbullying are available (Olweus, 1994, 2012) and promising programs now exist. For example, results from a randomized trial of an anti-bullying program delivered across 78 schools in Finland reported decreases in multiple forms of victimization, including cyberbullying (Salmivalli, Karna, & Poskiparta, 2011; Williford et al., 2012). It is also possible that new technologies can be used to assist victims targeted by both offline and online attacks. Adolescents may be encouraged to share their stories, seek support for mental health problems, and visit anti-bullying websites (Burns, Durkin, & Nicholas, 2009). Unfortunately, although many adolescents report cyberbullying incidents in anonymous research surveys, most youth still do not report either traditional or cyberbullying to their parents or teachers (Blumenfeld & Cooper, 2010; Juvonen & Gross, 2008). New strategies are required to create safe environments for victim disclosure.

This section has focused primarily on online victimization between peers, as the majority of communications using new technologies occurs between similar-aged peers who are known to each other offline. However, one of the most commonly voiced fears among parents is that their child will be solicited by a stranger online (boyd & Hargittai, 2013). A relatively large body of research exists regarding Internet solicitation risk and demonstrates that, for most adolescents, the risk of being solicited or victimized by a stranger in the virtual world is relatively low. Internet initiated crimes account for approximately 7% of all child sex crimes (Wolak, Finkelhor, Mitchell, & Ybarra, 2008), and estimates based on a national sample of 10–17 year olds (n=1500) indicated that approximately 1–3% of 10–12 year olds and 5–6% of 13–17 year olds had experienced ”aggressive sexual solicitation” (e.g., attempts to make offline contact) or reported experiencing “distressing sexual solicitation” (Mitchell, Jones, Finkelhor, & Wolak, 2013). In addition, the targeting of potential victims online does not appear to be random (Wolak et al., 2008). Adolescents with offline risks, such as substance use or delinquency, are more likely to be sexually solicited online (Ybarra et al., 2007). Notably, simply posting information about oneself does not appear to increase risk (Lenhart & Madden, 2007; Madden, Lenhart, Cortesi, et al., 2013). Rather, it is the interactive component of exchanges with unknown individuals, combined with offline risk factors, that are most predictive of solicitation and harassment (for a review see: Wolak et al., 2008). There has been very little research on how the introduction of the increasing use of mobile devices (versus prior Internet use at a stationary terminal in the child’s home or school) may be influencing adolescent’s risk for online solicitation. It is possible that the interactive components of new technologies (e.g., easy access to cameras, geo-location, and greater sharing of information), combined with a greater number of adolescents online, especially younger adolescents, may elevate risk. In sum, additional research is required to understand whether mobile technologies are modifying what we currently know about the risk for online Internet solicitation based on older research studies.

Fear 3: Adolescents’ Constant Connectivity Prevents them from being Present in ‘Real Life’ and Interferes with Offline Socialization Experiences and Friendships

In her most recent book Alone Together: Why We Expect More from Technology and Less from Each Other (2011), Sherry Turkle weaves together a series of case studies and argues that technology is interfering with the ability of everyone, but especially adolescents, to effectively communicate and achieve closeness in relationships. From this perspective, groups of adolescents seen interacting with their phones (rather than each other) are characterized as spending time “alone together” and missing out on important socialization experiences. Early research on the Internet in the 1990s tended to support the idea that online interactions with strangers were occurring at the expense of existing relationships (for a review see Valkenburg & Peter, 2009). However, these early studies captured online exchanges that were bounded by the state of the Internet at that time; that is, with only 11% of adolescents online and chat rooms typically used for interactions between strangers, the virtual context looked much different at that time than it does today.

Today, the majority of adolescents are online and the vast majority of online exchanges occur between peers who also identify as friends offline (see summary of research in Fear 1). For the most part, research conducted within the last decade has found that adolescents who report more frequent online communication also report higher quality friendships. For example, a survey of over 1,200 Dutch preadolescents and adolescents found positive associations between the time adolescents spent online, frequency of chats with friends, quality of friendships, and well-being (Valkenburg & Peter, 2007a). In general, online communication was primarily used to contact existing friends and more online chats with friends predicted higher friendship quality and well-being in ‘real’ life. In addition, adolescents who reported chatting more versus less often with friends online also spent more time with friends in person. Similarly, adolescents (n=2,000) in a large study in Bermuda who reported more frequent online communications with friends also reported higher perceived friendship quality (Davis (2013). Consistent with these findings, a major theme that has emerged from in-depth interviews with smaller samples of adolescents is that online communication between friends promotes self-disclosure and feelings of belonging (Davis, 2012). Theoretically, Valkenburg and Peter (2009) have argued that the greater ease of online self-disclosure (communication about personal topics not easily disclosed in person) accounts for the positive associations observed between online communication and social connectedness within contemporary studies.

Because most research to date has relied on self-reported information and cross-sectional data, it is difficult to determine whether the use of online tools is strengthening existing relationships, or alternatively, whether adolescents with strong relationships are simply more likely to engage in frequent online interactions. In one of the few studies to address this question, multi-informant data from the Child Development Supplement from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (n = 1,312) showed that children with stronger relationships early in life (prior to age 12) were more likely to use online communication frequently in adolescence (evidence of social selection) and, in turn, report having more close and cohesive friendships (evidence of social causation) (Lee, 2009). This set of findings was cited as supporting the “rich-get-richer” hypothesis, originally forwarded by Kraut et al. (2002) in which those with strong existing social networks and skills are the most likely to benefit from online interactions. Perhaps the most convincing evidence regarding the potential benefits of online communication stem from one of the few experimental studies conducted to date. Following experimentally induced social exclusion, adult and adolescent participants who were assigned to instant messaging versus solitary game play reported greater replenishment of self-esteem and perceptions of being accepted, valued, and respected (Gross, 2009). Online messaging versus solitary game place also resulted in a greater reduction of negative affect among adolescents as compared to adults. These findings suggest that online exchanges may help adolescents to “bounce back” emotionally following an experience of social rejection or isolation.

There is also reason to believe that time spent online may be beneficial for skill building and enhanced well-being among those with existing vulnerabilities (Bardi & Brady, 2010; Valkenburg & Peter, 2007b). For example, shy college students report that they Instant Message to increase interpersonal contacts, improve fluency of in-person conversations and decrease loneliness (Bardi & Brady, 2010). Additionally, among college students who report low self-esteem or life satisfaction, increasing amounts of time spent on Facebook predicts higher levels of positive affect and well-being (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007). Additional research using larger community versus college-based samples are required to understand how young people with existing vulnerabilities are spending their time online. In addition, experimental studies in this area are sorely needed and, as illustrated by Gross (2009), offer the ability to disentangle the effects of online experiences per se from how certain types of individuals choose to engage with new technologies.

Online conversations may help build networks and confidence for some young people. However, they may also mark or exacerbate problems for individuals with existing mental health problems. For example, among Dutch adolescents with depressive symptoms, greater technology use has been associated with inflated feelings of loneliness and isolation over time when used for entertainment rather than communication (Selfout, Branje, Delsing, ter Bogt, & Meeus, 2009). A naturalistic observational study of U.S. adolescents reported that adolescents who sent a greater number of text messages characterized by negative content (‘negative talk to or about others’) were more likely to suffer from informant-reported internalizing and depressive symptoms; however, there was little evidence that the frequency of texting predicted emotional problems (Underwood et al., 2015). Findings from this study also illustrated that adolescents whose text messages contained antisocial themes were more likely to experience increases in parent-reported, teacher-reported, and self-reported antisocial behavior over time (Ehrenreich et al., 2014). These findings are important as they go beyond simply characterizing the frequency of online exchanges and instead begin to address what the content of exchanges may signal for offline development. This study was also novel in that it integrated information from multiple informants, whereas most research has relied on single informant and self-reported information.

To summarize, time spent online does appear to displace in-person interactions. However, there is little evidence that it reduces friendship quality or leads to social isolation. For the most part, adolescents appear to be using mobile technologies to communicate and stay connected to existing friends and, in turn, may be strengthening the quality of existing relationships. Although primarily correlational, young people who replace in-person exchanges with virtual interactions appear to intensify their social impairments, while those who use online exchanges to supplement existing friendships report improvements in the quality and closeness of their existing relationships. However, it is important to note that nearly all of the research reviewed has relied on self-report data and non-experimental study designs, making it difficult to discern whether technology usage per se is influencing outcomes (for a key exception see Gross, 2009). Moving forward, experimental studies delivered via mobile devices may be one way to begin testing for whom and under what conditions online activities may influence key aspects of adolescents’ friendships and social development.

Fear #4: Mobile Phones are Creating a ‘Digital Divide’ between Parents and Adolescents

Many parents worry that the constant use of mobile phones by their teens is interfering with their ability to effectively communicate with their children. Although research has found that adolescents’ time spent online displaces face-to-face interactions with parents, moderate technology use does not appear to predict declining parent-child relationship quality (Williams & Merten, 2011). For example, a U.S. based diary study monitored the amount of time adolescents spent online versus interacting with others in person (Lee, 2009). Adolescents who reported higher versus lower levels of time online also reported spending less time with their parents. However, there were no differences in the child-reported quality of those parental relationships by time spent online (Lee, 2009). Some studies have found that higher than average reported Internet use by adolescents is associated with lower quality parental relationships, including parental attachment and knowledge (Willoughby, 2008). However, it is not clear whether technology use per se is affecting the relationship, or whether patterns of virtual parent-child communication are simply correlates of the existing offline relationship. Finally, there is some evidence that the shared use of new technologies across generations can foster stronger ties via more frequent parent-child contact. Families may engage in shared learning or play using new technologies (see for example research on video game play: Coyne, Padilla-Walker, Stockdale, & Day, 2011), and evidence from a small sample of U.S. college students suggests that mobile phones may increase the frequency of adolescents’ communications with parents while they are apart and, subsequently, improve the quality of parent-child relationships (Chen & Katz, 2009).

One thing that appears to matter is who is initiating contact. On one hand, new technologies can provide adolescents with quick, easy, and remote access to their parents. Adolescents who report calling their parents for support more often report better family cohesion and parental knowledge (Weisskirch, 2009). On the other hand, parents who call their children more frequently may not necessarily know more about their child’s behaviors. In families or situations where conflict is high, constant communication between parents and adolescents may exacerbate tensions. For example, among a sample of 196 parent-child dyads, frequency of parental calls was negatively related to adolescent reported truthfulness (Weisskirch, 2009). Similarly, parents who reported frequently calling their child when they were upset or for the purposes of monitoring also reported less knowledge of their child’s activities and poorer parent-child interactions than parents who relied on adolescent-initiated contact (Weisskirch, 2009, 2011).

One novel feature that mobile technologies offers is the potential for tech-savvy parents to unobtrusively monitor their adolescents’ behaviors, including monitoring the content of their online posts or exchanges and tracking their location. Findings based on a 2012 nationally representative phone survey of 802 U.S. parents and their teens indicated that most parents, especially those of younger teens, are taking steps to monitor their children’s online activities (Madden et al., 2012). That is, 50% of parents of online teens report using parental controls to block, filter, and monitor their children’s activities, and 59% of the parents of teens who use social networking sites have spoken with their child about something that concerned them on their account (Madden et al., 2012). However, it is important to note that parental monitoring alone has not been found to change adolescents behaviors within offline contexts (Kerr, Stattin, & Burk, 2010). Instead, adolescents’ own efforts to disclose information, versus parents attempts to monitor their children, has consistently emerged as the best predictor of involvement in risky behaviors. Given the lack of evidence for a causal association, it is unclear whether enhanced parental monitoring via mobile devices will lead to either increased parental knowledge or any changes in adolescents’ behaviors. In addition, mobile devices (versus computers in the home) and rapid changes in the tools adolescents are using for online communication are making it difficult to parents to effectively monitor their adolescents’ online activities and accounts.

To summarize, technology use among adolescents may take away from time spent with parents, but it does not necessarily weaken the parent-child relationship. Existing evidence suggests that if the quality of the parent-child relationship is strong offline, new technologies may confer benefits. Again, parallels are seen between the relationships that adolescents have in their offline versus online lives. More research is needed to understand how specific forms of virtual communication could be used to strengthen existing relationships, enhance feelings of adolescent autonomy, and increase parent knowledge. For example, experimental research is required to test whether adolescents find it easier to discuss sensitive topics with their parents online versus offline. Again, nearly all of the research conducted to date has been correlational and has relied on adolescent or parent report to describe relationship quality. Study designs that can facilitate causal inference are needed to test whether variations in online communications and monitoring lead to, or simply mark, differences in parent-child relationship quality.

Fear #5: Adolescents are Experimenting with Alternative Identities Online while Leaving a Digital Archive of Data that may Damage their Sense of Self and Future Lives

Adolescence has long been viewed as a time of self-exploration and discovery of one’s place in the social world (Erikson, 1968; Steinberg & Morris, 2001). In a classic sense, identity formation represents a key developmental task of adolescence involving the resolution of fundamental psychological conflicts or “crises”. During this process, adolescents become increasingly self-aware of their abilities, limitations, and defining qualities, while addressing critical questions about their values and roles in the social world. A successful progression through this stage-based process is characterized by the adolescent arriving at a cohesive, integrated sense of him or herself in the transition to adulthood (Erikson, 1968; Marcia, 1966). Over the years, the idea that adolescence is a critical period for self-reflection and locating oneself in the social world has held with a slight shift in more recent scholarship to focusing on the development of self-conceptions and self-worth among younger adolescents (Masten et al., 1995) and on ethnic identity development within diverse populations (Phinney, 1989). In short, adolescence is generally viewed as an optimal time for self-exploration and identity consolidation, processes that are dependent on interactions with peers and caregivers, as well as internal self-reflection and consolidation.

Mobile technologies offer a number of opportunities for adolescents to experiment with alternative identities and roles in the virtual world. In particular, there is now a growing body of research documenting how adolescents are using digital media for self-expression and experimentation, including the creation of online forums, posts, videos, and the creation of social media profiles (Buckingham, 2008; Calvert, Jordan, & Cocking, 2002; Davis & James, 2013; Gardner & Davis, 2013; Subrahmanyam & Smahel, 2011; Turkle, 1995). For example, in his edited volume, Buckingham (2008) brings together a diverse collection of case studies that illustrate how youth are engaging with digital tools and networks in ways that encourage adolescents’ growing autonomy and need for self-expression a provide tools that allow them to generate a “constant presences” and “write themselves into being” (p.10). Buckingham also illustrates how technology may be regarded as “… a force of liberation for young people—a means for them to reach past the constraining influence of their elders, and to create new, autonomous forms of communication and community. Far from corrupting the young, technology is seen to be creating a generation that is more open, more democratic, more creative, and more innovative than their parents’ generation.” (p.13). Narrative accounts also suggest that online spaces may offer safe places for some young people to explore sensitive topics about their sexuality and identity (Harper, Bruce, Serrano, & Jamil, 2009). These types of observations have sparked an interesting and ongoing debate about how the digital age may be influencing identity development among young people.

Consistent with research on adolescent friendships, adolescents’ online behaviors and presentations of self tend to closely mirror their offline activities, interests, and personalities. For example, in a daily diary study of virtual messaging in the U.S., one in ten adolescents reported frequently using the Internet to “pretend to be somebody else”, whereas the majority of adolescents reported using the Internet for communication with offline friends on everyday topics (Gross, 2004). In a more recent study of over 2,000 adolescents in Bermuda, online peer communications have been shown to indirectly influence adolescents’ self-concept via their positive influence on friendship quality (Davis, 2013). Similarly, thematic analyses of in-depth interviews with adolescents have indicated that online peer communications are often used to promote adolescents’ sense of belonging and self-disclosure (Davis, 2012), two key processes that support successful identity development during adolescence.

Access to the online world may also spark new interests and allow some adolescents to try out new identities in a relatively safe place. In an older survey study of Internet communication, close to 50% of Dutch adolescents (n=600) reported pushing the boundaries of their identities to reinforce social skills and relationships, or, most commonly, for self-exploration (Valkenburg, Schouten, & Peter, 2005). For example, adolescents in this study, especially girls and younger adolescents, pretended to be older or more attractive in order to see how others online might react to them differently. There are also virtual spaces, such as Second Life, where completely new environments allow for the creation of avatars that can model different social situations and personalities, potentially allowing adolescents to take on new identities and roles (Turkle, 2011). Unfortunately, very little information exists on how the majority of adolescents use these types of virtual spaces. Thus, it is not known whether adolescents are taking advantage of these virtual settings to try out new identities or roles versus simply recreating images that resemble their offline selves.

Small scale case studies have suggested some intriguing possibilities regarding the ways in which adolescents are “repurposing” technology to fulfill their desires or achieve their goals (e.g., encoding online messages to communicate privately with close peers, monitoring and sharing information online to position themselves in social groups) as well as using online spaces to, “take control over their lives, and find ways to be part of public life” (boyd, 2014 p. 212). However, one concern that has been voiced is that online presentations of self are closing adolescents off from face-to-face exploration while encouraging online self-promotion and the need to script a “flawless narrative” about their lives (Turkle, 2011). Indeed, previous interview-based research and content analyses of online posts suggests that adolescents struggle to reconcile wanting to present their lives honestly with wanting to impress others (Bortree, 2005). Unfortunately, much of this work has focused on Internet use in general versus mobile technology usage in particular and has been drawn from small, selective, and/or convenience samples.

Are there Reputational and Identity Costs Associated with the Digital Trail of Information Adolescents leave Online?

The accumulation of digital data (e.g., adolescents’ online communications, photos, videos) archived online over time has been termed a “digital dossier” (Palfrey & Gasser, 2008). Very little is known about how digital dossiers may influence adolescents’ evolving or future sense of self. Symbolic Interactionist theorists suggest that negative labels applied to adolescents by others (for example, ‘druggie’ and ‘delinquent’) may become internalized by the adolescents and ultimately lead to behaviors that are congruent with the label (e.g., Matsueda, 1992). Where prior generations of adolescents had the benefit of fading memories around potentially salient incidents (e.g., bullying incident, using drugs or alcohol at a party), online videos and photos may cause adolescents to relive emotionally charged experiences. As the first generation of adolescents who were “born digital” move through early adulthood, we will need to understand how the online archiving of their experiences during childhood and adolescence influences their evolving sense of self and test whether there are reputational or ‘identity costs’ associated with digital archives that carry into young adulthood (e.g., effects on college admissions, employment, future romantic partners).

To summarize, social scientists are just beginning to understand the ways in which online interactions are influencing how adolescents explore, form, and modify their sense of self over time and, to a lesser extent, how mobile devices are contributing to these effects. Most research has echoed a recurrent theme - that there is considerable overlap in how individuals present themselves to others both online and offline. However, there are important exceptions to this finding including, for example, the ability of LGBT youth to more fully explore and develop their identities in safe and shared spaces. Notably, research in this area has been generated from a very diverse set of perspectives, sources and methodologies. Ethnographic research and narrative reviews have been invaluable in showcasing the diversity of effects that mobile technologies may have on adolescents’ still developing sense of self. These studies have also provided powerful illustrations of the challenges that adolescents face when trying to integrate their online and offline personas (for a more detailed discussion see: Buckingham, 2008). However, large scale longitudinal and experimentally-based studies are now needed to test whether there is anything unique about mobile technologies per se that is influencing identity exploration and formation across adolescence, and to evaluate whether online experiences uniquely contribute to adjustment in the transition to adulthood.

Fear #6: Constant Multi-Tasking on Mobile Devices is Impairing Adolescents’ Cognitive Performance

The media has dubbed 21st century children and adolescents, “Generation M” for the unprecedented amount of time they spend consuming media and multi-tasking (Rideout et al., 2010). Among adults, experimental studies have consistently shown that multi-tasking, task switching, or distractions can lead to detrimental effects on immediate cognitive performance (e.g., Altmann, Trafton, & Hambrick, 2014; Rogers & Monsell, 1995). That is, multi-tasking increases error rates and tends to increase the amount of time that it takes to complete a given task. Adolescents report using new technologies to multi-task, for example talking to a friend while completing their homework online (Gross, 2004), and both adolescents and college students admit to frequently operating multiple types of new technologies at the same time (Jacobsen & Forste, 2011; Lenhart, Purcell, Smith, & Zuckuhr; Moreno et al., 2012). For example, when adolescents are ‘supposed to be doing homework’, students are typically multitasking (Shumow, Schmidt, & Kackar, 2008) and two-thirds of the time they are multi-tasking with some online activity (Rideout et al., 2010).

Research with college students suggests that multi-tasking associated with new technologies may have negative effects. For example, college students classified as “heavy users” of new technologies tend to exhibit more academic impairments, such as lower course grades (Englander, Terregrossa, & Wang, 2010), less time spent studying (Kirschner & Karpinski, 2010), and a greater number of missed classes (Kubey, Lavin, & Barrows, 2001) as compared to adolescents with average or low usage of new technologies. In addition, students who report frequent multitasking with Instant Messaging while doing schoolwork also report that they believe that these behaviors have a detrimental effect on their work and contribute to lower academic achievement (Junco & Cotten, 2011). However, the causal direction (if any) is unclear as it is possible that those who tend to use new technologies more frequently for recreation or non-academic purposes are simply more likely to experience difficulties across domains. Research with adults also suggests that those that engage who in high levels of multi-tasking are often the most easily distracted. In a quasi-experimental study, participants who self-reported more versus less daily multi-tasking were more distracted by experimental external interference and had lower scores on tests of task-switching (Ophir, Nass, & Wagner, 2009). Heavy multitaskers perceive their ability to multi-task to be high, but their actual ability is lower than that of light multi-taskers (Sanbonmatsu, Strayer, Medeiros-Ward, & Watson, 2013).

Unfortunately, very little is known about how ‘Generation M’ adolescents are performing in the face of unprecedented changes in the amount of time spent consuming and interacting with new technologies. Experimental paradigms need to be extended to adolescence and reconfigured to account the unique features of adolescents’ development and their use of mobile devices. There is also almost no research on the hypothesized neurological effects of multi-tasking using new technologies across adolescence (Giedd, 2012). It may be that the plasticity of the adolescent brain is allowing adolescents to optimize their performance to their new digital worlds or, conversely, digital overload may be leading to impairments in cognitive abilities and performance. Finally, it will be important to understand how mobile devices may be interfering with tasks that require sustained attention in daily life, such as distracted driving, as over one in three driving adolescents report that they have texted while driving (Madden & Lenhart, 2009). Experimental paradigms currently exist for evaluating how adolescents’ cognitive performance and decision making is influenced by the presence of peers (e.g., Gardner & Steinberg, 2005) and it will be informative to extend these paradigms to evaluate whether the presence of a virtual peer can have similar effects.

Fear #7: Adolescents are Losing Sleep due to their Devices

Adolescents require between 8.5 and 10 hours of sleep per night, yet the majority of adolescents (58%) are sleeping 7 hours or less per night (Emsellem et al., 2014). Poor sleep habits can lead to physical and mental health problems and are associated with reduced cognitive performance (for a review see Dahl & Lewin, 2002). Puberty itself has been linked to delayed phase preference, meaning adolescents’ brains become naturally wired to stay up and sleep later than children (Carskadon, Vieira, & Acebo, 1993). Research has shown that adolescents are sleeping less than both children and previous generations of adolescents (Iglowstein, Jenni, Molinari, & Largo, 2003). Sleep is one area where there is now compelling evidence that adolescents’ use of new technologies is having adverse effects on sleep duration and quality. There are at least three possible pathways through which new technologies may impair adolescents’ sleep: 1) media time displaces sleep time, 2) emotionally arousing media or online interactions make it more difficult for adolescents to fall and stay asleep, and/or 3) bright light from monitors or electromagnetic radiation from mobile phones disturbs melatonin activity and sleep rhythms.

Nearly all (97%) of U.S. adolescents have some type of electronic media (i.e., music player, TV, video game, phone, computer or Internet) in their bedrooms (Carskadon, Mindell, & Drake, 2006). Adolescents with four or more devices in their bedrooms report greater sleep related difficulties (e.g., feeling tired) and sleep less on weeknights and weekends than adolescents with three or fewer devices (Carskadon et al., 2006). Late night computer or mobile phone use is related to later bedtimes, less total sleep, greater tiredness, and lower sleep quality (Calamaro, Mason, & Ratcliffe, 2009; Van den Bulck, 2004). Over half of adolescents access the Internet and more than a third text or talk on their phones after 9pm (Calamaro et al., 2009). In addition, 4 in 5 adolescents in the U.S. now report sleeping with their phone on or near their bed. Adolescents who use their phone for texting are 42% more likely than adolescent mobile-phone owners who do not text to sleep with their phone by their bedside, and many report leaving their phones under their pillows so that they can respond to texts during the night (Lenhart et al., 2010). Among a large sample of 1,656 young adolescents in Belgium, 62% reported using their mobile phone after lights out (Van den Bulck, 2007). Adolescents who used their mobile phones “right after lights out” were twice as likely to report being “very tired” when compared to those who did not text after dark. Furthermore, adolescents who used their phones throughout the night were close to four times more likely to report being “very tired” the next day as compared to adolescents that did not respond (or allow) late night phone messages. Thus, media time appears to be displacing sleep time for a significant number of adolescents.

There is also some evidence that emotionally arousing media content and the light emitted from devices interfere with the amount and quality of sleep adolescents may be receiving. For example, adolescent study participants who were assigned to play an interactive video game before bed were more cognitively alert and took slightly longer to fall asleep than participants who passively watched a movie (Weaver, Gradisar, Dohnt, Lovato, & Douglas, 2010). A recent laboratory-based experimental study conducted with a small sample of adults (n=12) found that reading a book on a light emitting device (iPad) in the hours before bedtime versus reading a printed book resulted in a longer amount of time falling asleep, a 50% reduction in melatonin secretion (the sleep-promoting hormone), later timing of the circadian clock (participants melatonin rhythm was more than an hour and a half delayed), and reduced next morning alertness (Chang, Aeschbach, Duffy, & Czeisler, 2014). Experimental studies of this kind are needed outside of laboratory settings with adolescents, where device use prior to bedtime is common and the amount of exposure time varies widely.

In sum, research to date has consistently shown that mobile device usage prior to bedtime is associated with reduced sleep time and quality. Although many studies have relied on self-reported sleep duration and quality (for a review see Cain & Gradisar, 2010), experimental studies that manipulate pre-bedtime exposure to new technologies and capture sleep via more objective measures are now emerging (Chang et al., 2014). Future research is required to understand the effects of specific types of media consumption, such as peer messaging, on late night technology use and subsequent sleep quality. It is now possible to use high-quality ambulatory monitors of sleep duration and quality alongside tools that record the frequency and content of online activities. As adolescents’ lives become increasingly wired, it will also be important to understand how the use of multiple devices and modes of communication interfere with sleep and what can be done to offset these effects.

Conclusions

In this paper we reviewed seven commonly expressed fears about the effects of ubiquitous new technologies on adolescents’ safety, social development, cognitive performance, and sleep. The list of fears examined was not exhaustive, but included concerns that repeatedly emerged across national survey data, in-depth parent interviews, and recent popular press coverage. Three main sets of conclusions from this review are detailed below.

First, although there are cases where new technologies have introduced new risks to adolescent well-being (e.g., by creating a new platforms for bullying, interfering with sleep, and creating a digital archive that may carry reputational costs), the majority of behaviors and risks that are present in the online world appear to be mirrored offline. For example, there is a high degree of overlap in online versus offline friendship networks (e.g., Reich et al., 2012) as well as in the content of young adolescents’ exchanges (see Underwood et al., 2014, for a review). Adolescents at risk of being victimized or solicited in their offline lives are also at a heightened risk for being victimized online (e.g., Kowalski et al., 2014; Olweus, 2012). Similarly, adolescents with high quality relationships in real life are also likely to be strongly connected and to engage in positive interactions online (e.g., Valkenburg & Peter, 2007a). That is, while new technologies are offering new platforms for adolescents to interact with each other, online behaviors can often be predicted by offline behaviors and characteristics.

Second, the effects of new technologies on adolescent development are not uniform, nor should we expect them to be. Adolescents with strong familial and peer relationships exhibit enhanced relationship quality when virtual interactions are also present (e.g., Valkenburg & Peter, 2007b). In contrast, for adolescents who are struggling within existing relationships, high levels of technology use predict lower well-being and relationship quality (e.g., Weisskirch, 2009). Similarly, shy or isolated adolescents may seek out online interactions to decrease loneliness or build skills (e.g., Bardi & Brady, 2010), while high usage among adolescents with existing mental health problems, such as depression, may exacerbate existing vulnerabilities (Selfout, Branje, Delsing, ter Bogt, & Meeus, 2009). Similarly, posting personal information online is not associated with an elevated risk for unwanted sexual solicitation for most adolescents, but it does increase risk for those already at risk offline (Wolak et al., 2008).

Third, prior research has relied heavily, if not almost exclusively, on observational data. Study designs that can more readily facilitate causal inference are sorely needed. There is also a need to move beyond the sole reliance on self-reported technology usage and outcomes. Unobtrusive monitoring of online activities, sleep and physiology are now available through mobile applications and wireless sensors (for a review see George, Russell, & Odgers, in press). In addition, text-based mining algorithms and advancements in the “Big Data” space are opening up new opportunities to dive deeper into the content and meaning of online exchanges (see for example, text-based micro-coding methods by Underwood and colleagues, 2012, 2015). This is a rapidly advancing and diverse field in terms of research methods as well as usage patterns among adolescents. To provide more definitive answers to many of the concerns raised by parents and educators, enhanced methodological rigor and more innovative study designs will be required. A foundational question for future studies will be whether new technologies are introducing new risks or opportunities, or conversely, whether offline behaviors, preferences, and relationship features are simply mirrored within online spaces.

Future Directions

Many adults have expressed concern that the use of mobile devices and the seemingly constant connectivity among adolescents is impeding their development. Despite these fears, very few uniformly negative effects of new technologies on adolescent development have been documented. With some notable exceptions, many of the effects of new technologies on adolescents’ lives have been, or are expected to be, positive. There are also a number of exciting – and testable – ideas about the effects of the digital world on still developing adolescents. For example, some have suggested that high exposure to multi-tasking and digital media during this sensitive period could “rewire” the brain in ways that diminish concentration and hinder performance, while others cite the “adaptability” of the adolescent brain as allowing for optimization to the digital world (for a review see Giedd, 2012). In addition, mobile health platforms offer the tools for evaluating the mechanisms through which technology may influence adolescents’ development, as well new ways of fielding and evaluating interventions (George et al., in press). The medical field has already implemented mobile phone-based interventions that are effective in disease management and promoting healthy behaviors (for a review see: Cole-Lewis & Kershaw, 2010), and this could be an incredibly positive direction for psychological science (Mohr et al, 2013). Thus, even though our review is framed around seven fears related to new technology, our conclusions point to a number of promises of new technology for adolescent development and ends with a call for future research that can isolate how, for whom, and under what conditions new technologies are influencing the lives of adolescents.

Table 1.

Fear Fear Source Current Understanding Limitations Questions for Future Research
1. Parents worry about who their adolescents are interacting with online and what type of information they are sharing Parental surveys

Media reports
  • The majority of online interactions are with close, offline friends.

  • Texting content for younger adolescents is mostly positive or neutral.

  • A significant percentage of older adolescents are sharing sexually explicit materials.

  • Results based mainly on self-report and observational studies

  • Relatively few studies have focused on the content of online exchanges (for an exception see work by Underwood et al)

  • No large scale applications of social network analysis to date

  • How do adolescents’ offline and online social networks form and overlap over time?

  • What are adolescents’ motivations for sharing sexually explicit materials? Do motivations differ, or not, from offline exchanges?

2. Parents fear that their children will be victims of cyberbullying or be solicited by strangers online Parental surveys

Media reports

In-depth Parental interviews
  • Estimates of cyerbullying involvement typically fall between 11% to 48%.

  • There is a high degree of overlap between offline and online bullying.

  • Victims of cyberbullying are at risk for a wide range of offline problems and report negative feelings following incidents.

  • Programs that target traditional bullying have also been shown to reduce cyberbullying and victimization.

  • Estimates of cyberbullying involvement vary widely depending on the definition used, age and characteristics of the study members, and the reporting time frame

  • Nearly all research relies on self-reported involvement in cyberbullying.

  • What are the immediate and long-term effects of cyberbullying?

  • Is cyberbullying motivated or predicted by different factors than traditional bullying?

  • What can be done to increase the reporting of cyberbullying?

  • How does cyberbullying involvement vary across diverse subgroups of adolescents?

3. Adolescents’ constant connectivity prevents them from being present in ‘real life’ and interferes with offline socialization experiences and friendships Media reports

In-depth Case Studies
  • Research findings from the ‘internet’ era (1990s to early 2000s), when only a minority of teens were online and communicating via “chat rooms”, differs from current research focused on the vast majority of adolescents who are using mobile devices to communicate frequently with friends and family.

  • Adolescents who self-report more frequent online communication, also report higher offline friendship quality.

  • Longitudinal studies support a ‘rich-get-richer’ hypothesis, where children with strong relationships, engage in more online interactions, and in turn report higher friendship quality as adolescents.

  • Experimental research has shown that online interactions can help both adults and adolescents ‘bounce back’ following social exclusion.

  • The majority of evidence is based on self-reported technology use and relationship quality/skills.

  • Experimental and longitudinal research designs are needed in this area, as are other strategies that can facilitate causal inference.

  • Most research has adopted a ‘one-size fits all’ approach; however, results are not uniform; e.g., shy adolescents report going online to build skills, while greater use among already depressed adolescents appears to exacerbate symptoms. Effects are likely to vary widely depending on adolescents’ offline characteristics and contexts.

  • How do adolescents’ offline risks and characteristics moderate the potential positive and negative effects of online exchanges?

  • What skills are required and built during online exchanges? How do these skills differ from the development of traditional relationship and social skills?

  • Most online interactions take place with existing offline friends, but how do online only friendships form and develop? Do these friendships confer benefits, particularly among adolescents who may be isolated or otherwise vulnerable?

  • How do adolescents use online tools to recover or cope with offline stressors or events?

4. Mobile phones are creating a ‘digital divide’ between parents and adolescents Media reports

In-depth Parental interviews
  • Additional time spent online by adolescents often displaces in-person time with parents, but does not appear to reduce relationship quality.

  • New technologies may foster stronger ties between parents and children when separated (e.g., shared custody; college transition; children of migrant workers)

  • Parents are able to more closely monitor their children’s behavior using new technologies, but similar to offline monitoring, child initiated contact and disclosure is the key predictor what parents know about their child.

  • Nearly all of the research has been correlational and relied on self-reported measures of relationship quality

  • Very few studies have included both parents and adolescents views on the role of new technologies in enhancing or detracting from relationships,

  • Additional research with diverse populations of adolescents including, immigrant children, first-generation college students and others is needed.

  • Can new technologies enhance feelings of adolescent autonomy and parental knowledge?

  • How does virtual parental monitoring compare to in-person communication in predicting adolescent behavior and disclosure?

  • Many existing theories assume that adolescents will be more comfortable disclosing information about sensitive topics online; however, rigorous tests of this assumption are required.

5. Adolescents are experimenting with alternate identities online while leaving a digital archive of data that may damage their sense of self and future lives Parental surveys

Media reports
  • Adolescents are using a variety of different online forums (e.g., social media profiles, blogs, posts, videos) for self-expression.

  • Overall, online representations of self tend to closely mirror offline activities, interests and personalities.

  • Ethnographic research suggests adolescents use online interactions promote self-disclosure and a sense of belonging.

  • There is some evidence that online communities can provide LGBT youth with tools to explore their sexual identity.

  • Most research in this area has relied on case studies or highly selected samples of adolescents.

  • Adolescents uses of online spaces is evolving quickly, making it difficult for research to remain current

  • Large scale and representative studies on this topic are lacking, as are more in- depth studies with potentially vulnerable subgroups of adolescents.

  • What is the discrepancy, if any, between how adolescents present themselves in online versus offline contexts? How does this representation change over time?

  • What types of online tools are adolescents using (if any) to resolve identity crises?

  • Adolescents are accumulating a wealth of online data that has followed many of them since birth. The long long-term effects of this “digital dossier” on educational, employment and relationship prospects is largely unknown.

6. Constant multi-tasking on mobile devices is impairing adolescents’ cognitive performance Parental surveys

Media reports
  • The current generation of adolescents spends an unprecedented among of time consuming media and multi-tasking.

  • Media related multitasking among college students is associated with lower grades, less time studying, and more missed classes.

  • Experimental research with adults consistently documents negative effects of multi-tasking on cognitive performance.

  • The majority of research to date is with college students (correlational) or adults (some experimental)

  • Experimental research with “Generation M” adolescents is needed; such research should modify existing paradigms to account for the unique patterns of media use and potential distractions among youth (e.g., texting and driving)

  • What are the neurological effects of media multitasking? Are these effects different from other types of offline distracting activities?

  • How are mobile devices interfering with activities that require sustained attention, such as driving among adolescents?

  • Is adolescence a sensitive period for exposure to media-related multi-tasking? Does it “rewire” the adolescent brain?

7. Adolescents are losing sleep due to their devices Parental surveys

Media reports
  • The majority of US adolescent use or sleep with their mobile devices during bedtime hours, which is associated with reports of less and poorer quality sleep.

  • Media/texting content and light emission may also disrupt sleep.

  • Experimental research with adults shows disruption to the circadian clock, less sleep and reduced next morning alertness after reading from an iPad vs. book.

  • The majority of research with adolescents has relied on self-reported device usage and sleep quality.

  • The effects of light emission and emotionally arousing content of exchanges on sleep have been confounded in prior studies of adolescents.

  • How does the content of adolescents’ nightly texting influence sleep? Are these effects independent from disruptions linked to light emission?

  • Can the link between sleep disruptions and late-night tech use be clarified outside of the lab using ambulatory sleep monitoring devices and unobtrusive device usage measures?

Parental surveys included a representative phone-based survey of 802 parents of adolescents ages 12–17 in the United States in 2012 gathered by the Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project (e.g., Madden, Lenhart, Cortesi et al, 2013; Madden, Lenhart, Duggan et al, 2013) and a survey of over 1000 parents of 10 to 14 year old adolescents (boyd & Hargittai, 2013) as well as other smaller and more selective parent surveys. In-depth parental interviews/case studies refers to qualitative interviews with parents of 151 adolescents participating in our mobile-phone based research study (Russell et al, under review) as well as case studies provided by other authors (e.g., Turkle, 2012; boyd, 2013; Buckingham, 2008), respectively. Media reports reference how concerns related to adolescents and new technology (over)use was covered in major news outlets in the United States between 2011 and 2014.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funding from the National Institutions of Child Health and Human Development (HD061298), the Economic and Social Research Council (RES-062-1583), the William T. Grant Foundation (9837), and the Duke University Population Research Institute.

Footnotes

1

The Internet, social networking sites and mobile devices are not separate media. Adolescents often access multiple types of media or use them simultaneously, such as using a mobile phone with Internet access to post a message on Facebook (Rideout, 2011).

2

A number of additional fears were noted frequently in the popular press, but not included in this review due to insufficient empirical research on the topic, a lack of evidence that a sufficient number of parents were also concerned about the topic and/or space limitations, including: concerns that adolescents were becoming addicted to technology, fears that the reliance on truncated messaging and digital communication may be influencing communication and writing skills; and claims that mobile phones are physically damaging children’s brain.

3

The term ‘born digital’ is used by Palfrey & Gasser (2008) in their book “Born Digital: Understanding the First Generation of Digital Natives”. Digital natives is a term coined by Prenksy (2001) to mean individuals who were born after about 1980 and have grown up immersed in a culture of computers and mobile technologies as opposed to digital immigrants who’ve had to learn to use these devices later in life.

References

  1. Altmann EM, Trafton GJ, Hambrick DZ. Momentary interruptions can derail the train of thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2014;143(1):215–226. doi: 10.1037/a0030986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson CA, Shibuya A, Ihori N, Swing EL, Bushman BJ, Sakamoto A, Saleem M. Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136(2):151–173. doi: 10.1037/a0018251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bardi AC, Brady MF. Why shy people use instant messaging: Loneliness and other motives. Computers in Human Behavior. 2010;26(6):1722–1726. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blumenfeld WJ, Cooper RM. LGBT and allied youth responses to cyberbullying: Policy implications. International Journal of Critical Pedagogy. 2010;3(1):114–133. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bortree DS. Presentation of self on the Web: An ethnographic study of teenage girls’ weblogs. Education, Communication & Information. 2005;5(1):2005. doi: 10.1080/14636310500061102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boyd D. It’s complicated: The social lives of networked teens. New Haven, CT: Yal University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  7. boyd d, Hargittai E. Connected and concerned: Variation in parents’ online safety concerns. Policy & Internet. 2013;5(3):245–269. [Google Scholar]
  8. Buckingham D. Youth, identity, and digital media. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  9. Burns JM, Durkin LA, Nicholas J. Mental health of young people in the United States: what role can the internet play in reducing stigma and promoting help seeking? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;45(1):95–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Medicine. 2010;11(8):735–742. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Calamaro CJ, Mason TBA, Ratcliffe SJ. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):e1005–e1010. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Calvert SL, Jordan AB, Cocking RR. Children in the digital age: Influences of electronic media on development. Westport, CT: Praeger; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  13. Carskadon MA, Mindell JA, Drake C. 2006 Sleep in America Poll: Teens and sleep. Washington, D.C: National Sleep Foundation; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  14. Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep. 1993;16(3):258–262. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.3.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chang A-M, Aeschbach D, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014:201418490. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418490112. online early view. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chen YF, Katz JE. Extending family to social life: College students’ use of the mobile phone. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies. 2009;67:179–191. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2008.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cole-Lewis H, Kershaw T. Text messaging as a tool for behavior change in disease prevention and management. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2010;32(1):56–69. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxq004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Conley D. Wired for distraction: Kids in social media. TIME Magazine. 2011 Mar 19; Retrieved from http://content.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,2048363,00.html. [PubMed]
  19. Coyne SM, Padilla-Walker LM, Stockdale L, Day RD. Game on... girls: Associations between co-playing video games and adolescent behavioral and family outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2011;49(2):160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.11.249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dahl R, Lewin DS. Pathways to adolescent health: Sleep regulation and behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;31:175–184. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00506-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Davis K. Friendship 2.0: Adolescents’ experiences of belonging and self-disclosure online. Journal of Adolescence. 2012;35(6):1527–1536. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Davis K. Young people’s digital lives: The impact of interpersonal relationships and digital media use on adolescents’ sense of identity. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29(6):2281–2293. [Google Scholar]
  23. Davis K, James C. Tweens’ conceptions of privacy online: Implications for educators. Learning Media and Technology. 2013;38(1):4–25. doi: 10.1080/17439884.2012.658404. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Egli EA, Meyers LA. The role of video game playing in adolescent life: Is there reason to be concerned? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1984;22(4):309–312. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ehrenreich SE, Underwood MK, Ackerman RA. Adolescents’ text message communication and growth in antisocial behavior across the first year of high school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(2):251–264. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9783-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ellison NB, Steinfield C, Lampe C. The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2007;12(4):1143–1168. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Emsellem HA, Knutson KL, Hillygus DS, Buxton OM, Montgomery-Downs H, LeBourgeois MK, Spilsbury J. 2014 Sleep in America Poll: Sleep in the modern family. Arlington, VA: National Sleep Foundation; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  28. Englander EK. Bullying and Cyberbullying: What every educator needs to know. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  29. Englander F, Terregrossa RA, Wang Z. Internet use among college students: Tool or toy? Educational Review. 2010;62(1):85–96. doi: 10.1080/00131910903519793. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Erikson EH. Identity, youth, and crisis. 1. New York: Norton; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ferguson CJ, Kilburn J. Much ado about nothing: The misestimation and overinterpretation of violent video game effects in eastern and western nations: Comment on Anderson et al. (2010) Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136(2):174–178. doi: 10.1037/a0018566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Finn J. A survey of online harassment at a university campus. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2004;19(4):468–483. doi: 10.1177/0886260503262083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fleschler Peskin M, Markham CM, Addy RC, Shegog R, Thiel M, Tortolero SR. Prevalence and patterns of sexting among ethnic minority urban high school students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2013;16(6):454–459. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fowlkes J. Viewpoint: Why social media is destroying our social skills. USA TODAY. 2012 Oct 11; Retrieved from http://college.usatoday.com/2012/10/11/opinion-why-social-media-is-destroying-our-social-skills/
  35. Gardner H, Davis K. The App Generation: How today’s youth navigate identity, intimacy, and imaginiation in a digital world. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gardner M, Steinberg L. Peer influence on risk taking, risk perference, and risky descision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41(4):625–635. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. George MJ, Russell MA, Odgers CL. Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology. New York: Wiley-Blackwell; How mobile technologies can advance the study of psychopathology among children and adolescents. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  38. Giedd JN. The digital revolution and adolescent brain evolution. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2012;51(2):101–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gross EF. Adolescent Internet use: What we expect, what teens report. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2004;25:633–649. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2004.09.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gross EF. Logging on, bouncing back: An experimental investigation of online communication following social exclusion. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(6):1787–1793. doi: 10.1037/a0016541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Harper GW, Bruce D, Serrano P, Jamil O. The story of sexual identity: Narrative perspectives on the gay and lesbian life course. In: Hammack PL, Cohler BJ, editors. The story of sexual identity: Narrative perspectives on the gay and lesbian life course. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hartup WW. The company they keep: Friendships and their developmental significance. Child Development. 1996;67:1–13. 0009-3920/96/6701-0011$01.0. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hartup WW, Stevens N. Friendships and adaption in the life course. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:255–370. [Google Scholar]
  44. Heisler F. A comparison between those elementary school children who attend moving pictures, read comic books and listen to serial radio programs to an excess, with those who indulge in these activities seldom or not at all. The Journal of Educational Research. 1948;42(3):183–190. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hinduja S, Patchin JW. Bullying, cyberbullying, and suicide. Archives of Suicide Research. 2010;14(3):206–221. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2010.494133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hoffman J. As bullies go digital, parents struggle to catch up. The New York Times. 2010 Dec 4; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2010/12/05/us/05bully.html?pagewanted=all.
  47. Holson LM. Social media’s vampires: They text by night. The New York Times. 2014 Jul 3; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2014/07/06/fashion/vamping-teenagers-are-up-all-night-texting.html.
  48. Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics. 2003;111(2):302–307. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Jacobsen WC, Forste R. The wired generation: Academic and social outcomes of electronic media use among university students. Cyberpsychology Behavorial and Social Networking. 2011;14(5):275–280. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2010.0135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Junco R, Cotten SR. Perceived academic effects of Instant Messaging use. Computers & Education. 2011;56(2):370–378. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2010.08.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Juvonen J, Gross EF. Extending the school grounds? Bullying experiences in cyberspace. Journal of School Health. 2008;78(9):496–505. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2008.00335.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kerr M, Stattin H, Burk WJ. A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2010;20(1):39–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00623.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kirschner PA, Karpinski AC. Facebook and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior. 2010;26(6):1237–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kowalski RM, Giumetti GW, Schroeder AN, Lattanner MR. Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychological Bulletin. 2014 doi: 10.1037/a0035618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kowalski RM, Limber SP. Electronic bullying among middle school students. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2007;41:S22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kowalski RM, Limber SP. Psychological, physical, and academic correlates of cyberbullying and traditional bullying. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2014;53:S13–S20. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kraut R, Kiesler S, Boneva B, Cummings J, Helgeson V, Crawford A. Internet paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues. 2002;58(1):49–74. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kubey RW, Lavin LJ, Barrows JR. Internet use and collegiate academic performance decrements: Early findings. Journal of Communication. 2001:366–382. [Google Scholar]
  59. Larson R, Richards MH. Introduction: The changing life space of early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1989;18(6):501–509. doi: 10.1007/BF02139070. 0047-2.891/89/1200-0501506.00/0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lee SJ. Online communication and adolescent social ties: Who benefits more from Internet use? Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2009;14(3):509–531. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2009.01451.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lenhart A. Teens, Smartphones & Texting. Washington, D.C: The Pew Research Center Internet & American Life Project; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lenhart A, Ling R, Campbell S, Purcell K. Teens and Mobile Phones. Washington, D.C: University of Michigan Dept of Communication Studies; The Pew Research Center Internet & American Life Project; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lenhart A, Madden M. How teens manage their online identities and personal information in the age of MySpace. Washington, DC: Pew Internet and American Life Project; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  64. Longstaff HP. Preliminary results of a study of mothers’ opinions of children’s radio programs. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1936:416–419. [Google Scholar]
  65. Maccoby EE. Television: Its impact on school children. Public Opinion Quarterly, Fall. 1951;1951:421–444. [Google Scholar]
  66. Madden M, Cortesi S, Gasser U, Lenhart A, Duggan M. Parents, Teens, and Online Privacy. Washington, D.C: The Berkman Center for Internet & Society at Harvard University; The Pew Research Center Internet & American Life Project; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  67. Madden M, Lenhart A. Teens and Distracted Driving. Washington, D.C: The Pew Research Center Internet & American Life Project; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  68. Madden M, Lenhart A, Cortesi S, Gasser U, Duggan M, Smith A, Beaton M. Teens, Social Media, and Privacy. Washington, D.C: The Berkman Center for Internet & Society at Harvard University; The Pew Research Center Internet & American Life Project; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  69. Madden M, Lenhart A, Duggan M, Cortesi S, Gasser U. Teens and Technology 2013. Washington, D.C: The Berkman Center for Internet & Society at Harvard University; Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  70. Marcia JE. Development and validation of ego-identity status. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1966;3(5):551. doi: 10.1037/h0023281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Marwick AE, boyd d. I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: Twitter users, context collapse, and the imagined audience. New Media & Society. 2010;13(1):114–133. doi: 10.1177/1461444810365313. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Marwick AE, Ellison NB. “There isn’t Wifi in heaven!” Negotiating visibility on Facebook memorial pages. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 2012;56(3):378–400. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2012.705197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Masten AS, Coatsworth JD, Neemann J, Gest SD, Tellegen A, Garmezy N. The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through adolescence. Child development. 1995;66(6):1635–1659. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Mitchell KJ, Finkelhor D, Jones LM, Wolak J. Prevalence and characteristics of youth sexting: A national study. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):13–20. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Mitchell KJ, Jones LM, Finkelhor D, Wolak J. Understanding the decline in unwanted online sexual solicitations for US youth 2000–2010: Findings from three Youth Internet Safety Surveys. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2013;37(12):1225–1236. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Moreno MA, Jelenchick L, Koff R, Eikoff J, Diermyer C, Christakis DA. Internet use and multitasking among older adolescents: An experience sampling approach. Computers in Human Behavior. 2012;28(4):1097–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.01.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. Children’s friendship relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117(2):306–347. 0033-2909/95/$3.00. [Google Scholar]
  78. Olweus D. Annotation: Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a school based intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35(7):1171–1190. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01229.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Olweus D. Cyberbullying: An overrated phenomenon? European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2012;9(5):520–538. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2012.682358. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Ophir E, Nass C, Wagner AD. Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Psychological and Cognitive Sciences. 2009;106(37):15583–15587. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903620106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Ortega R, Elipe P, Mora-Merchan JA, Genta ML, Brighi A, Guarini A, Tippett N. The emotional impact of bullying and cyberbullying on victims: A European cross-national study. Aggressive Behavior. 2012;38:342–356. doi: 10.1002/ab.21440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Palfrey JG, Gasser U. Born digital: Understanding the first generation of digital natives. New York: Basic Books; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  83. Phinney JS. Stages of ethnic identity development in minority group adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 1989;9(1–2):34–49. [Google Scholar]
  84. Prenksy M. Digital natives, digital immigrants. On the Horizon. 2001;9(5):1–6. [Google Scholar]
  85. Raffaelli M, Duckett E. “We were just talking...”: Conversations in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1989;18:567–582. doi: 10.1007/BF02139074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Reich SM, Subrahmanyam K, Espinoza G. Friending, IMing, and hanging out face-to-face: Overlap in adolescents’ online and offline social networks. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48(2):356–368. doi: 10.1037/a0026980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Rice E, Rhoades H, Winetrobe H, Sanchez M, Montoya J, Plant A, Kordic T. Sexually explicit cell phone messaging associated with sexual risk among adolescents. Pediatrics. 2012;130(4):667–673. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Rideout VJ. Zero to eight children’s media use in America: Common Sense Media. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  89. Rideout VJ, Foehr UG, Roberts DF. Generation M: Media in the lives of 8 to 18 year olds. Menlo Park, CA: The Kaiser Family Foundation; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  90. Rogers RD, Monsell S. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1995;124(2):207–231. 0096-3445/95/$3.00. [Google Scholar]
  91. Russell MA, Odgers CL, Wang V. Witnessing substance use predicts same day antisocial behavior among adolescents: Evidence for a gene-environment interaction in daily life. (submitted) [Google Scholar]
  92. Salmivalli C, Karna A, Poskiparta E. Counteracting bullying in Finland: The KiVa program and its effects on different forms of being bullied. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2011:1–7. doi: 10.1177/0165025411407457. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Sanbonmatsu DM, Strayer DL, Medeiros-Ward N, Watson JM. Who multitasks and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. PLOS One. 2013;8(1):e54402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Selfout MHW, Branje SJT, Delsing M, ter Bogt TFM, Meeus WHJ. Different types of Internet use, depression, and social anxiety: The role of perceived friendship quality. Journal of Adolescence. 2009;32:819–833. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Shumow L, Schmidt JA, Kackar H. Adolescents’ experience doing homework: Associations among context, quality of experience, and outcomes. The School Community Journal. 2008;18(2):9–27. [Google Scholar]
  96. Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;53:83–112. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83. 0066-4308/01/0201-0083$14.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Strassberg DS, McKinnon RK, Sustaita MA, Rullo J. Sexting by high school students: An exploratory and descriptive study. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2013;42(1):15–21. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9969-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Subrahmanyam K, Smahel D. Digital Youth: The Role of Media in Development. New York: Springer; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  99. Temple JR, Paul JA, van den Berg P, Le VD, McElhany A, Temple BW. Teen sexting and its association with sexual behaviors. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2012;166(9):828–833. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Thrasher FM. The comics and delinquency: Cause or scapegoat. Journal of Educational Sociology. 1949;23(4):195–205. [Google Scholar]
  101. Turkle S. Life on the screen: Identity in the age of the Internet. New York: Simon & Schuster; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  102. Turkle S. Alone Together. New York: Basic Books; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  103. Underwood MK, Ehrenreich SE, More D, Solis JS, Brinkley DY. The BlackBerry Project: The hidden world of adolescents’ text messaging and relations with internalizing symptoms. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2015;25(1):101–117. doi: 10.1111/jora.12101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Underwood MK, Rosen LH, More D, Ehrenreich SE, Gentsch JK. The BlackBerry project: Capturing the content of adolescents’ text messaging. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48(2):295–302. doi: 10.1037/a0025914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Online communication and adolescent well-being: Testing the stimulation versus displacement hypothesis. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2007a;12:1169–1182. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00368.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Preadolescents’ and adolescents’ online communication and their closeness to friends. Developmental Psychology. 2007b;43(2):267–277. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Social consequences of the Internet for adolescents: A decade of research. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18(1):1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01595.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Valkenburg PM, Schouten AP, Peter J. Adolescents’ identity experiments on the Internet. New Media Society. 2005;7:383–402. doi: 10.1177/1461444805052282. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  109. Van den Bulck J. Television viewing, computer game paying, and Internet use and self-reported time to bed and time out of bed in secondary-school children. Sleep. 2004;27(1):101–104. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.1.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Van den Bulck J. Adolescent use of mobile phones for calling and for sending text messages after lights out: Results from a prospective cohort study with a one-year follow-up. Sleep. 2007;30(9):1220–1223. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.9.1220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. van Gell M, Vedder P, Tanilon J. Relationship between peer victizimation, cyberbullying and suicide in children and adolescents. JAMA Pediatrics. 2014;168(5):435–442. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.4143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Weaver E, Gradisar M, Dohnt H, Lovato N, Douglas P. The effect of presleep video-game playing on adolescent sleep. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2010;6(2):184–189. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Weisskirch RS. Parenting by cell phone: Parental monitoring of adolescents and family relations. Journal of Youth Adolescence. 2009;38(8):1123–1139. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9374-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Weisskirch RS. No crossed wires: Cell phone communication in parent-adolescent relationships. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking. 2011;14(7–8):447–451. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Williams AL, Merten MJ. A review of online social networking profiles by adolescents: Implications for future research and intervention. Adolescence. 2008;43(170):253. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Williams AL, Merten MJ. iFamily: Internet and social media technology in the family context. Family & Consumer Science Research Journal. 2011;40(2):150–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-3934.2011.02101.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Williford A, Boulton A, Noland B, Little TD, Karna A, Salmivalli C. Effects of KiVa antibullying program on adolescents’ depression, anxiety, and perception of peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40(2):289–300. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9551-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Willoughby T. A short-term longitudinal study of Internet and computer game use by adolescent boys and girls: Prevalence, frequency of use, and psychosocial predictors. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(1):195–204. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Wolak J, Finkelhor D, Mitchell K, Ybarra ML. Online “predators” and their victims: Myths, realities, and implications for prevention and treatment. American Psychologist. 2008;63(2):111–128. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Ybarra ML, Diener-West M, Leaf PJ. Examining the overlap in Internet harassment and school bullying: Implications for school intervention. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2007;41(6):S42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Ybarra ML, Mitchell KJ. Online aggressor/targets, aggressors, and targets: A comparison of associated youth characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(7):1308–1316. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00328.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES