Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2015 Nov 16;6:8877. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9877

Increased tolerance to humans among disturbed wildlife

Diogo S M Samia 1,a, Shinichi Nakagawa 2,3, Fausto Nomura 1, Thiago F Rangel 1, Daniel T Blumstein 4
PMCID: PMC4660219  PMID: 26568451

Abstract

Human disturbance drives the decline of many species, both directly and indirectly. Nonetheless, some species do particularly well around humans. One mechanism that may explain coexistence is the degree to which a species tolerates human disturbance. Here we provide a comprehensive meta-analysis of birds, mammals and lizards to investigate species tolerance of human disturbance and explore the drivers of this tolerance in birds. We find that, overall, disturbed populations of the three major taxa are more tolerant of human disturbance than less disturbed populations. The best predictors of the direction and magnitude of bird tolerance of human disturbance are the type of disturbed area (urbanized birds are more tolerant than rural or suburban populations) and body mass (large birds are more tolerant than small birds). By identifying specific features associated with tolerance, these results guide evidence-based conservation strategies to predict and manage the impacts of increasing human disturbance on birds.


Inline graphicSome animal species tolerate the presence of humans better than others. In a meta-analysis, Samia et al. find that populations of wildlife exposed to greater levels of human disturbance are more tolerant than undisturbed populations, with large birds in urbanized areas showing the highest levels of tolerance.


Animals often perceive humans as predators1. Thus, even non-consumptive human activities may affect their behaviour2,3 and generate population-level consequences4,5. For instance, declining populations of European and Australian birds are less tolerant of humans, as evidenced by them fleeing at greater distances from an approaching human (that is, flight initiation distance—FID), whereas populations from more tolerant species are increasing6,7. In addition, a synergic interaction between human tolerance and increased urbanization is expected8,9, resulting in an increased negative effect for less tolerant species. Hence, there is an urgent need to understand the factors that explain species tolerance of humans and to develop predictive models of tolerance10. One strategy to do so is to identify traits associated with species tolerance (that is, reduced responsiveness to people in areas with more people) or intolerance (that is, increased responsiveness to people in areas with more people) to human presence, which can be done by capitalizing on studies that compare the responses of animals in areas with varying levels of human presence.

We conducted a meta-analysis of 75 studies on birds (180 species), mammals (16 species) and lizards (16 species) to comprehensively evaluate species' tolerance of human disturbance. To address the potential non-independence among effect sizes, we incorporated study identity and species phylogeny as random factors. For each observation, we calculated the Hedges' g11, a measure of effect size, by comparing the mean FID of a population observed in areas of higher human disturbance with the mean FID of a population observed in areas of lower human disturbance. Negative effect sizes indicate tolerance of human disturbance, whereas positive effect sizes indicate intolerance of human disturbance. For the best-studied taxa—birds—we used a multi-model inference approach12 to identify the most important predictors of tolerance of human disturbance. Specifically, we tested for the effects of body mass, clutch size, habitat openness, group size, foraging habit, diet, migration behaviour and the type of habitats contrasted in populations under low and high human disturbance (for example, rural versus urban, natural versus urban; hereafter referred to as ‘habitat contrast') on bird's tolerance of human disturbance. Because it is well known that urbanization is a correlate of avian extinction13, we also conducted a separate meta-analysis focusing on bird populations from the rural–urban habitat contrast (the best studied contrast) to investigate if traits were similarly likely to explain differential behavioural responses13,14.

Here we find that, overall, disturbed populations of animals of the three major taxa are more tolerant of human disturbance than undisturbed (or less disturbed) populations. We also find that habitat contrast, body mass, clutch size and diet are the best predictors of direction and magnitude of tolerance of human disturbance. When we focused only on bird populations from the rural–urban habitat contrast, however, we find that body mass, diet and habitat openness are the best predictors of direction and magnitude of tolerance of these populations. Our results help to identify species that are potentially more vulnerable to increasing human disturbance and provide evidence-based guidance to future conservation actions.

Results

Overall results

In general, lizards, mammals and birds became equally more tolerant of human approaches with increasing human presence (Qb=0.09, df=2, P=0.96; Fig. 1 and Supplementary Data 1). Importantly, a regression between the mean and standard deviation of raw FID values showed that our effect size estimates genuinely reflect the magnitude of the mean FID differences of populations in areas of low versus high human disturbance (that is, they were not biased by any potential difference in variance of FIDs as a function of human disturbance level; Supplementary Fig. 1). Using I2, an index of heterogeneity among effect sizes15, we found substantial variation both in the meta-analysis of the three major taxa (I2total=91.44%, I2between-study=46.09%, I2phylogeny=10.15%, I2within-study(residuals)=35.20%) and in the birds-only meta-analysis (I2total=89.51%, I2study=36.87%, I2phylogeny=13.90%, I2residual=38.74%). This amount of heterogeneity justified our further exploration of the covariates. There was little evidence that our conclusions were affected by publication bias, where studies with low sample sizes are more prone to rejection because their higher probability of finding non-significant effects16 (Egger's regression; all taxa: intercept=−0.22, P=0.243; birds-only: intercept=−0.17, P=0.397; Supplementary Fig. 2). Using H2, an index for phylogenetic signal17, we found relatively small phylogenetic signal in the species tolerance of human disturbance (all taxa: H2=11.10%; birds-only: H2=15.53%). In fact, a separate set of analyses with meta-analytic models using studies and species as random factors, but not controlling for the phylogeny, yielded the same conclusions (Supplementary Figs 3–6, Supplementary Tables 1 and 2 and Supplementary Data 2 and 3).

Figure 1. Mean (±95% confidence interval) effect sizes (Hedges' g) by taxa which compare the difference between flight initiation distances of populations under high and low human disturbance.

Figure 1

Number of effect sizes and species (the latter in parenthesis) sampled by taxa is shown in the bottom of figure. Horizontal dashed line indicates zero effect size. Negative values illustrate tolerance of human disturbance.

Model selection using all bird species

The best predictors of bird response to human disturbance were habitat contrast, body mass, clutch size and diet (Table 1 and Supplementary Data 4). Magnitude and direction of birds responses varied according to the habitat contrast (Qb=20.34, df=8, P=0.009; Fig. 2). Birds in urbanized environments (that is, rural versus urban populations, and suburban versus urban populations) showed the greatest tolerance of humans (Table 1 and Fig. 2). Overall, the magnitude of the FID difference (that is, the effect sizes) of populations under low versus high human disturbance was lower, albeit marginally significant, in contrasts made ‘within' a same habitat type (for example, low versus high human disturbance in islands) than in contrasts made ‘between' habitat types (for example, rural versus urban areas; Qb=3.70, df=1, P=0.054). Larger birds were more tolerant of human disturbance than smaller birds (Table 1 and Fig. 2). Species that produced more eggs were less tolerant of human disturbance than those that produced fewer eggs (Table 1 and Fig. 2). Herbivorous and omnivorous species were more tolerant than carnivorous species (Table 1 and Fig. 2).

Table 1. Summary of the multi-model inference conducted to explain avian responses to human disturbance.

Predictor Levels Estimate s.e. Importance
All birds (180 species, 457 effect sizes)
 Intercept   0.736 0.599  
 Habitat contrast        
  Natural versus urban −0.941 0.632 1
  Rural versus suburban −1.438 0.762
  Rural versus urban −1.689 0.589
  Suburban versus urban −1.690 0.740
  Low versus high in urbanized areas −1.274 0.635
  Low versus high in recreational nature −0.684 0.593
  Low versus high in islands −0.894 0.717
  Low versus high in reserve −1.622 0.723
 Body mass   −0.315 0.075 0.98
 Clutch size   0.052 0.021 0.93
 Diet        
  Herbivorous −0.232 0.131 0.91
  Omnivorous −0.260 0.105
 Habitat openness   0.182 0.091 0.83
 Migration   0.127 0.080 0.70
 Group size        
  5–50 Individuals −0.060 0.088 0.51
  >100 Individuals 0.097 0.138
 Foraging habit   −0.007 0.109 0.41
         
Rural versus urban birds (103 species, 324 effect sizes)
 Intercept   −0.662 0.309  
 Body mass   −0.403 0.088 0.99
 Diet        
  Herbivorous −0.376 0.164 0.88
  Omnivorous −0.244 0.130
 Habitat openness   0.215 0.105 0.85
 Clutch size 0.035 0.025 0.64
 Migration   0.123 0.095 0.62
 Group size      
  5–50 Individuals −0.100 0.108 0.56
  >100 Individuals 0.091 0.176
 Foraging habit   0.079 0.130 0.46

Results are shown from both a meta-analysis using the full data set (all birds) and a meta-analysis focusing on the contrast between rural and urban populations. Values are average coefficients of models (estimate), their associated standard error (s.e.), and the importance of each factor in explaining species responses to human disturbance (the closer than 1, the most important the factor). Habitat contrasts presented as ‘low versus high in' refer to contrast between populations experiencing low and high human disturbance within a given habitat type.

Figure 2. Effects of eight predictors on the effect sizes (Hedges' g) of bird's tolerance of human disturbance using the full data set (180 species, 457 effect sizes).

Figure 2

Negative effect sizes show that species tolerate human disturbance, whereas positive effect sizes show that species are intolerant of human disturbance. Predictors are presented in their order of importance in explaining bird's tolerance (or intolerance) to human disturbance. (a) Type of habitats contrasted (contrasts are presented as habitat with ‘low versus high' human disturbance). Habitat contrasts presented as ‘low versus high in' refer to contrast between populations experiencing low and high human disturbance within a given habitat type. (b) Body mass (g). (c) Mean clutch size per reproductive period. (d) Diet. (e) Habitat openness. (f) Migration. (g) Group size. (h) Foraging habit. Horizontal dashed line indicates zero effect size. Different sizes of symbols in b and c reflect differences in sample size. The error bars illustrate 95% confidence intervals. The number of effect sizes in each categorical level is shown in the bottom of figures.

Model selection using birds from the rural–urban contrast

When we focused only on bird populations from the rural–urban habitat contrast, we found a slightly different rank order of importance of the best predictors (Table 1 and Supplementary Data 5). Once again, body mass was the most important species trait that explained variation in tolerance of human disturbance. However, both the larger regression coefficient and greater importance in relation to the other traits suggest that the effect of body mass on tolerance was even greater in the rural–urban habitat contrast (Fig. 3 and Table 1). Diet was the second most important trait explaining species tolerance in the rural–urban habitat contrast (Fig. 3 and Table 1). Species originally from open habitats were more tolerant than those from closed habitats (Fig. 3 and Table 1). Clutch size was substantially less important in explaining species tolerance in rural–urban habitat contrast (Fig. 3 and Table 1); an effect not explained by potential reductions in clutch size variability in urban species because of reduction in their body sizes (Supplementary Fig. 7). Similar to the analysis using the full avian data set, migration, group size and foraging habit were relatively less important in explaining variation in tolerance of human disturbance in the rural–urban habitat contrast (Fig. 3 and Table 1).

Figure 3. Effects of seven predictors on the effect sizes (Hedges g) of bird s tolerance of human disturbance from populations of rural-urban habitat contrast (103 species, 324 effect sizes).

Figure 3

Negative effect sizes show that species tolerate human disturbance, whereas positive effect sizes show that species are intolerant of human disturbance. Predictors are presented in their order of importance in explaining bird's response to human disturbance. (a) Body mass (g). (b) Diet. (c) Habitat openness. (d) Mean clutch size per reproductive period. (e) Migration. (f) Group size. (g) Foraging habit. Horizontal dashed line indicates zero effect size. Different sizes of symbols in a and d reflect differences in sample size. The error bars illustrate 95% confidence intervals. The number of effect sizes in each categorical level is shown in the bottom of figures.

Discussion

We found the habitat type contrasted in populations under low and high human disturbance (that is, the habitat contrasts) was one of the main drivers of the degree to which birds tolerate human disturbance. Rural versus urban and suburban versus urban populations had the largest effect sizes among the habitat contrasts, indicating that urbanized birds are those with the highest degree of tolerance of human disturbance. A previous study showed that time since urbanization was positively associated with the degree of tolerance in birds18, which could help to explain further variation in human tolerance within urban places. However, the lack of additional data18 prevents further tests of the effect of time since urbanization as a predictor of tolerance of human disturbance. Although sample sizes were admittedly smaller for the other habitat contrasts, the effect sizes of all contrasts (except inside versus outside reserves) were in the same direction, suggesting that different types of human exposure act generally increasing tolerance of populations. It is essential to realize that some species might be absent from certain habitat comparison because they are unable to survive in habitats with high human presence (for example, in urban areas) because of their low tolerance for human activity. Therefore, a more detailed examination of variation in human tolerance among habitats types would require more sampling in most habitats, a historical record of human or bird invasions18, and a systematic measurement of human presence (as conducted by few studies of our data set19,20). Nevertheless, our finding that FID differences were lower in contrasts within habitat types than between habitat types seems to support previous studies that showed a positive relationship between human exposure and degree of human tolerance20,21,22.

Based on a robust body of evidence23,24,25,26,27, we expected body mass to be one of the most important traits affecting how species respond to increased human disturbance. However, we did not initially expect the observed direction of the response (see Covariates in Methods). It is well established that large animals are more intolerants to human presence than small animals (that is, they have larger FID23,24,25,26,27), but, surprisingly, we found that large birds were those that had the greatest reduction in FID as human disturbance increased. Optimal escape theory states that animals must counterbalance both costs and benefits when making escape decisions27,28. Larger animals may suffer higher costs of flight either because they are less agile because of their size29,30 or because their energetic costs of not foraging are greater (opportunity cost26,31,32). Either way, we suggest that larger birds (and potentially, larger species of other taxa too) might be under intense pressure to be more tolerant of non-lethal human approaches. This finding may indicate that energetic costs of unnecessary escape are important variables driving tolerance.

There are at least two other reasons why body size may be associated with increased tolerance of humans in birds. First, larger birds may also be less likely to be killed by predators because of their body size33 and this reduced risk may select for increased tolerance of non-threatening humans in areas where humans are commonly encountered. Second, larger birds with relatively larger brains may have greater cognitive abilities and might be able to better assess risk34.

At a proximate level, our finding that carnivorous birds are less tolerant of humans may reflect a carry-over effect from their increased sensitivity to movement and thus their general degree of responsiveness23, or it may reflect the reduced foraging efficiency of carnivorous birds foraging around humans if their prey are similarly disturbed by humans. Carnivorous birds may also be persecuted because they compete with humans for prey, and, if so, may have been selected to be less tolerant of humans. Regardless of the mechanism, the evidence accumulated to date, and summarized in our meta-analysis, suggests that carnivorous birds are likely to be particularly vulnerable to human disturbance.

Animals in open habitats should be better able to monitor predator behaviour once they are detected, and to track risk dynamically34. Based on the ability to better track predation risk, we expect that animals in open habitats to be better at learning not fear encounters with non-threating humans than animals living in visually closed environments where sightings are either brief or interrupted34. Human-altered habitats, such as urban places, are often characterized by having reduced vegetative cover. Species naturally living in open habitats might suffer less in urbanized habitats once vegetation cover is reduced in cities, and consequently they might better tolerate humans. This may explain why habitat use (specifically, regarding to habitat openness) was a trait particularly important in explaining variation in tolerance in urbanized birds. As an extension of this reasoning, it is also probable that nature-based wildlife tourism may be less harmful to animals in relatively open habitats because humans can be monitored and animals would only escape when they assess a high risk of predation (but see ref. 35). Future studies are necessary to confirm this hypothesis.

Our meta-analysis shows that bird species that produce smaller clutches are more tolerant of human disturbance than those that produce larger clutches. Life history theory predicts that species that invest more in their offspring should be more likely to protect that investment36,37 (but see ref. 38). If we assume that small clutch-sized species generally have larger parental investment per offspring, then adults should accept more risk to guarantee their reproductive success. Abandoning a profitable patch, in addition to the energetic cost of flight, should thus be particularly costly to these species. Therefore, there should be a pressure for small clutch-sized birds to become more tolerant of non-threatening encounters. This hypothesis is consistent with the energetic hypothesis proposed by optimal escape theory27,28.

At least three mechanisms can explain variation in tolerance as a function of human disturbance. First, recent studies have suggested that differential selection among personality types could account for increased tolerance of humans in some species, whereby bold individuals settle in areas with higher human disturbance and shy individuals settle in areas with reduced disturbance14,39. Second, increased tolerance could result from local adaptation40. Third, increased tolerance could result from habituation41,42. We suggest that habituation-like processes are likely to be common and important because we know that individuals are exquisitely sensitive to unexpected behaviour from humans and potential predators27. Indeed, we expect location-specific habituation where even a slight deviation in a predator's (or human's) routine behaviour can re-elicit fearful responses43 (for example, when predator appears on a different side of lake44 or a human steps off a well-travelled hiking trail or crosses a fence45). Such a pattern is entirely consistent with habituation.

Discriminating between the three competing mechanistic hypotheses to explain tolerance of human disturbance would require a set of long-term studies on marked individuals (for example, because habituation is a learning process that occurs in individuals over time42), in addition to population-level genetic information to investigate selection and adaptation. These data are currently unavailable for the vast majority of the species studied. However, our results show that regardless of the mechanism leading to increasing tolerance of human disturbance, its magnitude and direction, at least in birds, is influenced by a species' traits and environmental differences. The present study thus helps address an urgent need to identify species potentially more vulnerable to the threats of human expansion10,46.

The results of our systematic review and meta-analysis have implications for wildlife conservation. For example, our findings suggest that small birds may benefit more from protection that reduces human disturbance than large birds, specifically because they are less likely to become tolerant of human disturbance. In practical terms, it is possible that set-back distance zones established within protected areas10 are more effective for small bird species. In addition, and contra previous suggestions based only on the relationship between body size and FID23, our findings suggest that ecotourism may not be as deleterious for larger birds (and perhaps larger animals) because they are better able to tolerate human disturbance and consequently may suffer smaller population-level consequences2,4,5.

Despite the conservation implications discussed above, at least two points related to our results require caution. First, large birds might become more vulnerable to illegal hunting if they become tolerant of humans as a function of exposure47. Thus, exposure to benign human presence (for example, ecotourists) could create an ecological trap if tolerant birds later encounter hunters. Therefore, wildlife managers interested in using our results to design a management programme should also consider actions aimed to curb poaching.

Second, the bird traits correlated with greater tolerance of human disturbance are the same found among many endangered species (that is, endangered species are typically large body sized and have low reproductive rates46,48,49). This apparently contradictory finding can be explained by considering the source of human disturbance. Large animals with low reproductive rates are in decline mainly because of habitat loss, overexploitation by humans and by climate change49,50. Large animals require larger home ranges, have reduced recruitment and are often hunted by humans for food and recreation46,48,49. Our results focus on behavioural responses to non-lethal human disturbance (that is, tolerance), which may influence a species' ability to live successfully with humans2,4,5,6,7,42. Therefore, although our findings may be used to predict bird species potentially most vulnerable to human disturbance, and to manage the impacts of increasing human presence on wildlife, other conservation actions must be adopted to decelerate the alarmingly rapid loss of biodiversity51.

The present study represents the first effort to develop an evidence-based model of wildlife tolerance of human disturbance. We hope the results of our meta-analysis serve as a starting point to predict and manage the impacts of increasing human disturbance on wildlife and guide conservation strategies. Additional studies must be conducted to understand the mechanisms that promote tolerance of human disturbance, because this mechanistic understanding should help us design more effective management strategies10,52. Also, studies are necessary to determine visitation rates that maximise the development of tolerance of humans in wildlife. Ultimately, it is evidence-based actions that will help reduce biodiversity loss in a rapidly urbanizing world.

Methods

Literature search and selection criteria

We first compiled all studies that cited or were cited by two key reviews28,31. We searched for studies published before 31 July 2014 on Web of Knowledge, Scopus and Google Scholar databases with the key words ‘flight initiation distance', ‘flight distance', ‘escape distance', ‘approach distance', ‘flushing distance' and ‘response distance'. The references cited in these studies were also examined. Our criteria to include a study were that FID of a given species (measured sensu27) had to be collected in areas of low and high human presence (=human disturbance). We followed the criteria used by authors to categorize each area as a function of its degree of disturbance. Hunted populations were not included in our data set. A PRISMA diagram describing our literature search and the detailed reasons for exclusion of studies are available in Supplementary Fig. 8 and Supplementary Table 3, respectively. Our final data set consisted of 504 effect sizes from 75 studies across 212 species distributed in three major taxa: birds, mammals and lizards (data available in Supplementary Data 1).

Estimating effect sizes

To quantify species response to disturbance, we used the effect size metric Hedges' g11; a bias-corrected measure of standardized mean differences, which does not overestimate the magnitude of effect when sample size is small11. For each species, we compared the mean FID of populations in areas of higher human disturbance with those in areas of lower human disturbance. These FID comparisons were restricted to populations of species studied in a same study. In our data set (Supplementary Data 1), positive effect sizes indicate sensitization, whereas negative values indicate tolerance of human disturbance. When mean, variance and sample size of FIDs were not provided in a paper, we estimated Hedges'g from the statistical results (t, F, χ2, Z and P)53. We directly contacted several authors for missing data (see Acknowledgements for details). Importantly, recent studies have shown that the starting distance of an approaching person (that is, animal–human distance when the approach begins) can affect FID27. However, the potential effects of starting distance on FID were controlled in most effect sizes either because studies used a fixed starting distance among experiments (for example, starting distance fixed in 30 m) or because starting distance was controlled analytically by using it as a covariate in statistical models, or because we used the marginal means for those studies that provided them or those studies in which we were able to obtain the raw data (see Supplementary Data 1 for details).

Meta-analysis

We used multi-level mixed-effects meta-analysis17 to test for both mean effect sizes and the importance of our predictors. We controlled for phylogenetic and study non-independence by including phylogeny (Supplementary Fig. 9) and study identity as random-factors in our models17. Although we also have multiple estimates per species in our data set (Supplementary Data 1), a model selection approach showed that the inclusion of ‘species identity' as an additional random-effect did not improve our models (Supplementary Table 4). Phylogenies of birds, mammals and lizards and how they were combined to test for difference among these taxa is described in Supplementary Methods 1. The mean effect sizes (that is, mean of the effect sizes weighted by the inverse of their variance) were considered significant if their 95% confidence intervals did not include zero53. We used the between-groups heterogeneity statistic (Qb) to test for significant difference between mean effect sizes53,54.

We used I2 index as a measure of heterogeneity in the effect sizes in which the value represents the proportion of total variation in data that is not sampling error (0%—all sampling error; 100%—no sampling error)15. We used an extended version of I2 that partitions the total heterogeneity among different sources: variation explained by study identity, by phylogenetic effect and by the residual variation (that is, that remaining to be explained by the predictor variables17). We calculated the degree of phylogenetic signal in our effect size estimates using the phylogenetic heritability index17, H2, which is the variance attributable to phylogeny in relation to the total variance expected in the data. When the unit of analysis is species, H2 is equivalent to Pagel's λ (ref. 55), in which higher values are associated with stronger phylogenetic signals. Primary studies can suffer from publication bias, where studies with low sample size are more prone to be rejected because of their higher probability of not finding significant effects16,53. We checked for publication bias using Egger's regression16, in which intercepts significantly different from zero suggest potential publication bias. To overcome the non-independent nature of our data, we applied the Egger's regression test on the meta-analytic residuals17. Analyses were conducted using the metafor54 R package v.1.9-4.

Covariates

The large number of observations in birds permitted us to investigate whether certain variables were potentially important predictors of tolerance of human disturbance. Based on previous findings in the literature and our own hypotheses, we collected information on eight variables. Seven were associated with a species' morphology, life-history and natural history traits. These data were obtained from Del Royo et al.56 (see Supplementary Data 1 for details). The eighth variable, the habitat contrast, describe the type of habitats contrasted in populations under low and high human disturbance. These data were extracted from each surveyed paper. Importantly, for these covariates, multi-collinearity was not an issue (variance inflation factor<1.50, below the suggested threshold57 of 3; see also correlation matrix in Supplementary Table 5). Below we justify the use of each variable and our predictions concerning their effect on bird response.

Body mass. A substantial amount of empirical evidence shows that large animals are less tolerant of human approaches—they generally flush at a greater distance from humans23,24,25,26,27. There are two non-exclusive main hypotheses to explain this response: (i) large animals are generally less maneuverable29,30 or (ii) large animals suffer a greater opportunity cost of not foraging because of their greater absolute metabolic needs27,32. Thus, we hypothesized that large animals would tolerate less human disturbance by showing larger positive effect sizes than small animals. Body mass was measured in grams and log10 transformed before analysis.

Group size. Three models of predation risk assessment predict a declining risk of predation as group size increases58,59,60. If predation risk decreases with group size, individuals in larger groups might thus tolerate closer approach. Moreover, if tolerance of non-lethal human disturbance is a socially transmitted behaviour, one could expect the effect of social transmission to be enhanced in larger groups61. Therefore, group size should be expected to influence tolerance of human disturbance by increasing the tolerance as group size increases. Following Burish et al.62, we coded species into three categories: alone or in pairs, in groups of 5–50 individuals, in groups of >100 individuals.

Habitat openness. Animals in open habitats can simultaneously detect predators at a greater distance and might have to travel a longer distance to reach protective cover. A recent study showed that birds originally from open habitats tend to delay the flight when compared with birds from closed habitats34. Moreover, highly altered habitats, such as urban and suburban places, usually have reduced vegetative cover. We thus hypothesized that species naturally living in open habitats would better tolerate humans because they would suffer less as vegetation cover was reduced. Either way, habitat openness might influence FID and must be accounted for if we are to isolate human disturbance effects. We categorized species as being originally from open habitats (for example, uplands and grassland) or closed habitats (for example, dense forests and woodlands).

Foraging habit. Prior work has shown that in areas of human disturbance, birds may place their nests higher in trees63,64 suggesting that being in trees affords enhanced safety. However, a previous study looking at the effects of height in a tree on FID found either no effects or found that birds that were higher in trees initiated flight at greater distances65. Our prediction is that species that typically forage on the ground would be under greater pressure to tolerate humans to minimize the opportunity cost of resuming foraging after a potentially unwarranted escape. We thus classified species as typically foraging on the ground versus species typically foraging above of ground level (for example, in trees or catching aerial insects while flying).

Diet. Birds that eat live prey, particularly carnivorous raptors, have especially good visual acuity and motion sensitivity66. Given this sensitivity, birds that eat live prey are disturbed at greater distances23. This increased sensitivity to disturbance could select for tolerance if they are better to learn that humans are not a threat. Alternatively, it could also reflect the possibility that foraging efficiency is reduced because their prey are similarly disturbed by humans. In the latter case, we might expect that these species would be less likely to tolerate the on-going disturbance. We categorized species as carnivorous, herbivorous or omnivorous.

Migration. Birds that migrate are exposed to a greater variety of habitats and might be selected to rapidly learn to assess predation risk in new habitats. Under this scenario, we expected that these species would be more likely to tolerate increased disturbance. We coded species as resident or migratory.

Clutch size. Like body size, clutch size is a life history trait that reflects energetic investment, and therefore need37. In general, we might expect that birds that produce fewer eggs per reproductive period might be more energetically stressed than those that produce more eggs because they have larger parental investment per offspring36. In this case, small clutch-sized species would tolerate closer approach because of the greater opportunity costs associated with flight27,28. Alternatively, enhanced energetic needs might select for small clutch-sized species to not tolerate the on-going monitoring costs associated with disturbance67 and may thus select them to move off and forage in areas without disturbance, resulting in a lower tolerance of these species. Either way, clutch size must be accounted for to isolate the effects of human disturbance on flight. We used the estimates of the number of eggs per reproductive period. Because there was a low correlation between clutch size and body mass (Supplementary Table 5), clutch size effects were not corrected by species body mass in our analyses.

Habitat contrast. The nine habitat contrasts were: (i) natural versus urban area, (ii) rural versus suburban areas, (iii) rural versus urban area, (iv) suburban versus urban area, (v) inside versus outside reserve, (vi) low versus high human disturbance in urbanized areas, (vii) low versus high human disturbance in recreational nature (for example, beaches, ski areas and other tourist locations), (viii) low versus high human disturbance in islands and (ix) low versus high human disturbance in reserve. We used these habitat contrasts either because of their difference in human disturbance degree or because a particular characteristic of habitat is expected to influence animal's tolerance of humans. Specifically, contrasts between natural, rural, suburban and urban populations were tested because they represented increasing human presence and thus a differential human tolerance is expected as a function of frequency of exposure to humans20,21,22. Previous studies have shown that even subtle temporal or spatial change in human disturbance within a given habitat type may triggers changes in animal's tolerance of humans27,41,42,43,44,45, justifying our exploration of the contrasts between low versus high human disturbance within a same habitat type (levels vi–ix). Overall, we expect a lower FID difference (that is, tolerance) in comparisons within than between habitat types. Recreational areas, islands and protected areas (reserves) were tested separately because their marked difference in the pattern of human disturbance. Because island populations often have reduced predation risk compared with mainland populations68, we expected either none or a small difference in FIDs of different populations found on islands. Animals living in natural areas with tourism may be more responsive to humans because they commonly experience seasonality in human disturbance (for example, visitation only in summer or winter). Beyond temporal variation, populations inside and outside protected areas may suffer marked spatial variation in human disturbance, as well as potential lethality associated with human presence (for example, individuals in protected area may feel safer). This spatial variation in human tolerance may also occur in populations ‘within' protected areas if the frequency of human visitation varies in the protected area. Importantly, our habitat contrasts were restricted to comparisons between populations of species tested in the same study. As explained in the Meta-analysis section, the variation among studies was controlled for by using study identity as a random factor in our models.

Multi-model inference

We used a multi-model inference approach based on Akaike's criteria corrected for small sample size to estimate the relative importance of the predictor variables12. To calculate the importance of each predictor, we first assessed the relative strengths of each candidate model by calculating its Akaike weight; analogous to the probability of that model is the best model. A constant term (intercept) was included in all models. In sequence, we estimated the importance of a predictor by summing the Akaike weights of all models in which that candidate variable appeared, which can be interpreted as the probability that a particular predictor is a component of the best model, which allowed us to rank predictors in order of importance12. We used a model averaging approach to estimate model parameters12. Multi-model analyses were conducted using the MuMIn69 R package v. 1.14.0.

Additional information

How to cite this article: Samia, D. S. M. et al. Increased tolerance to humans among disturbed wildlife. Nat. Commun. 6:8877 doi: 10.1038/ncomms9877 (2015).

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figures 1-9, Supplementary Tables 1-5, Supplementary Methods and Supplementary References

ncomms9877-s1.pdf (965.2KB, pdf)
Supplementary Data 1

Complete data set

ncomms9877-s2.xlsx (78.2KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 2

Model selection of all birds: phylogenetic effect not controlled

ncomms9877-s3.xlsx (44.3KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 3

Model selection of rural-urban birds: phylogenetic effect not controlled

ncomms9877-s4.xlsx (26.5KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 4

Model selection of all birds: phylogenetic effect controlled

ncomms9877-s5.xlsx (39.2KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 5

Model selection of rural-urban birds: phylogenetic effect controlled

ncomms9877-s6.xlsx (24.1KB, xlsx)

Acknowledgments

D.S.M.S. is grateful for support from CAPES, S.N. is supported by the Rutherford Discovery Fellowship (New Zealand) and by the Future Fellowship (Australia). F.N. thanks CNPq and FAPEG for support, T.F.R. is supported by CAPES and CNPq grants, and D.T.B. thanks the NSF for support. We thank Bill Cooper, Christoph Randler, Hayley Glover, James Boudreau, Jeremy Hyman, José Tella, Martina Carrete, Michael Weston, Penn Lloyd and Zhongqui Li for providing unpublished or missing data. We also thank Diogo Provete and Losia Lagisz for help with phylogenies, Marcus Cianciaruso and Fernando Sobral for sharing some data, and Kamil Barton for kindly modifying his R function to conduct multi-model inference using meta-regression models.

Footnotes

Author contributions D.S.M.S. and D.T.B. conceived the ideas for the paper and its structure; D.S.M.S. compiled published data and conducted meta-analysis with technical help from S.N.; D.S.M.S., S.N., F.N., T.F.R. and D.T.B. discussed results; D.S.M.S. and D.T.B. wrote the first draft of the manuscript; D.S.M.S., S.N., F.N., T.F.R. and D.T.B. edited the manuscript.

References

  1. Frid A. & Dill L. Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conserv. Ecol. 6, 1–11 (2002). [Google Scholar]
  2. Preisser E. L., Bolnick D. I. & Bernard M. F. Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86, 501–509 (2005). [Google Scholar]
  3. Peckarsky B. L. et al. Revisiting the classics: considering nonconsumptive effects in textbook examples of predator-prey interactions. Ecology 89, 2416–2425 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ellenberg U., Mattern T., Seddon P. J. & Jorquera G. L. Physiological and reproductive consequences of human disturbance in Humboldt penguins: the need for species-specific visitor management. Biol. Conserv. 133, 95–106 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  5. Baudains T. & Lloyd P. Habituation and habitat changes can moderate the impacts of human disturbance on shorebird breeding performance. Anim. Conserv. 10, 400–407 (2007). [Google Scholar]
  6. Møller A. P. Flight distance and population trends in European breeding birds. Behav. Ecol. 19, 1095–1102 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  7. Møller A. P., Samia D. S. M., Weston M. A., Guay P.-J. & Blumstein D. T. American exceptionalism: population trends and flight initiation distances in birds from three continents. PLoS ONE 9, e107883 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Shochat E., Warren P. S., Faeth S. H., McIntyre N. E. & Hope D. From patterns to emerging processes in mechanistic urban ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 186–191 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Markovchick-Nicholls L. et al. Relationships between human disturbance and wildlife land use in urban habitat fragments. Conserv. Biol. 22, 99–109 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blumstein D. T. & Fernández-Juricic E. A Primer of Conservation Behavior Sinauer (2010). [Google Scholar]
  11. Hedges L. V. Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J. Educ. Stat. 6, 107–128 (1981). [Google Scholar]
  12. Burnham K. P. & Anderson D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: a Practical Information-Theoretic Approach Springer (2002). [Google Scholar]
  13. McKinney M. L. Urbanization, biodiversity, and conservation. Bioscience 52, 883–890 (2002). [Google Scholar]
  14. Miranda A. C., Schielzeth H., Sonntag T. & Partecke J. Urbanization and its effects on personality traits: a result of microevolution or phenotypic plasticity? Glob. Chang. Biol. 19, 2634–2644 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Higgins J. P. T., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J. & Altman D. G. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Egger M., Smith G. D., Schneider M. & Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634 (1997). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Nakagawa S. & Santos E. S. A. Methodological issues and advances in biological meta-analysis. Evol. Ecol. 26, 1253–1274 (2012). [Google Scholar]
  18. Møller A. P. Interspecific variation in fear responses predicts urbanization in birds. Behav. Ecol. 21, 365–371 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  19. Li C., Monclús R., Maul T. L., Jiang Z. & Blumstein D. T. Quantifying human disturbance on antipredator behavior and flush initiation distance in yellow-bellied marmots. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 129, 146–152 (2011). [Google Scholar]
  20. Levey D. J. et al. Urban mockingbirds quickly learn to identify individual humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 8959–8962 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Engelhardt S. C. & Weladji R. B. Effects of levels of human exposure on flight initiation distance and distance to refuge in foraging eastern gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis). Can. J. Zool. 89, 823–830 (2011). [Google Scholar]
  22. McGiffin A., Lill A., Beckman J. & Johnstone C. P. Tolerance of human approaches by Common Mynas along an urban-rural gradient. Emu 113, 154–160 (2013). [Google Scholar]
  23. Blumstein D. T. Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Anim. Behav. 71, 389–399 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  24. Fernández-Juricic E. et al. Relationships of anti-predator escape and post-escape responses with body mass and morphology: a comparative avian study. Evol. Ecol. Res. 8, 731–752 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  25. Gotanda K. M., Turgeon K. & Kramer D. L. Body size and reserve protection affect flight initiation distance in parrotfishes. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 1563–1572 (2009). [Google Scholar]
  26. Samia D. S. M., Blumstein D. T., Stankowich T. & Cooper W. E. Jr Fifty years of chasing lizards: new insights advance optimal escape theory. Biol. Rev. doi: 10.1111/brv.12173 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cooper W. E. Jr & Blumstein D. T. (eds) Escaping from Predators: an Integrative View of Escape Decisions Cambridge Univ. (2015). [Google Scholar]
  28. Ydenberg R. C. & Dill L. M. The economics of fleeing from predators. Adv. Study Behav. 16, 229–249 (1986). [Google Scholar]
  29. Norberg U. M. Vertebrate Flight: Mechanics, Physiology, Morphology, Ecology and Evolution Springer Science & Business Media (2012). [Google Scholar]
  30. Witter M. S., Cuthill I. C. & Bonser R. H. C. Experimental investigations of mass-dependent predation risk in the European starling. Sturnus vulgaris. Anim. Behav. 48, 201–222 (1994). [Google Scholar]
  31. Stankowich T. & Blumstein D. T. Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 2627–2634 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Bennett P. M. & Harvey P. H. Active and resting metabolic rate in birds: allometry, phylogeny and ecology. J. Zool. 213, 327–344 (1987). [Google Scholar]
  33. Werner E. E., Gilliam J. F., Hall D. J. & Mittelbach G. G. An experimental test of the effects of predation risk on habitat use in fish. Ecology 64, 1540–1548 (1983). [Google Scholar]
  34. Samia D. S. M., Møller A. P. & Blumstein D. T. Brain size as a driver of avian escape strategy. Sci. Rep. 5, 11913 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fernández-Juricic E., Venier M. P., Renison D. & Blumstein D. T. Sensitivity of wildlife to spatial patterns of recreationist behavior: a critical assessment of minimum approaching distances and buffer areas for grassland birds. Biol. Conserv. 125, 225–235 (2005). [Google Scholar]
  36. Trivers R. L. in Sexual Selection & the Descent of Man ed. Campbell B. 136–179Aldine Publishing Company (1972). [Google Scholar]
  37. Yampolsky L. Y. Life history theory. Encycl. Life Sci. 1–6 doi: 10.1038/npg.els.0003219. (2002). [Google Scholar]
  38. Ghalambor C. K. & Martin T. E. Fecundity-survival trade-offs and parental risk-taking in birds. Science 292, 494–497 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Carrete M. & Tella J. L. High individual consistency in fear of humans throughout the adult lifespan of rural and urban burrowing owls. Sci. Rep. 3, 3524 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Møller A. P. Flight distance of urban birds, predation, and selection for urban life. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 63–75 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  41. Blumstein D. T. in Avian Urban Ecology: Behavioural and Physiological Adaptations eds Gil D., Brumm H. 41–53Oxford Univ. (2014). [Google Scholar]
  42. Herrero S., Smith T., DeBruyn T. D., Grunther K. & Matt C. A. From the field: Brown bear habituation to people—saftey, risks, and benefits. Wildl. Soc. Bull 33, 362–373 (2005). [Google Scholar]
  43. Shalter M. D. in Habituation, Sensitization, and Behavior ed. Peeke H. 349–391Elsevier (1984). [Google Scholar]
  44. Curio E. Probleme des feinderkennens bei vögeln. Proc. Int. Ornithol. Congr. 13, 206–239 (1963). [Google Scholar]
  45. Ikuta L. A. & Blumstein D. T. Do fences protect birds from human disturbance? Biol. Conserv. 112, 447–452 (2003). [Google Scholar]
  46. Dirzo R. et al. Defaunation in the Anthropocene. Science 345, 401–406 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kasereka B., Muhigwa J.-B. B., Shalukoma C. & Kahekwa J. M. Vulnerability of habituated Grauer's gorilla to poaching in the Kahuzi-Biega National Park, DRC. Afr. Study Monogr. 27, 15–26 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  48. Colinvaux P. A. Why Big Fierce Animals are Rare: an Ecologist's Perspective Princeton Univ. (1978). [Google Scholar]
  49. Primack R. B. Essentials of Conservation Biology Sinnauer Associates (2006). [Google Scholar]
  50. Urban M. C. Accelerating extinction risk from climate change. Science 348, 571–573 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ceballos G. et al. Accelerated modern human–induced species losses: Entering the sixth mass extinction. Sci. Adv. 1, e1400253 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Blumstein D. T. & Berger-Tal O. Understanding sensory mechanisms to develop effective conservation and management tools. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 6, 13–18 (2015). [Google Scholar]
  53. Koricheva J., Gurevitch J. & Mengersen K. Handbook of Meta-Analysis in Ecology and Evolution Princeton Univ. (2013). [Google Scholar]
  54. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36, 1–48 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  55. Pagel M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401, 877–884 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Del Hoyo J., Elliott A., Sargatal J., Christie D. A. & de Juana E. Handbook of the Birds of the World Alive. Available at http://www.hbw.com/ (Accessed on 14 November 2014).
  57. Zuur A. F., Ieno E. N. & Elphick C. S. A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1, 3–14 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  58. Alexander R. D. The evolution of social behavior. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5, 325–383 (1974). [Google Scholar]
  59. Hamilton W. D. Geometry for the selfish herd. J. Theor. Biol. 31, 295–311 (1971). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pulliam H. On the advantages of flocking. J. Theor. Biol. 38, 419–422 (1973). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Griffin A. S. Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Learn. Behav. 32, 131–140 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Burish M. J., Kueh H. Y. & Wang S. S.-H. Brain architecture and social complexity in modern and ancient birds. Brain. Behav. Evol. 63, 107–124 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Knight R. L. & Fitzner R. E. Human disturbance and nest site placement in black-billed magpies. J. F. Ornithol. 56, 153–157 (1985). [Google Scholar]
  64. Datta T. & Pal B. C. The effect of human interference on the nesting of the openbill stork Anastomus oscitans at the Raiganj Wildlife Sanctuary, India. Biol. Conserv. 64, 149–154 (1993). [Google Scholar]
  65. Blumstein D. T. et al. Avian risk assessment: effects of perching height and detectability. Ethology 110, 273–285 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  66. Tyrrell L. P. & Fernández-Juricic E. in Escaping from Predators: an Integrative View of Escape Decision eds Cooper Jr W. E., Blumstein D. T. Cambridge Univ. (2015). [Google Scholar]
  67. Blumstein D. T. Flush early and avoid the rush: a general rule of antipredator behavior? Behav. Ecol. 21, 440–442 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  68. Cooper W. E. Jr, Pyron R. A. & Garland T. Island tameness: living on islands reduces flight initiation distance. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20133019 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Barton K. MuMIn: Multi-model inference. R package version 3.1. 2. (2014).

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figures 1-9, Supplementary Tables 1-5, Supplementary Methods and Supplementary References

ncomms9877-s1.pdf (965.2KB, pdf)
Supplementary Data 1

Complete data set

ncomms9877-s2.xlsx (78.2KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 2

Model selection of all birds: phylogenetic effect not controlled

ncomms9877-s3.xlsx (44.3KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 3

Model selection of rural-urban birds: phylogenetic effect not controlled

ncomms9877-s4.xlsx (26.5KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 4

Model selection of all birds: phylogenetic effect controlled

ncomms9877-s5.xlsx (39.2KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 5

Model selection of rural-urban birds: phylogenetic effect controlled

ncomms9877-s6.xlsx (24.1KB, xlsx)

Articles from Nature Communications are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES