Abstract
Objective
To document the natural course of sensation seeking from childhood to adolescence, characterize distinct sensation-seeking trajectories, and examine how these trajectories vary according to selected predictors.
Method
Data were obtained from the Boricua Youth Study, a longitudinal study of 2,491 children and adolescents of Puerto Rican background (three assessments from 2000 to 2004). First, age-specific sensation-seeking levels were characterized. Then, age-adjusted residuals were analyzed using growth mixture models (GMM).
Results
On average, sensation seeking was stable in childhood (ages 5–10) and increased during adolescence (ages 11–17). Mean scores of sensation seeking were higher in the South Bronx vs. Puerto Rico and among males vs. females. Four classes of sensation-seeking trajectories were observed: most study participants had age-expected sensation-seeking trajectories following the average for their age (“normative,” 43.8%); others (37.2%) remained consistently lower than the expected average for their age (“low” sensation seeking); some (12.0%) had an “accelerated” sensation-seeking trajectory, increasing at a faster rate than expected, while a minority (7.0%) had a decreasing sensation-seeking trajectory that started high but decreased, reaching scores slightly higher than the age-average sensation-seeking scores (“stabilizers”). Site (South Bronx vs. Puerto Rico) and gender were predictors of membership in a specific class of sensation-seeking trajectory.
Conclusion
It is important to take a developmental approach when examining sensation seeking and to consider gender and the social environment when trying to understand how sensation seeking evolves during childhood and adolescence.
Keywords: Sensation Seeking, Puerto Rico, Trajectories, Children, Adolescence
INTRODUCTION
Sensation seeking is defined as “the need for varied, novel and complex sensations and experiences and the willingness to take physical and social risks for the sake of such experiences”.1,2 Sensation seeking has been consistently associated with risk-taking behaviors, such as alcohol and drug use, gambling and problem gambling, suicide, and sexual risk behaviors,3–6 as well as with an interest in stimulating but lower-risk activities (i.e., arts, group sports, debate clubs).1 Much of the research has focused on the relationship between sensation seeking as a predictor of different outcomes,1, 7–9 yet little is known about the developmental course of sensation seeking10–12 among young children. One potential reason for this gap in knowledge is that sensation seeking has been traditionally considered a personality trait, despite growing evidence that it can be modified.13
Studies examining sensation-seeking trajectories among US children and adolescents (e.g., 9–14 at baseline) show linear increases in sensation seeking over time,10–12, 14–16 and gradual declines among those about 17–24 years old.14 To date, no study has investigated sensation-seeking trajectories starting before age 9. Certain children already display more disinhibited patterns of behaviors than others early in life,17, 18 and some of them may also be more prone to novel and complex experiences. Therefore, it is important to investigate whether distinct trajectories of sensation seeking exist in childhood. Another understudied question is the existence of individual heterogeneity in the trajectories of sensation seeking, that is, different types of trajectories that may be followed by substantial proportions of individuals. Rather, most investigations have focused exclusively on the average trajectory of sensation seeking11, 12, 15, 16 and not on heterogeneity in trajectories.14 In our study, we aim to assess whether distinct sensation seeking trajectories exist among children and adolescents. Understanding individual heterogeneity is important when planning risk-taking prevention efforts.
Little is known about predictors of sensation seeking. Thus, for the purpose of this study, we selected likely predictors of sensation seeking, specifically, gender, maternal education, parental psychopathology, and sociocultural context. Gender has been associated with differences in sensation seeking in most cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Men usually have higher sensation-seeking scores as compared to women,1, 10, 15, 19, 20 with the exception of a study that showed no gender differences.12 In addition, it is possible that sensation seeking is influenced by factors related to future development of psychopathology, such as maternal education and parental psychopathology.21–24 Arnett7 has also argued that the sociocultural environment is crucial for how sensation seeking is expressed and evolves in adolescence: adolescents who live in more permissive environments (i.e., those in which engaging in risk-taking behaviors is more socially acceptable and those that may have less clear sets of rules and responsibilities for youth) will, on average, have higher sensation-seeking scores compared to those living in less permissive environments.7 However, no study has investigated the development of sensation seeking in a sample of children and adolescents from the same ethnic group growing up in two very different socio-cultural contexts, where the levels of exposure to risk (such as traumatic events and discrimination) differ. Identifying the role of these likely predictors of sensation-seeking trajectories in childhood and adolescence could help characterize individual variation in sensation seeking across time from childhood to adolescence and help identify mechanisms through which sensation seeking can be modified by the implementation of interventions.13
Knowledge in these areas will help inform interventions to encourage children and adolescents with high sensation-seeking levels to engage in stimulating activities unlikely to have negative consequences.1 Additionally, understanding how sensation seeking varies across different subgroups (e.g., socio-demographic factors) can help identify and reduce potential disparities in the sensation-seeking/risk-taking paradigm. The current study addresses the gaps in the literature by capitalizing on a large prospective cohort study of Puerto Rican youth growing up in two socio-cultural contexts (Puerto Rico and the US), including respondents as young as age 5, to examine the longitudinal trajectories of sensation seeking. Specifically, the aims of these analyses are to: (1) document the natural course of sensation seeking from early childhood to early adolescence (from ages 5 to 13) by socio-cultural context (Puerto Rico and the US) and time (i.e., three waves from 2000 to 2004); (2) to characterize groups of individuals with distinct trajectories of sensation seeking; and (3) to examine how sensation-seeking trajectories (i.e., subgroups of respondents characterized by different patterns of changes in sensation seeking) vary according to socio-demographic factors, maternal education, and parental psychopathology.
METHOD
Sample
The present study was based on the Boricua Youth Study, a longitudinal study of children and adolescents of Puerto Rican background, which conducted three yearly assessments from the summer of 2000 to the fall of 2004. A total of 2,491 Puerto Rican (PR) children aged 5 to 13 years old were recruited at baseline in the South Bronx (SBx), New York (n=1,138) and in the standard metropolitan area of San Juan and Caguas, Puerto Rico (n=1,353). Even though all children were recruited at ages 5 to 13, a few (n=100 interviewed at age 14 and 7 at age 15) were interviewed a year or more after recruitment. Each sample was a multistage probability sample of households of the target population, weighted to represent the populations of Puerto Rican children in the two socio-cultural contexts. The study sampled up to 3 children from each household, randomly selecting 3 children in households with more than 3 children. All children in the study had to be of Puerto Rican background, defined as having at least one primary caretaker being of Puerto Rican background. Details about the study sampling, design, and procedures can be obtained elsewhere.25, 26 In brief, there were 1,526 eligible children in Puerto Rico and 1,414 in the South Bronx. Out of these, 11.5% of the children selected to be study participants in Puerto Rico and 19.5% of those selected to participate in the South Bronx refused or were unavailable to participate in the study.25, 26
Procedures
Participants (caretakers and children) were interviewed in their homes in their preferred language (English or Spanish) by trained lay interviewers. Attrition rates across waves were low. Only 8.2% of the sample was not interviewed at wave 2, and 12.2% at wave 3. Retention rates were higher in PR than in NY at wave 2 (93.8% vs. 89.4%) and wave 3 (89.6% vs. 85.6%). Missing data analyses that took into account the complex survey design tested whether missing status at the two follow-ups were predicted by six socio-demographic measures collected at the first wave. Only one significant difference emerged: children living in the South Bronx were less likely to participate at waves 2 and 3 (89% of children in Puerto Rico participated in all waves vs. 83% of those in the South Bronx [p <.05]).
Measures
Outcome: Sensation seeking
Sensation seeking was measured by an abbreviated ten-item scale derived from Russo’s sensation-seeking scale,27 a shorter version of Zuckerman’s sensation-seeking scale.28 Seven of these selected items are from Zuckerman’s thrill-and adventure-seeking factor and three from the social disinhibition subscale. The structure of Russo’s scale was adapted for children as young as five years old: for each item, they were given two choices of behaviors and asked to identify which one was more like them (e.g., item 1: “You don’t like to do dangerous things” OR “Sometimes you like to do things that are a little scary”; item 2: “Riding very fast and doing tricks on a skateboard is scary” OR “You think that riding very fast and doing tricks on a skateboard are fun”). Cronbach alpha of this scale in this sample was 0.72. Two respondents out of the 2,491 had missing values on sensation seeking in all three waves and were excluded from the analyses. We assessed whether the scale was comparable across different ages, i.e., whether or not any items exhibited differential item functioning (DIF). Following standard methodology for assessing DIF, we fit a single-factor multiple indicator multiple cause (MIMIC) model of all 10 sensation-seeking items with age as a predictor in Mplus and tested modification indices of direct effects from age to specific items.29 There were no items that showed moderate or large effect size for DIF (all standardized direct effects <0.15).30 Hence, the sensation-seeking scale measures the trait consistently (i.e., is comparable) across different ages.
Correlates of Sensation seeking
We examined correlates (all measured at wave 1) of different sensation-seeking trajectories that had been consistently identified in prior studies as relevant for sensation seeking or as relevant for other possibly related outcomes in the study sample: study site (Puerto Rico and South Bronx), gender (male, female), maternal education (less than high school, high school, at least some college education), family socioeconomic status (household classified by information on income/number of household residents as above or below the US federal poverty guidelines), and parental psychopathology (any psychological or emotional problem defined based on the Family Psychiatric History Screen for Epidemiologic Studies scale)31.
Statistical Analysis
The three annual waves of data collected across a wide age range (5–13 years old at wave 1 to 8–17 at wave 3) represent an accelerated longitudinal design32 that provides the opportunity to 1) estimate mean sensation-seeking levels across the ages of 5–17, and 2) characterize different individual-level development in sensation seeking benchmarked by age-specific means. We first characterized age-specific sensation-seeking levels by modeling and plotting mean sensation seeking as a function of categorical age (5 to 17), site (Bronx or Puerto Rico), and gender. Sensation seeking means and 95% CIs were estimated from a linear mixed effects model with fixed effect for age, gender, site, and all two- and three-way interactions, including a random intercept to control for repeated measures within individuals across the three waves. Mean contrasts were tested between gender, site, and each age category. A subsequent piecewise linear model of continuous age was fitted to test differential slopes across ages 5–10 and 11–17, and to test whether differential slopes could be moderated by site or gender.
To examine individual-level heterogeneity in the development of sensation seeking, we used age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores based on the residuals from a linear regression with sensation seeking as the outcome, and categorical age, wave, and the interaction of age and wave as predictors. Age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores provide a meaningful way of characterizing within-individual development while controlling for age-specific trends. An advantage of this method based on residuals is its flexibility in controlling for age without needing to specify an oversimplified functional form for the population-age trend (e.g. linear, quadratic). Specifically, the individual-level age-adjusted sensation-seeking score represents how an individual differs at each wave from what her/his expected level of sensation seeking would be for her/his age. A score of 0 indicates the individual is average for her/his age, whereas positive (negative) values indicate higher (lower) sensation seeking for her/his age. Age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores were then analyzed using growth mixture models (GMMs).33 GMMs were used to determine whether there were distinct patterns of sensation-seeking trajectories and to empirically classify individuals into those latent classes. GMMs with one, two, three, four, and five classes were estimated using full information maximum likelihood that includes all individuals with at least one sensation-seeking score. Determination of the optimal number of classes relied primarily on the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), which balances model fit and parsimony34 and has been demonstrated to show good performance determining the optimal number of classes in mixture models.35 The sample-size-adjusted BIC and entropy were considered, as well. Slopes and intercepts of the sensation-seeking trajectories were allowed to vary between classes in each model. The KNOWNCLASS option in Mplus was used to test whether a model that allowed trajectories to differ by gender fit the data better than a model that constrained the trajectory classes to be the same for males and females. Associations between the estimated classes and socio-demographic covariates were tested using the 3-step auxiliary variable option in Mplus.36 All analyses incorporated sampling weights. The GMM incorporated the sampling clusters (primary sampling units) and strata and constrained all variance estimates to be positive. A sensitivity analysis of the GMM was conducted using family as the clustering unit instead of the primary sampling units. All analyses were conducted in Mplus 7.3. Level of significance was set at p<.01 to take into account multiple comparisons.
RESULTS
Sample characteristics have already been described elsewhere.25 Briefly, 54.4% of the wave-1 sample was based in Puerto Rico, 51.6% were male, and 50.1% were age10–13 at baseline. Figure 1 shows sensation seeking mean scores and 95% CIs by age, gender, and site. Mean scores of sensation seeking were higher in the South Bronx as compared to Puerto Rico (χ2(1) = 28.76, p< .0001) and among males compared to females ((χ2(1) = 75.28, df=1, p < .0001).
Tests comparing mean sensation seeking across each age category from 5 to 10 years old found no significant differences. The first incremental age to show a significant mean increase in sensation seeking was from 10 to 11 years old (t(df)=3.12, p = .002), and each additional age from 12–17 had significantly higher mean than at 10 years old. In addition, we examined a model treating age as continuous with a change point in the slope of sensation seeking at age 11. In all groups, mean sensation-seeking scores remained relatively stable until age 11 (slope = 0.06, χ2(1) = 3.13, df=1, p = 0.08), at which point mean sensation-seeking scores started increasing in all groups (slope=0.59, χ2(1)=55.93, df=1, p < .0001). These slopes did not differ significantly by site or gender.
Subgroups of Sensation-Seeking Trajectories
Model comparison statistics (Table 1) for the growth mixture models of age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores were better for the 4-class model than for the models with 1, 2, 3, or 5 distinct trajectory classes. A 4-class model that constrained the trajectories to be the same across gender fit the data better than one that allowed them to be different. Table 2 presents the estimates and tests for the intercept and slope associated with each class, the estimate of the variability of the random effect around each coefficient in each class, and the marginal probability of being in each class for the 4-class model. A GMM that accounted for the clustering of siblings within families produced virtually the same results as the model that accounted for clustering within primary sampling units.
Table 1.
Log-likelihood | Number of free parameters | BIC | Sample Size-Adjusted BIC | Entropy | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
|
|
|
1-class model | −15274.622 | 8 | 30611.801 | 30586.383 | |
2-class model | −15218.909 | 13 | 30539.473 | 30498.169 | 0.444 |
3-class model | −15169.842 | 18 | 30480.438 | 30423.248 | 0.448 |
4-class model | −15123.987 | 23 | 30427.825 | 30354.748 | 0.654 |
5-class model | −15115.065 | 28 | 30449.081 | 30360.118 | 0.640 |
Note: Bolding indicates best fit (i.e. smallest for Bayesian information criterion [BIC] and sample size-adjusted BIC [SSABIC], largest for entropy).
Two respondents did not have information on sensation seeking.
Table 2.
GMM sensationseeking classes | Fixed Coefficient | SE | p Value | Variance of random effect | % in class | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Normative | Intercept | 1.096 | 0.169 | <.001 | 0.254 | 43.80 |
Slope | −0.255 | 0.105 | .015 | 0.737 | ||
Low | Intercept | −2.02 | 0.224 | <.001 | 0.099 | 37.20 |
Slope | 0.008 | 0.206 | .970 | 0.000 | ||
|
||||||
Accelerated | Intercept | −1.916 | 0.181 | <.001 | 0.000 | 12.00 |
Slope | 1.668 | 0.512 | .001 | 0.641 | ||
|
||||||
Stabilizers | Intercept | 4.306 | 0.419 | <.001 | 0.208 | 7.00 |
Slope | −1.471 | 0.265 | <.001 | 1.195 | ||
|
Note: SE = standard error.
In the 4-class GMM of sensation-seeking trajectories (Figure 2), we identified one class that included 43.8% (n=1,089) of the youth (Class 1, the “normative” in this sample): in this class, the youths’ age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores remained near zero across all three waves, indicating that their sensation seeking increased (or stayed the same) as expected for youth of the same age. Another class comprised 37.2% (n=927) of the youth (Class 2, the “low sensation seeking”): in this class, individuals had age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores that were about two points lower than the average across all three waves as they aged. A third class represented 12.0% (n=299) of the sample (Class 3, the “accelerated”): in this class, individuals had below-average sensation-seeking scores at wave 1 that increased faster than expected with age, resulting in their having average (i.e., near zero) age-adjusted sensation seeking by wave 3. Finally, a fourth class, representing 7.0% of the sample (n=174), was comprised of individuals who started the trajectory at wave 1 with high age-adjusted sensation-seeking scores (more than 4 points above average) and decreased across waves but still remained higher than average for their age (Class 4, the “stabilizers”). GMM provides a probability for each person to be in a particular class; the higher these probabilities are, the more confidence we have in the assignment of persons to clusters. Individuals identified to be in the “normative” class have average probability of 83.9% to be in that class, those in the “low” class had average of 76.7% to be in that class, those in the “accelerated” class had 74.7% probability to be in that class, and those in the “stabilizer” class had 84.0% average probability to be in that class.
Adjusted Analysis of Predictors of Classes of Sensation-Seeking Trajectories
We then explored potential baseline predictors of sensation-seeking class membership (Table 3) in models adjusted by all covariates selected. Those in the “low” class of sensation seeking trajectories were less likely to live in the South Bronx (vs. Puerto Rico, adjusted odds ration, AOR [95% CI]: 0.45[0.31, 0.67]), but more likely to be females than males (AOR[95% CI]: 3.63 [2.54, 5.20]) compared to those in the “normative” class. Respondents in the “accelerated” class of sensation-seeking trajectory were less likely to live in the South Bronx (vs. Puerto Rico, AOR[95% CI]:0.52[0.32,0.82]) as compared to those in the “normative” class. In the “stabilizers” class of sensation-seeking trajectory, however, respondents were more likely to be males than females compared to those in the sample “normative” class (AOR[95% CI]: 0.41[0.22, 0.77]). There were no other statistically significant differences in predictors of trajectories at the p < .01 level.
Table 3.
Normative (n=1089, 43.8%) | Low (n=927, 37.2%) | Accelerated (n=299, 12.0%) | Stabilizers (n=174, 7.0%) | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|||||||||||||||
n | % | AOR | n | % | AOR (95% CI) | p–value | n | % | AOR (95% CI) | p-value | n | % | AOR (95% CI) | p-value | |
Site | |||||||||||||||
South Bronx (n=1,136) | 560 | 48.62 | Reference | 351 | 31.28 | 0.45 (0.31, 0.67) | <.0001 | 122 | 10.87 | 0.52 (0.32, 0.82) | .006 | 103 | 9.22 | 1.21 (0.70, 2.09) | .487 |
Puerto Rico (reference, n=1,353) | 529 | 37.95 | 576 | 42.98 | 177 | 14.12 | 71 | 4.95 | |||||||
Gender | |||||||||||||||
Female (n=1,206) | 443 | 41.26 | Reference | 573 | 43.06 | 3.63 (2.54, 5.20) | <.000 1 | 139 | 11.23 | 1.19 (0.72, 1.96) | .497 | 51 | 4.45 | 0.41 (0.22, 0.77) | .006 |
Male (reference, n=1,283) | 646 | 52.42 | 354 | 23.55 | 160 | 11.53 | 123 | 12.5 | |||||||
Maternal education | |||||||||||||||
Less than high school (n=850) | 358 | 45.22 | Reference | 326 | 34.67 | 1.41 (0.73, 2.73) | .300 | 95 | 10.14 | 0.92 (0.44, 1.92) | .833 | 71 | 9.96 | 2.73 (1.17, 6.39) | .020 |
High School (n=1,043) | 473 | 48.39 | 380 | 31.49 | 1.06 (0.60, 1.86) | .846 | 122 | 12.06 | 0.98 (0.50, 1.92) | .958 | 68 | 8.06 | 1.92 (0.74, 4.98) | .181 | |
At least some college (reference, n=596) | 248 | 47.91 | 214 | 34.03 | 80 | 12.98 | 31 | 5.08 | |||||||
Single parent household | |||||||||||||||
Yes (n=908) | 405 | 47.69 | Reference | 319 | 31.40 | 0.72 (0.46, 1.12) | .145 | 108 | 11.68 | 1.27 (0.75, 2.12) | .371 | 76 | 9.23 | 1.17 (0.70, 1.95) | .540 |
No (reference, n=1,569) | 678 | 46.25 | 603 | 34.37 | 191 | 11.31 | 97 | 8.08 | |||||||
Parental Psychopathology | |||||||||||||||
Yes (n=827) | 372 | 46.81 | Reference | 303 | 33.37 | 1.02 (0.82, 1.26) | .885 | 99 | 11.79 | 1.02 (0.80, 1.31) | .858 | 53 | 8.03 | 0.93 (0.68, 1.27) | .642 |
No (reference, n=1,641) | 707 | 46.94 | 617 | 33.03 | 197 | 11.19 | 120 | 8.84 | |||||||
SES | |||||||||||||||
Below federal poverty level(n=1,712) | 722 | 46.37 | Reference | 682 | 34.77 | 1.46 (0.44, 1.05) | .086 | 194 | 10.62 | 0.73 (0.83, 2.25) | .223 | 114 | 8.24 | 0.80 (0.73, 2.15) | .420 |
Above federal poverty level(reference, n=777) | 367 | 48.05 | 245 | 29.95 | 105 | 12.86 | 60 | 9.13 |
Note: AOR = adjusted odds ratio; SES = socioeconomic status.
The sample age-corrected class is the reference category.
Model-weighted and adjusted for sampling design and adjusted for all covariates described in the table.
DISCUSSION
This study examined the natural course of sensation seeking from early childhood to adolescence by sociocultural context (Puerto Rico and the US) over time, characterized groups of individuals with distinct sensation-seeking trajectories, and examined how sensation-seeking trajectories varied by subgroups. There were three essential findings. First, among children of Puerto Rican background ages 5–13, over the course of 3 years, sensation seeking was stable from ages 5–10 and increased from ages 11–17. Second, four different classes of sensation-seeking trajectories were observed from ages 5–17: most children had age-expected increasing sensation-seeking trajectories, some starting off from medium levels and increasing as expected with age (“normative” across childhood and adolescence, 43.8%); others (37.2%) starting at low sensation-seeking levels and increasing as expected with age (“low”); some (12.0%) had an “accelerated” sensation-seeking trajectory, starting with average–low sensation-seeking scores and increasing faster than expected with age, while a minority (7.0%) had a decreasing sensation-seeking trajectory that started high but decreased, reaching scores slightly higher than the average age sensation-seeking scores (“stabilizers”). Third, site and gender were the only predictors of membership in a specific class of sensation-seeking trajectory. Specifically, as compared to the “normative” class of sensation-seeking trajectory, those living in the South Bronx (vs. Puerto Rico) were less likely to belong to the “low” and “accelerated” classes (vs. the “normative” class); males were less likely than females to be in the “low” class of sensation-seeking trajectory and were more likely to be in the “stabilizer” class of sensation-seeking trajectory, versus the “normative” class.
Overall, our study findings are concordant with previous studies that focused on adolescents and show that sensation seeking is not stable and homogeneous over time, at least during later developmental stages.9, 11, 12, 14–16, 37,38 Our study expands previous findings by indicating that the average sensation-seeking scores in both the South Bronx and in Puerto Rico remained relatively stable from age 5 until age 10, when they started increasing. To our knowledge, ours is the first study on classes of sensation-seeking trajectories that includes respondents as young as age 5. In addition, our findings are consistent with findings from studies showing that sensation seeking has high values in mid-adolescence.11, 12, 15 High sensation seeking among youth may be related to still-developing prefrontal executive function and to rapid maturation of the subcortical motivation system in adolescence.39–41 This could partially explain why we found stable trajectories only until age 10, when average sensation seeking started to increase. Social and family environments can influence these trajectories, with adolescents being more susceptible to influences from the environment outside their homes (i.e., school, peers)42, 43 than children (socialization processes are different by age). No other study has investigated the trajectory of sensation seeking prior to age 9, and the stability of average scores of sensation seeking in this age range needs to be investigated in other samples.
Our study’s main contribution is elucidating that sensation-seeking trajectories do not follow only one single pattern from childhood to adolescence; rather, there are different classes of sensation-seeking trajectories as described in our results. Our study goes above and beyond former studies11, 12, 14 that focused on the overall mean trajectory of sensation seeking because we examined heterogeneity in the trajectories of sensation seeking in our sample by age, site, and study wave. Investigating heterogeneity in trajectories is important to identify individual- level trajectories of sensation seeking and understand how sensation seeking evolves for different subgroups of children and adolescents. For example, we were able to determine that not all children have the same starting point and that for a small group of children, sensation seeking actually decreases as they age. Understanding how sensation seeking evolves from childhood to adolescence for different subgroups can better help identify who the children and youth are who should be targeted when developing prevention and intervention efforts. As expected, the great majority (76.62%) of the sample belonged to the “normative” and “low–sensation-seeking” classes, in which sensation-seeking scores increased as expected with age (albeit in the “low–sensation-seeking” class, wave 1 scores were lower than in the “normative” class). However, two distinct subgroups of respondents are worth further examination. One, the 16.31% of respondents classified as being in the “accelerated” class, that is, those who had average–low sensation-seeking scores that increased faster than expected with age, and the 7.1% of the sample who started the trajectory with high sensation-seeking scores that decreased quickly with age (the “stabilizers”). Future studies are needed to investigate whether different trajectories are associated with different behavioral outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood.
Interestingly, mean scores of sensation seeking were consistently higher in the South Bronx as compared to Puerto Rico, suggesting that the socio-cultural context matters in the expression of sensation-seeking phenotypes in childhood and adolescence. When we examined specific classes of sensation-seeking trajectories, we saw that South Bronx youth are less likely to be in classes that start with low sensation-seeking levels (“low sensation seeking” and “accelerated” classes) as compared to being in the “normative” class, meaning that they had a tendency to start with higher sensation-seeking levels. This could be explained if sensation seeking could potentially be associated with, for example, migration patterns, and that children born into families of migrants would score higher in sensation seeking either because they inherited a “novelty-seeking” trait from their parents,44–46 or they were exposed to family environments (e.g., different parenting practices) that promoted certain behaviors.10, 47 As described in prior reports from this study, the characteristics of the socio-cultural and neighborhood contexts from which the samples were selected could partially explain these differences. The area sampled in Puerto Rico is more demographically heterogeneous, whereas the South Bronx is a poverty-stricken area (the poorest congressional district in the US) with a more homogeneous Puerto Rican population.25 Besides poverty, children living in the South Bronx sociocultural context very frequently face environmental risks such as exposure to violence, peer delinquency, or stressful life events.48, 49 Moreover, this is in line with Arnett’s argument7 that the socio-cultural environment (i.e., peer influence, community socialization) is crucial for how sensation seeking is expressed and evolves in adolescence.
Consistent with prior findings,1, 10, 15, 19, 20 in our study, males had higher sensation-seeking levels than females. Studies have shown that sex hormones (particularly testosterone) are associated with sensation seeking.50,51 Sex differences in sensation seeking have also been explained by gender differences in culturally transmitted social norms,52 while evolutionary psychologists have related these sex differences to differences in selection pressure.52–54 Respondents in the “low–sensation-seeking” class were more likely to be female than male (class with lower sensation-seeking scores across time than the “normative” class), while respondents in the “stabilizers” class were more likely to be male than female (class with higher sensation-seeking scores across time than the “normative” class). Nevertheless, more than half of the males and 42% of the females were in the “normative” class and followed what is considered the average development for sensation seeking in childhood and adolescence years. None of the other specific factors were significantly associated with classes of sensation-seeking trajectories in this sample. The differences by socio-cultural context that we identified, however, indicate the need to examine a wider array of potential factors to be able to understand environmental influences on sensation seeking.
Limitations are noted. First, this study assumes that respondents in both socio-cultural contexts come from the same population, and that selection factors that distinguish the two groups can be adjusted. Second, experiences of Puerto Rican youth in the South Bronx might be different from experiences of Puerto Rican youth in other parts of the US. Third, the follow-up of study respondents spanned only three years and started more than ten years ago, thus data are not available to examine the full spectrum of sensation-seeking trajectories from childhood to young adulthood, and only linear trajectories could be tested. Fourth, we had to adapt an existing measure of sensation seeking to younger children. Fifth, we relied solely on self- and parent-reported data for all of our measures; in particular, parental psychopathology was determined based on parental report of family history.
These limitations notwithstanding, this study has important clinical implications and contributions to the literature on trajectories of sensation seeking in children and youth. To our knowledge, no other study has documented the trajectories of sensation seeking in Puerto Rican youth, even though Puerto Ricans are the second largest minority group in the US and are at highest risk among all Latino subgroups for risk-taking behaviors such as substance use disorders.55 In addition, no study to date has investigated heterogeneity in the trajectories of sensation seeking in an epidemiologic sample of children and adolescents from any racial/ethnic group. From a clinical point of view, this means that there are several different ways in which sensation seeking develops over time for different subgroups of children and adolescents. The findings of this study highlight the importance of taking a developmental approach when examining sensation seeking and considering the socio-cultural context when trying to understand how sensation seeking evolves from childhood to adolescence.
Acknowledgments
The authors acknowledge the contribution of all of the dedicated staff who participated in this study.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Dr. Wall served as the statistical expert for this research.
Disclosure: Dr. Martins has served as a consultant for Purdue Pharma to conduct secondary data analysis of data on alcohol and prescription opioid use, which is unrelated to data reported in this manuscript. Drs. Wall, Blanco, Santaella, Ramos-Olazagasti, Canino, Bird, Brown, Duarte, and Ms. Eisenberg report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Contributor Information
Silvia S. Martins, Columbia University, Mailman School of Public Health, New York.
Melanie M. Wall, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Ruth Eisenberg, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Carlos Blanco, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Julian Santaella, Columbia University, Mailman School of Public Health, New York.
Maria Ramos-Olazagasti, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Glorisa Canino, Behavioral Sciences Research Institute, University of Puerto Rico School of Medicine, San Juan.
Hector R. Bird, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Qiana Brown, Columbia University, Mailman School of Public Health, New York.
Cristiane S. Duarte, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
References
- 1.Roberti JW. A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. J Res Pers. 2004;38:256–79. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Zuckerman M, Neeb M. Sensation seeking and psychopathology. Psychiatry research. 1979;1:255–264. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(79)90007-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ortin A, Lake AM, Kleinman M, Gould MS. Sensation seeking as risk factor for suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in adolescence. J affect disord. 2012;143:214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sheer VC, Cline RJ. The Development and Validation of a Model Explaining Sexual Behavior among College Students – Implications for Aids Communication Campaigns. Hum Commun Res. 1994;21:280–304. [Google Scholar]
- 5.McDaniel SR. Investigating the roles of gambling interest and impulsive sensation seeking on consumer enjoyment of promotional games. Social Behavior & Personality. 2002;30:53–64. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Greene K, Krcmar M, Walters L, Rubin D, Jerold LH. Targeting adolescent risk-taking behaviors: the contributions of egocentrism and sensation-seeking. Adolesc. 2000;23:439–461. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Arnett J. Sensation Seeking – a New Conceptualization and a New Scale. Pers Indiv Differ. 1994;16:289–96. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zuckerman M. Sensation seeking and crime, antisocial behavior, and delinquency. In: Zuckerman M, editor. Sensation seeking and risky behavior. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hansen EB, Breivik G. Sensation seeking as a predictor of positive and negative risk behaviour among adolescents. Pers Indiv Differ. 2001;30:627–640. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zuckerman M. Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Romer D, Hennessy M. A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: the role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention science: the official journal of the Society for Prevention Research. 2007;8:89–101. doi: 10.1007/s11121-007-0064-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Collado A, Felton JW, MacPherson L, Lejuez CW. Longitudinal trajectories of sensation seeking, risk taking propensity, and impulsivity across early to middle adolescence. Addictive behaviors. 2014;39:1580–8. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Vallone D, Allen JA, Clayton RR, Xiao H. How reliable and valid is the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS-4) for youth of various racial/ethnic groups? Addiction. 2007;102(Suppl 2):71–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01957.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM. Individual differences in the development of sensation seeking and impulsivity during adolescence: further evidence for a dual systems model. Dev psychol. 2011;47:739–46. doi: 10.1037/a0023279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Shulman EP, Harden KP, Chein JM, Steinberg L. Sex Differences in the Developmental Trajectories of Impulse Control and Sensation-Seeking from Early Adolescence to Early Adulthood. J Youth Adolesc. 2015;44:1–17. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0116-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev psychol. 2008;44:1764–78. doi: 10.1037/a0012955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Degnan KA, Hane AA, Henderson HA, Moas OL, Reeb-Sutherland BC, Fox NA. Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. Dev psychol. 2011;47:765–80. doi: 10.1037/a0021316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fernandez JP, Aguero TH, Vega Lopez GA, Marranzino G, Cerrizuela S, Aybar MJ. Developmental expression and role of Kinesin Eg5 during Xenopus laevis embryogenesis. Dev Dyn. 2014;243:527–540. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.24094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zuckerman M. Sensation Seeking: The balance between risk. In: Lipsitt L, Mitnick L, editors. Self-regulatory Behavior and Risk Taking: Causes and Consequences. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation; 1991. pp. 143–152. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Zuckerman M. Psychobiology of personality. 2nd. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal Depression and Child Psychopathology: A Meta-Analytic Review. Clin Child Fam Psych. 2011;14:1–27. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Klein DN, Lewinsohn PM, Rohde P, Seeley JR, Olino TM. Psychopathology in the adolescent and young adult offspring of a community sample of mothers and fathers with major depression. Psychol Med. 2005;35:353–65. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wills TA, McNamara G, Vaccaro D. Parental education related to adolescent stress-coping and substance use: development of a mediational model. Health psychol. 14:464–478. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.14.5.464. 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bird HR, Canino GJ, Davies M, et al. A study of disruptive behavior disorders in Puerto Rican youth: I. Background, design, and survey methods. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45(9):1032–1041. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000227878.58027.3d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bird HR, Shrout PE, Davies M, et al. Longitudinal development of antisocial behaviors in young and early adolescent Puerto Rican children at two sites. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(1):5–14. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000242243.23044.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Russo MF, Lahey BB, Christ MAG, et al. Preliminary Development of a Sensation Seeking Scale for Children. Pers Indiv Differ. 1991;12(5):399–405. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Zuckerman M, Eysenck S, Eysenck HJ. Sensation seeking in England and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1978 Feb;46(1):139–149. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.46.1.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Finch H. The MIMIC Model as a Method for Detecting DIF: Comparison With Mantel-Haenszel, SIBTEST, and the IRT Likelihood Ratio. Applied Psychological Measurement. 2005;29:278–295. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Jin Y, Myers N, Ahn S, Penfield R. A Comparison of Uniform DIF Effect Size Estimators Under the MIMIC and Rasch Models. Educational and psychological measurement. 2013;73(2):339–358. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lish JD, Weissman MM, Adams PB, Hoven CW, Bird H. Family Psychiatric Screening Instrument for Epidemiologic Studies – Pilot Testing and Validation. Psychiatry research. 1995;57:169–180. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02632-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Galbraith S, Bowden J, Mander A. Accelerated longitudinal designs: An overview of modelling, power, costs and handling missing data [epub ahead of print] Stat methods med res. 2014 Aug 20; doi: 10.1177/0962280214547150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Muthén B. Latent variable analysis: Growth mixture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fraley C, Raftery A. How many clusters? Which clustering method? Answers via model based cluster analysis. Computer Journal. 1998;41:578–588. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nylund K, Asparouhov T, Muthen B. Deciding on the number of classes in LCA and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo Simulation Study. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Asparouhov T, Muthen B. Auxiliary Variables in Mixture Modeling: 3-step approaches using Mplus. Mplus Web Notes. 2014;15(8) [Google Scholar]
- 37.MacPherson L, Magidson JF, Reynolds EK, Kahler CW, Lejuez CW. Changes in sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity predict increases in alcohol use among early adolescents. Alcohol clin exp res. 2010;34:1400–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01223.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Pedersen SL, Molina BS, Belendiuk KA, Donovan JE. Racial differences in the development of impulsivity and sensation seeking from childhood into adolescence and their relation to alcohol use. Alcohol clin exp res. 2012;36:1794–1802. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01797.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Romer D. Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: implications for prevention. Developmental psychobiology. 2010;52:263–276. doi: 10.1002/dev.20442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Romer D, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Giannetta JM, Yang W, Hurt H. Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence. Dev Sci. 2011;14:1119–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01061.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. The American journal of psychiatry. 2003;160(6):1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Alexander C, Piazza M, Mekos D, Valente T. Peers, schools, and adolescent cigarette smoking. J adolesc health. 2001;29:22–30. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(01)00210-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ryan AM. Peer groups as a context for the socialization of adolescents’ motivation, engagement, and achievement in school. Educ Psychol. 2000;35(2):101–111. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Koopmans JR, Boomsma DI, Heath AC, Vandoornen LJP. A Multivariate Genetic-Analysis of Sensation Seeking. Behav Genet. 1995;25(4):349–356. doi: 10.1007/BF02197284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Stoel RD, De Geus EJC, Boomsma DI. Genetic analysis of sensation seeking with an extended twin design. Behav Genet. 2006;36(2):229–237. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-9028-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Fulker DW, Eysenck SBG, Zuckerman M. A Genetic and Environmental-Analysis of Sensation Seeking. J Res Pers. 1980;14(2):261–281. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Calzada E, Eyberg S. Self-reported parenting practices in Dominican and Puerto Rican mothers of young children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2002;31(3):354–363. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3103_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Morcillo C, Duarte CS, Shen S, Blanco C, Canino G, Bird HR. Parental familism and antisocial behaviors: development, gender, and potential mechanisms. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:471–479. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ramos-Olazagasti MA, Shrout PE, Yoshikawa H, Canino GJ, Bird HR. Contextual risk and promotive processes in Puerto Rican youths’ internalizing trajectories in Puerto Rico and New York. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25:755–71. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Campbell BC, Dreber A, Apicella CL, et al. Testosterone exposure, dopaminergic reward, and sensation-seeking in young men. Physiology & behavior. 2010;99:451–6. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rosenblitt JC, Soler H, Johnson SE, Quadagno DM. Sensation seeking and hormones in men and women: Exploring the link. Horm Behav. 2001;40:396–402. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Cross CP, Cyrenne DL, Brown GR. Sex differences in sensation-seeking: a meta-analysis. Scientific reports. 2013;3:2486. doi: 10.1038/srep02486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.MacDonald KB. Effortful control, explicit processing, and the regulation of human evolved predispositions. Psychological review. 2008;115:1012–1031. doi: 10.1037/a0013327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wilson M, Daly M. Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: the young male syndrome. Ethol Sociobiol. 1985;73:59–73. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Alegria M, Canino G, Shrout PE, et al. Prevalence of mental illness in immigrant and non-immigrant U.S. Latino groups. Am J Psychiat. 2008;165:359–69. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]