Skip to main content
Annals of General Psychiatry logoLink to Annals of General Psychiatry
. 2015 Dec 1;14:42. doi: 10.1186/s12991-015-0081-z

The neurocognitive functioning in bipolar disorder: a systematic review of data

Eirini Tsitsipa 1, Konstantinos N Fountoulakis 2,
PMCID: PMC4666163  PMID: 26628905

Abstract

Background

During the last decades, there have been many different opinions concerning the neurocognitive function in Bipolar disorder (BD). The aim of the current study was to perform a systematic review of the literature and to synthesize the data in a comprehensive picture of the neurocognitive dysfunction in BD.

Methods

Papers were located with searches in PubMed/MEDLINE, through June 1st 2015. The review followed a modified version of the recommendations of the Preferred Items for Reporting of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses statement.

Results

The initial search returned 110,403 papers. After the deletion of duplicates, 11,771 papers remained for further evaluation. Eventually, 250 were included in the analysis.

Conclusion

The current review supports the presence of a neurocognitive deficit in BD, in almost all neurocognitive domains. This deficit is qualitative similar to that observed in schizophrenia but it is less severe. There are no differences between BD subtypes. Its origin is unclear. It seems it is an enduring component and represents a core primary characteristic of the illness, rather than being secondary to the mood state or medication. This core deficit is confounded (either increased or attenuated) by the disease phase, specific personal characteristics of the patients (age, gender, education, etc.), current symptomatology and its treatment (especially psychotic features) and long-term course and long-term exposure to medication, psychiatric and somatic comorbidity and alcohol and/or substance abuse.

Background

The neurocognitive dysfunction in BD has been the focus of debate for many years. It was not clear whether the observed neurocognitive deficit could be explained by iatrogenic or alcohol and/or drug abuse effects or by the temporary functional changes which constitute the result of mood changes. Also, it was unclear whether the impairment is the product of degenerative structural brain changes or of some kind of structural changes of a neurodevelopmental origin (trait), or it is secondary to mood dysregulation (state). Recent data suggested that the neurocognitive deficit is not only an enduring component of the illness, but also represents a core primary characteristic of the illness, rather than being secondary to the mood state or medication [1]. It has been suggested by recent data that 84 % of patients suffering from schizophrenia, 58.3 % of psychotic major depressive patients, and 57.7 % of psychotic BD patients are neurocognitively impaired (at least one SD below healthy controls in at least two domains) [2].

It has also been suggested that patients with BD are more creative (e.g., artists, scientists, etc.) and have higher IQ in comparison to the general population [37]. However, more recent data reported a significant and broad neurocognitive deficit, which seems to be present even before the first manifestation of mood symptoms, and it persists across the different phases and even worsens during the course of the illness [813]. Several studies suggest that 40 % of BD patients are impaired in one neurocognitive domain, one-third or more are impaired in at least two neurocognitive domains and 22 % in three or more domains [14, 15]. This deficit is rather stable and relatively independent from mood changes, probably reflecting trait features of BD [1619] Even after controlling for confounding variables, such as education and social class and clinical symptoms, it has been indicated that the neurocognitive impairment in BD is less pronounced in comparison to that in schizophrenia [20, 21].

Gender, age, education, phase of the illness and medication status constitute some of the identified confounding factors. Additionally, patients in a severe depression or mania cannot be tested.

A significant limitation in this kind of research is that the performance in most tests is influenced by more than one neurocognitive process. It is a fact that the boundaries between neurocognitive processes are unclear and no process is completely independent from the others. Different approaches in their classification and nomenclature have been proposed, adding to the confusion. The domain of executive functions, particularly, is open to several different approaches and conceptualizations.

The aim of the current study was to perform a systematic review of the literature to identify all studies pertaining to the topic of neurocognitive dysfunction in BD and afterwards to synthesize the findings in a comprehensive picture.

Review

Materials and methods

Reports investigating the neurocognitive dysfunction in BD patient samples were located with searches in Pubmed/MEDLINE, through June 1st 2015. Only reports in English language were included.

The Pubmed database was searched using the search terms ‘Bipolar’ OR ‘Mania’ OR ‘manic’ OR ‘Manic-depression’ OR Manic-depressive AND ‘Neurocognition’ OR ‘Neurocognitive’ OR ‘Neuropsychology’ OR ‘Neuropsychological’ OR ‘Cognitive’ OR ‘Cognition’ OR ‘Intelligence quotient’ OR ‘IQ’ OR ‘VIQ’ OR ‘PIQ’ OR ‘North American Adult Reading Test’ OR ‘NAART’ OR ‘wide Range Achievement Test’ OR ‘WRAT’ OR ‘Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale’ OR ‘WAIS’ OR ‘Mental speed’ OR ‘Digit Symbol Substitution Test’ OR ‘DSST’ OR ‘Trail Making Test’ OR ‘TMT’ OR ‘Reaction time’ OR ‘Attention’ OR ‘Attentional’ OR ‘Vigilance’ OR ‘Concentration’ OR ‘Continuous Performance Test’ OR ‘CPT’ OR ‘Digits Forward’ OR ‘Learning’ OR ‘Memory’ OR ‘Working memory’ OR ‘Declarative memory’ OR ‘Verbal memory’ OR ‘Non-verbal memory’ OR ‘Visual memory’ OR ‘Logical memory’ OR ‘Autobiographical memory’ OR ‘Prospective memory’ OR ‘Immediate memory’ OR ‘Delayed memory’ OR ‘Verbal learning’ OR ‘Digits Backward’ OR ‘California Verbal Learning Test’ OR ‘CVLT’ OR ‘Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test’ OR ‘RAVLT’ OR ‘Wechsler Memory Scale’ OR ‘WMS’ OR ‘Free recall’ OR ‘Rey Complex Figure Test’ OR ‘RCFT’ OR ‘Verbal skills’ OR ‘Verbal fluency’ OR ‘Category fluency’ OR ‘Letter fluency’ OR ‘Controlled Oral Word Association Test’ OR ‘COWA-FAS’ OR ‘Animal Naming’ OR ‘Visuospatial’ OR ‘Constructional’ OR ‘Block design’ OR ‘Rey Complex Figure Test’ OR ‘RCFT’ OR ‘Clock test’ OR ‘Executive function’ OR ‘Reasoning’ OR ‘Inhibitory control’ OR ‘Executive control’ OR ‘Concept formation’ OR ‘Wisconsin Card Sorting Test’ OR ‘WCST’ OR ‘Stroop Color Word Test’ OR ‘Stroop’ OR ‘SCWT’ OR ‘Theory of Mind’ OR ‘ToM’ OR ‘Emotion processing’ OR ‘Emotional decision-making’ OR ‘Benton Facial Recognition Test’ OR ‘BFRT’ OR ‘Faces Test’ OR ‘Eyes Test’ OR ‘Hinting Task’ OR ‘False belief and deception’ OR ‘Picture sequencing’ OR ‘Character intention’ OR ‘Faux Pas’.

Only papers in English language were included.

This review followed a modified version of the recommendations of the Preferred Items for Reporting of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement [22].

Results

The initial search returned 110,403 papers. Αfter the deletion of duplicates 11,771 remained. The reference lists of review papers and books were scanned and eventually and after assessing these papers on the basis of title and abstract 250 papers remained for further study (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The PRISMA flowchart

A list of neurocognitive domains and the neuropsychological tools usually used for their assessment are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Neurocognitive domains assessed in the literature and neuropsychological tools used

Domain Tool
Premorbid IQ Single-word reading score from the North American Adult Reading Test (NAART)
Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)
Vocabulary subtest score from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)
Current IQ Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)
Psychomotor and mental speed Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)
Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A)
Reaction time tests
Attention Continuous Performance Test (CPT)
Digits forward
Working memory Digits backward
Verbal memory
 Learning California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)
 Short delayed recall Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)
 Long delayed recall Wechsler Memory Scale-Logical Memory (WMS-LM)
 Recognition Free recall
Non-verbal Memory Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT)—Immediate and delayed recall
Wechsler Memory Scale-Visual Reproduction (WMS-VR)
Visuospatial function Block design
Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT)-copy
Language/verbal fluency Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWA-FAS)
Animal naming (AN)
Executive function Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)—categories achieved and perseverative errors
Stroop Color Word Test (SCWT)
Trail Making Test-B (TMT-B)

General neurocognitive functioning and Intelligence quotient (IQ)

It has been reported that both patients with BD and their families have above average IQ and general intellectual functioning [2329], or at least they have intelligence similar to healthy controls [3034]. On the other hand, higher functioning and preserved neurocognitive performance during the premorbid phase, as well as higher social class could push towards a mood disorders diagnosis rather than schizophrenia [25, 30, 3445].

Overall, the literature reports that patients with BD manifest moderate global reduction in their neurocognitive functioning as reflected in their IQ scores and their performance in neuropsychological batteries, irrespective of illness phase [43, 44, 4648]. It has also been shown that these deficits are milder but may be qualitatively similar to those seen in patients with schizophrenia [21, 49]. The impairment is more severe in the presence of psychotic features [50], and, in line with this, it has been suggested that the deficit is comparable to that seen in schizophrenia, especially during the acute psychotic manic phase [5154].

One study suggested the presence of a more complex relationship between BD and IQ since it has been reported that although BD was related to higher premorbid IQ, further analysis revealed that men with the lowest and the highest IQ (especially verbal or practical ability) were at the greatest risk for pure BD (without comorbidity) [55]. There are inconclusive data concerning whether psychotic BD is related to impaired premorbid IQ [56, 57].

It is well known that the full IQ is composed of two composite scores; the Verbal IQ (VIQ) and the non-verbal or Performance IQ (PIQ). The effect size of the deficit during the manic phase is 0.06 and 0.28 for PIQ and VIQ, respectively [53]. It seems that during acute mania the effect size is equal to 0.47 for the full IQ [54]. There are no studies concerning the IQ in patients during the acute depressive phase. Since it is possible, the premorbid IQ of BD patients is higher in comparison to the normal population, the true magnitude of decline cannot be evaluated accurately, and all studies reporting this decline in comparison to population norms underestimate it.

According to a meta-analysis, patients with BD show higher VIQ in comparison to PIQ scores [58, 59]. This is due to the uniform reduction in all PIQ subtest scores with an accompanying preservation of VIQ scores. This impairment is not because of the slowing in mental speed which is observed in BD patients (since four PIQ subtests in comparison to one VIQ subtest are timed) and this is obvious from the quality of the deficit which persists during periods of euthymia, and it is confirmed from targeted research [60]. The deficit might be already present during the early stages of BD [61] although more recent data argue against this [62]. One study has reported that the severity of depression seems to reduce VIQ scores and thus it might diminish the VIQ-PIQ discrepancy [63]. It is important to notice that this discrepancy does not seem to be present in patients with unipolar depression [64].

It has been reported that the VIQ-PIQ discrepancy might reflect a specific effect of BD on ‘fluid intelligence’ (the capacity to think logically and solve problems in novel situations, independently of acquired knowledge) with a simultaneous respect of the ‘crystallized intelligence’ (the ability to use skills, knowledge, and experience; it does rely on long-term memory). However, any attempt for a deep understanding of this deficit is risky and premature [63].

When all phases of the illness are taken into consideration, the effect sizes concerning current IQ reduction range from 0.36 to 070 [63, 65]. For patients in remission, the results of meta-analyses are inconclusive. The effect sizes reported for PIQ range from 0.40 to 0.50 [63, 66] to lower and within the normal range, that is 0.11–0.16 [8, 12]. A problem is that most meta-analyses report an effect size for the full IQ and not separately for VIQ and PIQ. The effect size of premorbid IQ change in euthymic BD patients is reported to be low and not significant (0.04–0.20) [66, 67].

Psychomotor and mental speed

Although mental speed and psychomotor activation are two concepts which overlap and include reaction time, cognitive and motor speed and, manual dexterity, they are clearly not identical. Additionally, most of the neuropsychological tools which are used for the evaluation of psychomotor and mental speed also assess other neurocognitive functions, and this is at least partially a consequence of a methodology effect, because to measure ‘speed’, you need to initiate a ‘procedure’ whose ‘speed’ is going to be measured.

It has been reported that the reaction time in bipolar depressive patients is prolonged [68]. Also, euthymic patients had not only prolonged reaction times but also more error rates in a visual backward masking test. Overall, reaction time prolongation has been found to be associated with burden of illness and especially past history of depressions but not with current medication [69].

Irrespective of illness phase and symptom severity, the speed of mental processing in BD patients seems to be slower [34, 4345, 59, 7087]. Moreover, it has been reported that the speed of mental processing is less affected in comparison to schizophrenia [38, 40, 61, 88], although there are some data suggesting the presence of a similar degree of impairment between patients with BD and patients with schizophrenia [89]. On the other hand, it has been suggested that there were no significant differences between depressed BD patients, euthymic BD patients and healthy controls in psychomotor speed [90]. Another study suggested that patients with BD showed a linear improvement in processing speed in the first year after resolution of their initial manic episode [91]. It has been reported that bipolar depressives manifest slower mental speed in comparison to manic and unipolar depressives, even after corrected for motor speed. The interesting feature in that particular study was that distraction improved the mental speed of BD depressed patients while it adversely influenced the speed of the other two groups [92].

The deficit could be present already during the early stages of BD [93]. Individual studies suggested that the magnitude of mental speed impairment in patients with BD is reported to correspond to an effect size of 0.82–1.08 [48, 77, 94]. When all phases of the illness are taken into consideration, the effect size 0.50–0.55 (which is similar to that observed concerning the IQ) [63, 65]. In euthymic BD patients, one meta-analysis reported that the effect size with the use of the TMT-A was 0.52 and with the DSST was 0.59 [13]. A second study reported effect sizes of 0.60 and 0.79, respectively [67], a third one reported effect sizes equal to 0.64 and 0.76 [12], a fourth 0.71 and 0.84 [8] and a fifth 0.7 and 07–0.8, respectively [66].

It is important to note that the deficit in the processing speed might have a significant confounding effect on the performance in almost all neurocognitive testing and controlling for it might make any difference between groups concerning other neurocognitive domains disappear [95]. Also, it is suggested that global functional impairment is strongly associated with poor performance on a cognitive measure of processing speed (e.g., WAIS Digit Symbol or the TMT) [75, 96].

Attention

Attention is a concept that includes a number of processes which work together, influence one another or prerequisite one another. These processes are: working memory (which refers to the ability to keep a limited number of mental objects in awareness for a limited duration of time), vigilance (which is the capacity to identify a specific target among many other stimuli), freedom from distraction or interference and the ability to split or to rapidly shift attention. Concentration is a term which refers to the ability to sustain attention over prolonged periods of time. There are many tests, with each of them assessing one of the previously mentioned processes. For example, the Continuous Performance Test assesses vigilance while ‘span tasks’ assess working memory. However, all these tests except from the specific aspect of attention they assess are also influenced from the other processes which are related to attention as well. Working memory is often classified as belonging to the executive functions and it is often considered in relation to them.

The magnitude of the attentional impairment is independent of current symptomatology and of the phase of the illness; however, significant variability is present in the literature [34, 44, 48, 71, 85, 87, 91, 97113]. It has been also reported that the impairment is present already during the early stages of the disorder [93] and it is less pronounced in comparison to the deficit seen in patients with schizophrenia [38, 114, 115]. On the other hand, however, some studies reported that there is a similar magnitude of impairment between patient with BD and patients with schizophrenia [89, 116, 117]. It has been suggested that the performance in divided attention (DA) varied considerable over time within patients. It was also found a significant quadratic relationship between manic symptoms and DA performance, even after corrected for the effect of psychotropic medication. It has been suggested that mild hypomanic symptoms have a positive influence on divided attention scores and moderate to severe manic symptoms have a negative influence. No association between depressive symptoms and DA performance was found [118]. The magnitude of effect sizes ranges from 0.36 to 0.82 [48, 94, 119] depending on the domain assessed and the composition of the study sample.

The overall effect size calculated after a meta-analysis is 0.64 which is similar to that reported concerning the rest of neurocognitive functions [63]. Another meta-analysis reported that attention is impaired during the acute manic/mixed state, with effect sizes ranging from 0.79 to 0.90, during the acute depressed state with an effect size up to 0.80, but also during euthymia with effect size from 0.41 to 0.65 [11]. Another meta-analysis concerning BD patients during euthymia has reported medium effect sizes (0.48–0.60 depending on the testing condition) [13], while a third one found effect sizes of similar magnitude (0.62 and 0.74 for CPT hits and reaction time, respectively) [67]. A fourth meta-analysis gave effect size 0.58 for CPT and 0.37 for digits forward [8] and a fifth reported an effect size equal to 0.8 for CPT [66].

There are also a limited number of studies which indicate no impairment in attention [79, 120123], and this concerns especially euthymic patients [124]. One study has found that depressed BD patients, euthymic BD patients and healthy controls had no significant differences in attention [90].

Learning and memory

Learning refers to the ability to acquire and store new information. Memory is the mental process that allows individuals to retrieve the new information at a later time. Learning and memory involve a number of processes including attention and concentration, encoding and allocation of effort. These processes are distinct from one another but interrelated and interdependent. Moreover, there are different strategies and processes involved, depending on whether a short- or a long-term effect is desirable and also depending on the quality and nature of the information and the frame it is presented in.

A typical classification of learning and memory structure is shown in Table 2 [125]. Due to the fact that much of research on memory is focused on ‘depression’ and does not distinguish between unipolar and bipolar depression, the results and the conclusions from these studies should be received with reservation because it is uncertain whether they apply specifically to BD, and to which extent.

Table 2.

Effect sizes concerning the various neurocognitive domains during different phases of BD as well as in high-risk relatives (endophenotypes)

Domain All phases Acute mania Acute bipolar depression Euthymia Endophenotype
Intelligence Quotient (IQ)
 Premorbid IQ Normal Normal
 Current IQ 0.36–0.70 028–0.47 0.11–0.50 0.20
Psychomotor and mental speed 0.50–0.55 0.52–0.80 0.17–022
Attention 0.64 0.79–0.90 0.80 0.41–0.80 0.18–0.36
Memory
 Working memory 0.60 0.54–1.02
 Verbal memory
  Immediate 0.43 0.73–0.82 0.33–0.42
  Delayed 0.34 1.05 1.20 0.71–0.85 0.27–0.33
  Verbal learning 0.91 1.43 0.66–0.90 0.28
 Non-Verbal memory
  Immediate 0.26 0.73
  Delayed 0.51 0.62–0.80 0.13
 Episodic memory 0.62
Visuospatial function 0.65 022–0.57
Language/verbal fluency 0.63 0.51–0.59 0.93 0.34–0.90 0.27
Executive function 0.34–0.79 0.64–0.72 0.54-0.75 0.52–0.88 0.24–0.51
Social cognition
 ToM 0.75–0.86
 Emotion recognition 0.35
 Emotion decision-making Normal

The range of values reflects heterogeneity in study samples but also heterogeneity because of the different neuropsychological tools used

It has been suggested that there is a deficit in working memory [45, 57, 72, 73, 77, 8487, 126129] and specifically in the visuospatial working memory [33, 130134]. Psychotic patients might have worse performance [48, 135]. The impairment is probably present already since the early stages of the illness [136]. Some studies have shown that the impairment in working memory affects only patients with acute mania [53, 131, 132]. Comparing to patients with schizophrenia, the impairment in visuospatial working memory is less pronounced [115].

Additionally, there are studies indicating a deficit in declarative memory [137] and specifically in the semantic [138] and the episodic memory [48, 109, 139, 140], verbal learning [45, 47, 72, 86, 141147] and this is true both for BD-I and BD-II [134, 148]. A deficit is also present concerning verbal memory [38, 41, 73, 75, 87, 102, 111, 143, 144, 147, 149151], also during periods of euthymia [152]. One study, however, has shown that depressed BD patients showed greater impairments in verbal memory than the euthymic BD patients [90]. It has been suggested that the magnitude of the effect size of the verbal memory deficit is 0.7–0.9 [41, 139] and it is less pronounced in comparison to that seen in schizophrenia [153, 154] and cannot be explained by the attentional deficit [120]. Patients at early stages of BD have better performance in the total immediate free recall and in delayed free recall compared to patients at a later stage and to patients with schizophrenia. Additionally, concerning the ability to retain words learned, BD patients at a later stage and chronic patients with schizophrenia were more impaired than BD patients at early stage and patients with recent onset schizophrenia [155]. It has been also indicated that delayed free recall is worse in patients with BD compared to healthy controls [156].

There are impairments in total learning as well as short- and long-delay verbal recall, recognition, discriminability and learning slope [157], associative learning [158], implicit motor learning [159], immediate memory [134], delayed memory [34, 45, 72, 77, 84, 85, 94, 108, 160], non-verbal memory [161], visual memory [38, 43, 44, 74, 79, 102, 108, 111], autobiographical [162, 163] and prospective memory [164]. It has been shown that prospective memory deficits [165] and short-term non-affective memory [166] are also present in remitted BD patients suggesting that they constitute a trait deficit. Moreover, it has been shown that BD patients manifest a deficit in incidental contextual memory in the absence of a binding cue at encoding. There was no difference found between the groups for contextual memory even under incidental encoding with the binding cue. One study has indicated that the impairment in the contextual memory was reduced by providing cognitive support at encoding [167].

There are also negative studies concerning the presence of impairment in working memory [168170], spatial working memory [171] and in verbal learning [138] and verbal [123, 172, 173] and visual memory [173]. One study has shown that the ability to learn is maintained both by BD patients and by patients with schizophrenia [155] and another study has reported that there were no differences between BD patients and healthy controls in terms of their slope of learning, retrieval index, retention percentage, semantic or serial clustering, errors, or level of retrieval [174]. It seems that most memory impairments are due to the presence of confounding variables except maybe for verbal recall [101]. The possibility that difficulties in the semantic clustering or other strategic processing deficits are the cause for the verbal memory impairment has both positive [139, 160] and negative data [137, 139, 161, 175].

In meta-analytic studies, when all phases of the illness are taken into consideration, the magnitude of the effect size is 0.60 for working memory, 0.43 for immediate verbal memory and 0.34 for delayed, 0.26 for immediate visual memory and 0.51 for delayed [65]. The meta-analysis of short-term memory studies revealed an effect size of 0.58 when span tasks were utilized, without any difference between auditory or visual tasks. When verbal learning tasks were used, the effect size was 0.91 [63].

During the acute manic/mixed state, the magnitude of the effect sizes was 1.43 for verbal learning and 1.05 for delayed free verbal recall. Additionally, concerning the acute depressed state, the effect size for verbal memory was 1.20, while during euthymia the domains impaired were working memory (0.65), verbal learning (0.81), long-delay verbal free recall (0.78), immediate non-verbal memory (0.73) and delayed non-verbal recall (0.80) [11].

The average effect size for episodic memory in euthymic BD patients is reported to be equal to 0.62 and for working memory equal to 0.60 [12]. Again in euthymic BD patients, a large effect size (0.90) is reported for verbal learning and working memory (0.98) and somewhat lower effect sizes for aspects of immediate (0.73) and delayed (0.71) verbal memory [13]. A third meta-analysis reported effect sizes of 0.81, 0.54, 0.74 and 0.72, respectively [67]. Another meta-analysis reported an effect size of 1.02 for working memory. 0.85 for delayed recall, 0.82 for immediate recall and 0.62 for visual memory [8]. Finally, a last meta-analysis gave an effect size of 0.85 for verbal learning, and 0.73 for verbal memory-early recall [9, 66]. It seems there is a publication bias especially concerning verbal learning and after correction for this, the effect size is attenuated (from 0.85 down to 0.66) [9].

Overall, the literature suggests that BD, irrespective of illness phase, is characterized by a severe deficit in the acquisition of new information, but not in the retention [41, 63, 161, 175, 176]. In spite of some opposing data, the most probable interpretation which derives from empirical studies is that the attention and concentration deficits impair the acquisition of information and learning, by disrupting the engagement of effortful processing which results in a shallow rather than deeper level of processing (e.g., acoustic rather than semantic) [137, 175, 177186].

Verbal skills

The evaluation of verbal skills includes mostly the evaluation of verbal fluency. Although the literature has reported that verbal skills are impaired during all phases of BD [84, 87, 108, 129, 147, 149, 166, 169, 187189], there are some studies reporting that this impairment is not present during euthymia [41, 52] or during the first mood episode [121]. One study has indicated that there are errors in speech during the acute manic state, and these errors are independent from the co-existence of an attentional deficit [106].

Concerning the magnitude of the effect size, it has been reported that it is small [169] and even smaller in comparison to the effect size that seen in schizophrenia [61], however, patients with psychotic BD have a higher effect size (0.68–1.73) [94, 117, 190]. Even smaller (below 0.50) effect sizes have been reported [63], while when all phases of the illness are taken into consideration, an effect size equal to 0.63 emerges [65]. Both letter fluency and semantic fluency are impaired during the acute manic/mixed state and the effect sizes are equal to 0.51 and 0.59, respectively. The phonemic fluency is impaired during the acute depressed state with an effect size equal to 0.93 while during euthymia impaired are both phonemic fluency (0.51), and semantic verbal fluency (0.75) [11]. In euthymic BD patients, the average effect size for verbal fluency ranges between 0.56 and 0.90 [12, 67] but it is smaller (0.34) for verbal fluency by letter [13].

Visuospatial skills

The evaluation of visuospatial skills is usually made with the use of the complex Rey figure or with the WAIS-R block design. Patients with BD and their unaffected relatives show impairment in the visuospatial/constructional abilities [45, 147, 191] and in visual learning and memory [191, 192]. It is interesting that euthymic BD patients and patients with schizophrenia have similar impairment when the results are controlled for possible confounding factors [193]. Also, one study has shown that BD patients were significantly impaired on all three object location memory processes (positional memory, object-location binding, and a combined process), with the largest effect found in exact positional memory (d = 1.18) [194]. Unaffected relatives demonstrated an intermediate level of performance in comparison to patients with BD and to normal controls [195]. Contrawise, one study suggested that the visual motion integration is intact in BD patients [196]. Some authors suggest that the impairment in visuospatial skills is restricted in the acute phase while these skills might not be affected during remission [41, 63]. One study has reported that the overall effect size is equal to 0.65 [63] and other studies have reported that in remitted patients the effect size is 0.22–0.57 [8, 12].

Executive function

The executive system is considered to be involved in the planning, the decision-making, the error correction and the troubleshooting, in situations where responses are not well rehearsed or contain novel sequences of actions, are dangerous or constitute technically difficult situations or situations requiring the overcoming of a strong habitual response or resisting temptation. In other words, ‘Controlling of mental and neurocognitive processes’ seems to be the key phrase describing the role of executive functions. In patients with BD, reasoning should be considered separately from the rest executive functions due to the fact that it seems to rely heavily on verbal and linguistic skills [63].

It has been suggested a severe impairment in executive functions except reasoning during all phases of BD [34, 36, 4345, 48, 70, 72, 73, 7577, 86, 87, 90, 94, 101103, 108, 109, 111, 120122, 129, 143, 146, 149, 190, 195, 197209] and early during the course of BD [93], but the deficit is less pronounced in comparison to schizophrenia [38, 40, 61, 89, 116, 122, 210]. However, at least a subgroup of patients is as severely impaired as patients with schizophrenia [153, 211, 212]. One study indicates that the depressed BD patients showed greater impairments in executive functions comparing to the euthymic BD patients [90]. It is suggested that this impairment might be particularly severe concerning interference and inhibitory control [47, 80, 82, 108, 168, 190, 192, 213215]. Additionally, patients with BD might have more risky [216] or erratic choices [217, 218] especially when a history of alcohol abuse exists [219].

However, not all data are straightforward. Normal overall executive function has been suggested by one study [144], while another reported only prolonged time to complete the test [207]. Also, a bimodal distribution of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) scores in patients with BD is reported, with some patients at near-control levels and others significantly impaired [41]. According to another study, the performance on the executive function measures is bimodal among euthymic BD patients (one subgroup with relatively normal and one subgroup with impaired executive functioning) [41]. Some authors argue that there is no difference between BD patients and controls in executive functions [78, 123, 170, 188, 220, 221], while others argue that the deficit is present only in the more severe and chronic cases [188]. It has been also indicated that BD patients show an improvement in the executive functions in the first year after resolution of their initial manic episode [91].

The deficit in executive functions might be overall independent from illness phase, however, there are data suggesting that some aspects of the deficit are related to affective lability [222], duration of illness, residual mood symptoms and current antipsychotic treatment [197] and history of psychosis [48]. One study found that the impairment in executive function was related to the severity of general psychopathology as it is measured by the PANSS [198] and another one correlated impaired insight with impaired executive functions [223].

A meta-analysis suggested that when taking all phases of the illness together, the effect size of this impairment is equal to 0.79 when reasoning (which was reported to be intact) is excluded [63]. Moreover, another meta-analysis reported an effect size of 0.34 for concept formation and of 0.55 for executive control, with no significant effect for current clinical condition [65]. During the acute manic/mixed state, the general executive function and the speeded set-shifting are impaired with the magnitude of effect sizes to be equal to 0.72 and to 0.64, respectively. During the acute depressed state, speeded set-shifting is impaired (0.64) while during euthymia problem-solving tasks (0.54), set-switching tasks (0.73) and verbal interference (0.75) are impaired [11]. Three recent meta-analytic studies in euthymic patients reported that the effect sizes were for executive functioning equal to 0.80 for the TMT-B, 0.56 for the WCST and 0.80 for the Stroop test [12] according to the first one, 0.78, 0.62 and 0.63, respectively, according to the second one [13] and 0.55, 0.69 and 0.71, respectively, according to a third meta-analysis [67]. Another meta-analysis reported an effect size of 0.99 for the TMT-B and 0.88 for the WCST perseverative errors and 0.52 for the WCST categories, 0.73 for the Stroop time and 0.65 for the stroop-correct [8]. Similarly, another study reported 0.70 for the WCST perseveration score and 0.8 for the TMT-B and the stroop test [9, 66].

Social cognition and theory of mind (ToM)

The term ‘social cognition’ constitutes a psychological domain with several dimensions. It refers not only to the ability of the person to assume that other people have minds similar to his/her own and to interpret, but also to understand and predict the emotions, desires, intentions, behaviors and speech of others (including non-verbal elements). Social cognition shapes communication and interaction with others and in this way enabling adaptive social adaptation. It involves a complex set of processes including the representation of internal somatic states, knowledge about the self, perception of others, and interpersonal motivations [224].

The broad theory of mind (ToM) includes three main processes (a narrow definition of ToM, emotion processing, and affective decision-making). The narrow definition of ToM (mentalizing or mindreading) refers to the ability to attribute mental states (e.g., beliefs, desires, and intents) to oneself and to others. Emotion processing is the ability to identify and discriminate basic emotions. Affective decision-making is crucial for an appropriate social behavior, and concerns weighing up choices in association with reward and punishment [225].

The tests which are used to evaluate these domains are both verbal (scenarios) and non-verbal (pictures). They demand the subject to identify and comprehend the situation, the roles and the interactions and to make appropriate planning. So far, empirical data have confirmed the universality of facial emotions. This means that the specific ability to process and identify facial emotions is a substantial feature of human communication and social interaction, which is independent of culture. It has been found, even in “ecological” tests that mimic real life scenarios that patients with BD showed less impairment on neurocognitive performance compared to patients with schizophrenia [154].

ToM

It has been shown that here is a deficit concerning ToM and social cognition during all phases of BD [162, 201, 216, 226232], although there are also negative studies [233, 234]. It is possible that after controlling for medication and other confounding factors, the performance of patients is the same with the performance of controls [231]. There is also evidence that the impairment in ToM is restricted to the acute phases of the illness even when memory was controlled for [134] and thus there is no impairment during remission [235]. Moreover, patients with BD with psychotic features and patients with schizophrenia were found to be equally impaired in their scores for ToM stories. However, patients with schizophrenia manifested a worse performance [154, 236]. Another study reported no impairment in the accuracy of responses, but only the presence of a prolonged latency time for the response [237].

Generally, the literature supports the presence of a robust deficit in ToM in BD patients [78, 216, 232, 238241]. This, in spite of one study which has shown that there are subtle deficits [229] and a second one which has reported no deficit at all [242]. Probably, the impairment in ToM in BD is associated with mood symptoms and it might reflect more fundamental underlying neurocognitive deficits rather than representing a specific trait marker of the disorder [134].

Emotion processing

There are inconclusive data concerning the recognition of emotions in BD patients. On one hand, there are studies indicating an impairment in emotion recognition, and in the identification and discrimination of emotions even during remission [193, 209, 229, 231, 240, 242251]; on the other hand, however, there are studies reporting no impairment in these domains, especially after controlling for medication and other confounding variables [78, 231, 238, 239, 252257].

Patients with BD I, seem to be more impaired in comparison to controls on face emotional recognition (FER) fear subtests, happiness, on the surprise test and on FER total scores [209]. It has been also reported that patients with BD do not have any impairment in face recognition in general, but the impairment exists specifically in the facial affect labeling, even during euthymia [246, 258]. Maybe specific phases affect specific emotions. For instance, in acute mania, the recognition of fear and disgust is reported to be impaired [247] while during euthymia patients recognize disgust better [259]. Reduced biases in the emotion recognition are related with acute bipolar depression while on the contrary increased biases in emotion recognition are related with acute mania [260]. This mood-congruent bias is state rather than trait and contributes as a core characteristic [245, 258, 261]. Younger BD participants performed worse than expected relative to healthy comparison participants of similar age. The deficits were found both concerning child and adult faces and were particularly strong for angry child faces, which were most often undertaken as sad. The results of this particular study were not influenced by medications, comorbidities/substance use, or mood state/global functioning [262].

Emotional decision-making

The literature reports that there is little or no difference between patients with BD and controls on the emotional decision-making component [207, 216, 218, 231].

Reviews and meta-analyses

One meta-analysis reported an effect size concerning ToM equal to 0.75–0.86 and concerning emotion processing equal to 0.35. The same analysis found that there was no difference between patients with BD and healthy controls concerning the emotional decision-making. That specific meta-analysis suggested that the performance in ToM and in emotion recognition was not associated with years of education, age, sex, duration of illness, and medication [225].

The significant heterogeneity in the results may be due to the differences among studies. These differences concern the neuropsychological tools used and the study samples. The specific tests might play a significant role since the performance of BD patients might be similar to controls in some aspects of emotion recognition but other aspects could be impaired or not, depending on the clinical state. For example, it has been found that stable BD patients might exhibit impaired facial emotion discrimination [193], while on the contrary the depressed BD patients’ performance could be similar to healthy controls in the perception of chimeric faces. In contrast, manic patients might perceive all chimeric faces as positive [263]. It is interesting that patients might outperform normal controls in specific domains (e.g., euthymic BD patients in the recognition of disgust) [259]. Depressed BD patients might show impairment only in the most difficult tasks [248]. It is important to be noted, however, that the effect of the clinical state is controversial since it is not consistent across studies [201, 216]. An additional problem is the fact that these are highly sophisticated neurocognitive functions and depend on the intact performance of lower ones; for example, the recognition of facial affect requires a compilation of attentional, executive and emotional abilities. Thus, it is difficult to be determined where exactly is the impairment located within these mechanisms [249].

In this frame, it is interesting that it has been found not only that the impairment in ToM is independent of other neurocognitive dysfunctions [201, 244] but also that this impairment is unrelated to a history of psychotic symptoms [227]. Of course, there are data supporting the opposite opinion as well, which seems more reasonable. Even in euthymic BD, executive dysfunction and some other neurocognitive impairments such as basic emotion recognition might be at least partially responsible for the impairment in ToM and social cognition tests [228].

A number of factor have been identified as contributing to of confounding the deficit. Low level of education and family history of BD might predict this impairment [216]. Moreover, the perception of emotion may be affected by the use of psychotropic medication and particularly by the use of benzodiazepines and both use and non-use of antidepressants. In healthy subjects, benzodiazepines impair the recognition of anger, citalopram and reboxetine reduce the perception of negative expressions and propranolol increases the reaction time to recognize sadness [216, 258].

Conclusively, although the available data suffer from significant methodological drawbacks, the literature suggests that there is an impairment in ToM in patients with BD [264]. Additionally, the theory that the deficit in emotional recognition occurs due to an impairment located in right hemisphere does not seem to be strong or sufficient [265] and any neurobiological dysfunction is likely to be state-dependent [266]

Clinical correlations

Neuropsychological dysfunction in BD may also be related to the clinical symptom pattern and severity, and it has been correlated with age, earlier age at onset, medication status, as well as with idiosyncratic factors affecting the long-term course.

The effect of medication

Medication constitutes an important confounding variable when comparing the different phases of BD. Some acutely ill patients might be medication-free during testing, however, this is not the case with patients in remission. As a result, medication status not only constitutes a confounding variable which is difficult to control for, but also might introduce a bias towards the detection of a deficit, especially in patients in remission. On the other hand, however, patients with severe mania or severe depression cannot be tested and are rarely off medication. Medication could be a possible reason why patients with BD have poor performance on certain neurocognitive tasks. This is in accord with the traditional concept that BD is considered to belong to the ‘functional psychoses’. According to this approach, the attentional impairment is considered to be the core neurocognitive deficit and the cause of all other deficits in neurocognition.

Overall, medication is considered to be an important factor, especially given also the possible neuroprotective or neurotoxic effect of several agents. For example, while most authors argue that lithium is neuroprotective, there is a possible neurotoxic effect in the long term, even at therapeutic levels, especially when it is prescribed in combination with antipsychotics [267].

It is of prime importance to differentiate the neurocognitive deficit caused by the illness itself and the deficit which could be medication induced. This differentiation determines not only the long-term therapeutical design but also the overall outcome. Moreover, such a differentiation requires a comprehensive assessment, on the basis of knowledge of those neurocognitive domains which are most affected by specific medication agents [268].

Patients under lithium often report that lithium inhibits their productivity and creativity [269]. There is no specific reason for this although it has been shown that lithium reduces unusual associations, and this could be the possible underlying mechanism [269]. On the other hand, however, it is not clear whether this constitutes a true deficit or it reflects a subjective feeling as a consequence of the transition from the manic/hypomanic to the euthymic state. It should be noted that this loss of creativity might be specifically related to lithium and not to divalproex [270].

Additionally, it seems that lithium has a negative impact on neurocognition especially on memory and psychomotor functioning [271275] but fortunately the insult does not seem to be cumulative [276]. More specifically, it has been found that lithium impairs both mental and motor speed, short-term memory, and verbal or associative fluency, but the impairment is reversible when lithium is withdrawn and re-establishes when lithium is re-administered [272, 277, 278]. Lithium also causes a deficit in the long-term recall (retrieval) without having an effect on attention or on encoding [271, 278281]. This impairment might especially concern verbal memory [94, 282]. Fortunately, it has been shown that cognitive complaints do not seem to be significant predictors of discontinuation of lithium treatment [283, 284].

There are limited data which disagree with the suggestion that lithium causes a significant neurocognitive impairment [285]. These data especially argue that lithium has no adverse effect on the reaction time [69] and executive functions [221]. Moreover, a longitudinal study found no evidence for a neurocognitive deterioration over a 6-year period in a sample of BD patients treated with lithium [276].

Overall, it seems that the effect size of the neurocognitive deficit related to lithium treatment is small and equal to 0.30 [8]. This is especially true concerning immediate verbal learning and memory (0.24), creativity (0.33) and psychomotor speed (0.62). Delayed verbal memory, visual memory, attention and executive function might not be affected at all [286].

The data on the possible deleterious effect of antipsychotics and antiepileptics on neurocognition are rare and conflicting [268, 287, 288]. Valproate and carbamazepine might cause an impairment in attention [289]. Topiramate, which is an agent which is not used in the treatment of BD per se, but is often administered in patients to treat a comorbid substance abuse disorder or to lose weight, impairs verbal memory, attention, causes psychomotor slowing, and impairs word finding even at very low dosages (25–50 mg/day). This deficit is reversible after discontinuation of the drug [277, 290].

In general, neuroleptics cause impairment in sustained attention and in visuomotor speed [291]. Moreover, even after controlling for clinical features, current antipsychotic treatment is related to worse performance across all executive function tests as well as in verbal learning and recognition memory and in semantic fluency in BD patients [41, 197, 292]. One study did not find any adverse effects concerning risperidone [293]. While another one suggested that years of exposure to antipsychotic medication were related to the impairment in executive functions [108]. It is unclear whether this deficit constitutes a true medication adverse effect or it is the consequence of the manifestation of psychotic symptoms, for the treatment of which, antipsychotics were prescribed.

Overall, it has been shown that medications have a limited adverse effect on neurocognitive function [294] if any at all [295297]. On the contrary, there seems to be a close relationship between poor treatment adherence and neurocognitive impairment, but the causal inferences of these findings are uncertain. It is unclear whether it is the poor treatment adherence which leads to a worse neurocognitive performance through worsening of the overall course of BD, or, on the contrary, it is the neurocognitive impairment which causes poor treatment adherence and reflects a more severe form of the illness [298].

It is known that patients with BD are often treated with benzodiazepines which interfere with memory [299]. Also, some patients are treated with complex combinations of lithium, antipsychotics, antiepileptics, antidepressants, and benzodiazepines, and the combinatorial effects of these drugs on neurocognition are a matter of speculation rather than research.

Medication probably causes some degree of neurocognitive deficit particularly in sustained attention and in psychomotor speed [9]. It is difficult to differentiate this impairment from the impairment caused by the illness per se. It has been reported by studies comparing euthymic patients with or without medication that there are little effects of medication on neurocognitive test performance [104, 300]. It has also been mentioned that patients which were assessed during their first episode, thus before any exposure to medication drugs, also showed a neurocognitive impairment that was more or less similar, to the impairment observed in chronically medicated patients [301]. However, there is one study which reported that medicated BD-II patients performed worse in sustained attention than unmedicated BD-II patients [287].

In those individuals with full inter-episode remission doing well off medication, this adverse effect might be obvious, however it is also well known that staying off medication will adversely affect the overall course of the illness. It is unfortunate that no currently known pharmacotherapy improves neurocognition in BD substantially. Preliminary findings suggest some potential value for adjunctive stimulants such as modafinil and novel experimental agents [268].

The effect of psychotic symptoms

It has been shown that the presence of psychotic symptoms is strongly related to a worse overall neurocognitive performance [48, 57, 83, 84, 94, 100, 108, 129, 190, 227, 302307] and in psychotic BD patients various aspects of the neurocognitive impairment are similar in magnitude to those observed in patients with schizophrenia [85, 133, 307]. Some negative studies exist as well [66, 77, 190, 227] and they suggest that the deficit in BD is overall less pronounced in comparison with schizophrenia. However, in several neurocognitive domains such as working memory and executive function, the deficit is similar to that seen in schizophrenia although as a general profile the neurocognitive deficit in psychotic BD patients is similar to that seen in unipolar psychotic depression [66].

A meta-analysis reported that patients with BD perform better than patients with schizophrenia, and the effect sizes of the difference varied between 0.26 and 0.63 for IQ, mental speed, verbal working memory, immediate visual memory, verbal fluency (with had the largest effect size equal to 0.63), executive control, and concept formation, but without any difference concerning the rest of domains [65]. It is important to note that there were only quantitative, and not qualitative, differences. Significant heterogeneity in effect sizes was present between studies, and this was partially due to the methodological issues and the size and clinical characteristics of the study samples [307].

Even in the absence of current psychotic symptoms, a history of psychotic features is also strongly related to a worse neurocognitive performance [129, 188], especially concerning measures of executive functioning, verbal and spatial working memory [48, 84]. However, it does not seem to exist a complete categorical distinction between psychotic and non-psychotic BD since this effect is modest [308]. The presence of family history correlates with a worse visuomotor attention in psychotic BD patients [309]; however, this load is less severe in comparison with schizophrenia since the offspring of mothers with BD has less neurocognitive impairment in comparison to offspring of mothers with schizophrenia [310]. Probably depending on the study sample (proportion of BD-I and BD-II patients) and the definition of psychosis, the results vary, and are suggestive of a nosological continuum between psychotic and non-psychotic bipolar cases. It is interesting that such a history of psychotic symptoms is inversely related to the neurocognitive function in the patients’ relatives [311].

Another meta-analysis reported that between patients with and without a history of psychotic symptoms, there were no differences concerning attention and visual memory. The observed differences in global IQ, mental speed, working memory, planning and reasoning and executive functions are small and only after excluding one outlier study [227], the effect size in the executive functions increases to 0.55 [308].

It is possible that the neurocognitive differences between psychotic and non-psychotic BD patients are in fact the result of an earlier onset of illness or medication use rather than a result of psychosis per se. In accord with this, one meta-analysis of cases suggested that psychotic BD patients in the above studies had a younger age of illness onset, more hospital admissions and a larger proportion of them were using antipsychotics. Also, they had lower education [308].

Schizoaffective patients perform poorer in a global way, maybe because current psychotic symptoms or history of psychosis is related to more severe neurocognitive impairment no matter the specific diagnosis [83, 129, 312]. Euthymic and stabilized schizoaffective patients are reported to perform worse than BD patients in attention, concentration, declarative memory, executive function, and perceptuomotor function [312, 313]

The effect of mood symptoms

Data are not available for very severely manic or depressed patients because most of these patients cannot be tested. Additionally, extrapolating conclusions on these patients from the study of less severe cases is problematic.

The overall severity of mood symptoms might not affect memory performance at least in those patients whose neurocognitive functions can be assessed [175]. There are studies showing no effect of either acute phase on the neurocognition of patients with BD [71, 142]. However, the bulk of the literature suggests that acute mania is associated to impulse control [314] and executive function impairments [315]. Acute bipolar depression is related with an attentional bias [287] with lowering of mental speed and impaired attention in general [100, 316]. There is also a verbal fluency [71], verbal recall and fine motor skills deficit [101]. It has been found impaired performance on theory of mind tests in both acutely depressed and manic patients, even when memory was controlled for [235]. The fluctuation in both manic and depressive symptoms have an appreciable impact on neurocognitive functioning [11]. While moderate changes in affective symptoms did not co-vary with neurocognitive ability [317] when changes are more severe or when rapid-cycling emerges, there is neurocognitive dysfunction [318].

It has been shown by a number of research studies and meta-analyses that in many domains there is a severe neurocognitive deficit even during remission [8, 9, 66, 307, 319], although there are also studies which keep some reservations [235, 320]. One meta-analysis showed that the illness phase had no effect on the short-term memory deficit [63] and some authors indicate that the consequence of antipsychotic therapy alone might be responsible for the impairment observed during the euthymic phase [292].

The data concerning the impairment in executive functions are inconclusive since there are studies suggesting that the deficit is independent from illness phase, however, there are reports indicating that some aspects of it are related to affective lability [222], duration of illness, residual mood symptoms, current antipsychotic treatment [197] and history of psychosis [48]. A relationship between the impairment in executive function with the severity of general psychopathology as it is measured by the PANSS [198] and with impaired insight have been reported [223]. Finally, differences in the neurocognitive performance could be the result of differences in somatic comorbidity (and co-medication) between BD patients and normal controls [321].

An important methodological problem is that the definition criteria for euthymia differ among studies and as a result, conclusions are suspect. Maybe even subsyndromal conditions might affect verbal memory [120] and also the presence of residual mood symptoms, regardless of polarity, might have a negative impact, on measures of attentional interference [197]. The impairment in ideation fluency is associated with residual mania, but not depression [285]. Our picture concerning the presence of the neurocognitive deficit during euthymia might change after controlling for residual symptoms in stabilized euthymic patients [19, 97, 192].

One study reported that apart from current symptomatology, the number of previous hospitalizations and family history of mood disorder are associated with the impairment in memory in patients with BD [137]. Particularly, the impairment in verbal memory might relate to the presence of subsyndromal mood symptoms, the duration of illness and the numbers of previous manic episodes, suicide attempts and hospitalizations [120, 144, 149].

The first follow-up study found that from patients without any neurocognitive impairment at first episode, one-third had significant deficits after 5–7 years [322]. The overall picture could be more complex since during the first episode of the illness, in non-psychotic patients, there might be no neurocognitive deficits at all, while, patients with psychotic features manifest a deficit comparable to that seen in patients with schizophrenia [302]. After the first episode, the time to recover was associated with executive function and possibly with verbal fluency [323].

A meta-analysis confirmed that neurocognitive impairment is present during all phases of BD. That meta-analysis calculated the respected effect sizes for specific neurocognitive domains separately for each phase. During the acute manic/mixed states, these effect sizes showed a clear impairment in attention (0.79–0.90), verbal learning (1.43) and delayed free verbal recall (1.05), letter fluency (0.51) and semantic fluency (0.59), general executive function (0.72) and speeded set-shifting (0.64). During acute bipolar depression, there were impairments in attention (0.80), verbal memory (1.20), phonemic fluency (0.93) and executive function in speeded set-shifting (0.64). During the euthymic phase, these effect sizes showed a clear impairment in auditory (0.41) and sustained visual vigilance (0.69) and speeded visual scanning (0.65), working memory (0.65), verbal learning (0.81) and long-delay verbal free recall (0.78), executive functions concerning problem-solving tasks (0.54), verbal interference (0.75) and set-switching tasks (0.73), immediate non-verbal memory (0.73), delayed non-verbal recall (0.80), visuospatial function (0.55), phonemic (0.51), and semantic (0.75) verbal fluency and finally in psychomotor speed (0.66) [11]. Overall, these results suggested that patients in a manic or depressed state had significantly greater effect size impairment in verbal learning than patients in an euthymic state.

The overall evidence suggests that the observed neurocognitive deficit in BD patients is not secondary and does not constitute a by-product of mood symptomatology or of exposure to medication. This is in spite of the observed strong relationship between mood symptoms and neurocognitive impairment. The most probable explanation is that neurocognitive impairment reflects a deeper neurobiological dysfunction which probably includes the presence of premorbid developmental abnormalities [324].

The effect of age and age at onset and personal psychiatric history

It has been well established that the overall progression of the illness causes and worsens the neurocognitive deterioration. The progression is a concept that cannot be easily defined and operationalized; however, there are a number of factors and indices which can be used to conceptualize it. These factors include the age at onset, the duration of illness and the number of previous episodes. A general belief is that the neurocognitive function is strongly associated with the severity of the disease [325].

Age The age of the study sample seems to play an important role, since young patients with BD have better performance compared to young unipolar patients, but the reverse is true in the elderly [326]. Overall, age seems to play a complex role. It has been suggested by one meta-analysis that there is significant impairment of neurocognitive function with advancing age [8], while a second one reported that the difference between patients with BD and healthy controls attenuates with age, and probably this happens because the neurocognitive performance of healthy people deteriorates with age, at a rate which seems to be faster in comparison to what is observed in patients with BD [12] thus leading to a floor effect. A similar phenomenon has been observed in schizophrenia [327329]. Another meta-analysis reported no effect for age [66]

Age at onset The severity of the neurocognitive is correlated with age at onset [94, 330], especially in psychomotor speed and in verbal memory [9]. The early onset of illness and particularly the onset during childhood or adolescence is associated with more severe impairment [319]. In pediatric BD patients, the observed neurocognitive deficit is similar to the deficit seen in adult BD patients [331]. Especially, the impairment in attention is correlated with the age at onset [332].

Personal anamnestic During the course of the illness, the number of episodes [333], the number of prior hospitalizations [137, 188, 333, 334] and the longer duration [333] were associated with a worse neurocognitive function. Illness duration is related to the loss of inhibitory control [197] and to a general memory deficit [175] and verbal memory [149]. Also, worse neurocognitive function might be associated with the number of episodes but not with the duration of the illness [188, 275]. The impairment in attention is correlated with the age of first hospitalization and with the duration of the illness [332].

Some studies found that any history of mood disorder has an adverse effect on neurocognition and especially on memory [137, 175]. The number of past manic episodes, hospitalizations, and suicide attempts was correlated with more severe neurocognitive deficit [149]. It has been suggested that manic episodes were correlated with impairment in verbal learning and memory [144] and in attention and executive function [297], while the number of past depressive episodes was reported to have adverse effect on verbal memory [335] and reaction times [69].

The reverse explanation has also been suggested, with the neurocognitive deterioration being the cause rather than the effect of worse course and outcome [336]. The possibility that neurocognitive differences between psychotic and non-psychotic BD patients are in fact the result of an earlier onset of illness or current medication use rather than a result of psychosis per se cannot be excluded. In line with this, it has been reported that psychotic BD patients had more hospital admissions, a younger age at illness onset, and a larger proportion of them were using antipsychotics. They also had less years of education [308], which is something that should be taken into consideration since it has been suggested that education plays a significant moderatory role [11]. Finally, two meta-analyses suggested no significant effect for age at onset and duration or severity of illness, as defined by the number of episodes [9, 66].

The role of other clinical factors

There are many other clinical factors which could influence the neurocognitive performance of patients with BD. These include brain white matter lesions that are sometimes found in remitted BD patients and rarely in patients with schizophrenia but apparently do not underlie neurocognitive deficits per se [337]. Likewise, the lifetime comorbid alcohol use disorder, which also does not seem to correlate with the neurocognitive performance at least at earlier stages [316]. One study found that overweight and obese BD patients might have worse performance on verbal fluency [338].

Education could constitute an additional confounding variable, since patients with BD have lower educational level despite their IQ level which is comparable with that of controls. Thus, controlling for it might attenuate the magnitude of the observed neurocognitive impairment [339]. It has been suggested that the neurocognitive impairment effect sizes seem to decrease as a function of education [8, 12] and shorter duration of education is related to a more pronounced deficit in different domains such as letter fluency, WCST categories, and the stroop test [66]. The explanation might include two arms. The first concerns the possibility that education is a marker related to the onset and severity of illness, since early and severe illness interferes with educational attainment, and the second concerns the possibility that education is a protective factor per se.

Neurocognitive disorder in BD-II vs. BD-I

In spite of the research efforts during the last few decades, there has not been found a specific neurocognitive profile for the different bipolar subtypes [340]. This is probably because research on BD-II patients is rare, and there are large methodological differences between studies. As a result, inconclusive data are in place. Research on the other subtypes of the bipolar spectrum is essentially lacking. There is only one study reporting data on ‘bipolar spectrum’ patients. That paper suggested the presence of a broad neurocognitive impairment, particularly affecting verbal memory and the executive functions [341].

Some unsystematic reports in the literature suggest that BD-II patients have similar performance to controls [250, 342, 343], but others supported the notion that BD-II patients perform in-between healthy controls and BD-I patients [72, 73, 75, 79, 250, 344346], but this maybe specific concerning verbal memory [149, 346] and executive functions [346]. On the contrary, there are data suggesting that BD-II patients perform similar to BD-I [72, 75, 138, 160], or even perform worse than BD-I patients, at least in some specific neurocognitive domains, including reaction time and inhibition [347, 348].

The literature cannot answer the question whether there is any qualitative difference between bipolar subtypes. Such a difference would suggest (although that would not be mandatory) that possibly there are different neurobiological mechanisms which underlie BD subtypes. However, both questions (concerning a quantitative and a qualitative difference) remain unanswered. For both there are data in favor and against.

A global neurocognitive impairment might be present in BD-II patients, with only phonemic verbal fluency being preserved and with moderate to strong effect sizes ranging between 0.62 and 1.34 [345]. For premorbid IQ, there is only one study which have not found any differences between BD-I and BD-II patients, since both groups had worse performance in comparison to the performance of controls [72]. It has been also shown that the intellectual decline was less pronounced in patients with BD-II in comparison to those with BD-I [347, 349]. There are several studies suggesting an impairment in psychomotor speed and in attention [72, 73, 287, 345, 346, 348]; however, other studies did not support this [342, 344]. One study suggested that reaction time was similar to that of controls [350]. Moreover, another study showed that attention was intact but psychomotor speed was impaired [79]. As far as memory, there seems to be a deficit in verbal memory and verbal learning [79, 149, 345347] but some studies disagree [72, 73, 342, 344, 348]. Other authors found that in BD-II patients there is a presence of a less disorganized semantic system in comparison to patients with BD-I [138, 160]. Additionally, delayed memory is rather intact [72]. There are controversial data concerning the deficit in visual memory, since some authors reported an impairment [72, 345, 347] while others disagreed [73, 287, 342, 343, 348]. Similarly, some authors supported the presence of impairment in executive functions and in working memory [72, 79, 149, 344348] while others did not [342, 343]. Additionally, there is one study which showed that there is a deficit only in working memory but not in executive functions [73]. It has been observed that all the studies using the stroop color–word test, which assesses interference, showed a deficit in inhibitory control in BD-II patients [344347]. Also, there might be a deficit in the emotional processing domain [287, 347]; however, the emotion recognition seems to be intact [250]. It has been also found that unmedicated depressed BD-II patients had intact decision-making performance [343]. On the other hand, one study suggested that medicated BD-II patients had worse performance in comparison to unmedicated BD-II patients in sustained attention [287].

One meta-analyses suggested that there is no difference between BD-II and healthy controls neither in the estimated current intelligence quotient (IQ) nor the premorbid IQ [340, 351]. Another meta-analyses found that neurocognitive impairment in BD-II patients is as severe as in BD-I patients except for the domains of semantic fluency and memory [10]. There are contradictory data concerning psychomotor speed, verbal and visual memory and the impairment in these domains is probably small in magnitude. On the contrary, there are robust data concerning the presence of a working memory deficit and a decrease of cognitive flexibility and impaired inhibitory control in BD-II. Patients with BD-II manifest a deficit also in recognizing emotions [351]. In quantitative terms, BD-II does not differ much from BD-I [119] although it seems that the opinion which prevails is that BD-II patients manifest better performance in comparison to BD-I but worse than healthy controls and are positioned in between these two groups.

Long-term development of the neurocognitive deficit

Methodological issues

It is difficult to chart the long-term course of BD since there are no reliable indices to describe and chart the course in a global way. This happens because BD is a complex illness with different phases and clinical characters. Moreover, there is no clear direction of causality. One possibility is that the accumulation of mood episodes impacts negatively the neurocognitive function; however, the reverse is equally possible. A third possibility is that both the neurocognitive deficit and mood symptoms, independently from each other, reflect a specific pattern of clinical course and disease phenotype without any direct relationship to each other.

Premorbid period

Patients with BD have a relatively intact neurocognitive functioning throughout childhood and adolescence, and the neurocognitive impairment emerges only after the overt symptom onset, and this is in contrast to what is known concerning patients with schizophrenia [352]. In accord with this, it has been found that children who later develop BD exhibit good academic functioning prior to illness onset [32, 353356].

However, some studies argue for the opposite. A prospective investigation of executive functioning in at-risk adolescents showed an impairment in executive function in those who later developed BD [357]. Also, an increased prevalence of abnormal developmental history has been shown with delayed language acquisition, and motor and social development in a group of adolescents with BD [358]. A large Finnish cohort study which evaluated verbal, arithmetic and visuospatial reasoning in healthy male conscripts (mean age 19.9 years) showed that the premorbid visuospatial impairment was associated with later development of both BD and schizophrenia [359]. Also, another prospective study from Sweden suggested that 7 % of 56 adolescents with developmental deficits at the age of 6 years, went on to develop BD, compared with none from the control group [360].

Early stages of BD

The neurocognitive maturation and the development of the child is probably adversely affected by the development of BD during childhood [361]. Generally, the overall neuropsychological deficit has been associated with earlier age at onset which, however, is unclear to which extend it represents simply a longer duration [330]. Immediately following illness onset, adolescents with BD exhibit poor performance in social and neurocognitive domains [353]. Similarly, in adults, the impairment is present already during the first mood episode [301]. At illness onset, there is a deficit in many domains such as sustained attention [97], spatial/non-verbal reasoning, learning and recall, and several aspects of executive function [362] as well as memory, verbal fluency and executive function [121].

Medium stages of BD

Patients with two or more illness episodes manifest poorer performance than patients with just one episode [275] and within 1–3 years, a significant further deterioration in the executive function can be observed [363], which along with processing speed are considered to be the main long-term neurocognitive deficits in BD [364]. One study found that the rest neurocognitive functions seem to be stable at 1–3-year follow-up [363, 365]. However, the complete picture of the results is controversial concerning the short-term deterioration [53, 147, 151, 153, 192, 334].

Latter in the course of the illness, repeated acute episodes negatively impact the neurocognitive functioning [352], which seems to correlate with both the number of affective episodes and the overall duration of illness [81, 97, 108, 144, 333, 366]. In turn, duration of illness and disease course are reported to correlate with verbal and visual memory and executive function [102, 108, 367]. The comparison of young, elderly, and chronic BD patients revealed that a greater number of chronic patients scored in the severely impaired range on a memory and executive battery than their counterparts with fewer past episodes [318]. Thus, it is possible that, instead of reflecting long-term damage to the brain because of repeated acute episodes, poor neurocognitive performance in multi-episode patients may be the result of the presence of chronic residual mood symptoms. It is known that BD patients experience mood liability [368, 369] and residual symptoms, usually depressive, during periods of euthymia in spite of the fact that they were rated as euthymic by clinicians [162].

Later stages of BD

Complex comorbidity is frequent in chronic patients and it is often accompanied with incomplete remission. Thus, the length of BD could act as a confounding factor in patients with comorbidity, e.g., with alcohol dependence in comparison to a non-alcoholic group [151]. A weak point in the literature is that although the acute effects of alcohol or drug intoxication have been controlled for in some studies [147, 176, 337], the effect of past exposures has not been taken into account.

Continuous medication treatment is also a confounding variable especially at later stages and when chronicity and complex comorbidity is in place. For example, although one study reported that there has not been observed further decline of neurocognitive function in patients under long-term lithium treatment [276], another study suggested that the executive function deficit was negatively correlated with years of exposure to antipsychotic drugs [108]. This latter finding could reflect either a toxic effect of long-term antipsychotic medication, the toxic effect of chronic psychosis or both.

In the long term, the neurocognitive deficit seems to be associated with the functional outcome. This relationship is particularly true for processing speed, attention, memory [370] as well as the visual/motor processing domain [371]. One study used cross-sectional data from a large case-register study of 14,000 people which were hospitalized with a mood disorder, 81,380 patients with osteoarthritis and 69,149 patients with diabetes. The results indicated that patients with BD have a 6 % increase in the risk of dementia with every episode leading to admission and overall this risk was higher in comparison to the two control groups [372, 373]. Although these data are in favor of a neurodegenerative process, it is also possible that the findings reflect different courses of the illness plus a different probability of long-term medication treatment and electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) with unknown long-term effects [123]. This effect has already been discussed since patients with more severe and frequent affective episodes perform more poorly on neurocognitive testing [333].

In accord with this, a course characterized by chronicity and residual symptoms with lack of remission between episodes is related to a progressive neurocognitive deficit and in this frame an important role is attributed to the specific clinical course of BD [318, 333]. As previously mentioned, there is a deleterious effect of psychotic symptoms that have deleterious effects [352] and as already previously mentioned antipsychotic medication is associated with poorer performance on IQ, memory and working memory assessments [46].

The overall longitudinal course suggests that neurodevelopmental factors play a minor role in the emergence of neuropsychological dysfunction in BD [324, 352]. Psychopathological factors during the course of the disorder itself are probably related to the neurocognitive impairment, but the nature of this association remains unknown. A graphical representation of the long-term development of the neurocognitive deficit in BD patients in comparison to schizophrenia and normal controls is shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Long-term evolution of the neurocognitive deficit in BD patients, in comparison to patients with schizophrenia and normal subjects. Overall, in contrast to schizophrenia patients, BD patients exhibit a relatively intact neurocognitive functioning throughout childhood and adolescence, and the neurocognitive deterioration is observed only after the overt symptom onset. Reproduced after permission from Lewandowski et al. [352]

Awareness of the neurocognitive deficit

Although many patients with BD frequently complain about neurocognitive problems in attention, concentration and memory, there are limited data on the relationship between subjective cognitive complaints with objective neuropsychological deficits.

The patients’ subjective cognitive complaints do not seem to correlate or predict objective neuropsychological deficits [142, 164, 374]. One study has suggested that there might be a weak correlation between subjective complains and deficits in attention, memory and execute function [375]. Moreover, it has been shown that neither mood symptoms nor the severity of mania or depression correlate with patients’ self-report [142] One study reported that patients with BD might show more subjective complaints when there is a higher number of episodes, and especially a higher number of mixed episodes, a longer duration of the illness and when the onset of the illness occurred at an earlier age [375]. There seems to be some association between depressive symptoms and self-report neurocognitive complaints; however, the association between complaints and objective neurocognitive functioning is not moderated by mood symptoms [374].

Gender and the neurocognitive deficit

There is limited literature concerning the role that gender might play and no conclusions can be derived. Males with BD manifest a more severe deficit in immediate memory [376] and executive function [126] whilst one study suggested that gender does not play any role on emotion recognition [250]. A better memory performance might be related with female gender [175], while another study suggested that the verbal memory deficit might relate to female gender as an endophenotype [377]. Overall, the data are conflicting with one out of three recent meta-analysis being in favor of a gender effect [8], while the two others were against [12, 66].

The neurocognitive deficit as an endophenotype for BD

Endophenotype constitutes a stable pattern of behavioral symptoms with a clear genetic connection. It can be used to bridge the gap between high-level symptom presentation and low-level genetic variability. Ideally, an endophenotype might be useful in the differential diagnosis between disorders when they present with similar symptoms. Additionally, an endophenotype is associated with a specific disorder in the population, and should be heritable and state-independent. It is expected that endophenotype and the specific diagnosis co-segregate within families and the endophenotype is found in non-affected family members at a higher rate than in the general population.

The neurocognitive impairment in patients with BD is at least partially different in comparison to the deficit seen in patients with schizophrenia and it is also at least partially state-independent and definitely present during periods of complete recovery and euthymia. Although the literature so far does not support the use of global measures of neurocognition as endophenotypes [378380]. On the other hand, however, specific neurocognitive deficits might be relevant, although further research is needed. It has been reported that inaffective relatives of patients occupy an intermediate position between patients and healthy controls concerning specific neurocognitive functions [195].

Twin studies

There is not much data concerning discordant monozygotic or dizygotic twins. Healthy co-twins performance in aspects of concentration, verbal and visual memory and verbal recognition and executive function is worse than controls [381], although a population-based study reported that the verbal memory deficit as an endophenotype might relate to female gender alone [377]. It has been also reported that neither BD patients nor their unaffected co-twins differed from control subjects in spatial working memory [171]. Another population-based study on healthy monozygotic and dizygotic twins showed that monozygotic high-risk twins manifested a significant deficit on selective and sustained attention, executive function, language processing and working and declarative memory, while the dizygotic high-risk twins manifested lower scores only on language processing and episodic memory [382]. No deficit was found on response inhibition in discordant twins of BD patients (event with a psychotic or familial form). Lower performance in twins during testing could be explained by current depressive symptoms [383]. It is interesting than in most of these studies, the unaffected co-twins suffered from minor or subthreshold mental disorders and dysthymia. Overall, twin studies provide some but not strong evidence for the presence of an endophenotype on the basis of neurocognitive impairment.

Studies on first-degree relatives

There are a limited number of studies in children whose parents suffer from BD. They suggest that high-risk children differ from control children in terms of reaction time; however, the quality data are rather low [384]. The same research group also reported that both high-risk and control children have similar performance in speech competence tests [385]. In two other studies, high-risk children were impaired in PIQ but not in VIQ; however, half of them were suffering from depression [28, 386]. Another study found that high-risk children and controls had similar IQ; however, controls had better performance in some reading and arithmetic cognitive tasks and there was an increased rate of VIQ-PIQ discrepancy [387]. A study on visual backward masking was also negative [69]. High-risk relatives had slower reaction time on a sustained auditory attention task [388]. Moreover, it has been shown by a recent study that specific executive functions which are supposedly located in the ventral frontal cortex might be impaired in high-risk children in comparison to controls [389]. Also, there might be a deficit in healthy high-risk children in spatial memory, in attention and in executive functions, even after correction for multiple confounding factors [390]. Overall, these studies provide evidence for the usefulness of executive function impairment as an endophenotype for BD.

Studies on mixed samples of relatives

There are more data concerning families of BD patients. These studies utilized mixed samples of first- and second-degree patients.

One study which utilized only female relatives of patients (most of them psychotic) reported that their IQ was superior to the IQ of controls. Also, it argued in favor of the usefulness of visual memory and executive function as endophenotypes [380]. Another study indicated that IQ was similar to controls [30] while on the contrary, another one reported the presence of a general and non-specific deficit in IQ, with some pronounced effect on verbal fluency [59]. More recent data suggest a higher discrepancy between VIQ and PIQ in families of patients with BD in comparison to healthy controls; however, these results were inconsistent with previous findings of higher VIQ in comparison to PIQ [356].

A deficit in psychomotor speed is also reported [52, 70, 143, 391], and while studies suggested an impairment in concentration [392], others have found that the impairment concerns shifting, but not sustaining attention (concentration) [98, 393, 394]. One study found a deficit in delayed recall in well-educated siblings with high IQ [395]. The literature also suggests the presence of an impairment in concentration, in the visuospatial declarative memory [392], in verbal working memory [396], and verbal [70, 143, 391, 397], visual [195] and auditory verbal learning [398] in working memory [399] and in executive function domain [52, 70, 143, 391, 396, 400].

There is one report on increased emotional interference with a bias towards mood-related information [401]. At-risk youths make more errors when identifying facial emotions, and the magnitude of the deficit was similar to that observed in patients with BD [243]; however, another study reported no deficit either in BD patients or in their first-degree relatives [257].

It is interesting that in spite of an overall normal neurocognitive functioning, there might be present some kind of subtle impairment in first-degree relatives of BD patients. A study which utilized l-tryptophan challenge showed that under challenge, a deficit emerges in relatives of BD patients in comparison to controls in memory, focused and divided attention, and psychomotor performance. Additionally, the relatives of patients with BD-I performed worse than the relatives of BD-II patients [402, 403].

Reviews and meta-analyses

Overall, the literature supports the idea that the deficit in executive function could constitute an endophenotype for BD, especially concerning psychotic cases. However, there are some data, which dispute the usefulness of specific executive functions, including cognitive control during episodic memory retrieval [397], response inhibition [383] and cognitive set-shifting [393]. Also, while a widespread deficit in attention and memory in families of patients with BD is reported, there are some negative data concerning concentration [98], psychomotor performance [396, 400], working memory [393], spatial working memory [171], verbal learning [391], verbal memory [396] and executive functions [400]. It is almost certain that IQ cannot serve as an endophenotype of the illness.

Reviews and meta-analyses suggest that the executive function and the verbal memory deficit could serve as core endophenotypes for BD [8, 367, 404], but conclusions are premature. The effect sizes reported are 0.49 with the stroop test and 0.37 with the TMT-B, while concerning immediate verbal memory the effect size is 0.42. Thus, in first-degree relatives, effect sizes were small (d < 0.5), but significantly different from healthy controls in particular concerning executive function and verbal memory [8].

In line with the above, another meta-analysis reported small effect sizes, that is, 0.20 for current IQ, 0.18–0.36 for attention and 0.17–0.22 for mental speed, 0.13–0.33 for various aspects of memory, 0.27 for verbal fluency and 0.24–0.51 for executive functions with the effect for the stroop test being the highest (0.51). This meta-analytic study demonstrated that impaired response inhibition might constitute the most prominent neurocognitive endophenotype of BD. Another executive measure, set-shifting (TMT-B, 0.38; WCST perseveration, 0.36) and two other neurocognitive domains, verbal memory (0.27–0.33) and sustained attention (0.36) also met the criteria as potential endophenotypes of BD. These results suggest that the response inhibition deficit, a potential marker of ventral prefrontal dysfunction, seems to be the most prominent endophenotype of BD [9].

In Table 2, the effect sizes of neurocognitive impairment in relatives of BD patients are shown. These effect sizes determine whether a specific impairment is of sufficient magnitude to be considered as an endophenotype.

Discussion

Many areas of the neurocognitive function of BD still remain uncharted. It seems that a little less than half of BD patients suffer from a significant neurocognitive deficit and although the majority of BD patients might not differ from healthy controls, it is also true that some others do not differ from patients with schizophrenia. Both patient groups share a similar cognitive impairment profile with different degrees of deficits and thus the difference between them seems to be more quantitative rather than qualitative [405].

The complexity of the illness is evident also in the patients’ neurocognitive function and its assessment. The core deficit, which constitutes a direct consequence of the disease itself, seems to be independent not only from the other components of the disease but also from mood symptoms. This core deficit is either increased or on the contrary it is attenuated by many factors such as the disease phase, specific personal characteristics of the patients (age, gender, education, etc.), current symptomatology and its treatment and the long-term course and the long-term exposure to medication, psychiatric and somatic comorbidity and alcohol and/or substance abuse.

However, the origin and the etiopathogenesis of the core neurocognitive impairment remains elusive. Probably, there is a neurodegenerative component as a consequence of repeated mood episodes and psychotic features. Additionally, the effect of chronicity is quite strong while the neurodevelopmental component is either absent or very weak. This probably differentiates BD from schizophrenia, in which the neurodevelopmental component is strong. Such a neurodevelopmental effect is evident in some but not all patients with BP. A model suggesting that only BD patients who share common genetic risk factors with schizophrenia have premorbid neurodevelopmental neurocognitive deficits has been proposed [406]. On the other hand, there is no consistent evidence supporting that BD, as a group of disorders, have a progressively deteriorating course of neurocognitive functions [407].

Overall, the neurocognitive deficit concerns almost all domains with only a few exceptions and its magnitude is at the severe range during the acute episodes and at the medium range during euthymia. A summary of effect sizes by neurocognitive domain and illness phase is shown in Table 2.

There seems to be a small to moderate reduction in current IQ and general neurocognitive function during all phases of the illness; however, premorbid IQ and general neurocognitive function seems to be intact. Impairments in verbal memory, attention, and executive functions tend to be present during and after the first episode. Preliminary evidence suggests that these deficits in specific neurocognitive domains might precede the onset of illness [408]. The pattern and the severity of the neuropsychological impairment seems to be in place already during the first mood episode and these patients seem to lie intermediate between patients with a first episode of schizophrenia and healthy controls [409]

There is an impairment in psychomotor speed, attention, working memory and most of the verbal and some non-verbal components of memory as well as verbal fluency, executive functions and social cognition which is typically severe during the acute phases and moderate during euthymia. BD, irrespective of illness phase, is characterized by a deficit in the acquisition of new information, but not in retention, probably because of a disruption in the engagement of effortful processes. An important exception is the deficit in visuospatial skills which is present only during the acute phases and not during euthymia. An important methodological problem is that different studies utilize different definitions of euthymia and as a result, it is unclear whether subthreshold residual symptoms are present or not, what their polarity is and whether they are responsible for the deficit which is observed during remission.

The overall negative impact of medication is small and it is confounded by the specific clinical symptoms, for which medication is used for their treatment. This is especially true concerning antipsychotics and psychotic symptoms.

Although the prevailing opinion is that BD-II patients perform better than BD-I but worse than healthy controls, the literature has shown that in quantitative terms, BD-II does not differ much from BD-I.

The specific clinical course of the illness seems to play an important role. A course characterized by residual symptoms, chronicity and lack of remission between episodes is related to a progressive neurocognitive deficit. Of significant importance is the adverse effect specifically of psychotic symptoms, while manic episodes seem to affect neurocognitive function more than depressive episodes do. Age, age at onset, duration of the illness and number of episodes, not only reflect distinct but overlapping aspects of the overall disease burden but also are related to the neurocognitive impairment and its progression. However, the data are inconclusive concerning the magnitude and the true nature of this relationship.

The verbal memory and the executive function deficit could be the central endophenotypes of the illness. The most prominent endophenotype of BD seems to be the response inhibition deficit. However, in most family studies, unaffected at-risk family members who do not suffer from BD but they do manifest dysthymic or other, often subthreshold mood states, often in the form of temperament, and this might again act as a confounding variable.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the literature suggests that the neurocognitive deficit in BD patients concerns almost all domains with only a few exceptions. Its magnitude is at the severe range during the acute episodes and at the medium range during euthymia, while the origin of the deficit remains unclear. In terms of neurocognitive function, BD patients do quantitatively better than patients with schizophrenia but the qualitative pattern of the deficit is similar in the two disorders. There are no clear differences between BD subtypes. The deficit is present early in the course of the disorder. At least in some patients, it might emerge before the onset of the first mood episode and in the majority of patients it progresses probably in relationship with the manifestation of psychotic symptoms. The verbal memory and the executive function deficit probably constitute endophenotypes, while the role of medication as a causative factor is limited.

Authors’ contributions

KNF and ET carried out the literature search and the interpretation of the results. KNF wrote the first draft and ET contributed to the revision including the final draft. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Contributor Information

Eirini Tsitsipa, Email: tsitsipa_eir@yahoo.gr.

Konstantinos N. Fountoulakis, Email: kostasfountoulakis@gmail.com

References

  • 1.Fountoulakis K. Bipolar disorder: an evidence-based guide to manic depression. Berlin: Springer; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Reichenberg A, Harvey PD, Bowie CR, Mojtabai R, Rabinowitz J, Heaton RK, Bromet E. Neuropsychological function and dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(5):1022–1029. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Andreasen NC. The relationship between creativity and mood disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2008;10(2):251–255. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2008.10.2/ncandreasen. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Andreasen NC, Glick ID. Bipolar affective disorder and creativity: implications and clinical management. Compr Psychiatry. 1988;29(3):207–217. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(88)90044-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Andreasen NC. Creativity and mental illness: prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144(10):1288–1292. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.10.1288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jamison KR. Manic-depressive illness and creativity. Sci Am. 1995;272(2):62–67. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0295-62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jamison KR. Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British writers and artists. Psychiatry. 1989;52(2):125–134. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1989.11024436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol Med. 2008;38(6):771–785. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord. 2009;113(1–2):1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C, Berk M. Meta-analytic review of neurocognition in bipolar II disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011;123(3):165–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kurtz MM, Gerraty RT. A meta-analytic investigation of neurocognitive deficits in bipolar illness: profile and effects of clinical state. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(5):551–562. doi: 10.1037/a0016277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mann-Wrobel MC, Carreno JT, Dickinson D. Meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: an update and investigation of moderator variables. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(4):334–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00935.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Goswami U, Young AH, Ferrier IN, Moore PB. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2006;93(1–3):105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gualtieri CT, Morgan DW. The frequency of cognitive impairment in patients with anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder: an unaccounted source of variance in clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69(7):1122–1130. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Scapola M, Igoa A, Marengo E, Ais ED, Perinot L. Heterogeneity in cognitive functioning among patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2008;109(1–2):149–156. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.12.232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Clark L, Goodwin GM. State- and trait-related deficits in sustained attention in bipolar disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;254(2):61–68. doi: 10.1007/s00406-004-0460-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Quraishi S, Frangou S. Neuropsychology of bipolar disorder: a review. J Affect Disord. 2002;72(3):209–226. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00091-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Malhi GS, Ivanovski B, Szekeres V, Olley A. Bipolar disorder: it’s all in your mind? The neuropsychological profile of a biological disorder. Can J Psychiatry. 2004;49(12):813–819. doi: 10.1177/070674370404901204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Hughes JH, Watson S, Gray JM, Ferrier IN, Young AH. Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic patients with bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:32–40. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dickerson F, Boronow JJ, Stallings C, Origoni AE, Cole SK, Yolken RH. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: comparison of performance on the repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status. Psychiatry Res. 2004;129(1):45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schretlen DJ, Cascella NG, Meyer SM, Kingery LR, Testa SM, Munro CA, Pulver AE, Rivkin P, Rao VA, Diaz-Asper CM, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(2):179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Int J Surg. 2010;8(5):336–341. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2010.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mason CF. Pre-illness intelligence of mental hospital patients. J Consult Psychol. 1956;20(4):297–300. doi: 10.1037/h0047921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Woodruff RA, Jr, Robins LN, Winokur G, Walbran B. Educational and occupational achievement in primary affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1968;124(11):Suppl:57–Suppl:64. doi: 10.1176/ajp.124.11S.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Woodruff RA, Jr, Robins LN, Winokur G, Reich T. Manic depressive illness and social achievement. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1971;47(3):237–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1971.tb02211.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Petterson U. Manic-depressive illness. A clinical, social and genetic study. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1977;269:1–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Waters BG, Marchenko-Bouer I, Smiley D. Educational achievement, IQ and affective disorder in the adult offspring of bipolar manic-depressives. Br J Psychiatry. 1981;139:457–462. doi: 10.1192/bjp.139.5.457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Decina P, Kestenbaum CJ, Farber S, Kron L, Gargan M, Sackeim HA, Fieve RR. Clinical and psychological assessment of children of bipolar probands. Am J Psychiatry. 1983;140(5):548–553. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.5.548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A. Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(1):50–57. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gilvarry C, Takei N, Russell A, Rushe T, Hemsley D, Murray RM. Premorbid IQ in patients with functional psychosis and their first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res. 2000;41(3):417–429. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00092-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Zammit S, Allebeck P, David AS, Dalman C, Hemmingsson T, Lundberg I, Lewis G. A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ Score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(4):354–360. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Rabinowitz J, Caspi A, Schmeidler J, Mark M, Kaplan Z, Davidson M. A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(12):2027–2035. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gooding DC, Tallent KA. The association between antisaccade task and working memory task performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189(1):8–16. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200101000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Seidman LJ, Kremen WS, Koren D, Faraone SV, Goldstein JM, Tsuang MT. A comparative profile analysis of neuropsychological functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar psychoses. Schizophr Res. 2002;53(1–2):31–44. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00162-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Coryell W, Endicott J, Keller M, Andreasen N, Grove W, Hirschfeld RM, Scheftner W. Bipolar affective disorder and high achievement: a familial association. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146(8):983–988. doi: 10.1176/ajp.146.8.983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Morice R. Cognitive inflexibility and pre-frontal dysfunction in schizophrenia and mania. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:50–54. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Souza VB, Muir WJ, Walker MT, Glabus MF, Roxborough HM, Sharp CW, Dunan JR, Blackwood DH. Auditory P300 event-related potentials and neuropsychological performance in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37(5):300–310. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00131-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Greenberg R, Griffin S, Schulz SC, Pickar D, Kleinman JE, Weinberger DR. Contrasts between patients with affective disorders and patients with schizophrenia on a neuropsychological test battery. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(9):1355–1362. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.9.1355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Jones PB, Bebbington P, Foerster A, Lewis SW, Murray RM, Russell A, Sham PC, Toone BK, Wilkins S. Premorbid social underachievement in schizophrenia. Results from the Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:65–71. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gard D, Harrell EH, Poreh A. Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia on the WAIS-R NI sentence arrangement subtest. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Neuropsychological Inventory. J Clin Psychol. 1999;55(9):1085–1094. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199909)55:9&#x0003c;1085::AID-JCLP6&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Altshuler LL, Ventura J, van Gorp WG, Green MF, Theberge DC, Mintz J. Neurocognitive function in clinically stable men with bipolar I disorder or schizophrenia and normal control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(8):560–569. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Silverstein ML, McDonald C, Fogg L. Intelligence and neuropsychological functioning in psychiatric disorders. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1990;5(3):317–323. doi: 10.1093/arclin/5.3.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Coffman JA, Bornstein RA, Olson SC, Schwarzkopf SB, Nasrallah HA. Cognitive impairment and cerebral structure by MRI in bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1990;27(11):1188–1196. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90416-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fitzgerald D, Lucas S, Redoblado MA, Winter V, Brennan J, Anderson J, Harris A. Cognitive functioning in young people with first episode psychosis: relationship to diagnosis and clinical characteristics. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2004;38(7):501–510. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2004.01403.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Dittmann S, Seemuller F, Grunze HC, Schwarz MJ, Zach J, Fast K, Born C, Dargel S, Engel RR, Bernhard B, et al. The impact of homocysteine levels on cognition in euthymic bipolar patients: a cross-sectional study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69(6):899–906. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Donaldson S, Goldstein LH, Landau S, Raymont V, Frangou S. The Maudsley Bipolar Disorder Project: the effect of medication, family history, and duration of illness on IQ and memory in bipolar I disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2003;64(1):86–93. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n0116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Varga M, Magnusson A, Flekkoy K, Ronneberg U, Opjordsmoen S. Insight, symptoms and neurocognition in bipolar I patients. J Affect Disord. 2006;91(1):1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Barguil M, Barrett J, Reichenberg A, Bowden CL, Soares JC, Velligan DI. The neurocognitive signature of psychotic bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(8):910–916. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Czobor P, Jaeger J, Berns SM, Gonzalez C, Loftus S. Neuropsychological symptom dimensions in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(1–2):71–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00428.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Seidman LJ, Lanca M, Kremen WS, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Organizational and visual memory deficits in schizophrenia and bipolar psychoses using the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: effects of duration of illness. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25(7):949–964. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.7.949.16482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dickerson FB, Sommerville J, Origoni AE, Ringel NB, Parente F. Outpatients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder: do they differ in their cognitive and social functioning? Psychiatry Res. 2001;102(1):21–27. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00247-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zalla T, Joyce C, Szoke A, Schurhoff F, Pillon B, Komano O, Perez-Diaz F, Bellivier F, Alter C, Dubois B, et al. Executive dysfunctions as potential markers of familial vulnerability to bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2004;121(3):207–217. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00252-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hoff AL, Shukla S, Aronson T, Cook B, Ollo C, Baruch S, Jandorf L, Schwartz J. Failure to differentiate bipolar disorder from schizophrenia on measures of neuropsychological function. Schizophr Res. 1990;3(4):253–260. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(90)90006-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Dalby JT, Williams R. Preserved reading and spelling ability in psychotic disorders. Psychol Med. 1986;16(1):171–175. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700002609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Gale CR, Batty GD, McIntosh AM, Porteous DJ, Deary IJ, Rasmussen F. Is bipolar disorder more common in highly intelligent people? A cohort study of a million men. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(2):190–194. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Badcock JC, Michiel PT, Rock D. Spatial working memory and planning ability: contrasts between schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder. Cortex. 2005;41(6):753–763. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70294-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Cakir S, Barrett JA, Najt P, Serap Monkul E, Maples N, Velligan DI, Soares JC. Differential working memory impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: effects of lifetime history of psychosis. Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(2):117–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00296.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kluger A, Goldberg E. IQ patterns in affective disorder, lateralized and diffuse brain damage. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1990;12(2):182–194. doi: 10.1080/01688639008400966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.McIntosh AM, Harrison LK, Forrester K, Lawrie SM, Johnstone EC. Neuropsychological impairments in people with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:378–385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.5.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Sackeim HA, Freeman J, McElhiney M, Coleman E, Prudic J, Devanand DP. Effects of major depression on estimates of intelligence. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14(2):268–288. doi: 10.1080/01688639208402828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mojtabai R, Bromet EJ, Harvey PD, Carlson GA, Craig TJ, Fennig S. Neuropsychological differences between first-admission schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(9):1453–1460. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.9.1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sorensen HJ, Saebye D, Urfer-Parnas A, Mortensen EL, Parnas J. Premorbid intelligence and educational level in bipolar and unipolar disorders: a Danish draft board study. J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):1188–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Goodwin F, Jamison K. Manic-depressive illness. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gorlyn M, Keilp JG, Oquendo MA, Burke AK, Sackeim HA, John Mann J. The WAIS-III and major depression: absence of VIQ/PIQ differences. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(7):1145–1157. doi: 10.1080/13803390500246944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Krabbendam L, Arts B, van Os J, Aleman A. Cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a quantitative review. Schizophr Res. 2005;80(2–3):137–149. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive impairment in affective psychoses: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36(1):112–125. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Torres IJ, Boudreau VG, Yatham LN. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2007;434:17–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01055.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Marvel CL, Paradiso S. Cognitive and neurological impairment in mood disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2004;27(1):19–36, vii–viii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.MacQueen GM, Grof P, Alda M, Marriott M, Young LT, Duffy A. A pilot study of visual backward masking performance among affected versus unaffected offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(5):374–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00133.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Antila M, Partonen T, Kieseppa T, Suvisaari J, Eerola M, Lonnqvist J, Tuulio-Henriksson A. Cognitive functioning of bipolar I patients and relatives from families with or without schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. J Affect Disord. 2009;116(1–2):70–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Chaves OC, Lombardo LE, Bearden CE, Woolsey MD, Martinez DM, Barrett JA, Miller AL, Velligan DI, Glahn DC. Association of clinical symptoms and neurocognitive performance in bipolar disorder: a longitudinal study. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(1):118–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00888.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Dittmann S, Hennig-Fast K, Gerber S, Seemuller F, Riedel M, Emanuel Severus W, Langosch J, Engel RR, Moller HJ, Grunze HC. Cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II patients. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(8):877–887. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00640.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Hsiao YL, Wu YS, Wu JY, Hsu MH, Chen HC, Lee SY, Lee IH, Yeh TL, Yang YK, Ko HC, et al. Neuropsychological functions in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(5):547–554. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00723.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Langenecker SA, Saunders EF, Kade AM, Ransom MT, McInnis MG. Intermediate: cognitive phenotypes in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2010;122(3):285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Martino DJ, Igoa A, Marengo E, Scapola M, Strejilevich SA. Neurocognitive impairments and their relationship with psychosocial functioning in euthymic bipolar II disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2011;199(7):459–464. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182214190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Mur M, Portella MJ, Martinez-Aran A, Pifarre J, Vieta E. Persistent neuropsychological deficit in euthymic bipolar patients: executive function as a core deficit. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007;68(7):1078–1086. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v68n0715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Basso MR, Lowery N, Ghormley C, Ward T, Purdie R, Neel J, Combs DR, Bornstein RA. Neuropsychological impairment and psychosis in mania. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;31(5):523–532. doi: 10.1080/13803390802304516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Olley AL, Malhi GS, Bachelor J, Cahill CM, Mitchell PB, Berk M. Executive functioning and theory of mind in euthymic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7(Suppl 5):43–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00254.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Xu G, Lin K, Rao D, Dang Y, Ouyang H, Guo Y, Ma J, Chen J. Neuropsychological performance in bipolar I, bipolar II and unipolar depression patients: a longitudinal, naturalistic study. J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):328–339. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.McGrath J, Scheldt S, Welham J, Clair A. Performance on tests sensitive to impaired executive ability in schizophrenia, mania and well controls: acute and subacute phases. Schizophr Res. 1997;26(2–3):127–137. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00070-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.El-Badri SM, Ashton CH, Moore PB, Marsh VR, Ferrier IN. Electrophysiological and cognitive function in young euthymic patients with bipolar affective disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2001;3(2):79–87. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2001.030206.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Stoddart SD, Craddock NJ, Jones LA. Differentiation of executive and attention impairments in affective illness. Psychol Med. 2007;37(11):1613–1623. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Szoke A, Meary A, Trandafir A, Bellivier F, Roy I, Schurhoff F, Leboyer M. Executive deficits in psychotic and bipolar disorders—implications for our understanding of schizoaffective disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23(1):20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Martinez-Aran A, Torrent C, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Salamero M, Daban C, Balanza-Martinez V, Sanchez-Moreno J, Manuel Goikolea J, Benabarre A, Colom F, et al. Neurocognitive impairment in bipolar patients with and without history of psychosis. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69(2):233–239. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Hill SK, Reilly JL, Harris MS, Rosen C, Marvin RW, Deleon O, Sweeney JA. A comparison of neuropsychological dysfunction in first-episode psychosis patients with unipolar depression, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2009;113(2–3):167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Gallagher P, Gray JM, Watson S, Young AH, Ferrier IN. Neurocognitive functioning in bipolar depression: a component structure analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44(5):961–974. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Volkert J, Kopf J, Kazmaier J, Glaser F, Zierhut KC, Schiele MA, Kittel-Schneider S, Reif A. Evidence for cognitive subgroups in bipolar disorder and the influence of subclinical depression and sleep disturbances. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25(2):192–202. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Hawkins KA, Hoffman RE, Quinlan DM, Rakfeldt J, Docherty NM, Sledge WH. Cognition, negative symptoms, and diagnosis: a comparison of schizophrenic, bipolar, and control samples. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;9(1):81–89. doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Hobart MP, Goldberg R, Bartko JJ, Gold JM. Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status as a screening test in schizophrenia, II: convergent/discriminant validity and diagnostic group comparisons. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(12):1951–1957. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.12.1951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Ha TH, Chang JS, Oh SH, Kim JS, Cho HS, Ha K. Differential patterns of neuropsychological performance in the euthymic and depressive phases of bipolar disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(7):515–523. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Torres IJ, Kozicky J, Popuri S, Bond DJ, Honer WG, Lam RW, Yatham LN. 12-month longitudinal cognitive functioning in patients recently diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2014;16(2):159–171. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Blackburn IM. Mental and psychomotor speed in depression and mania. Br J Psychiatry. 1975;126:329–335. doi: 10.1192/bjp.126.4.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Kolur US, Reddy YC, John JP, Kandavel T, Jain S. Sustained attention and executive functions in euthymic young people with bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:453–458. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.022921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Bora E, Vahip S, Akdeniz F, Gonul AS, Eryavuz A, Ogut M, Alkan M. The effect of previous psychotic mood episodes on cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(5):468–477. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00469.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Antila M, Kieseppa T, Partonen T, Lonnqvist J, Tuulio-Henriksson A. The effect of processing speed on cognitive functioning in patients with familial bipolar I disorder and their unaffected relatives. Psychopathology. 2011;44(1):40–45. doi: 10.1159/000317577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Burdick KE, Goldberg JF, Harrow M. Neurocognitive dysfunction and psychosocial outcome in patients with bipolar I disorder at 15-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;122(6):499–506. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01590.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Clark L, Iversen SD, Goodwin GM. Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;180:313–319. doi: 10.1192/bjp.180.4.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Clark L, Kempton MJ, Scarna A, Grasby PM, Goodwin GM. Sustained attention-deficit confirmed in euthymic bipolar disorder but not in first-degree relatives of bipolar patients or euthymic unipolar depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(2):183–187. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Fleck DE, Shear PK, Strakowski SM. Processing efficiency and directed forgetting in bipolar disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11(7):871–880. doi: 10.1017/s1355617705051027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Liu SK, Chiu CH, Chang CJ, Hwang TJ, Hwu HG, Chen WJ. Deficits in sustained attention in schizophrenia and affective disorders: stable versus state-dependent markers. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(6):975–982. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.6.975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Malhi GS, Ivanovski B, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Mitchell PB, Vieta E, Sachdev P. Neuropsychological deficits and functional impairment in bipolar depression, hypomania and euthymia. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(1–2):114–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00324.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Sanchez-Morla EM, Barabash A, Martinez-Vizcaino V, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Balanza-Martinez V, Cabranes-Diaz JA, Baca-Baldomero E, Gomez JL. Comparative study of neurocognitive function in euthymic bipolar patients and stabilized schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 2009;169(3):220–228. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Elshahawi HH, Essawi H, Rabie MA, Mansour M, Beshry ZA, Mansour AN. Cognitive functions among euthymic bipolar I patients after a single manic episode versus recurrent episodes. J Affect Disord. 2011;130(1–2):180–191. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Strakowski SM, Adler CM, Holland SK, Mills N, DelBello MP. A preliminary FMRI study of sustained attention in euthymic, unmedicated bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(9):1734–1740. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Rund BR, Orbeck AL, Landro NI. Vigilance deficits in schizophrenics and affectively disturbed patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1992;86(3):207–212. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1992.tb03253.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Thomas P, Kearney G, Napier E, Ellis E, Leuder I, Johnson M. Speech and language in first onset psychosis differences between people with schizophrenia, mania, and controls. Br J Psychiatry. 1996;168(3):337–343. doi: 10.1192/bjp.168.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Harmer CJ, Clark L, Grayson L, Goodwin GM. Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder is not a working memory impairment in disguise. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(9):1586–1590. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00019-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Zubieta JK, Huguelet P, O’Neil RL, Giordani BJ. Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar I disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2001;102(1):9–20. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00242-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Malloy-Diniz LF, Neves FS, Abrantes SS, Fuentes D, Correa H. Suicide behavior and neuropsychological assessment of type I bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 2009;112(1–3):231–236. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Wilder-Willis KE, Sax KW, Rosenberg HL, Fleck DE, Shear PK, Strakowski SM. Persistent attentional dysfunction in remitted bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2001;3(2):58–62. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2001.030202.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Ali SO, Denicoff KD, Altshuler LL, Hauser P, Li X, Conrad AJ, Mirsky AF, Smith-Jackson EE, Post RM. A preliminary study of the relation of neuropsychological performance to neuroanatomic structures in bipolar disorder. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2000;13(1):20–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Asarnow RF, MacCrimmon DJ. Span of apprehension deficits during the postpsychotic stages of schizophrenia. A replication and extension. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38(9):1006–1011. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780340058007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Harmell AL, Mausbach BT, Moore RC, Depp CA, Jeste DV, Palmer BW. Longitudinal study of sustained attention in outpatients with bipolar disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(2):230–237. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Addington J, Addington D. Attentional vulnerability indicators in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 1997;23(3):197–204. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00105-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Park S, Holzman PS. Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49(12):975–982. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820120063009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Tam WC, Liu Z. Comparison of neurocognition between drug-free patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192(7):464–470. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000131805.72855.a3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Gruzelier J, Seymour K, Wilson L, Jolley A, Hirsch S. Impairments on neuropsychologic tests of temporohippocampal and frontohippocampal functions and word fluency in remitting schizophrenia and affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45(7):623–629. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800310027003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Koenders MA, Spijker AT, Hoencamp E, Haffmans JP, Zitman FG, Giltay EJ. Effects of mood state on divided attention in patients with bipolar disorder: evidence for beneficial effects of subclinical manic symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220(1–2):302–308. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J. A 2-year naturalistic study on cognitive functioning in bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011;123(3):190–205. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01601.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Goswami U, Sharma A, Khastigir U, Ferrier IN, Young AH, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Moore PB. Neuropsychological dysfunction, soft neurological signs and social disability in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:366–373. doi: 10.1192/bjp.188.4.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Barrett SL, Mulholland CC, Cooper SJ, Rushe TM. Patterns of neurocognitive impairment in first-episode bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(1):67–72. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.054874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Tam WC, Sewell KW, Deng HC. Information processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a discriminant analysis. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1998;186(10):597–603. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199810000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Paradiso S, Lamberty GJ, Garvey MJ, Robinson RG. Cognitive impairment in the euthymic phase of chronic unipolar depression. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1997;185(12):748–754. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199712000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Swann AC, Pazzaglia P, Nicholls A, Dougherty DM, Moeller FG. Impulsivity and phase of illness in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2003;73(1–2):105–111. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00328-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Stern Y, Sackeim HA. The neuropsychology of memory and amnesia. In: Yudofsky SC, Hales RE, editors. The American Psychiatric Press textbook of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences. 4. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press; 2002. pp. 597–622. [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Barrett SL, Kelly C, Bell R, King DJ. Gender influences the detection of spatial working memory deficits in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(5):647–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00592.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Brown GG, Lohr J, Notestine R, Turner T, Gamst A, Eyler LT. Performance of schizophrenia and bipolar patients on verbal and figural working memory tasks. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116(4):741–753. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Wiegand LC, Warfield SK, Levitt JJ, Hirayasu Y, Salisbury DF, Heckers S, Dickey CC, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW, et al. Prefrontal cortical thickness in first-episode psychosis: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(2):131–140. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Simonsen C, Sundet K, Vaskinn A, Birkenaes AB, Engh JA, Faerden A, Jonsdottir H, Ringen PA, Opjordsmoen S, Melle I, et al. Neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar and schizophrenia spectrum disorders depends on history of psychosis rather than diagnostic group. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(1):73–83. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Thompson JM, Hamilton CJ, Gray JM, Quinn JG, Mackin P, Young AH, Ferrier IN. Executive and visuospatial sketchpad resources in euthymic bipolar disorder: implications for visuospatial working memory architecture. Memory. 2006;14(4):437–451. doi: 10.1080/09658210500464293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Mintz J. Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania. II. Specifying the visual channels. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(12):945–951. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950120017004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Mintz J. Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania. I. Specifying a mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(12):939–944. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950120011003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.McGrath J, Chapple B, Wright M. Working memory in schizophrenia and mania: correlation with symptoms during the acute and subacute phases. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2001;103(3):181–188. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.00114.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Ioannidi N, Konstantakopoulos G, Sakkas D, Oulis P. The relationship of theory of mind with symptoms and cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: a prospective study. Psychiatrike. 2015;26(1):17–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Frydecka D, Eissa AM, Hewedi DH, Ali M, Drapala J, Misiak B, Klosinska E, Phillips JR, Moustafa AA. Impairments of working memory in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: the effect of history of psychotic symptoms and different aspects of cognitive task demands. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:416. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Ayres AM, Busatto GF, Menezes PR, Schaufelberger MS, Coutinho L, Murray RM, McGuire PK, Rushe T, Scazufca M. Cognitive deficits in first-episode psychosis: a population-based study in Sao Paulo, Brazil. Schizophr Res. 2007;90(1–3):338–343. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Bearden CE, Glahn DC, Monkul ES, Barrett J, Najt P, Villarreal V, Soares JC. Patterns of memory impairment in bipolar disorder and unipolar major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2006;142(2–3):139–150. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Chang JS, Choi S, Ha K, Ha TH, Cho HS, Choi JE, Cha B, Moon E. Differential pattern of semantic memory organization between bipolar I and II disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35(4):1053–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Deckersbach T, Savage CR, Reilly-Harrington N, Clark L, Sachs G, Rauch SL. Episodic memory impairment in bipolar disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: the role of memory strategies. Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(3):233–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00118.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Oertel-Knochel V, Reinke B, Feddern R, Knake A, Knochel C, Prvulovic D, Fusser F, Karakaya T, Loellgen D, Freitag C, et al. Verbal episodic memory deficits in remitted bipolar patients: a combined behavioural and fMRI study. J Affect Disord. 2013;150(2):430–440. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Bonnin CM, Sanchez-Moreno J, Martinez-Aran A, Sole B, Reinares M, Rosa AR, Goikolea JM, Benabarre A, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Ferrer M, et al. Subthreshold symptoms in bipolar disorder: impact on neurocognition, quality of life and disability. J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):650–659. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Burdick KE, Endick CJ, Goldberg JF. Assessing cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: are self-reports valid? Psychiatry Res. 2005;136(1):43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Antila M, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Kieseppa T, Eerola M, Partonen T, Lonnqvist J. Cognitive functioning in patients with familial bipolar I disorder and their unaffected relatives. Psychol Med. 2007;37(5):679–687. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Cavanagh JT, Van Beck M, Muir W, Blackwood DH. Case-control study of neurocognitive function in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder: an association with mania. Br J Psychiatry. 2002;180:320–326. doi: 10.1192/bjp.180.4.320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Kaya E, Aydemir O, Selcuki D. Residual symptoms in bipolar disorder: the effect of the last episode after remission. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2007;31(7):1387–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Torrent C, Sanchez-Moreno J, Goikolea JM, Salamero M, Malhi GS, Gonzalez-Pinto A, Daban C, Alvarez-Grandi S, et al. Functional outcome in bipolar disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(1–2):103–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00327.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Atre-Vaidya N, Taylor MA, Seidenberg M, Reed R, Perrine A, Glick-Oberwise F. Cognitive deficits, psychopathology, and psychosocial functioning in bipolar mood disorder. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1998;11(3):120–126. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Bourne C, Bilderbeck A, Drennan R, Atkinson L, Price J, Geddes JR, Goodwin GM. Verbal learning impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: BDI v BDII. J Affect Disord. 2015;182:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.04.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Reinares M, Colom F, Torrent C, Sanchez-Moreno J, Benabarre A, Goikolea JM, Comes M, Salamero M. Cognitive function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(2):262–270. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Fleck DE, Shear PK, Zimmerman ME, Getz GE, Corey KB, Jak A, Lebowitz BK, Strakowski SM. Verbal memory in mania: effects of clinical state and task requirements. Bipolar Disord. 2003;5(5):375–380. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2003.00055.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.van Gorp WG, Altshuler L, Theberge DC, Wilkins J, Dixon W. Cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients with and without prior alcohol dependence. A preliminary study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(1):41–46. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Bas TO, Poyraz CA, Bas A, Poyraz BC, Tosun M. The impact of cognitive impairment, neurological soft signs and subdepressive symptoms on functional outcome in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2015;174:336–341. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Verdoux H, Liraud F. Neuropsychological function in subjects with psychotic and affective disorders. Relationship to diagnostic category and duration of illness. Eur Psychiatry. 2000;15(4):236–243. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(00)00238-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Caletti E, Paoli RA, Fiorentini A, Cigliobianco M, Zugno E, Serati M, Orsenigo G, Grillo P, Zago S, Caldiroli A, et al. Neuropsychology, social cognition and global functioning among bipolar, schizophrenic patients and healthy controls: preliminary data. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:661. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Czepielewski LS, Massuda R, Goi P, Sulzbach-Vianna M, Reckziegel R, Costanzi M, Kapczinski F, Rosa AR, Gama CS. Verbal episodic memory along the course of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a new perspective. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25(2):169–175. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Santos JL, Aparicio A, Bagney A, Sanchez-Morla EM, Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Mateo J, Jimenez-Arriero MA. A 5-year follow-up study of neurocognitive functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2014;16(7):722–731. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Chakrabarty T, Kozicky JM, Torres IJ, Lam RW, Yatham LN. Verbal memory impairment in new onset bipolar disorder: relationship with frontal and medial temporal morphology. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2015;16(4):249–260. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2014.1000373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Brambilla P, Cerruti S, Bellani M, Perlini C, Ferro A, Marinelli V, Giusto D, Tomelleri L, Rambaldelli G, Tansella M, et al. Shared impairment in associative learning in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35(4):1093–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Chrobak AA, Siuda-Krzywicka K, Siwek GP, Arciszewska A, Siwek M, Starowicz-Filip A, Dudek D. Implicit motor learning in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2015;174:250–256. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Ha TH, Kim JS, Chang JS, Oh SH, Her JY, Cho HS, Park TS, Shin SY, Ha K. Verbal and visual memory impairments in bipolar I and II disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2012;9(4):339–346. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.4.339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Deckersbach T, McMurrich S, Ogutha J, Savage CR, Sachs G, Rauch SL. Characteristics of non-verbal memory impairment in bipolar disorder: the role of encoding strategies. Psychol Med. 2004;34(5):823–832. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703001685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Scott J, Stanton B, Garland A, Ferrier IN. Cognitive vulnerability in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2000;30(2):467–472. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Kim WJ, Ha RY, Sun JY, Ryu V, Lee SJ, Ha K, Cho HS. Autobiographical memory and its association with neuropsychological function in bipolar disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(2):290–297. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Chan RC, Lui SS, Wang Y, Liu AC, Chui WW, Shum DH, Cheung EF. Patients with bipolar disorders share similar but attenuated prospective memory impairments with patients with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2012;43(8):1639–1649. doi: 10.1017/S003329171200236X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Zhou JJ, Xiang YT, Wang CY, Zhou FC, Ungvari GS, Dickerson F, Chiu HF, Lai KY, Shum DH, Lee E, et al. Prospective memory deficits in euthymic bipolar disorder patients: a preliminary study. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry. 2013;5(3):183–190. doi: 10.1111/appy.12019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Bauer IE, Keefe RS, Sanches M, Suchting R, Green CE, Soares JC. Evaluation of cognitive function in bipolar disorder using the brief assessment of cognition in affective disorders (BAC-A) J Psychiatr Res. 2015;60:81–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Correa MS, da Silveira EM, de Lima DB, Balardin JB, Walz JC, Kapczinski F, Bromberg E. The role of encoding strategies in contextual memory deficits in patients with bipolar disorder. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2015;25(1):122–136. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2014.969281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Larson ER, Shear PK, Krikorian R, Welge J, Strakowski SM. Working memory and inhibitory control among manic and euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11(2):163–172. doi: 10.1017/s1355617705050228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Docherty NM, Hawkins KA, Hoffman RE, Quinlan DM, Rakfeldt J, Sledge WH. Working memory, attention, and communication disturbances in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105(2):212–219. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.2.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Thompson JM, Gray JM, Hughes JH, Watson S, Young AH, Ferrier IN. Impaired working memory monitoring in euthymic bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(5):478–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00470.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Pirkola T, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Glahn D, Kieseppa T, Haukka J, Kaprio J, Lonnqvist J, Cannon TD. Spatial working memory function in twins with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(12):930–936. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Landro NI, Orbeck AL, Rund BR. Memory functioning in chronic and non-chronic schizophrenics, affectively disturbed patients and normal controls. Schizophr Res. 1993;10(1):85–92. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(93)90080-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Kerry RJ, McDermott CM, Orme JE. Affective disorders and cognitive performance. A clinical report. J Affect Disord. 1983;5(4):349–352. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(83)90025-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Van Rheenen TE, Rossell SL. Investigation of the component processes involved in verbal declarative memory function in bipolar disorder: utility of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(7):727–735. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Bearden CE, Glahn DC, Monkul ES, Barrett J, Najt P, Kaur S, Sanches M, Villarreal V, Bowden C, Soares JC. Sources of declarative memory impairment in bipolar disorder: mnemonic processes and clinical features. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40(1):47–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.van Gorp WG, Altshuler L, Theberge DC, Mintz J. Declarative and procedural memory in bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46(4):525–531. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00336-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Christensen H, Griffiths K, Mackinnon A, Jacomb P. A quantitative review of cognitive deficits in depression and Alzheimer-type dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1997;3(6):631–651. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Weingartner H, Cohen RM, Murphy DL, Martello J, Gerdt C. Cognitive processes in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38(1):42–47. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780260044004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Weingartner H, Miller H, Murphy DL. Mood-state-dependent retrieval of verbal associations. J Abnorm Psychol. 1977;86(3):276–284. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.86.3.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Roy-Byrne PP, Weingartner H, Bierer LM, Thompson K, Post RM. Effortful and automatic cognitive processes in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43(3):265–267. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800030083008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.el Massioui F, Lesevre N. Attention impairment and psychomotor retardation in depressed patients: an event-related potential study. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1988;70(1):46–55. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90193-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Hartlage S, Alloy LB, Vazquez C, Dykman B. Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychol Bull. 1993;113(2):247–278. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Den Hartog HM, Derix MM, Van Bemmel AL, Kremer B, Jolles J. Cognitive functioning in young and middle-aged unmedicated out-patients with major depression: testing the effort and cognitive speed hypotheses. Psychol Med. 2003;33(8):1443–1451. doi: 10.1017/S003329170300833X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Hammar A. Automatic and effortful information processing in unipolar major depression. Scand J Psychol. 2003;44(5):409–413. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-9450.2003.00361.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Hammar A, Lund A, Hugdahl K. Selective impairment in effortful information processing in major depression. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(6):954–959. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703960152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Hammar A, Lund A, Hugdahl K. Long-lasting cognitive impairment in unipolar major depression: a 6-month follow-up study. Psychiatry Res. 2003;118(2):189–196. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00075-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Kremen WS, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Is there disproportionate impairment in semantic or phonemic fluency in schizophrenia? J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(1):79–88. doi: 10.1017/s1355617703910095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.de Almeida Rocca CC, de Macedo-Soares MB, Gorenstein C, Tamada RS, Isller CK, Dias RS, de Almeida KM, Schwartzmann AM, Amaral JA, Lafer B. Verbal fluency dysfunction in euthymic bipolar patients: a controlled study. J Affect Disord. 2008;107(1–3):187–192. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Sung K, Gordon B, Vannorsdall TD, Ledoux K, Schretlen DJ. Impaired retrieval of semantic information in bipolar disorder: a clustering analysis of category-fluency productions. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(3):624–634. doi: 10.1037/a0033068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Selva G, Salazar J, Balanza-Martinez V, Martinez-Aran A, Rubio C, Daban C, Sanchez-Moreno J, Vieta E, Tabares-Seisdedos R. Bipolar I patients with and without a history of psychotic symptoms: do they differ in their cognitive functioning? J Psychiatr Res. 2007;41(3–4):265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Sapin LR, Berrettini WH, Nurnberger JI, Jr, Rothblat LA. Mediational factors underlying cognitive changes and laterality in affective illness. Biol Psychiatry. 1987;22(8):979–986. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(87)90007-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Ferrier IN, Stanton BR, Kelly TP, Scott J. Neuropsychological function in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;175:246–251. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.3.246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Addington J, Addington D. Facial affect recognition and information processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 1998;32(3):171–181. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00042-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Gallagher P, Gray JM, Kessels RP. Fractionation of visuo-spatial memory processes in bipolar depression: a cognitive scaffolding account. Psychol Med. 2015;45(3):545–58. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 195.Frantom LV, Allen DN, Cross CL. Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(3):387–399. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00529.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Chen Y, Bidwell LC, Holzman PS. Visual motion integration in schizophrenia patients, their first-degree relatives, and patients with bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 2005;74(2–3):271–281. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Frangou S, Donaldson S, Hadjulis M, Landau S, Goldstein LH. The Maudsley Bipolar Disorder Project: executive dysfunction in bipolar disorder I and its clinical correlates. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(11):859–864. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Martinez-Aran A, Penades R, Vieta E, Colom F, Reinares M, Benabarre A, Salamero M, Gasto C. Executive function in patients with remitted bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and its relationship with functional outcome. Psychother Psychosom. 2002;71(1):39–46. doi: 10.1159/000049342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Rossi A, Arduini L, Daneluzzo E, Bustini M, Prosperini P, Stratta P. Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar patients, stabilized schizophrenic patients, and healthy controls. J Psychiatr Res. 2000;34(4–5):333–339. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(00)00025-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Torralva T, Strejilevich S, Gleichgerrcht E, Roca M, Martino D, Cetkovich M, Manes F. Deficits in tasks of executive functioning that mimic real-life scenarios in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(1):118–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.00987.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Wolf F, Brune M, Assion HJ. Theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2010;12(6):657–666. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00854.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Borkowska A, Rybakowski JK. Neuropsychological frontal lobe tests indicate that bipolar depressed patients are more impaired than unipolar. Bipolar Disord. 2001;3(2):88–94. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2001.030207.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Tien AY, Ross DE, Pearlson G, Strauss ME. Eye movements and psychopathology in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1996;184(6):331–338. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199606000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Brambilla P, Macdonald AW, 3rd, Sassi RB, Johnson MK, Mallinger AG, Carter CS, Soares JC. Context processing performance in bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(3):230–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00398.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Waddington JL, Brown K, O’Neill J, McKeon P, Kinsella A. Cognitive impairment, clinical course and treatment history in out-patients with bipolar affective disorder: relationship to tardive dyskinesia. Psychol Med. 1989;19(4):897–902. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700005614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Savard RJ, Rey AC, Post RM. Halstead-Reitan Category Test in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Relationship to age and phase of illness. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1980;168(5):297–304. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198005000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Rubinsztein JS, Michael A, Paykel ES, Sahakian BJ. Cognitive impairment in remission in bipolar affective disorder. Psychol Med. 2000;30(5):1025–1036. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799002664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Daniel BD, Montali A, Gerra ML, Innamorati M, Girardi P, Pompili M, Amore M. Cognitive impairment and its associations with the path of illness in affective disorders: a comparison between patients with bipolar and unipolar depression in remission. J Psychiatr Pract. 2013;19(4):275–287. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000432597.79019.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 209.David DP, Soeiro-de-Souza MG, Moreno RA, Bio DS. Facial emotion recognition and its correlation with executive functions in bipolar I patients and healthy controls. J Affect Disord. 2014;152–154:288–294. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.Park S. Association of an oculomotor delayed response task and the Wisconsin Card Sort Test in schizophrenic patients. Int J Psychophysiol. 1997;27(2):147–151. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00045-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 211.Young DA, Zakzanis KK, Bailey C, Davila R, Griese J, Sartory G, Thom A. Further parameters of insight and neuropsychological deficit in schizophrenia and other chronic mental disease. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1998;186(1):44–50. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199801000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 212.Arduini L, Kalyvoka A, Stratta P, Rinaldi O, Daneluzzo E, Rossi A. Insight and neuropsychological function in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with psychotic features. Can J Psychiatry. 2003;48(5):338–341. doi: 10.1177/070674370304800510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Barbosa IG, Rocha NP, Huguet RB, Ferreira RA, Salgado JV, Carvalho LA, Pariante CM, Teixeira AL. Executive dysfunction in euthymic bipolar disorder patients and its association with plasma biomarkers. J Affect Disord. 2012;137(1–3):151–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.Blumberg HP, Leung HC, Skudlarski P, Lacadie CM, Fredericks CA, Harris BC, Charney DS, Gore JC, Krystal JH, Peterson BS. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of bipolar disorder: state- and trait-related dysfunction in ventral prefrontal cortices. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(6):601–609. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.Kerr N, Scott J, Phillips ML. Patterns of attentional deficits and emotional bias in bipolar and major depressive disorder. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005;44(Pt 3):343–356. doi: 10.1348/014466505X57755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 216.Adida M, Jollant F, Clark L, Besnier N, Guillaume S, Kaladjian A, Mazzola-Pomietto P, Jeanningros R, Goodwin GM, Azorin JM, et al. Trait-related decision-making impairment in the three phases of bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(4):357–365. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 217.Yechiam E, Hayden EP, Bodkins M, O’Donnell BF, Hetrick WP. Decision making in bipolar disorder: a cognitive modeling approach. Psychiatry Res. 2008;161(2):142–152. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 218.Chandler RA, Wakeley J, Goodwin GM, Rogers RD. Altered risk-aversion and risk-seeking behavior in bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(9):840–846. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 219.Kathleen Holmes M, Bearden CE, Barguil M, Fonseca M, Serap Monkul E, Nery FG, Soares JC, Mintz J, Glahn DC. Conceptualizing impulsivity and risk taking in bipolar disorder: importance of history of alcohol abuse. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(1):33–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00657.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Fleck DE, Shear PK, Madore M, Strakowski SM. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in bipolar disorder: effects of mood state and early course. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(4):539–545. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00582.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Friedman MJ, Culver CM, Ferrell RB. On the safety of long-term treatment with lithium. Am J Psychiatry. 1977;134(10):1123–1126. doi: 10.1176/ajp.134.10.1123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 222.Aminoff SR, Jensen J, Lagerberg TV, Hellvin T, Sundet K, Andreassen OA, Melle I. An association between affective lability and executive functioning in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2012;198(1):58–61. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 223.Dias VV, Brissos S, Carita AI. Clinical and neurocognitive correlates of insight in patients with bipolar I disorder in remission. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2008;117(1):28–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01110.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 224.Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(4):268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 225.Samame C, Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA. Social cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder: systematic review and meta-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;125(4):266–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01808.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 226.Cusi A, Macqueen GM, McKinnon MC. Altered self-report of empathic responding in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(2):354–358. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 227.Lahera G, Montes JM, Benito A, Valdivia M, Medina E, Mirapeix I, Saiz-Ruiz J. Theory of mind deficit in bipolar disorder: is it related to a previous history of psychotic symptoms? Psychiatry Res. 2008;161(3):309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 228.Bora E, Vahip S, Gonul AS, Akdeniz F, Alkan M, Ogut M, Eryavuz A. Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(2):110–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00570.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 229.Donohoe G, Duignan A, Hargreaves A, Morris DW, Rose E, Robertson D, Cummings E, Moore S, Gill M, Corvin A. Social cognition in bipolar disorder versus schizophrenia: comparability in mental state decoding deficits. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(7):743–748. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 230.Cusi AM, Macqueen GM, McKinnon MC. Patients with bipolar disorder show impaired performance on complex tests of social cognition. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200(2–3):258–264. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 231.Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Fassi G, Marengo E, Igoa A. Theory of mind and facial emotion recognition in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2011;189(3):379–384. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 232.Inoue Y, Tonooka Y, Yamada K, Kanba S. Deficiency of theory of mind in patients with remitted mood disorder. J Affect Disord. 2004;82(3):403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 233.Barrera A, Vázquez G, Tannenhaus L, Lolich M, Herbst L. Theory of mind and functionality in bipolar patients with symptomatic remission [Article in English, Spanish] Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. 2013;6(2):67–74. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsm.2012.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 234.Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle MC. How do people with schizophrenia explain the behaviour of others? A study of theory of mind and its relationship to thought and speech disorganization in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1999;29(3):613–620. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 235.Kerr N, Dunbar RI, Bentall RP. Theory of mind deficits in bipolar affective disorder. J Affect Disord. 2003;73(3):253–259. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 236.Rossell SL, Van Rheenen TE. Theory of mind performance using a story comprehension task in bipolar mania compared to schizophrenia and healthy controls. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18(5):409–421. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.725820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 237.Kim E, Jung YC, Ku J, Kim JJ, Lee H, Kim SY, Kim SI, Cho HS. Reduced activation in the mirror neuron system during a virtual social cognition task in euthymic bipolar disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33(8):1409–1416. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 238.Montag C, Ehrlich A, Neuhaus K, Dziobek I, Heekeren HR, Heinz A, Gallinat J. Theory of mind impairments in euthymic bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 2010;123(1–3):264–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 239.Shamay-Tsoory S, Harari H, Szepsenwol O, Levkovitz Y. Neuropsychological evidence of impaired cognitive empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;21(1):59–67. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2009.21.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 240.Lahera G, Ruiz-Murugarren S, Iglesias P, Ruiz-Bennasar C, Herreria E, Montes JM, Fernandez-Liria A. Social cognition and global functioning in bipolar disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2012;200(2):135–141. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182438eae. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 241.Bonshtein U, Leiser D, Levine J. Naive theory impairment in schizophrenia: is it domain-specific? J Nerv Ment Dis. 2006;194(10):753–759. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000243083.75008.ae. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 242.Rubinow DR, Post RM. Impaired recognition of affect in facial expression in depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry. 1992;31(9):947–953. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90120-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 243.Brotman MA, Guyer AE, Lawson ES, Horsey SE, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Facial emotion labeling deficits in children and adolescents at risk for bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(3):385–389. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06122050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 244.Bozikas VP, Tonia T, Fokas K, Karavatos A, Kosmidis MH. Impaired emotion processing in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2006;91(1):53–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 245.Lennox BR, Jacob R, Calder AJ, Lupson V, Bullmore ET. Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania. Psychol Med. 2004;34(5):795–802. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 246.Getz GE, Shear PK, Strakowski SM. Facial affect recognition deficits in bipolar disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(4):623–632. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703940021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 247.Lembke A, Ketter TA. Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(2):302–304. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 248.Loughland CM, Williams LM, Gordon E. Schizophrenia and affective disorder show different visual scanning behavior for faces: a trait versus state-based distinction? Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52(4):338–348. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01356-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 249.Yurgelun-Todd DA, Gruber SA, Kanayama G, Killgore WD, Baird AA, Young AD. fMRI during affect discrimination in bipolar affective disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2000;2(3 Pt 2):237–248. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2000.20304.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 250.Derntl B, Seidel EM, Kryspin-Exner I, Hasmann A, Dobmeier M. Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Br J Clin Psychol. 2009;48(Pt 4):363–375. doi: 10.1348/014466509X404845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 251.Benito A, Lahera G, Herrera S, Muncharaz R, Benito G, Fernandez-Liria A, Montes JM. Deficits in recognition, identification, and discrimination of facial emotions in patients with bipolar disorder. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2013;35(4):435–438. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2013-1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 252.Vaskinn A, Sundet K, Friis S, Simonsen C, Birkenaes AB, Engh JA, Jonsdottir H, Ringen PA, Opjordsmoen S, Andreassen OA. The effect of gender on emotion perception in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116(4):263–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00991.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 253.Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Sachdev PS, Ivanovski B, Shnier R, Ketter T. Is a lack of disgust something to fear? A functional magnetic resonance imaging facial emotion recognition study in euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(4):345–357. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00485.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 254.Kucharska-Pietura K, David AS. The perception of emotional chimeric faces in patients with depression, mania and unilateral brain damage. Psychol Med. 2003;33(4):739–745. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702007316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 255.Bellack AS, Blanchard JJ, Mueser KT. Cue availability and affect perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1996;22(3):535–544. doi: 10.1093/schbul/22.3.535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 256.Robinson JL, Monkul ES, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, Franklin C, Bearden CE, Fox PT, Glahn DC. Fronto-limbic circuitry in euthymic bipolar disorder: evidence for prefrontal hyperactivation. Psychiatry Res. 2008;164(2):106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 257.Surguladze SA, Marshall N, Schulze K, Hall MH, Walshe M, Bramon E, Phillips ML, Murray RM, McDonald C. Exaggerated neural response to emotional faces in patients with bipolar disorder and their first-degree relatives. Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 258.Venn HR, Gray JM, Montagne B, Murray LK, Michael Burt D, Frigerio E, Perrett DI, Young AH. Perception of facial expressions of emotion in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(4):286–293. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00121.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 259.Harmer CJ, Grayson L, Goodwin GM. Enhanced recognition of disgust in bipolar illness. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51(4):298–304. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01249-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 260.David AS, Cutting JC. Affect, affective disorder and schizophrenia. A neuropsychological investigation of right hemisphere function. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;156:491–495. doi: 10.1192/bjp.156.4.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 261.Almeida JR, Versace A, Hassel S, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Elevated amygdala activity to sad facial expressions: a state marker of bipolar but not unipolar depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 262.Wegbreit E, Weissman AB, Cushman GK, Puzia ME, Kim KL, Leibenluft E, Dickstein DP. Facial emotion recognition in childhood-onset bipolar I disorder: an evaluation of developmental differences between youths and adults. Bipolar Disord. 2015;17(5):471–485. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 263.Lior R, Nachson I. Impairments in judgment of chimeric faces by schizophrenic and affective patients. Int J Neurosci. 1999;97(3–4):185–209. doi: 10.3109/00207459909000660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 264.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment: a distinct trait-marker for schizophrenia spectrum disorders and bipolar disorder? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;120(4):253–264. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01414.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 265.Bearden CE, Hoffman KM, Cannon TD. The neuropsychology and neuroanatomy of bipolar affective disorder: a critical review. Bipolar Disord. 2001;3(3):106–150. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2001.030302.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 266.Rocca CC, Heuvel E, Caetano SC, Lafer B. Facial emotion recognition in bipolar disorder: a critical review. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2009;31(2):171–180. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462009000200015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 267.Fountoulakis KN, Vieta E, Bouras C, Notaridis G, Giannakopoulos P, Kaprinis G, Akiskal H. A systematic review of existing data on long-term lithium therapy: neuroprotective or neurotoxic? Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;11(2):269–287. doi: 10.1017/S1461145707007821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 268.Goldberg JF, Chengappa KN. Identifying and treating cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(Suppl 2):123–137. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 269.Shaw ED, Mann JJ, Stokes PE, Manevitz AZ. Effects of lithium carbonate on associative productivity and idiosyncrasy in bipolar outpatients. Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143(9):1166–1169. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.9.1166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 270.Stoll AL, Locke CA, Vuckovic A, Mayer PV. Lithium-associated cognitive and functional deficits reduced by a switch to divalproex sodium: a case series. J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57(8):356–359. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 271.Squire LR, Judd LL, Janowsky DS, Huey LY. Effects of lithium carbonate on memory and other cognitive functions. Am J Psychiatry. 1980;137(9):1042–1046. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.9.1042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 272.Kocsis JH, Shaw ED, Stokes PE, Wilner P, Elliot AS, Sikes C, Myers B, Manevitz A, Parides M. Neuropsychologic effects of lithium discontinuation. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1993;13(4):268–275. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199308000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 273.Honig A, Arts BM, Ponds RW, Riedel WJ. Lithium induced cognitive side-effects in bipolar disorder: a qualitative analysis and implications for daily practice. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 1999;14(3):167–171. doi: 10.1097/00004850-199905030-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 274.Lund Y, Nissen M, Rafaelsen OJ. Long-term lithium treatment and psychological functions. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1982;65(3):233–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1982.tb00843.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 275.Kessing LV. Cognitive impairment in the euthymic phase of affective disorder. Psychol Med. 1998;28(5):1027–1038. doi: 10.1017/S0033291798006862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 276.Engelsmann F, Katz J, Ghadirian AM, Schachter D. Lithium and memory: a long-term follow-up study. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1988;8(3):207–212. doi: 10.1097/00004714-198806000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 277.Goldberg J. Adverse cognitive effects of psychotropic medications. In: Goldberg J, Burdick K, editors. Cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder: a guide for clinicians. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 2008. pp. 137–158. [Google Scholar]
  • 278.Shaw ED, Stokes PE, Mann JJ, Manevitz AZ. Effects of lithium carbonate on the memory and motor speed of bipolar outpatients. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96(1):64–69. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.96.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 279.Reus VI, Targum SD, Weingarter H, Post RM. Effect of lithium carbonate on memory processes of bipolar affectively ill patients. Psychopharmacology. 1979;63(1):39–42. doi: 10.1007/BF00426919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 280.Karniol IG, Dalton J, Lader MH. Acute and chronic effects of lithium chloride on physiological and psychological measures in normals. Psychopharmacology. 1978;57(3):289–294. doi: 10.1007/BF00426753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 281.Kropf D, Muller-Oerlinghausen B. Changes in learning, memory, and mood during lithium treatment. Approach to a research strategy. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1979;59(1):97–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1979.tb06951.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 282.Senturk V, Goker C, Bilgic A, Olmez S, Tugcu H, Oncu B, Atbasoglu EC. Impaired verbal memory and otherwise spared cognition in remitted bipolar patients on monotherapy with lithium or valproate. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(Suppl 1):136–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00481.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 283.Connelly CE, Davenport YB, Nurnberger JI., Jr Adherence to treatment regimen in a lithium carbonate clinic. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39(5):585–588. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290050057011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 284.Maarbjerg K, Aagaard J, Vestergaard P. Adherence to lithium prophylaxis: I. Clinical predictors and patient’s reasons for nonadherence. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1988;21(3):121–125. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-1014662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 285.Jaeger J, Berns S, Loftus S, Gonzalez C, Czobor P. Neurocognitive test performance predicts functional recovery from acute exacerbation leading to hospitalization in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9(1–2):93–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00427.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 286.Wingo AP, Wingo TS, Harvey PD, Baldessarini RJ. Effects of lithium on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. J Clin Psychiatry. 2009;70(11):1588–1597. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08r04972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 287.Holmes MK, Erickson K, Luckenbaugh DA, Drevets WC, Bain EE, Cannon DM, Snow J, Sahakian BJ, Manji HK, Zarate CA., Jr A comparison of cognitive functioning in medicated and unmedicated subjects with bipolar depression. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(7):806–815. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00628.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 288.Pan YJ, Hsieh MH, Liu SK. Visuospatial working memory deficits in remitted patients with bipolar disorder: susceptibility to the effects of GABAergic agonists. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(4):365–376. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00931.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 289.Thompson PJ, Trimble MR. Anticonvulsant drugs and cognitive functions. Epilepsia. 1982;23(5):531–544. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1982.tb05439.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 290.Salinsky MC, Storzbach D, Spencer DC, Oken BS, Landry T, Dodrill CB. Effects of topiramate and gabapentin on cognitive abilities in healthy volunteers. Neurology. 2005;64(5):792–798. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152877.08088.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 291.King DJ. Psychomotor impairment and cognitive disturbances induced by neuroleptics. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1994;380:53–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1994.tb05833.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 292.Jamrozinski K, Gruber O, Kemmer C, Falkai P, Scherk H. Neurocognitive functions in euthymic bipolar patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119(5):365–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01320.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 293.Reinares M, Martinez-Aran A, Colom F, Benabarre A, Salamero M, Vieta E. Long-term effects of the treatment with risperidone versus conventional neuroleptics on the neuropsychological performance of euthymic bipolar patients. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2000;28(4):231–238. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 294.Maalouf FT, Klein C, Clark L, Sahakian BJ, Labarbara EJ, Versace A, Hassel S, Almeida JR, Phillips ML. Impaired sustained attention and executive dysfunction: bipolar disorder versus depression-specific markers of affective disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(6):1862–1868. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 295.Goswami U, Sharma A, Varma A, Gulrajani C, Ferrier IN, Young AH, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Moore PB. The neurocognitive performance of drug-free and medicated euthymic bipolar patients do not differ. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;120(6):456–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 296.Roiser JP, Cannon DM, Gandhi SK, Taylor Tavares J, Erickson K, Wood S, Klaver JM, Clark L, Zarate CA, Jr, Sahakian BJ, et al. Hot and cold cognition in unmedicated depressed subjects with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(2):178–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00669.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 297.Lopez-Jaramillo C, Lopera-Vasquez J, Gallo A, Ospina-Duque J, Bell V, Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E. Effects of recurrence on the cognitive performance of patients with bipolar I disorder: implications for relapse prevention and treatment adherence. Bipolar Disord. 2010;12(5):557–567. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00835.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 298.Martinez-Aran A, Scott J, Colom F, Torrent C, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Daban C, Leboyer M, Henry C, Goodwin GM, Gonzalez-Pinto A, et al. Treatment nonadherence and neurocognitive impairment in bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2009;70(7):1017–1023. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 299.Stein RA, Strickland TL. A review of the neuropsychological effects of commonly used prescription medications. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;13(3):259–284. doi: 10.1093/arclin/13.3.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 300.Joffe RT, MacDonald C, Kutcher SP. Lack of differential cognitive effects of lithium and carbamazepine in bipolar affective disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1988;8(6):425–428. doi: 10.1097/00004714-198812000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 301.Nehra R, Chakrabarti S, Pradhan BK, Khehra N. Comparison of cognitive functions between first- and multi-episode bipolar affective disorders. J Affect Disord. 2006;93(1–3):185–192. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 302.Albus M, Hubmann W, Wahlheim C, Sobizack N, Franz U, Mohr F. Contrasts in neuropsychological test profile between patients with first-episode schizophrenia and first-episode affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1996;94(2):87–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb09830.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 303.Levy B, Weiss RD. Neurocognitive impairment and psychosis in bipolar I disorder during early remission from an acute episode of mood disturbance. J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(2):201–206. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04663yel. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 304.Sheffield JM, Williams LE, Cohen N, Heckers S. Relational memory in psychotic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(5):537–546. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01036.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 305.Glahn DC, Barrett J, Bearden CE, Mintz J, Green MF, Serap Monkul E, Najt P, Soares JC, Velligan DI. Dissociable mechanisms for memory impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2006;36(8):1085–1095. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 306.Savitz J, van der Merwe L, Stein DJ, Solms M, Ramesar R. Neuropsychological status of bipolar I disorder: impact of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;194(3):243–251. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.052001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 307.Daban C, Martinez-Aran A, Torrent C, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Balanza-Martinez V, Salazar-Fraile J, Selva-Vera G, Vieta E. Specificity of cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder versus schizophrenia. A systematic review. Psychother Psychosom. 2006;75(2):72–84. doi: 10.1159/000090891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 308.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Neurocognitive markers of psychosis in bipolar disorder: a meta-analytic study. J Affect Disord. 2010;127(1–3):1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 309.Tabares-Seisdedos R, Balanza-Martinez V, Salazar-Fraile J, Selva-Vera G, Leal-Cercos C, Gomez-Beneyto M. Specific executive/attentional deficits in patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder who have a positive family history of psychosis. J Psychiatr Res. 2003;37(6):479–486. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(03)00071-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 310.Schubert EW, McNeil TF. Neuropsychological impairment and its neurological correlates in adult offspring with heightened risk for schizophrenia and affective psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(4):758–766. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.4.758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 311.Jabben N, Arts B, Krabbendam L, van Os J. Investigating the association between neurocognition and psychosis in bipolar disorder: further evidence for the overlap with schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(2):166–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00663.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 312.Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, Amann B, Daban C, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Gonzalez-Pinto A, Reinares M, Benabarre A, Salamero M, McKenna P, et al. Cognitive impairment in schizoaffective disorder: a comparison with non-psychotic bipolar and healthy subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;116(6):453–460. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01072.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 313.Studentkowski G, Scheele D, Calabrese P, Balkau F, Hoffler J, Aubel T, Edel MA, Juckel G, Assion HJ. Cognitive impairment in patients with a schizoaffective disorder: a comparison with bipolar patients in euthymia. Eur J Med Res. 2010;15(2):70–78. doi: 10.1186/2047-783X-15-2-70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 314.Ryan KA, Vederman AC, McFadden EM, Weldon AL, Kamali M, Langenecker SA, McInnis MG. Differential executive functioning performance by phase of bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(5):527–536. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01032.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 315.Dixon T, Kravariti E, Frith C, Murray RM, McGuire PK. Effect of symptoms on executive function in bipolar illness. Psychol Med. 2004;34(5):811–821. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703001570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 316.van der Werf-Eldering MJ, Burger H, Holthausen EA, Aleman A, Nolen WA. Cognitive functioning in patients with bipolar disorder: association with depressive symptoms and alcohol use. PLoS One. 2010;5(9). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 317.Depp CA, Savla GN, de Dios LA, Mausbach BT, Palmer BW. Affective symptoms and intra-individual variability in the short-term course of cognitive functioning in bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2012;42(7):1409–1416. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 318.McKay AP, Tarbuck AF, Shapleske J, McKenna PJ. Neuropsychological function in manic-depressive psychosis. Evidence for persistent deficits in patients with chronic, severe illness. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167(1):51–57. doi: 10.1192/bjp.167.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 319.Jamrozinski K. Do euthymic bipolar patients have normal cognitive functioning? Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2010;23(3):255–260. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328338620a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 320.Gruber S, Rathgeber K, Braunig P, Gauggel S. Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in manic and depressed bipolar patients compared to patients with Major Depression. J Affect Disord. 2007;104(1–3):61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 321.Newcomer JW. Medical risk in patients with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67(11):e16. doi: 10.4088/JCP.1106e16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 322.Dhingra U, Rabins PV. Mania in the elderly: a 5–7 year follow-up. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39(6):581–583. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb03597.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 323.Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neuropsychological performance predicts clinical recovery in bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 2008;105(1–3):253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 324.Savitz J, Solms M, Ramesar R. Neuropsychological dysfunction in bipolar affective disorder: a critical opinion. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7(3):216–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00203.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 325.Yates DB, Dittmann S, Kapczinski F, Trentini CM. Cognitive abilities and clinical variables in bipolar I depressed and euthymic patients and controls. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(4):495–504. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 326.Burt T, Prudic J, Peyser S, Clark J, Sackeim HA. Learning and memory in bipolar and unipolar major depression: effects of aging. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2000;13(4):246–253. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 327.Fountoulakis KN, Panagiotidis PT, Siamouli M, Magiria S, Sokolaki S, Kantartzis S, Rova K, Papastergiou N, Shoretstanitis G, Oral T, et al. Development of a standardized scoring method for the Graphic Sequence Test suitable for use in psychiatric populations. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008;21(1):18–27. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e318165b120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 328.Fountoulakis KN, Siamouli M, Magiria S, Panagiotidis PT, Kantartzis S, Terzoglou VA, Oral T. A standardized scoring method for the copy of cube test, developed to be suitable for use in psychiatric populations. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2011;10(1):19. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-10-19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 329.Fountoulakis KN, Siamouli M, Panagiotidis PT, Magiria S, Kantartzis S, Terzoglou VA, Oral T. The standardised copy of pentagons test. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2011;10(1):13. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-10-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 330.Osuji IJ, Cullum CM. Cognition in bipolar disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2005;28(2):427–441. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2005.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 331.Dickstein DP, Treland JE, Snow J, McClure EB, Mehta MS, Towbin KE, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Neuropsychological performance in pediatric bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(1):32–39. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00701-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 332.Ancin I, Santos JL, Teijeira C, Sanchez-Morla EM, Bescos MJ, Argudo I, Torrijos S, Vazquez-Alvarez B, De La Vega I, Lopez-Ibor JJ, et al. Sustained attention as a potential endophenotype for bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;122(3):235–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01532.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 333.Denicoff KD, Ali SO, Mirsky AF, Smith-Jackson EE, Leverich GS, Duncan CC, Connell EG, Post RM. Relationship between prior course of illness and neuropsychological functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 1999;56(1):67–73. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00028-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 334.Tham A, Engelbrektson K, Mathe AA, Johnson L, Olsson E, Aberg-Wistedt A. Impaired neuropsychological performance in euthymic patients with recurring mood disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. 1997;58(1):26–29. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v58n0105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 335.Fossati P, Harvey PO, Le Bastard G, Ergis AM, Jouvent R, Allilaire JF. Verbal memory performance of patients with a first depressive episode and patients with unipolar and bipolar recurrent depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2004;38(2):137–144. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2003.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 336.Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Marengo E, Igoa A, Fassi G, Teitelbaum J, Caravotta P. Relationship between neurocognitive functioning and episode recurrences in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2013;147(1–3):345–351. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 337.Krabbendam L, Honig A, Wiersma J, Vuurman EF, Hofman PA, Derix MM, Nolen WA, Jolles J. Cognitive dysfunctions and white matter lesions in patients with bipolar disorder in remission. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2000;101(4):274–280. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 338.Yim CY, Soczynska JK, Kennedy SH, Woldeyohannes HO, Brietzke E, McIntyre RS. The effect of overweight/obesity on cognitive function in euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(3):223–228. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 339.Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Bowden CL, Soares JC. Reduced educational attainment in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. 2006;92(2–3):309–312. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.01.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 340.Sole B, Bonnin CM, Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, Popovic D, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Vieta E. Neurocognitive impairment across the bipolar spectrum. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2012;18(3):194–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2011.00262.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 341.Smith DJ, Muir WJ, Blackwood DH. Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic young adults with bipolar spectrum disorder and recurrent major depressive disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(1):40–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00275.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 342.Savitz JB, van der Merwe L, Stein DJ, Solms M, Ramesar RS. Neuropsychological task performance in bipolar spectrum illness: genetics, alcohol abuse, medication and childhood trauma. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(4):479–494. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 343.Taylor Tavares JV, Clark L, Cannon DM, Erickson K, Drevets WC, Sahakian BJ. Distinct profiles of neurocognitive function in unmedicated unipolar depression and bipolar II depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(8):917–924. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 344.Simonsen C, Sundet K, Vaskinn A, Birkenaes AB, Engh JA, Hansen CF, Jonsdottir H, Ringen PA, Opjordsmoen S, Friis S, et al. Neurocognitive profiles in bipolar I and bipolar II disorder: differences in pattern and magnitude of dysfunction. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(2):245–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00492.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 345.Andersson S, Barder HE, Hellvin T, Lovdahl H, Malt UF. Neuropsychological and electrophysiological indices of neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar II disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(8):888–899. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 346.Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, Daban C, Sanchez-Moreno J, Comes M, Goikolea JM, Salamero M, Vieta E. Cognitive impairment in bipolar II disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:254–259. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.017269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 347.Summers M, Papadopoulou K, Bruno S, Cipolotti L, Ron MA. Bipolar I and bipolar II disorder: cognition and emotion processing. Psychol Med. 2006;36(12):1799–1809. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 348.Harkavy-Friedman JM, Keilp JG, Grunebaum MF, Sher L, Printz D, Burke AK, Mann JJ, Oquendo M. Are BPI and BPII suicide attempters distinct neuropsychologically? J Affect Disord. 2006;94(1–3):255–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 349.Bruno SD, Papadopoulou K, Cercignani M, Cipolotti L, Ron MA. Structural brain correlates of IQ changes in bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2006;36(5):609–618. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 350.Berns GS, Martin M, Proper SM. Limbic hyperreactivity in bipolar II disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(2):304–306. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 351.Sole B, Martinez-Aran A, Torrent C, Bonnin CM, Reinares M, Popovic D, Sanchez-Moreno J, Vieta E. Are bipolar II patients cognitively impaired? A systematic review. Psychol Med. 2011;41(9):1791–1803. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 352.Lewandowski KE, Cohen BM, Ongur D. Evolution of neuropsychological dysfunction during the course of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2011;41(2):225–241. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 353.Quackenbush D, Kutcher S, Robertson HA, Boulos C, Chaban P. Premorbid and postmorbid school functioning in bipolar adolescents: description and suggested academic interventions. Can J Psychiatry. 1996;41(1):16–22. doi: 10.1177/070674379604100106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 354.Kutcher S, Robertson HA, Bird D. Premorbid functioning in adolescent onset bipolar I disorder: a preliminary report from an ongoing study. J Affect Disord. 1998;51(2):137–144. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00212-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 355.Uzelac S, Jaeger J, Berns S, Gonzales C. Premorbid adjustment in bipolar disorder: comparison with schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2006;194(9):654–658. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000235767.00901.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 356.Toulopoulou T, Quraishi S, McDonald C, Murray RM. The Maudsley Family Study: premorbid and current general intellectual function levels in familial bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(2):243–259. doi: 10.1080/13803390500360513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 357.Meyer SE, Carlson GA, Wiggs EA, Martinez PE, Ronsaville DS, Klimes-Dougan B, Gold PW, Radke-Yarrow M. A prospective study of the association among impaired executive functioning, childhood attentional problems, and the development of bipolar disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;16(2):461–476. doi: 10.1017/S095457940404461X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 358.Sigurdsson E, Fombonne E, Sayal K, Checkley S. Neurodevelopmental antecedents of early-onset bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174:121–127. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.2.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 359.Tiihonen J, Haukka J, Henriksson M, Cannon M, Kieseppa T, Laaksonen I, Sinivuo J, Lonnqvist J. Premorbid intellectual functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: results from a cohort study of male conscripts. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1904–1910. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 360.Hellgren L, Gillberg IC, Bagenholm A, Gillberg C. Children with deficits in attention, motor control and perception (DAMP) almost grown up: psychiatric and personality disorders at age 16 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35(7):1255–1271. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01233.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 361.Pavuluri MN, West A, Hill SK, Jindal K, Sweeney JA. Neurocognitive function in pediatric bipolar disorder: 3-year follow-up shows cognitive development lagging behind healthy youths. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(3):299–307. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318196b907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 362.Torres IJ, DeFreitas VG, DeFreitas CM, Kauer-Sant’Anna M, Bond DJ, Honer WG, Lam RW, Yatham LN. Neurocognitive functioning in patients with bipolar I disorder recently recovered from a first manic episode. J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(9):1234–1242. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04997yel. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 363.Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, del Mar Bonnin C, Reinares M, Daban C, Sole B, Rosa AR, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Popovic D, Salamero M, et al. Long-term outcome of cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73(7):e899–e905. doi: 10.4088/JCP.11m07471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 364.Mur M, Portella MJ, Martinez-Aran A, Pifarre J, Vieta E. Long-term stability of cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: a 2-year follow-up study of lithium-treated euthymic bipolar patients. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69(5):712–719. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 365.Balanza-Martinez V, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Selva-Vera G, Martinez-Aran A, Torrent C, Salazar-Fraile J, Leal-Cercos C, Vieta E, Gomez-Beneyto M. Persistent cognitive dysfunctions in bipolar I disorder and schizophrenic patients: a 3-year follow-up study. Psychother Psychosom. 2005;74(2):113–119. doi: 10.1159/000083170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 366.Lebowitz BK, Shear PK, Steed MA, Strakowski SM. Verbal fluency in mania: relationship to number of manic episodes. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001;14(3):177–182. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 367.Robinson LJ, Ferrier IN. Evolution of cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: a systematic review of cross-sectional evidence. Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(2):103–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00277.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 368.Kruger S, Seminowicz D, Goldapple K, Kennedy SH, Mayberg HS. State and trait influences on mood regulation in bipolar disorder: blood flow differences with an acute mood challenge. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(11):1274–1283. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00691-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 369.Tsuang MT, Woolson RF, Fleming JA. Long-term outcome of major psychoses. I. Schizophrenia and affective disorders compared with psychiatrically symptom-free surgical conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36(12):1295–1301. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1979.01780120025002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 370.Bearden CE, Woogen M, Glahn DC. Neurocognitive and neuroimaging predictors of clinical outcome in bipolar disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2010;12(6):499–504. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0151-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 371.Tabares-Seisdedos R, Balanza-Martinez V, Sanchez-Moreno J, Martinez-Aran A, Salazar-Fraile J, Selva-Vera G, Rubio C, Mata I, Gomez-Beneyto M, Vieta E. Neurocognitive and clinical predictors of functional outcome in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder at 1-year follow-up. J Affect Disord. 2008;109(3):286–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.12.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 372.Kessing LV, Andersen PK. Does the risk of developing dementia increase with the number of episodes in patients with depressive disorder and in patients with bipolar disorder? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75(12):1662–1666. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.031773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 373.Kessing LV, Nilsson FM. Increased risk of developing dementia in patients with major affective disorders compared to patients with other medical illnesses. J Affect Disord. 2003;73(3):261–269. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00004-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 374.van der Werf-Eldering MJ, Burger H, Jabben N, Holthausen EA, Aleman A, Nolen WA. Is the lack of association between cognitive complaints and objective cognitive functioning in patients with bipolar disorder moderated by depressive symptoms? J Affect Disord. 2011;130(1–2):306–311. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 375.Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Colom F, Torrent C, Reinares M, Goikolea JM, Benabarre A, Comes M, Sanchez-Moreno J. Do cognitive complaints in euthymic bipolar patients reflect objective cognitive impairment? Psychother Psychosom. 2005;74(5):295–302. doi: 10.1159/000086320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 376.Carrus D, Christodoulou T, Hadjulis M, Haldane M, Galea A, Koukopoulos A, Kumari V, Frangou S. Gender differences in immediate memory in bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2010;40(8):1349–1355. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 377.Kieseppa T, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Haukka J, Van Erp T, Glahn D, Cannon TD, Partonen T, Kaprio J, Lonnqvist J. Memory and verbal learning functions in twins with bipolar-I disorder, and the role of information-processing speed. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):205–215. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 378.Burdick KE, Gunawardane N, Woodberry K, Malhotra AK. The role of general intelligence as an intermediate phenotype for neuropsychiatric disorders. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2009;14(4–5):299–311. doi: 10.1080/13546800902805347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 379.Jabben N, Arts B, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Neurocognitive functioning as intermediary phenotype and predictor of psychosocial functioning across the psychosis continuum: studies in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(6):764–774. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04837yel. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 380.Kremen WS, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ, Pepple JR, Tsuang MT. Neuropsychological risk indicators for schizophrenia: a preliminary study of female relatives of schizophrenic and bipolar probands. Psychiatry Res. 1998;79(3):227–240. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00042-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 381.Gourovitch ML, Torrey EF, Gold JM, Randolph C, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. Neuropsychological performance of monozygotic twins discordant for bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45(5):639–646. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00148-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 382.Christensen MV, Kyvik KO, Kessing LV. Cognitive function in unaffected twins discordant for affective disorder. Psychol Med. 2006;36(8):1119–1129. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 383.Kravariti E, Schulze K, Kane F, Kalidindi S, Bramon E, Walshe M, Marshall N, Hall MH, Georgiades A, McDonald C, et al. Stroop-test interference in bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;194(3):285–286. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.052639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 384.Winters KC, Stone AA, Weintraub S, Neale JM. Cognitive and attentional deficits in children vulnerable to psychopathology. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1981;9(4):435–453. doi: 10.1007/BF00917794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 385.Harvey PD, Weintraub S, Neale JM. Speech competence of children vulnerable to psychopathology. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1982;10(3):373–387. doi: 10.1007/BF00912328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 386.Kron L, Decina P, Kestenbaum CJ, Farber S, Gargan M, Fieve R. The offspring of bipolar manic-depressives: clinical features. Adolesc Psychiatry. 1982;10:273–291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 387.McDonough-Ryan P, DelBello M, Shear PK, Ris DM, Soutullo C, Strakowski SM. Academic and cognitive abilities in children of parents with bipolar disorder: a test of the nonverbal learning disability model. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24(3):280–285. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.3.280.980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 388.Pierson A, Jouvent R, Quintin P, Perez-Diaz F, Leboyer M. Information processing deficits in relatives of manic depressive patients. Psychol Med. 2000;30(3):545–555. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799001944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 389.Frangou S, Haldane M, Roddy D, Kumari V. Evidence for deficit in tasks of ventral, but not dorsal, prefrontal executive function as an endophenotypic marker for bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(10):838–839. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 390.Klimes-Dougan B, Ronsaville D, Wiggs EA, Martinez PE. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescent children of mothers with a history of bipolar or major depressive disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(9):957–965. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 391.Antila M, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Kieseppa T, Soronen P, Palo OM, Paunio T, Haukka J, Partonen T, Lonnqvist J. Heritability of cognitive functions in families with bipolar disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B(6):802–808. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 392.Ferrier IN, Chowdhury R, Thompson JM, Watson S, Young AH. Neurocognitive function in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder: a preliminary report. Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(4):319–322. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 393.Schulze KK, Walshe M, Stahl D, Hall MH, Kravariti E, Morris R, Marshall N, McDonald C, Murray RM, Bramon E. Executive functioning in familial bipolar I disorder patients and their unaffected relatives. Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(2):208–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00901.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 394.Clark L, Sarna A, Goodwin GM. Impairment of executive function but not memory in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar I disorder and in euthymic patients with unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1980–1982. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 395.Keri S, Kelemen O, Benedek G, Janka Z. Different trait markers for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a neurocognitive approach. Psychol Med. 2001;31(5):915–922. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701004068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 396.Bora E, Vahip S, Akdeniz F, Ilerisoy H, Aldemir E, Alkan M. Executive and verbal working memory dysfunction in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2008;161(3):318–324. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 397.Christodoulou T, Messinis L, Papathanasopoulos P, Frangou S. The impact of familial risk for schizophrenia or bipolar disorder on cognitive control during episodic memory retrieval. Psychiatry Res. 2012;197(3):212–216. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 398.Kulkarni S, Jain S, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T. Impairment of verbal learning and memory and executive function in unaffected siblings of probands with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2010;12(6):647–656. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00857.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 399.Glahn DC, Almasy L, Barguil M, Hare E, Peralta JM, Kent JW, Jr, Dassori A, Contreras J, Pacheco A, Lanzagorta N, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder identified in multiplex multigenerational families. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):168–177. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 400.Szoke A, Schurhoff F, Golmard JL, Alter C, Roy I, Meary A, Etain B, Bellivier F, Leboyer M. Familial resemblance for executive functions in families of schizophrenic and bipolar patients. Psychiatry Res. 2006;144(2–3):131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 401.Besnier N, Richard F, Zendjidjian X, Kaladjian A, Mazzola-Pomietto P, Adida M, Azorin JM. Stroop and emotional stroop interference in unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenic and bipolar disorders: distinct markers of vulnerability? World J Biol Psychiatry. 2009;10(4 Pt 3):809–818. doi: 10.1080/15622970903131589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 402.Sobczak S, Honig A, Schmitt JA, Riedel WJ. Pronounced cognitive deficits following an intravenous l-tryptophan challenge in first-degree relatives of bipolar patients compared to healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(4):711–719. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 403.Sobczak S, Riedel WJ, Booij I, Aan Het Rot M, Deutz NE, Honig A. Cognition following acute tryptophan depletion: difference between first-degree relatives of bipolar disorder patients and matched healthy control volunteers. Psychol Med. 2002;32(3):503–515. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702005342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 404.Balanza-Martinez V, Rubio C, Selva-Vera G, Martinez-Aran A, Sanchez-Moreno J, Salazar-Fraile J, Vieta E, Tabares-Seisdedos R. Neurocognitive endophenotypes (endophenocognitypes) from studies of relatives of bipolar disorder subjects: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(8):1426–1438. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 405.Vohringer PA, Barroilhet SA, Amerio A, Reale ML, Alvear K, Vergne D, Ghaemi SN. Cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:87. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 406.Bora E. Developmental trajectory of cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: comparison with schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25(2):158–168. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 407.Strejilevich SA, Samame C, Martino DJ. The trajectory of neuropsychological dysfunctions in bipolar disorders: a critical examination of a hypothesis. J Affect Disord. 2015;175:396–402. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 408.Martino DJ, Samame C, Ibanez A, Strejilevich SA. Neurocognitive functioning in the premorbid stage and in the first episode of bipolar disorder: a systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 2015;226(1):23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 409.Bora E, Pantelis C. Meta-analysis of cognitive impairment in first-episode bipolar disorder: comparison with first-episode schizophrenia and healthy controls. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(5):1095–1104. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Annals of General Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES