Abstract
Objective
We examined whether an empirically-based, randomized controlled trial of a preventive intervention for divorced mothers and children had a long-term impact on offspring cortisol regulation.
Design
Divorced mothers and children (age 9-12) were randomly assigned to a literature control condition or the 11-week New Beginnings Program (NBP), a family-focused group preventive intervention for mothers and children in newly divorced families.
Main Outcome Measures
Fifteen years after the trial, offspring salivary cortisol (n=161) was measured before and after a social stress task.
Results
Multilevel mixed models were used to predict cortisol from internalizing symptoms, externalizing symptoms, group assignment and potential moderators of intervention effects. Across the sample, higher externalizing symptoms were associated with lower cortisol reactivity. There was a significant group by age interaction such that older offspring in the control group had higher reactivity relative to the intervention group, and younger offspring in the control group exhibited a decline across the task relative to younger offspring in the intervention group.
Conclusions
Preventive interventions for youth from divorced families may have a long-term impact on cortisol reactivity to stress. Results highlight the importance of examining moderators of program effects.
Keywords: parental divorce, cortisol, intervention, externalizing
Children in divorced families may experience a number of adversities, including interparental conflict, financial stressors, parental depression, and separation from a parent. A considerable literature documents health risks associated with such family-related adversities (Miller et al., 2011). Meta-analyses find that youth in divorced families have more conduct, internalizing, social, and academic problems than those in non-divorced families (e.g., Amato, 2001). Parental divorce has also been empirically linked to tobacco use, physical symptoms in children and adolescents (Troxel & Mathews, 2004), health problems in adolescence and adulthood (Fabricius & Luecken, 2007; Fuller-Thomson & Dalton, 2012; Roustit et al., 2011), and decreased longevity (Larson & Halfon, 2013).
The impact of adverse experiences during a sensitive period of neurological development on the calibration and function of biological stress response systems is a commonly theorized mechanism linking early adversity and health (e.g., Luecken & Lemery, 2004; Miller et al., 2011; Repetti et al., 2002). The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, which regulates the production and release of the hormone cortisol, undergoes extensive development during childhood and adolescence, and is highly sensitive to environmental influences (Gunnar et al., 2009). A large research literature demonstrates that adverse childhood experiences can significantly modify HPA activity, but yields conflicting results about the form or direction of effects. Prolonged or intense childhood stress can lead to exaggerated or attenuated reactivity to psychosocial stress, higher or lower basal cortisol, or flattened diurnal slopes (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007; Koss et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2007). Both exaggerated and attenuated cortisol reactivity to stress and daily cortisol output have been linked to long-term mental and physical health problems, including affective and externalizing disorders, heart disease, and infectious diseases (Heim, Ehlert, & Hellhammer, 2000; Miller et al., 2007; Shirtcliff et al., 2014). Research suggests complex relations between childhood adversity and associated cortisol alterations, with exaggerated reactivity as an outcome for some vulnerable youth, and attenuated reactivity for others.
The few studies that have examined the impact of parental divorce and interparental conflict on offspring cortisol reactivity find similarly complex results. Kraft and Luecken (2009) found lower cortisol reactivity to a videotaped lab-based social challenge task (a 4-minute preparation period followed by a 4-minute speech defending oneself from a false accusation of shoplifting; Saab et al., 1989) in emerging adults from divorced families compared to young adults from married families. Bloch et al. (2007) reported that parental divorce in childhood predicted lower cortisol response to a corticotropin-releasing hormone stimulation test. Although not specific to parental divorce, both exaggerated and attenuated cortisol reactivity to simulated conflict between parents have been reported in grade-school aged children (Davies et al., 2008, 2011; Koss et al., 2013). Higher cortisol reactivity to a 10-minute role-play peer conflict task was exhibited among young adults exposed to high interparental conflict during childhood relative to those who experienced moderate conflict (Hagan, Roubinov, Purdom Marreiro, & Luecken, 2014). The investigation of predictors of individual differences in outcomes is a critical direction for future research.
Concurrent or life-course psychological symptomatology may help explain the impact of parental divorce on cortisol regulation. A recent study of adults exposed to childhood adversity found divergent patterns of cortisol reactivity depending on whether or not the participant also experienced recurrent psychological problems (Goldman-Mellor, Hamer, & Steptoe, 2012). Children who experience parental divorce and interparental conflict are at elevated risk of internalizing and externalizing problems (Amato, 2001; Kim, 2011), and a number of studies document cortisol dysregulation in children and adults with psychological disorders. Internalizing symptoms, for example, have been associated with higher baseline cortisol and cortisol stress reactivity (Lopez-Duran, Kovacs, & George, 2009), while reduced basal cortisol and lower cortisol reactivity have been associated with externalizing problems (Alink et al., 2008; Hagan, Roubinov, Mistler, & Luecken, 2014). Offspring of parental divorce who develop internalizing or externalizing problems may show diverging patterns of cortisol reactivity.
Given clear evidence that childhood adversity can impact health across the lifespan, the development of interventions capable of mitigating negative effects is critical (Shonkoff et al. 2009). As a potential mediator, the HPA axis represents an intriguing target of interventions for at risk youth (e.g., Bruce et al., 2013; Cicchetti & Gunnar, 2008). Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) offer a powerful means to assess the ability of interventions to alter biological functions because the process of randomization minimizes the possibility that program effects are due to pre-existing biological or individual characteristics. Several studies provide intriguing evidence of short- and long-term adrenocortical benefits of interventions for children in foster care and parentally-bereaved children (Brotman et al., 2007; Fisher et al., 2007, 2011; O’Neal et al., 2010; Luecken et al., 2010).
Although recent findings are promising, it remains an open question whether the long-term impact of parental divorce on neuroendocrine response systems can be modified, and whether there are individual differences in intervention effects - a task that requires consideration of program moderators. Examination of program moderators has considerable public health significance as it can help identify who is most likely to benefit from intervention, the conditions under which benefits are maximized, and individual differences in treatment response (Kraemer et al., 2002, 2006; MacKinnon, et al., 2013). Ideally, moderators are baseline characteristics (e.g., age, sex) that are uncorrelated with program condition (Kraemer et al., 2002). Moderators of intervention effects on psychological outcomes are commonly examined, but little is known about moderators of program effects on neurobiological systems.
The New Beginnings Program
The New Beginnings Program (NBP) is an 11-week family-focused intervention for recently divorced mothers and children. The NBP was designed to promote children’s positive adaptation by minimizing risk factors such as exposure to interparental conflict and maximizing protective factors such as high quality parent-child relationships. An RCT of the NBP conducted when children were 9-12 years old revealed program benefits on several indices of mental health at post-test, six years, and 15 years later (Wolchik et al., 2000, 2002, 2007, 2013).
The current study investigated NBP program effects on cortisol 15 years later. Drawing from research about intervention effects on psychosocial functioning in at-risk youth, we theorized that age and gender would moderate NBP effects on neurobiological systems. Sensitive periods for environmental influences on brain development occur at different ages for varied aspects of cognitive, emotional, and biological development, and children are particularly receptive to negative experiences during sensitive periods. The peripubertal period represents one such sensitive period. The effects of parenting interventions on child psychosocial functioning have been found to vary by age (e.g., Jayson et al., 1998; Sandler et al., 1992). For example, the Incredible Years Program found stronger effects on conduct problems for younger children (Gardner et al., 2010). Further, age effects on HPA activity and the neurobiological consequences of stress have been reported (Kudielka et al., 2004; Miller et al., 2011). Sex differences in children’s mental health outcomes have been reported for interventions after parental bereavement or divorce (e.g., Sandler et al., 2003; Wolchik et al., 2013). Moreover, sex differences in HPA activity, the impact of childhood adversity on HPA activity, and the impact of parental divorce on adult physical health outcomes have been noted (Doom et al., 2013; Fuller-Thomson & Dalton, 2012; Trickett et al., 2014).
The current study examined the impact of NBP participation on cortisol reactivity to a psychosocial challenge in young adulthood. Prior research finds evidence of both blunted and exaggerated reactivity in youth affected by adversity relative to a non-affected group. The current sample was all affected by parental divorce, and we expected that those who received the preventive intervention would exhibit less evidence of dysregulation (exaggerated or blunted responses) relative to the control group. We also examined the relation of concurrent mental health to cortisol reactivity. Finally, we evaluated child age and gender as moderators of intervention effects on cortisol.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Participants included 161 young adults from divorced families who participated in a RCT of the NBP 15 years earlier. Demographic information is displayed in Table 1 and a CONSORT flowchart is shown in Figure 1. Most families (80%) were recruited from randomly selected court records of divorce decrees within two years of the intervention’s start; the remainder responded to media advertisements. Eligibility criteria included: 1) primary residential parent was female, 2) a 9-12 year-old child who resided at least 50% of the time with the mother, 3) neither mother nor any child was in treatment for mental health problems, 4) mother had not remarried, did not plan to remarry, and did not have a live-in boyfriend (see Wolchik et al, 2000 for complete eligibility criteria). Children signed informed assent forms; parents and youths older than 18 signed informed consent.
Table 1.
Sample Characteristics
Full Sample | NBP | LC | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age at W0 (study entry; M, SD) | 10.76 (1.4) | 10.77(1.1) | 10.67 (1.1) | 0.62 |
Age at W5 (M, SD) | 25.6 (1.2) | 25.6 (1.2) | 25.5 (1.2) | 0.73 |
Age at parental divorce (M, SD) | 9.7 (1.3) | 9.8 (2.3) | 9.7 (1.2) | 0.63 |
Sex (N, %) | ||||
Male | 84 (52%) | 61 (54%) | 23 (51%) | 0.49 |
Female | 77 (48%) | 53 (46%) | 24(49%) | |
Ethnicity (N, %) | ||||
Hispanic | 18 (11%) | 14 (12%) | 4 (9%) | 0.68 |
Anglo/Caucasian | 132 (82%) | 92 (81%) | 40 (85%) | |
African American | 5 (3%) | 5 (4%) | 0 (0%) | |
Native American | 3 (2%) | 0 (0%) | 3 (7%) | |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 3 (2%) | 3 (3%) | 0 (0%) | |
Hormonal contraceptive use (N, %) | 24 (31%) | 17 (32%) | 7 (29%) | 0.80 |
Regular Smoker (N, %) | 50 (31%) | 35 (31%) | 15 (33%) | 0.82 |
Time from divorce to study entry (M, SD)1 | 12.5 (6.3) | 12.4 (6.3) | 12.5 (6.5) | 0.89 |
Use of medications (N, %)2 | 37 (23%) | 26 (23%) | 11 (23%) | 0.80 |
W0 Psychosocial risk (M, SD) 3 | 0 (1.0) | 0.081 (.99) | −0.15 (.69) | 0.15 |
W5 Internaliz. Symptoms (M, SD) 4 | 56.4 (6.8) | 56.54 (6.5) | 55.8 (6.4) | 0.63 |
W5 Externaliz. Symptoms (M, SD) 4 | 49.2 (10.8) | 48.8 (10.9) | 49.2 (9.3) | 0.80 |
W5 Total Problems Index (M, SD) 4 | 50.2 (9.2) | 50.5 (9.4) | 49.4 (8.6) | 0.51 |
Pre-task cortisol (nmol/L; M, SD)5 | 2.41 (1.3) | 2.37 (1.3) | 2.53 (1.3) | 0.77 |
Post-task cortisol (nmol/L; M, SD) 5 | 2.34 (1.5) | 2.29 (1.6) | 2.49 (1.4) | 0.65 |
20-min post cortisol (nmol/L; M, SD) 5 | 2.48 (1.9) | 2.46 (2.0) | 2.61 (1.7) | 0.84 |
40-min post cortisol (nmol/L; M, SD)5 | 2.02 (1.3) | 1.98 (1.3) | 2.14 (1.3) | 0.66 |
Note: “NBP” = intervention participants; “LC” = participants in the control condition
in months;
includes allergy, antidepressant, anti-anxiety, or other medications that could potentially affect cortisol
z-score
t-score
non-transformed data
Figure 1.
Flow of study participants
Assessments were conducted in participants’ homes at baseline (prior to assignment to condition, W0), post-test (W1), and at 3-month (W2), 6-month (W3), 6-year (W4) and 15-year (W5) follow-ups. The current analyses include W0 and W5 data. At W0, families were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: Mother Program (n = 81), Mother and Child Program (n = 83) or Literature Control (LC; n = 76). Prior analyses indicated that the child component did not increase program benefits on mental health outcomes beyond the mother component (Wolchik et al., 2002; 2013)1; therefore the Mother and the Mother and Child intervention groups were combined for analyses, hereafter labeled the New Beginnings Program (NBP).
Of the 240 youth assigned to condition, 194 (89.6% of the families; NBP n = 134; LC n = 60) completed the 15-year follow-up (see Figure 1). Attrition analyses showed that the rate of attrition did not differ significantly across NBP (9.8%) and LC (11.8%) (χ2 [1, N = 240] = .24, p = .65) conditions. We found no differential attrition or attrition by group interaction effects on demographic or psychosocial risk variables from W0.
Cortisol samples were not obtained from 12 participants (8 refused, 4 lived outside the country). Two participants had cortisol levels outside normal physiological levels (>50 nmol/L; Nicolson, 2008), indicating interference in the assay. An additional nineteen (11 from the NBP group, 8 from the LC group) were excluded a priori from analyses due to pregnancy (n = 8), thyroid or other medications that affect glucocorticoids (n = 9), or cortisol levels > 4 SD’s from the mean (n = 2; Nicolson, 2008) 2. The final sample consisted of 115 participants in the NBP and 46 in the LC group.
Procedure
All components of the assessment were conducted at participants’ homes. Saliva samples were collected before, immediately after, and 20 and 40-minutes after a modified Trier Social Stress task (TSST; Kirschbaum et al., 1993) that included 3 minutes of mental arithmetic and an 6-minute video-recorded speech about personal strengths and weaknesses (2 minutes preparation, 4 minutes speech). The mental arithmetic task included three trials of serial subtraction problems performed out loud. A new starting number was provided each minute, adjusted for difficulty to hold effort constant across participants. It was conducted under time pressure, with prompting from the research assistant (e.g., “faster”, “incorrect, begin again with ___”). The speech was video-recorded and given in front of the experimenter who displayed a neutral expression and took notes on a clipboard throughout the speech. In addition, prior to the speech task, participants were told their speech would be evaluated and graded by a team of experts. An a priori start time for cortisol samples was 2 – 8:30 PM (range = 1:54 – 8:27 PM; mean = 5:55 PM).
Intervention and Control Conditions
Intervention condition (NBP)
The Mother Program consisted of 11 group sessions (1.75 hours each; average group size = 9) and two individual sessions. Groups were led by two Master’s level clinicians using structured manuals. Mother program sessions focused on skills to improve mother-child relationship quality and effective discipline, decrease barriers to child contact with the father, and decrease interparental conflict. In the Mother and Child Program, children participated in separate groups that aimed to increase active coping, decrease avoidant coping and negative appraisals of divorce stressors, and improve mother-child relationship quality. Clinical methods were based on social learning and cognitive behavioral theories. As noted earlier, the two intervention conditions were combined in the analyses and the combined group is referred to as NBP. See Wolchik et al (2000) for a detailed description of the clinical methods.
Control condition (LC)
In the LC, mothers and children each received three books about children’s divorce adjustment, along with a syllabus to guide their reading. Families received their first books a week after assignment to condition, their second books three weeks later, and their last books six weeks after assignment to condition (see Wolchik et al., 2000 for details).
Measures
Mental health problems
Mental health problems during the past six months were assessed at W5 using the Adult Self Report form (ASR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003). Subscale scores included Internalizing problems (α = .90), Externalizing problems (α = .84), and a Total Problems Index (α = .90).
Baseline psychosocial risk
A baseline risk index served as a control variable in the prediction of cortisol reactivity. All measures in the risk index were completed by mothers at W0, prior to assignment to condition. The risk index was the composite of standardized scores on child externalizing problems (Child Behavior Checklist; Achenbach, 1991; α = .88) and divorce-related adversity, including interparental conflict (Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict, Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992; α = .88), post-divorce negative events (Negative Life Events Scale; Sandler, Wolchik, & Braver, 1988), maternal mental health problems (Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Interview; Dohrenwend, Shrout, Egri, & Mendelsohn, 1980; α = .91), low contact with non-residential father (mother reported visits with father in the past month), and low per capita income (total income divided by the number of household members). The index was predictive of mental disorder and substance use in the LC group 6 years later (Dawson-McClure et al., 2004).
Cortisol sampling
Samples were obtained with the Salivette (Sarstedt, Rommelsdorf, Germany) and shipped on dry ice to Salimetrics (State College, PA) for analysis of free cortisol using high-sensitive enzyme immunoassay. The test has a range of sensitivity from .007 to 1.8 μg/dl, and mean intra-and inter-assay coefficients of variation 4.13% and 8.89%. Cortisol values were transformed from μg/dl to nmol/L and then log-transformed to correct for deviations from normality.
Statistical analyses
Preliminary analyses
We tested group equivalence on W0 demographic variables (ethnicity, sex, income, psychosocial risk, age at the divorce, and months since the divorce) using χ2 or t-statistics. Next, we evaluated covariates and confounders. Covariates do not explain relations between the independent variable (IV: group assignment) and dependent variable (DV: cortisol reactivity) because they are not in a causal pathway between them (MacKinnon & Luecken, 2008). Potential covariates included W0 demographics (e.g., sex, income, ethnicity) as well as information from the day of testing (W5; time of day, recent meals, exercise, caffeine consumption, medications, or hormonal contraceptives). It is not necessary to control for covariates, but doing so may increase efficiency in estimation (Sauer et al., 2013). Confounders are variables potentially associated with both the IV and the DV that could introduce bias into estimations (e.g., smoking). Covariates and confounders were selected empirically with the same criteria: any variable significantly related to the intervention condition and/or cortisol reactivity was selected for inclusion in analyses.
Analyses of outliers, conducted by evaluation of Cook’s D, studentized residuals, and leverage scores, identified two influential cases. The first, from the NBP group, self-reported heavy nonprescription drug use (99th percentile) and had cortisol levels > 3 SD from the mean. The second, from the LC group, began cortisol sampling at 1:54 PM, had smoked 15 minutes prior to the first sample, and reported heavy alcohol use and binge-drinking (95th percentile). Although the statistical significance of the results was not affected by the removal of these cases, the graphical pattern of results was influenced by their extreme values. These two cases were not considered reliable and were removed from the analyses.
A preliminary analysis was conducted to evaluate whether the challenge task resulted in significant cortisol reactivity across the sample. A multilevel model revealed a significant quadratic pattern (capturing cortisol’s rise and fall across the task), p = .008, indicating reactivity to the task across the sample.
Primary analyses
We used multilevel modeling (MLM) to evaluate group differences in cortisol reactivity. The data were modeled using SPSS MIXED, with the repeated cortisol measures forming the within-person dimension. A variable ‘time’ reflected within-person cortisol sampling order (‘0’ = pretask, ‘1’ = immediately posttask, ‘2’ = 20 minutes posttask, ‘3’ = 40 minutes posttask), and was included as a random linear effect. ‘Reactivity’ was modeled with the quadratic term ‘time × time’. Group assignment served as the between-persons dimension, coded as NBP group = 1 and LC group = −1. Sex was coded as 0 = male and 1 = female. Continuous variables were centered at the sample mean. Significant models were re-estimated with the inclusion of concurrent mental health symptoms.
Results
Evaluation of group equivalence
The NBP and LC groups did not significantly differ on W0 measures of age, ethnicity, sex, income, psychosocial risk, age at the divorce, months since the divorce, or concurrent internalizing symptoms, externalizing symptoms, or total behavioral problems (see Table 1). On the day of testing (W5), the groups did not differ in regard to session start time, time of saliva sampling, cigarettes smoked, caffeine or energy drinks, BMI, exercise, medications, time of most recent meal, or negative mood before or after the task (all p’s > .18).
Selection of covariates, and confounds
Zero-order correlations are shown in Table 2, and demonstrate significant relations among time of day and participant sex with cortisol levels at individual time points. Additional analyses were conducted using multilevel modeling to evaluate effects of covariates or confounds on cortisol reactivity. Men showed stronger cortisol reactivity (quadratic pattern of response) than women (p = .01). The time of day predicted cortisol intercept (p = .02), but did not predict reactivity (p = .25). Age, ethnicity, W0 income, W0 psychosocial risk, child age at divorce, and time since the divorce did not predict cortisol intercept, slope, or reactivity, nor did smoking, caffeine or energy drinks, BMI, exercise, pre- or post-task negative mood, medications, or recent meals on the day of testing (all p’s ≥ .10). Therefore, time of day and sex were included as covariates in all models.
Table 2.
Zero-order correlations1
1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. W0 age | 1.0 | |||||||||
2. Sex | −.047 | 1.0 | ||||||||
3. Time of day | .044 | .021 | 1.0 | |||||||
4. Time since divorce |
−.047 | .013 | .053 | 1.0 | ||||||
5. W5 Internalizing |
−.089 | −.038 | .020 | −.019 | 1.0 | |||||
6. W5 Externalizing |
−.020 | .050 | .042 | −.030 | .74** | 1.0 | ||||
7. W5 Total Problems |
−.066 | −.064 | −.003 | −.032 | .65** | .73** | 1.0 | |||
8. Baseline cortisol |
−.026 | −.055 | −.20** | −.068 | −.037 | −.021 | −.036 | 1.0 | ||
9. Post-task cortisol |
−.046 | −.16* | −.13† | −.006 | −.033 | −.019 | −.028 | .81** | 1.0 | |
10. 20-min post cortisol |
−.050 | −.25** | −.08 | −.006 | −.11 | −.13† | −.22** | .62** | .83** | 1.0 |
11. 40-min post cortisol |
−.044 | −.13 | −.14* | −.027 | −.054 | −.045 | −.12 | .66** | .82** | .83** |
non-transformed cortisol (nmol/L); sex is coded male = 0, female = 1; Time of day is in minutes past midnight; Time since divorce is in months; Internalizing symptoms, Externalizing symptoms, and Total Problems are t-scores.
p < .10
p < .05
p < .01
Mental health symptoms and cortisol reactivity
A multilevel model controlling for time of day and sex found that higher externalizing symptoms were associated with lower cortisol reactivity (quadratic effect), β = .001, 95% CI [.0001, 0.001], p = .047, and a declining linear slope, β = −.003, 95% CI [−0.005, −0.001], p = .017 (see Figure 2). Internalizing symptoms did not predict cortisol reactivity (quadratic effect), p = .22, linear slope, p = .12, or intercept, p = .16. The total mental health problems index was a statistically significant predictor of cortisol linear slope, β = −.004, 95% CI [−0.006, −0.001], p = .003, such that more problems were associated with a more steeply declining slope across the task. Total mental health problems did not predict cortisol reactivity (p = .06) or intercept (p = .53).
Figure 2.
Cortisol reactivity by externalizing symptoms (“ext”)
Note: Covariates = time of day and sex. Externalizing symptoms was analyzed as a continuous variable but set to one SD above and below the mean for display purposes. Predicted log-transformed cortisol values were subjected to anti-log-transformation in order to display cortisol values in an interpretable metric
Main effect of intervention on cortisol reactivity
A multilevel model controlling for time of day and sex did not find significant prediction of cortisol reactivity by group, p = .57. The linear slope, p = .67, and intercept, p = .51, were also not statistically significant. Inclusion of externalizing symptoms or total mental health problems in the models did not significantly change the results.
Moderation of intervention effect by youth age
We next examined individual differences in response to the intervention. The group by age interaction was a statistically significant predictor of cortisol reactivity, β = .011, 95% CI [.004, 0.018], p = .004, and linear slope, β = −.040, 95% CI [−.064, −.016], p = .001, but did not predict the intercept, p = .56. The group by age interaction remained statistically significant after adjusting for externalizing symptoms, β = .012, 95% CI [.005, 0.019], p = .002, and total mental health problems, β = .011, 95% CI [.004, 0.019], p = .003. As shown in Figure 3, the NBP participants showed similar reactivity and recovery regardless of age, while older LC participants had elevated reactivity and younger LC participants exhibited declining cortisol across the task.
Figure 3.
Intervention × age effects on cortisol reactivity; comparison of New Beginnings Program (NBP) to the literature control condition (LC).
Note: Age groupings represent median split. Covariates = time of day, sex, and externalizing symptoms. Predicted log-transformed cortisol values were subjected to anti-log-transformation in order to display cortisol values in an interpretable metric.
Probing the Group*Age Interaction
The significant group by age interaction was probed by dividing the sample into younger (age ≤ 10.5 at program entry; n = 79) and older (age > 10.5; n = 82) groups using a median split and evaluating each age group separately. Older participants across both the intervention and control conditions exhibited significant cortisol reactivity to the task (p = .001); however, older LC participants showed exaggerated reactivity compared to older NBP participants, β = .016, 95% CI [.004, 0.028], p = .010. Among older participants, the NBP and LC groups did not differ on pre-task cortisol (p = .71), sex (p = .33), time of day (p = .37), externalizing symptoms (p = .13), or total mental health problems (p = .13), and these factors did not explain the group effect on cortisol reactivity.
When examined separately, younger LC participants did not show cortisol reactivity (p = .99), whereas younger NBP participants did exhibit reactivity (p = .006). When directly compared, the group difference in reactivity did not reach statistical significance, β = .011, 95% CI [−.022, 0.0009], p = .072. However, younger LC participants’ cortisol response showed a steeper linear slope relative to younger NBP participants, β = .045, 95% CI [.007, 0.084], p = .021. Among younger participants, the NBP and LC groups did not differ on pre-task cortisol (p = .91), sex (p = .93), time of day of testing (p = .94), externalizing symptoms or total mental health symptoms (p’s > .36), and these factors did not explain the group difference in slope.
Moderation of intervention effect by sex
A multilevel model predicting cortisol reactivity from group, sex, and the group by sex interaction, controlling for time of day found that the group by sex interaction was not significant in the prediction of cortisol reactivity, p = .69, linear slope, p = .44, or intercept, p = .86. Inclusion of externalizing symptoms or total mental health symptoms in the models did not change the results.
Discussion
Our results suggest that an evidence-based intervention for recently divorced families may influence offspring cortisol stress responses 15 years later. Compared to youth in families who received the NBP, participants in the LC group showed diverging patterns of cortisol stress response depending on their age: older LC participants had elevated reactivity relative to NBP participants, whereas younger LC participants showed declining cortisol across the task relative to younger NBP participants. Higher externalizing symptoms predicted lower cortisol reactivity in both the NBP and LC groups, but did not explain the age effects on cortisol reactivity. The study is strengthened by the use of a randomized experimental design and an evidence-based intervention with documented mental health benefits. We found no evidence of pre-intervention group differences that could explain the current results. The use of a randomized design is critical because it minimizes the possibility that third variables (e.g., genetic or other pre-intervention factors) accounted for intervention effects (Cicchetti & Gunnar, 2008).
The study is unique in finding evidence of both exaggerated and attenuated cortisol reactivity in the LC group, similar to the sensitization and attenuation patterns of dysregulation following childhood adversity that have been noted in other studies (e.g., Miller et al., 2007), and suggest that age at the time of exposure to divorce-related stressors may have influenced long-term neuroendocrine response patterns in the control group. Relative to older participants in the NBP and younger LC participants, older LC participants had significantly elevated reactivity. Although pubertal status was not assessed, older participants were 11 or 12 years old during the program and likely transitioning into puberty. The peripubertal period is a sensitive period of development marked by increasing parent-child conflict and decreasing parent-child closeness (Steinberg & Morris, 2001), as well as heightened emotional and hormonal reactivity to stress (Gunnar et al., 2009; Shirtcliff et al., 2012; Sumter, et al., 2010). Hormonal activity during puberty also influences parent-child relationships and youth psychological health (Marceau et al., 2012). This transitional period is particularly challenging for youth coping with parental divorce (Anderson et al., 1989). As a result, older youth may have been particularly susceptible to the effects of parental divorce and related stressors, and program-induced improvements in family factors or sensitivity to stress may have contributed to lower cortisol reactivity for NBP participants.
We also found evidence of attenuated reactivity among younger participants in the LC group. Younger participants in the intervention and control conditions did not differ in pre-task cortisol, thus the declining cortisol in the young LC participants is unlikely to be a result of higher anticipatory cortisol. Although speculative, participants who were younger during their exposure to the stressors in the early part of the divorce process may have initially exhibited high reactivity, but continuing stress across puberty may have promoted blunted reactivity, as suggested by theories of the long-term impact of chronic stress across development (e.g., Miller et al., 2007; Ruttle et al., 2011). Pathways by which parental divorce may affect HPA regulation include exposure to interparental conflict, parental mental health problems, offspring risky health behaviors, decreased father-child relationship quality, or impairments in academic, social, or occupational competence (Troxel & Matthews, 2004; Luecken & Lemery, 2004). The processes by which the NPB affected young adult HPA regulation are likely to be complex and involve cascading effects of changes across time in environmental, family, and individual-level processes. Future studies evaluating mechanistic pathways will be critical for testing theories of age-related effects of interventions on biological processes.
Interpretation of the health implications of different cortisol response patterns is complicated, as both higher and lower cortisol reactivity have been linked to psychological or physical health problems. In addition, interpretation of what constitutes a response that is too high or too low is necessarily context-dependent. In general, an adaptive response rises to the extent needed to meet the unique demands of the situation and decreases upon cessation of the stressor. However, both high and low cortisol reactivity could confer advantage in a given situation. Evolutionary models theorize that early life conditions prepare a child’s HPA axis to respond optimally to an idiosyncratic environment, and both exaggerated and blunted reactivity could have short-term survival advantages in adverse environmental conditions (Pluess & Belsky, 2013; Shirtcliff, 2014). Indeed, emerging research points to nonlinear relations between childhood adversity and HPA outcomes, in which characteristics of the child (e.g., age, temperament, mental health, personality), the caregiving environment (e.g., warmth, harsh discipline), and/or environment (e.g., severity and type of stressors, cumulative stressors) may influence the HPA stress response.
However, what is adaptive in childhood may be maladaptive later in development and in other environmental conditions (Shirtcliff, 2014). Both sensitization and attenuation patterns of cortisol stress response have been linked to maladaptive psychosocial outcomes in children and young adults exposed to family adversity (Hagan et al., in press; Koss et al., 2013), and poor long-term physical health outcomes (Heim et al., 2000). Higher reactivity may indicate an overly sensitive HPA axis or disruptions in the HPA axis negative feedback system, whereas attenuated responses may reflect a failure of the HPA to initiate a response or an overactive negative feedback system. In the current study, the higher reactivity of older LC participants could be considered an adaptive response in that moment, but might also reflect heightened sensitivity to stress relative to other participants. However, even if adaptive in a specific situation, consistent higher reactivity across time and in other contexts may confer future health risk. The association of concurrent mental health problems (externalizing symptoms and total problem index) with steeply declining cortisol responses suggests that the response pattern seen in younger LC participants likely indicates poor adaptation.
There are limitations to the analyses. We did not assess cortisol reactivity prior to assignment to condition, thus we cannot distinguish between whether the NBP prevented or alleviated adversity-induced alterations to cortisol. However, the randomized design of the trial is a “gold standard for establishing causation because randomization creates probabilistically equivalent treatment and control groups” (Mercer et al., 2007, p. 143), and we found no evidence of group differences in attrition or pre-intervention demographic or psychosocial factors that might suggest the intervention and control groups differed on cortisol at baseline. Because cortisol reactivity was not assessed at waves before the 15-year-follow-up, we cannot examine cortisol reactivity patterns over time. It was necessary for the study that assessments be conducted in participants’ homes, and the average cortisol reactivity was smaller than responses to the TSST commonly found in lab-based studies in front of “audiences” (e.g., Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). Further, a larger, more diverse sample would allow examination of additional moderators of intervention effect, such as socioeconomic status or ethnicity. Finally, because the intervention was specifically for children from divorced families, all participants experienced parental divorce. It cannot be determined if results generalize to youth experiencing other forms of adversity.
Conclusions
Alterations in the neuroendocrine stress response system may represent a mechanism linking childhood parental divorce to adult mental and physical health outcomes. This study demonstrated age-related effects on cortisol reactivity of a preventive intervention for children who experienced parental divorce. Older youth in the control group displayed higher cortisol reactivity 15 years later relative to the intervention group, whereas younger participants in the control group showed a declining response to the task relative to the intervention group. Across the sample, higher externalizing symptoms and more mental health problems were associated with lower cortisol reactivity, but did not explain intervention effects. The findings contribute to a growing literature documenting varied patterns of cortisol response among those who experience childhood adversities and suggest that preventive interventions may alter the potential long-term neuroendocrine consequences.
Acknowledgments
Funding: This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (5R01MH071707; 5P30MH068685, 5P30MH039246). Trial Registration: clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT01407120. The funding source had no role in study design, collection, analysis, interpretation, or writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.
Footnotes
The authors have no financial interest in the application of this research.
Preliminary analyses also confirm that the two intervention groups did not differ in cortisol response to the task (p = .17)
Primary analyses were repeated without excluding these 19 participants, as recommended by Cohen, Cohen, Aiken West & Aiken (2013). The results retained statistical significance. However, because the identified factors are known to alter cortisol levels (e.g., Jung et al., 2011; Walter et al., 2012), these cases were excluded from analyses.
References
- Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. University of Vermont, Department of Psychology; Burlington, VT: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms & Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; Burlington, VT: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Alink LR, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Mesman J, Juffer F, Koot HM. Cortisol and externalizing behavior in children and adolescents: mixed meta-analytic evidence for the inverse relation of basal cortisol and cortisol reactivity with externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50:427–450. doi: 10.1002/dev.20300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amato P. Children of divorce in the 1990s: An update of the Amato and Keith (1991) meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology. 2001;15:355–370. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.15.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson ER, Hetherington EM, Clingempeel WG. Transformations in family relations at puberty: Effects of family context. Journal of Early Adoescence. 1989;9:310–334. [Google Scholar]
- Bloch M, Peleg I, Koren D, Aner H, Klein E. Long-term effects of early parental loss due to divorce on the HPA axis. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;51:516–23. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brotman LM, Gouley KK, Huang KY, Kamboukos D, Fratto C, Pine DS. Effects of a psychosocial family-based preventive intervention on cortisol response to a social challenge in preschoolers at high risk for antisocial behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1172–1179. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce J, Gunnar MR, Pears KC, Fisher PA. Early adverse care, stress neurobiology, and prevention science: Lessons learned. Prevention Science. 2013;14:247–256. doi: 10.1007/s11121-012-0354-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Gunnar MR. Integrating biological processes into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:737–743. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Personality, adrenal steroid hormones, and resilience in maltreated children: A multilevel perspective. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:787–809. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cicchetti D. Adrenocortical underpinnings of children’s psychological reactivity to interparental conflict. Child Development. 2008;79:1693–1706. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01219.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cicchetti D. Interparental aggression and children’s adrenocortical reactivity: Testing an evolutionary model of allostatic load. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:801–814. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson-McClure S, Sandler IN, Wolchik SA, Millsap RE. Prediction and reduction of risk for children of divorce: A six-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2004;32:175–190. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000019769.75578.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological bulletin. 2004;130:355. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dohrenwend BP, Shrout PE, Egri G, Mendelsohn FS. Non-specific psychological distress and other dimensions of psychopathology. Archives of General Psychology. 1980;39:1229–1236. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1980.01780240027003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doom JR, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Dackis MN. Child maltreatment and gender interactions as predictors of differential neuroendocrine profiles. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:1442–1454. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fabricius WV, Luecken LJ. Postdivorce living arrangements, parent conflict, and long-term physical health correlates for children of divorce. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21:195–205. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M, Gunnar MR, Burraston BO. Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:892–905. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PA, Van Ryzin MJ, Gunnar MR. Mitigating HPA axis dysregulation associated with placement changes in foster care. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:531–539. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuller-Thomson E, Dalton AD. Gender differences in the association between parental divorce during childhood and stroke in adulthood: findings from a population-based survey. International Journal of Stroke. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2012.00935.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2012.00935.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner F, Hutchings J, Bywater T, Whitaker C. Who benefits and how does it work? Moderators and mediators of outcome in an effectiveness trial of a parenting intervention. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39(4):568–580. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.486315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Mellor S, Hamer M, Steptoe A. Early-life stress and recurrent psychological distress over the lifecourse predict divergent cortisol reactivity patterns in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(11):1755–1768. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grych JH, Seid M, Fincham FD. Assessing marital conflict from the child’s perspective: The Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Child Development. 1992;63:558–572. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:145–173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Talge N, Herrera A. Stressor paradigms in developmental studies: What does and does not work to produce mean increases in salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:953–967. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Wewerka S, Frenn K, Long JD, Griggs C. Developmental changes in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: Normative changes and associations with puberty. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:69–85. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagan MJ, Roubinov DS, Mistler AK, Luecken LJ. Mental health outcomes in Emerging adults exposed to childhood maltreatment: The moderating role of stress reactivity. Child maltreatment. 2014 doi: 10.1177/1077559514539753. epub. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagan MJ, Roubinov DS, Purdom Marreiro CL, Luecken LJ. Childhood interparental conflict and HPA axis activity in young adulthood: Examining nonlinear relations. Developmental psychobiology. 2014;56(4):871–880. doi: 10.1002/dev.21157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim C, Ehlert U, Hellhammer DH. The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25:1–35. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(99)00035-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jayson D, Wood A, Kroll L, Fraser J, Harrington R. Which depressed patients respond to cognitive-behavioral treatment? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37:35–39. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199801000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung C, Ho JT, Torpy DJ, Rogers A, Doogue M, Lewis JG, Inder WJ. A longitudinal study of plasma and urinary cortisol in pregnancy and postpartum. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2011;96(5):1533–1540. doi: 10.1210/jc.2010-2395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim HS. Consequences of parental divorce for child development. American Sociological Review. 2011;76(3):487–511. [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The “Trier Social Stress Test”: a tool For investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28:76–8. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koss KJ, George MRW, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, Sturge-Apple ML. Patterns of children's adrenocortical reactivity to interparental conflict and associations with child adjustment: A growth mixture modeling approach. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:317–26. doi: 10.1037/a0028246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer HC, Frank E, Kupfer DJ. Moderators of treatment outcomes: Clinical, research, and policy importance. JAMA. 2006;296:1286–1289. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.10.1286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer HC, Wilson T, Fairburn CG, Agras WS. Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59:877–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraft AJ, Luecken LJ. Childhood parental divorce and cortisol in young adulthood: evidence for mediation by family income. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1363–1369. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudielka BM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:83–98. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00146-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larson K, Halfon N. Parental divorce and adult longevity. International Journal of Public Health. 2013;58:89–97. doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0373-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Duran NL, Kovacs M, George CJ. Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis dysregulation in depressed children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(9):1272–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luecken LJ, Hagan MJ, Sandler IN, Tein JY, Ayers TS, Wolchik SA. Cortisol levels six years after participation in the Family Bereavement Program. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:785–789. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luecken LJ, Lemery K. Early caregiving and adult physiological stress responses. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24:171–191. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Luecken LJ. How and for whom? Mediation and moderation in health psychology. Health Psychology. 2008;27:99–100. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2(Suppl.).S99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Lockhart G, Baraldi A, Gelfand L. Evaluating treatment mediators and moderators. In: Comer JS, Kendall PC, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Research Strategies for Clinical Psychology. Oxford University Press; New York: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Marceau K, Dorn LD, Sussman EJ. Stress and puberty-related hormone reactivity, negative emotionality, and parent-adolescent relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1286–1298. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mercer SL, DeVinney BJ, Fine LJ, Green LG, Dougherty D. Study designs for effectiveness and translation research: Identifying trade-offs. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2007;33:139–154. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137:959–997. doi: 10.1037/a0024768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GE, Chen E, Zhou ES. If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133(1):25–45. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Neal CR, Brotman LM, Huang K, Gouley KK, Kamboukos D, Calzada EJ, et al. Understanding relations among early family environment, cortisol response, and child aggression via a prevention experiment. Child Development. 2010;81:290–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01395.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:330–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roustit C, Campoy E, Renahy E, King G, Parizot I, Chauvin P. Family social environment in childhood and self-rated health in young adulthood. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:949. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruttle PL, Shirtcliff EA, Serbin LA, Ben-Dat Fisher D, Stack DM, Schwartzman AE. Disentangling psychobiological mechanisms underlying internalizing and externalizing behaviors in youth: Longitudinal and concurrent associations with cortisol. Hormones and behavior. 2011;59(1):123–132. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saab PG, Matthews KA, Stoney CM, McDonald RH. Premenopausal and postmenopausal women differ in their cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses to behavioral stressors. Psychophysiology. 1989;26:270–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1989.tb01917.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandler IN, Ayers TS, Wolchik SA, Tein J-Y, Kwok O-M, Haine RA, et al. The Family Bereavement Program: Efficacy evaluation of a theory-based prevention program for parentally-bereaved children and adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:587–600. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.3.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandler IN, West SG, Baca L, Pillow DR, Gersten JC, Rogosch F, Virdin L, Beals J, Reynolds JD, Kallgren C, Tein JY, Kreige G, Cole E, Ramirez R. Linking empirically based theory and evaluation: The Family Bereavement Program. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1992;20:491–521. doi: 10.1007/BF00937756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandler IN, Wolchik SA, Braver SL. The stressors of children’s postdivorce environments. In: Wolchik S, Karoly P, editors. Children of divorce: Empirical perspectives on adjustment. Gardner; New York: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Sauer B, Brookhart MA, Roy JA, Vanderweele TJ. Covariate selection. In: Venentgas P, Dreyer NA, Nourjah P, Smith SR, Torchia MM, editors. Developing a Protocol for Observational Comparitive Effectiveness Research: A User's Guide. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; Rockville, MD: 2013. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shirtcliff EA, Allison AL, Armstrong JM, Slattery MJ, Kalin NH, Essex MJ. Longitudinal stability and developmental properties of salivary cortisol levels and circadian rhythms from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology. 2012;54:493–502. doi: 10.1002/dev.20607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shirtcliff EA, Peres JC, Dismukes AR, Lee Y, Phan JM. Riding the physiological roller coaster: Adaptive significance of cortisol stress reactivity to social contexts. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2014;28:40–51. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2014.28.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. JAMA. 2009;301:2252–2259. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simeon D, Yehuda R, Cunill R, Knutelska M, Putnam FW, Smith LM. Factors associated with resilience in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:1149–1152. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:83–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sumter SR, Bokhorst CL, Miers AC, Van Pelt J, Westenberg PM. Age and puberty differences in stress responses during a public speaking task: Do adolescents grow more sensitive to social evaluation? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1510–1516. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trickett PK, Gordis E, Peckins MK, Susman EJ. Stress reactivity in maltreated and comparison male and female young adolescents. Child Maltreatment. 2014;19:27–37. doi: 10.1177/1077559513520466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troxel WM, Matthews KA. What are the costs of marital conflict and dissolution to children's physical health? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2004;7:29–57. doi: 10.1023/b:ccfp.0000020191.73542.b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter KN, Corwin EJ, Ulbrecht J, Demers LM, Bennett JM, Whetzel CA, Klein LC. Elevated thyroid stimulating hormone is associated with elevated cortisol in healthy young men and women. Thyroid research. 2012;5(1):13. doi: 10.1186/1756-6614-5-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolchik SA, Sandler IN, Millsap RE, Plummer BA, Greene SM, Anderson ER, et al. Six-year followup of preventive interventions for children of divorce. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;68:843–856. [Google Scholar]
- Wolchik S, Sandler IN, Tein JY, Mahrer NE, Millsap RE, Winslow E, Velez C, Porter MM, Luecken LJ, Reed A. Prevention of mental and substance abuse disorders in young adults who experienced parental divorce fifteen years earlier: Results of a randomized trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2013;81:660–673. doi: 10.1037/a0033235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolchik SA, Sandler I, Weiss L, Winslow EB. New Beginnings: An empirically-based program to help divorced mothers promote resilience in their children. In: Briesmeister JM, Schaefer CE, editors. Handbook of parent training: Helping parents prevent and solve problem behaviors. John Wiley & Sons; New York: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Wolchik SA, West SG, Sandler IN, Tein J-Y, Coatsworth JD, Lengua L, et al. An experimental evaluation of theory-based mother and mother-child programs for children of divorce. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:843–856. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]