Skip to main content
Sage Choice logoLink to Sage Choice
. 2015 Dec;29(12):1236–1247. doi: 10.1177/0269881115612404

Caffeine consumption and self-assessed stress, anxiety, and depression in secondary school children

Gareth Richards 1,, Andrew Smith 1
PMCID: PMC4668773  PMID: 26508718

Abstract

Previous research suggests that effects of caffeine on behaviour are positive unless one is investigating sensitive groups or ingestion of large amounts. Children are a potentially sensitive subgroup, and especially so considering the high levels of caffeine currently found in energy drinks. The present study used data from the Cornish Academies Project to investigate associations between caffeine (both its total consumption, and that derived separately from energy drinks, cola, tea, and coffee) and single-item measures of stress, anxiety, and depression, in a large cohort of secondary school children from the South West of England. After adjusting for additional dietary, demographic, and lifestyle covariates, positive associations between total weekly caffeine intake and anxiety and depression remained significant, and the effects differed between males and females. Initially, effects were also observed in relation to caffeine consumed specifically from coffee. However, coffee was found to be the major contributor to high overall caffeine intake, providing explanation as to why effects relating to this source were also apparent. Findings from the current study increase our knowledge regarding associations between caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression in secondary school children, though the cross-sectional nature of the research made it impossible to infer causality.

Keywords: Adolescent behaviour, anxiety, caffeine, depression, energy drinks, sex differences, stress

Introduction

Dose-dependent effects of caffeine on behaviour

Short-term effects of caffeine consumption include enhanced mood and alertness (Ferré, 2008; Kaplan et al., 1997; Lorist and Tops, 2003), improved exercise performance (Doherty and Smith, 2004), increased blood pressure (Riksen et al., 2009), improved ability to remain awake and mentally alert after fatigue (Smit and Rogers, 2002), faster information processing speed and reaction time, and heightened awareness and attention (Cysneiros et al., 2007). When consumed in moderation it appears that there are no serious adverse health effects associated with its use by adults (Nawrot et al., 2003) or children (Higdon and Frei, 2006; Mandel, 2002). However, it has been advised that those who are highly sensitive should not consume >400 mg/d, in order to avoid headaches, drowsiness, anxiety, and nausea (Nawrot et al., 2003). A sensitive individual might experience adverse effects at a lower dose than less sensitive individuals. Children are often considered as sensitive individuals because of their size and developing central nervous system. This is concerning because many children and adolescents are frequent caffeine consumers (for instance, a recent US study found 73% of children to consume caffeine on a given day; Branum et al., 2014). It is important, therefore, to identify thresholds above which negative effects might occur. In the context of the current study, the thresholds in question relate to the group as a whole, with potential sensitivity to caffeine being defined by the participants being children.

The relatively recent introduction of ‘energy drinks’ to the consumer market has been highlighted as a cause for concern (e.g. Reissig et al., 2009). Energy drinks are soft drinks that manufacturers claim boost performance and endurance (Meadows-Oliver and Ryan-Krause, 2007), with the main active ingredient being caffeine (McLellan and Lieberman, 2012). These products are often strategically marketed towards the young consumer (Reissig et al., 2009), with 30–50% of adolescents and young adults now known to consume them (Seifert et al., 2011). Energy drinks have also been associated with behavioural problems (Richards et al., 2015a), and a number of serious health complications (Reissig et al., 2009).

A potential avenue by which energy drink use may negatively affect health is through their association with risk-taking behaviours (see Arria et al., 2014). Miller (2008a), for instance, reported that the frequency of energy drink consumption in US undergraduates was positively associated with smoking, drinking, alcohol problems, use of illicit prescription drugs and marijuana, sexual risk-taking, fighting, seatbelt omission, and taking risks on a dare. However, it should be noted that such effects might also be explainable by personality characteristics of high users of energy drinks (for example, adherence to a ‘toxic jock’ identity; Miller, 2008b), rather than necessarily to the products themselves.

Another potential route that energy drinks may negatively affect health is through caffeine’s capacity to disrupt sleep. Energy drink use has been associated with daytime sleepiness and weekly ‘jolt and crash’ episodes (Kristjánsson et al., 2011; Malinauskas et al., 2007), though the products also appear to be used to counter the effects of insufficient sleep (Malinauskas et al., 2007). Although findings such as these may implicate energy drinks in particular, Kristjánsson et al. (2013) have reported that caffeine consumption itself is positively associated with self-reported violent behaviour and conduct disorder. Furthermore, James et al. (2011) observed a strong inverse relationship between caffeine intake and academic attainment, 32% of which was explained by mediating effects of daytime sleepiness and other licit substance use. Due to findings such as these it is considered to be of particular importance to investigate the effects of caffeine from difference sources, as well as its overall intake.

Associations between caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression

The consumption of caffeinated beverages is known to be a coping strategy used by college students in the management of stressful academic situations (Lazarus, 1993; Thoits, 1995), with 49% of a representative stratified sample of Puerto Rican students reporting caffeinated products to be useful for coping with stress (Ríos et al., 2013). Pettit and DeBarr (2011) have also reported a positive relationship between energy drink consumption and perceived stress levels in undergraduate students. Though the use of caffeine is moderately related to a range of psychiatric and substance use disorders in the general population, the relationships appear not to be causal (Kendler et al., 2006), and results between studies are equivocal (for a review of the area see Lara, 2010). Discerning the nature and direction of relationships between such variables becomes even more difficult when considering the self-medication hypothesis (e.g. Khantzian, 1997). The idea here is that people may self-medicate with legal and/or illicit substances, with evidence having already been provided to suggest that some individuals with mental health problems use caffeinated energy drinks for such purposes (Chelben et al., 2008).

In some cases positive effects of caffeine have been observed. For instance, low doses have been shown to reduce anxiety and elevate mood (Haskell et al., 2005; Lieberman et al., 1987, 2002; Smith, 2009a; Smith et al., 1999). Smith (2009b) also reported that caffeine consumption was associated with reduced risk of depression compared with non-consumption in a population study.

Negative effects of caffeine on stress and mental health have also been observed. Gilliland and Andress (1981), for instance, reported higher anxiety levels in moderate and high caffeine consumers compared with abstainers in a student sample. Case reports also suggest that mania can be induced by a high intake of caffeine (Ogawa and Ueki, 2003) or energy drinks (Sharma, 2010). These results are supported by the finding of Kaplan et al. (1997), that 250 mg of caffeine can increase elation in healthy volunteers, whereas 500 mg increases irritability. Other studies, however, have reported null findings. James et al. (1989), for instance, found no relationships between caffeine intake and anxiety or depression in medical students.

In the general population, negative effects of caffeine are usually observed in relation to excessive intake. At extremely high doses its consumption can induce a condition known as ‘caffeinism’. Symptoms include anxiety, nervousness, restlessness, insomnia, excitement, psychomotor agitation, dysphoria, and a rambling flow of thoughts and speech (Gilliland and Andress, 1981; Greden, 1974), which have been considered to mimic a clinical picture known as ‘mixed mood state’ (Lara, 2010).

Larger effects of caffeine seem to occur in sensitive individuals, with psychiatric patients appearing to make up one such group. Higher sensitivity to the anxiogenic effects of high doses (typically >400 mg), for instance, has been observed in patients with panic disorder (Boulenger et al., 1984; Charney et al., 1985), generalised panic disorder (Bruce et al., 1992), and to a lesser extent, depression (Lee et al., 1988). Similar findings have also been made in patients with performance social anxiety disorder (though not generalised social anxiety disorder; Nardi et al., 2009), and excessive intake may interfere with the recovery of patients with bipolar disorder and manic-type mood episodes (Caykoylu et al., 2008; Dratcu et al., 2007; Tondo and Rudas, 1991).

Another potentially sensitive subgroup is that of young consumers. Certain psychiatric symptoms appear to occur at an alarming rate in this group. For example, the prevalence of major depressive disorder is known to range from 0.4% to 8% in adolescents (Birmaher et al., 1996; Fleming and Offord, 1990; Roberts et al., 1995), with approximately 30% reporting at least one current symptom of a major depressive episode (Roberts et al., 1995). Depressive symptoms have also been found to correlate positively with coffee consumption in middle- and high-school students (Fulkerson et al., 2004), and positive associations with the Children’s Depression Inventory have been reported in both children and adolescents (Luebbe and Bell, 2009). However, as with research in adults, some studies have also reported null findings. Luebbe and Bell (2009), for instance, found no relationship between anxiety and caffeine in children and adolescents.

Aims of the current research

The general lack of research relating to the effects of caffeine on stress, anxiety, and depression in children is an area that the current paper will try to address. In order to do this, the Diet and Behaviour Scale (DABS; Richards et al., 2015b), a measure of intake of food and drinks (including caffeinated products) that may affect psychological outcomes, was administered to a large cohort of secondary school children from the South West of England. The current paper used the DABS for two purposes: (1) to provide estimates of weekly caffeine intake from energy drinks, cola, tea, and coffee, and (2) so that additional aspects of diet could be controlled for in multivariate analyses.

Along with the DABS, single-item measures of self-assessed stress, anxiety, and depression were administered. Single items were chosen as they have been shown to be valid and reliable, allowing for the identification of overall risk whilst reducing the time costs associated with administering multi-item measures (Williams and Smith, 2012). The items themselves came from the Wellbeing Process Questionnaire (Williams, 2014), have been validated against full measures, demonstrated to correlate well, and appear to be as sensitive as the full-length measures with which they were compared (Williams, 2015; Williams and Smith, 2013).

It was hypothesised that high consumption of caffeine would be associated with high stress, anxiety, and depression, and that such relationships would not be dependent on the source from which caffeine was obtained. However, as no interventions were conducted, and data presented here are only cross-sectional in nature, it should be acknowledged that it is not possible to infer causality or the direction of relationships observed.

Method

The Cornish Academies Project was a large-scale longitudinal programme of research designed to investigate dietary effects on school performance, general health, and stress, anxiety, and depression in secondary school children. Two cross-sections of data were collected from three academies in the South West of England. The first cross-section (T1) was collected 6 months prior to the second (T2). The current paper presents analyses using data from the latter cross-section only, as information relating to stress, anxiety, and depression were not collected at the former.

Participants

In total, 3071 secondary school pupils were asked to take part in the Cornish Academies Project at T1; 2610 (85%) agreed to participate. At T2, the cohort consisted of 3323 pupils, and 2307 completed the questionnaires. A relatively balanced sex ratio (48.5% male, 51.5% female), and an age range of 11–17 (M = 13.6, SD = 1.49) were observed (for a more detailed description of the sample see Richards et al., 2015b).

Apparatus/materials

The DABS (Richards et al., 2015b) is a 29-item questionnaire developed for the purpose of assessing intake of common dietary variables with an onus on functional foods, and foods and drinks of current concern. The DABS contains 18 questions that assess frequency of consumption on a five-point scale (1 = never, 2 = once a month, 3 = once or twice a week, 4 = most days [3–6], 5 = every day), and 11 questions to assess amounts typically consumed. It has been associated with a four-factor structure in secondary school children labelled Junk Food, Caffeinated Soft Drinks/Gum, Healthy Foods, and Hot Caffeinated Beverages (see Richards et al., 2015b).

Because caffeine content is known to vary considerably between energy drink products (Reissig et al., 2009), participants were asked to state the brand names of those that they consumed. This measure was included in order to increase the accuracy of estimating caffeine consumption. In addition to this, as diet may reflect general lifestyle (e.g. Akbaraly, 2009), five further questions were administered. Three items were used to gauge exercise frequency (mildly energetic, moderately energetic, and vigorous), with answers being given on a four-point scale (1 = three times a week or more, 2 = once or twice a week, 3 = about once to three times a month, 4 = never/hardly ever). In addition to this, participants were asked to state how many hours per night they typically spent asleep, and to give an indication of how good they perceived their general health to have been over the previous 6 months (1 = very good, 2 = good, 3 = fair, 4 = bad, 5 = very bad). Participants were then asked to state how frequently they had experienced stress, anxiety, and depression over the previous 6 months, on a five-point scale (1 = not at all, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = frequently, 5 = very frequently), though no clinical evaluations were made. No further descriptions of ‘stress’, ‘anxiety’, or ‘depression’ were provided as it was assumed that participants would understand the concepts at hand.

Design and procedure

Schoolteachers administered the DABS as well as the lifestyle, stress, anxiety, and depression questions to the pupils at their respective academies. Demographic information was acquired through the School Information Management System (SIMS) and stored in a confidential database in Cardiff. This information included age, sex, school attendance, number of detentions/behavioural points received, English and maths attainment at Key Stage 3/Key Stage 4, school year, ethnicity, presence/absence of a special educational needs (SEN) status, eligibility/ineligibility to receive free school meals (FSM; a proxy indication of socioeconomic status; Shuttleworth, 1995), whether or not English was spoken as an additional language, and whether or not children were cared for by a non-parental guardian.

All questionnaire and demographic data were anonymised prior to being merged into a single database. Ethical clearance was granted by Cardiff University’s School of Psychology Ethics Committee, and informed consent was acquired from all participants (as well as their parents) before data were collected. All data analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 20.

Statistical analysis

The representativeness of the sample was investigated by comparing SIMS data for those who completed the questionnaires with that of those who did not, though frequency data relating to stress, anxiety, and depression are not provided here because they have already been reported elsewhere (see Richards and Smith, 2015). Weekly caffeine consumption was calculated from the DABS items used to measure the amount of consumption of energy drinks, cola, tea, and coffee. Linear-by-linear trends were then investigated between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression, and were followed up with binary logistic regression analyses (using the ‘enter’ method), so that additional variance from diet, demography, and lifestyle could be controlled for statistically. In order to investigate interactions between caffeine use and sex, multivariate analyses were conducted for males and females separately. It was also deemed important to examine the effects of each individual source of caffeine (i.e. that consumed specifically from energy drinks, cola, tea, and coffee). As with the analyses of total weekly caffeine intake, these effects were initially investigated using linear-by-linear trends, and then with binary logistic regression to control for additional covariates (though in this instance separate analyses were not conducted for males and females).

Results

Demographic and lifestyle variables

Considerable variance in demographic background and lifestyle was observed within the sample; for frequency data, see Table 1. Participants’ average number of sleep hours and frequency of exercise (a single-factor analysed variable derived from items measuring mild, moderate, and vigorous exercise) that are used as control variables in the current study have been described elsewhere (see Richards et al., 2015b).

Table 1.

Frequency information for demographic variables.

N %
Academy 1 971 29.2%
2 1375 41.4%
3 977 29.4%
School year 7 573 18.8%
8 602 19.7%
9 618 20.3%
10 616 20.2%
11 640 21%
Sex Male 1018 48.5%
Female 1079 51.5%
SEN status Yes 899 29.2%
No 2184 70.8%
Eligible for FSM Yes 398 13.1%
No 2651 86.9%
Ethnicity White 2946 97.2%
Not white 84 2.8%
English as additional language Yes 51 1.7%
No 2868 98.3%
Non-parental guardian Yes 17 .6%
No 2909 99.4%

Representativeness of the sample

A relatively high response rate of 88.4% was observed for completion of the DABS. To investigate how representative the sample was in reference to the academies from which it came, Chi-square tests were conducted to determine if SIMS data for those who completed the DABS differed from SIMS data of those that did not. It should be noted that a similar analysis presented in Richards et al. (2015b) relates to T1 from the Cornish Academies Project, whereas that presented here relates to T2.

It was found that the academy a pupil came from was significantly related to their likelihood of responding to the questionnaires, with Academy 1 and Academy 3 providing fewer respondents, and Academy 2 providing more respondents, than expected, χ2 (2, N = 3323) = 241.172, p < .001. The school year that a participant came from was also related to their likelihood of completing the questionnaires, χ2 (4, N = 3049) = 34.681, p < .001. A significant linear-by-linear association was observed, with the likelihood of responding being negatively associated with school year, χ2 (1, N = 3049) = 30.245, p < .001. Children with a SEN status were also less likely to answer the questionnaire, χ2 (1, N = 3083) = 23.142, p < .001, as were children who were eligible to receive FSM, χ2 (1, N = 3049) = 25.116, p < .001.

Associations between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression

Univariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression

Single items from the DABS were used to estimate weekly caffeine intake, with the following values being assigned: cup of coffee (80 mg), cup of tea (40 mg), can of cola (25 mg), can of energy drink (133 mg). The values used for coffee, tea, and cola, were based on updated versions of those reported by Brice and Smith (2002), which were themselves based on values provided by Barone and Roberts (1996) and Scott et al. (1989); the value used for energy drinks was the mean caffeine content of the three brands most commonly reported by the current sample (which together accounted for 53.2% of all cases). Caffeine totals consumed from coffee, tea, energy drinks, and cola were then added together to create a variable for total weekly consumption. It was found that caffeine intake was higher in males than females, both in total amount, as well as in that consumed from energy drinks, cola, and coffee (though there was no difference regarding caffeine consumed from tea; for descriptive statistics, see Table 2). Total weekly caffeine was subsequently recoded into a categorical variable consisting of the following six consumption groups: 0 mg/w, 0.1–250 mg/w, 250.1–500 mg/w, 500.1–750 mg/w, 750.1–1000 mg/w, >1000 mg/w.

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics and sex differences for self-reported stress, anxiety and depression, and weekly caffeine intake as calculated from the DABS.

Overall
Males
Females
Sex difference
N M SD N M SD N M SD t df p
Mental health Stress 2249 2.88 1.08 984 2.67 1.07 1060 3.08 1.05 −8.7 2024.191 < .001
Anxiety 2239 2.43 1.05 979 2.16 1 1058 2.66 1.05 −10.89 2033.779 < .001
Depression 2237 2.17 1.15 980 1.97 1.06 1054 2.34 1.19 −7.411 2028.44 < .001
Caffeine intake (mg/w) Total 2200 421.77 550 963 467.34 557.01 1033 364.99 512.84 4.262 1948.766 < .001
Energy drinks 2254 123.74 246.99 989 158.69 270.54 1055 89.51 223.12 6.284 1918.455 < .001
Cola 2253 36.7 55.52 991 41.45 60.31 1053 32 49.49 3.857 1917.632 < .001
Coffee 2265 113.77 322.51 996 130.6 348.42 1061 92.97 278.13 2.696 1902.424 .007
Tea 2267 152.32 261.65 996 142.49 241.87 1063 155.6 266.93 −1.165 2057 .244

Self-assessed stress, anxiety, and depression were all found to be significantly higher in females compared with males (for descriptive statistics, see Table 2). The single-item measures were then dichotomised, with those answering with 1 or 2 (‘not at all’ or ‘rarely’ experienced stress, anxiety, or depression) making up the above average mental health group, and those answering with 3, 4, or 5 (‘sometimes’, ‘frequently’, or ‘very frequently’ experienced stress, anxiety, or depression) making up the below average mental health group.

Linear-by-linear associations were investigated between the dichotomous variables for stress, anxiety, and depression, and the categorical variable created from total weekly caffeine intake. The analysis found the >1000 mg/w condition to be associated with high stress, anxiety, and depression. In addition to this, consuming 0.1–250 mg/w was associated with low stress, and non-consumption was associated with low depression, though the latter effect was not significant. For linear-by-linear associations and cross-tabulations between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression, see Table 3.

Table 3.

Cross-tabulations between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression.

0 mg/w 0.1–250 mg/w 250.1–500 mg/w 500.1–750 mg/w 750.1–1000 mg/w >1000 mg/w
Stress Low Count 81 342 165 89 42 66
Expected count 81.6 318.5 166.9 88.2 44.8 84.9
Column % 36.2% 39.1% 36% 36.8% 34.1% 28.3%
Adjusted residual −.1 2.1 −.2 .1 −.5 −2.7
High Count 143 532 293 153 81 167
Expected count 142.4 555.5 291.1 153.8 78.2 148.1
Column % 63.8% 60.9% 64% 63.2% 65.9% 71.7%
Adjusted residual .1 −2.1 .2 −.1 .5 2.7
Linear-by-linear 6.599, p = .01
Anxiety Low Count 134 519 258 143 75 110
Expected count 128.7 502.9 262.7 139.1 71 134.5
Column % 60.1% 59.6% 56.7% 59.3% 61% 47.2%
Adjusted residual .8 1.4 −.5 .5 .7 −3.4
High Count 89 352 197 98 48 123
Expected count 94.3 368.1 192.3 101.9 52 98.5
Column % 39.9% 40.4% 43.3% 40.7% 39% 52.8%
Adjusted residual −.8 −1.4 .5 −.5 −.7 3.4
Linear-by-linear 6.976, p = .008
Depression Low Count 158 574 308 157 77 131
Expected count 146.1 569.5 300.1 157.3 80.6 151.4
Column % 70.9% 66.1% 67.2% 65.4% 62.6% 56.7%
Adjusted residual 1.8 .4 .9 .0 −.7 −3
High Count 65 295 150 83 46 100
Expected count 76.9 299.5 157.9 82.7 42.4 79.6
Column % 29.1% 33.9% 32.8% 34.6% 37.4% 43.3%
Adjusted residual −1.8 −.4 −.9 .0 .7 3
Linear-by-linear 9.101, p = .003

Note. Mean weekly caffeine intake for each consumption group was as follows: 0 mg M = 0 mg (SD = 0), 0.1–250 mg/w M = 117.83 (SD = 69.32), 250.1–500 mg/wM = 355.94 (SD = 70.61), 500.1–750 mg/w M = 616.37 (SD = 69.99), 750.1–1000 mg/w M = 865.09 (SD = 72.71), >1000 mg/w M = 1651.74 (SD = 750.33).

Multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression

The analyses described in the previous section indicate that being a very high consumer of caffeine is a predictor of high levels of stress, anxiety, and depression. It was therefore deemed important to further investigate such effects at the multivariate level, so that additional variance could be controlled for statistically. In order to do this, binary logistic regression analyses (using the ‘enter’ method) were conducted upon the dependent variables of stress, anxiety, and depression. The same categorical variable for total weekly caffeine intake described in the previous section was used, and the non-consumption group was set as the comparison. The additional covariates entered were diet (the DABS subscale scores for Junk Food and Healthy Foods; Caffeinated Soft Drinks/Gum and Hot Caffeinated Beverages were not entered as they were comprised of caffeinated products; for a description of these variables see Richards et al., 2015b), demography (sex, school, school year, presence/absence of a SEN status, and the eligibility/ineligibility to receive FSM), and lifestyle (sleep hours, exercise frequency, and school attendance). It was, however, deemed inappropriate to attempt to control for ethnicity, whether English was spoken as an additional language, and whether or not the child was cared for by a non-parental guardian, due to the numbers present in the minority groups being particularly small.

After controlling for covariates, the overall effect of caffeine on stress was not significant, Wald = 6.252, p = .283, and none of the consumption groups differed from the non-consumption group. However, total weekly caffeine intake remained a significant predictor of anxiety, Wald = 12.39, p = .03. This effect reflected increased risk of high anxiety occurring in the >1000 mg/w group, though none of the other conditions differed significantly from the non-consumers. For odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for the multivariate association between total weekly caffeine intake and anxiety, see Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and anxiety.

The effect of caffeine on depression also remained significant after controlling for covariates, Wald = 14.682, p = .012. In this case increased risk was associated with each of the consumption groups compared with the non-consumers (though the effect relating to the 250.1–500 mg/w group was only marginally significant, and the effect relating to the 500.1–750 mg/w group was not significant). For odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for the multivariate associations between caffeine and depression, see Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and depression.

Sex differences in associations between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression

Due to the large sample size available, and because sex differences in responses to caffeine in adolescents have been reported (e.g. Temple and Ziegler, 2011), it was deemed meritorious to investigate interactions between sex and caffeine intake. To do this, the same methodology outlined in the previous section was used (i.e. binary logistic regression analyses were conducted, and the same covariates were entered), except that the caffeine*sex interaction term was included instead of the main effects of caffeine and sex. Significant interactions were observed for each of the outcome variables: stress, Wald = 31.927, p < .001, anxiety, Wald = 50.341, p < .001, depression, Wald = 45.038, p < .001.

In order to further investigate the interactions between sex and caffeine intake on stress, anxiety, and depression, separate multivariate analyses were conducted in males and females. The overall effect of caffeine on stress was not significant in males, Wald = 5.193, p = .393, or females, Wald = 4.243, p = .515, though males who consumed >1000 mg/w were marginally more likely to report high stress compared with controls, OR = 1.891, 95% CI [.943, 3.792], p = .073. The effect of caffeine on anxiety was not significant in females, Wald = 8.307, p = .14, and none of the consumption groups differed significantly from the control. In males, however, the effect was significant, Wald = 13.186, p = .022. This reflected increased risk of high anxiety in the 0.1–250 mg/w, 250.1–500 mg/w, and >1000 mg/w conditions, with the effect being most apparent in the last condition. For odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for the multivariate association between total weekly caffeine intake and anxiety in males see Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and anxiety in males.

The overall effect of caffeine on depression in males was not significant, Wald = 7.882, p = .163. However, each caffeine consumption group was associated with increased risk compared with the control (though the effect relating to the 500.1–750 mg/w group was only marginally significant, and the effect in the 750.1–1000 mg/w condition was not significant). The overall effect in females was significant, Wald = 13.137, p = .022, and reflected increased risk in both the 750.1–1000 mg/w and >1000 mg/w groups. For odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for the multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and depression in males and females, see Figures 4 and 5, respectively.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and depression in males.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for multivariate associations between total weekly caffeine intake and depression in females.

Associations between individual caffeine sources and stress, anxiety, and depression

Univariate associations between individual caffeine sources and stress, anxiety, and depression

In order to determine whether the source of caffeine was important regarding the relationships reported in the previous section, caffeine from energy drinks, cola, tea, and coffee were recoded into three groups (non-consumption, low consumption, and high consumption), and linear-by-linear associations were investigated in relation to stress, anxiety, and depression. Because the distributions were skewed, the cut-off points to define what constituted ‘low consumption’ and ‘high consumption’ were determined in a manner that assigned relatively balanced numbers of participants to each group. These distinctions are shown in Table 4; essentially ‘low consumption’ related to one can of energy drink, one can of cola, two cups of coffee, and three cups of tea per week, and ‘high consumption’ related to any values in excess of these.

Table 4.

Cross-tabulations between weekly caffeine intake from energy drinks, cola, coffee, and tea, and stress, anxiety, and depression.

Caffeine from energy drinks
Caffeine from cola
Caffeine from coffee
Caffeine from tea
0 mg 0.1–133 mg >133 mg 0 mg 0.1–25 mg >25 mg 0 mg 0.1–160 mg >160 mg 0 mg 0.1–120 mg >120 mg
Low Count 493 164 145 245 295 266 602 110 94 333 227 248
Stress Expected count 476.6 175.6 149.8 275.7 272.4 257.8 576.3 110.5 119.2 335 216.3 256.7
Column % 37.6% 34% 35.2% 32.5% 39.6% 37.7% 38% 36.2% 28.7% 36.2% 38.2% 35.2%
Adjusted residual 1.5 −1.2 −.5 −2.9 2.1 .8 2.5 −.1 −3.1 −.2 1.1 −.8
High Count 818 319 267 509 450 439 984 194 234 587 367 457
Expected count 834.4 307.4 262.2 478.3 472.6 447.2 1009.7 193.5 208.8 585 377.7 448.3
Column % 62.4% 66% 64.8% 67.5% 60.4% 62.3% 62% 63.8% 71.3% 63.8% 61.8% 64.8%
Adjusted residual −1.5 1.2 .5 2.9 −2.1 −.8 −2.5 .1 3.1 .2 −1.1 .8
Linear-by-linear 1.426, p = .232 4.477, p = .034 9.308, p = .002 .121, p = .728
Low Count 755 277 233 412 447 408 933 172 166 524 352 394
Anxiety Expected count 752.8 276.3 236 432.9 427.7 406.4 907.9 174.9 188.1 525.8 339.6 404.6
Column % 57.7% 57.7% 56.8% 54.9% 60.3% 58% 59.1% 56.6% 50.8% 57.3% 59.6% 56%
Adjusted residual .2 .1 −.3 −1.9 1.8 .2 2.4 −.4 −2.7 −.2 1.2 −1
High Count 553 203 177 338 294 296 645 132 161 391 239 310
Expected count 555.2 203.7 174 317.1 313.3 297.6 670.1 129.1 138.9 389.2 251.4 299.4
Column % 42.3% 42.3% 43.2% 45.1% 39.7% 42% 40.9% 43.4% 49.2% 42.7% 40.4% 44%
Adjusted residual −.2 −.1 .3 1.9 −1.8 −.2 −2.4 .4 2.7 .2 −1.2 1
Linear-by-linear .081, p = .776 1.434, p = .231 7.62, p = .006 .196, p = .658
Depression Low Count 864 316 255 497 491 447 1048 198 193 612 369 460
Expected count 853.4 313.7 267.9 489.5 485.5 460 1029.5 197.6 211.9 597.8 384.4 458.8
Column % 66.2% 65.8% 62.2% 66.4% 66.2% 63.6% 66.4% 65.3% 59.4% 66.8% 62.6% 65.4%
Adjusted residual 1 .3 −1.5 .7 .5 −1.3 1.8 .1 −2.4 1.3 −1.6 .1
High Count 442 164 155 251 251 256 531 105 132 304 220 243
Expected count 452.6 166.3 142.1 258.5 256.5 243 549.5 105.4 113.1 318.2 204.6 244.2
Column % 33.8% 34.2% 37.8% 33.6% 33.8% 36.4% 33.6% 34.7% 40.6% 33.2% 37.4% 34.6%
Adjusted residual −1 −.3 1.5 −.7 −.5 1.3 −1.8 −.1 2.4 −1.3 1.6 −.1
Linear-by-linear 1.805, p = .179 1.288, p = .257 5.164, p = .023 .465, p = .495

Note. Caffeine amounts listed relate to weekly consumption.

Caffeine consumed from energy drinks and tea was not associated with stress, anxiety, or depression. Interestingly, although consumption of caffeine from cola was not associated with anxiety or depression, its non-consumption was associated with high stress levels, and being a low consumer was associated with low stress levels.

Positive linear relationships were observed between caffeine consumption from coffee and stress, anxiety, and depression (for linear-by-linear associations and cross-tabulations between stress, anxiety, and depression, and caffeine consumed from individual sources, see Table 4). However these associations are likely explained by coffee being the major contributor to high overall caffeine intake. This is reflected in the observation that those above the median for caffeine intake from coffee consumed more total caffeine than did those above the median for each of the other sources: caffeine from coffee low M = 261.42 (SD = 331.82), high M = 827.65 (SD = 748.51); caffeine from energy drinks low M = 247.63 (SD = 382.38), high M = 674.24 (SD = 649.38); caffeine from tea low M = 225.97 (SD = 365.43), high M = 640.55 (SD = 633.11); caffeine from cola low M = 295.12 (SD = 448.63), high M = 486.88 (SD = 585).

Multivariate associations between individual caffeine sources and stress, anxiety, and depression

In order to further investigate associations between caffeine from different sources and stress, anxiety, and depression, the non-consumption/low consumption/high consumption variables for caffeine from energy drinks, cola, tea, and coffee were entered together into binary logistic regression analyses using the ‘enter’ method. The same dietary, demographic, and lifestyle variables that were controlled for in the multivariate analyses of total weekly caffeine intake were again entered as covariates here.

Low consumption of caffeine from energy drinks was associated with high stress, though the overall effect was not significant. Both low and high consumption of caffeine from cola, on the other hand, were significantly associated with low stress. Low caffeine from energy drinks and high caffeine from coffee were both marginally associated with high anxiety, though neither effect was significant overall. Low consumption of caffeine from tea was associated with high depression, and the overall effect was significant. High caffeine consumption from coffee was also associated with high depression, though in this case the overall effect was not significant. For odds rations, 95% confidence intervals, and p-values for all multivariate level associations between individual caffeine sources and stress, anxiety, and depression, see Table 5.

Table 5.

Multivariate associations between individual sources of caffeine and stress, anxiety, and depression.

Caffeine source OR 95% CI p
Stress Energy drinks Low 1.377 1.051, 1.803 .02
High 1.099 .804, 1.502 .555
Wald 5.41, p = .067
Cola Low .721 .557, .935 .013
High .68 .517, .895 .006
Wald 8.986, p = .011
Coffee Low .957 .705, 1.3 .779
High 1.293 .93, 1.8 .127
Wald 2.625, p = .269
Tea Low 1.014 .784, 1.312 .915
High 1.052 .818, 1.353 .693
Wald .159, p = .923
Anxiety Energy drinks Low 1.259 .967, 1.638 .087
High 1.05 .77, 1.43 .759
Wald 3.008, p = .222
Cola Low .862 .669, 1.109 .248
High .83 .635, 1.085 .173
Wald 2.151, p = .341
Coffee Low 1.138 .842, 1.538 .401
High 1.348 .988, 1.838 .059
Wald 3.829, p = .147
Tea Low .944 .731, 1.217 .655
High .958 .75, 1.224 .731
Wald .231, p = .891
Depression Energy drinks Low .994 .756, 1.306 .964
High 1.11 .811, 1.52 .516
Wald .449, .779
Cola Low 1.184 .911, 1.539 .206
High 1.227 .93, 1.619 .148
Wald 2.443, p = .295
Coffee Low .931 .681, 1.273 .655
High 1.369 1.001, 1.872 .049
Wald 4.514, p = .105
Tea Low 1.408 1.086, 1.825 .01
High 1.104 .856, 1.422 .447
Wald 6.809, p = .033

Discussion

The current study aimed to present cross-sectional data from the Cornish Academies Project to investigate associations between caffeine consumption and stress, anxiety, and depression in secondary school children. Based on findings from the literature it was predicted that excessive caffeine intake would be associated with high stress, anxiety, and depression, and that such effects would not be dependent on any particular source of caffeine. In addition to this, separate analyses were conducted in males and females in order to investigate interactions between caffeine and sex.

Relationships between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression

Initial positive relationships were observed between total weekly caffeine intake and stress, anxiety, and depression. After adjusting for dietary, demographic, and lifestyle covariates, the effect on stress disappeared. However, consuming >1000 mg/w remained a predictor of high anxiety, and caffeine consumption in general appeared to be associated with higher instances of depression compared with non-consumption (although the effect was also most pronounced in those who consumed >1000 mg/w).

Though the above findings mainly replicated those reported in adults (e.g. Gilliland and Andress, 1981; Pettit and DeBarr, 2011), the effects appeared to occur at lower doses, which is most likely a reflection of the lower bodyweight of children compared with adults. One finding that differed considerably from those made in adult populations was that of depression. Smith (2009b) observed caffeine consumption to be beneficial compared with its abstinence, whereas the opposite pattern of results was observed here. This finding is therefore likely to highlight differences between the populations studied.

As significant interactions between total weekly caffeine intake and sex were observed in relation to each of the outcome variables, separate multivariate analyses were conducted for males and females. No association between caffeine and anxiety appeared in females; in males, higher instances of anxiety occurred in the 0.1–250 mg/w, 250.1–500 mg/w, and >1000 mg/w conditions, with the largest effect occurring in the last group. For depression, effects occurred in both males and females. In males, increased risk was associated with each group that consumed caffeine compared with non-consumers (though consuming 500.1–750 mg/w was only a marginally significant predictor, and consuming 750.1–1000 mg/w was not significantly related). In females, consuming either 750.1–1000 mg/w or >1000 mg/w was significantly associated with higher reporting of depression. These observations are consistent with other findings, such as caffeine having been shown to produce greater arousal effects in young males compared with females (Adan et al., 2008), and to have a higher propensity for reinforcement in adolescent males compared with females (Temple et al., 2009). Though it may be that male adolescents are more vulnerable to harmful effects of caffeine than are females, these results may also reflect sexually dimorphic personality characteristics, or the observation that overall caffeine consumption in the current study was higher in males than in females.

When individual caffeine sources were investigated, negative effects were observed in relation to coffee, tea, and energy drinks, though they were not consistent across variables and often only marginally statistically significant. One relationship of particular interest was however observed: both low (0.1–25 mg/w) and high (>25 mg/w) levels of caffeine consumed from cola were associated with low stress. This finding may reflect reports of students using caffeinated products to cope with stress (e.g. Ríos et al., 2013). However, the general lack of consistent findings from this analysis as a whole suggests that, when investigating its effects on stress, anxiety, and depression, caffeine is best examined in terms of total intake rather than by differentiating between individual sources.

Methodological limitations and directions for future research

Though the current study has addressed a gap in the literature, several methodological limitations should be considered when interpreting the findings. One such limitation is that the participants who completed the questionnaires were not fully representative of the schools from which they came. However, taking a multivariate approach to data analysis in which demographic and lifestyle variables could be controlled for statistically is deemed to have been an effective method for addressing this issue. Nevertheless, as the population studied came from a very specific demographic group (i.e. 11–17-year-old White children from the South West of England), further research is needed that focuses on more representative samples.

Another limitation of the current research was that the chronicity of caffeine use was not taken into account. For instance, a weekly cycle of caffeine use in adolescents was reported by Pollak and Bright (2003), in which consumption peaked during the weekend (Saturday), and was lowest in the middle of the week (Wednesday). Coupled with the observations that adolescents sometimes use caffeinated products to delay sleep onset (e.g. Calamaro et al., 2009) and to counteract the effects of sleepiness during the day (Malinauskas et al., 2007), it is possible that the timing of administration of the questionnaire may have been of importance.

A further limitation of the current study is that it utilised a cross-sectional design. This means that all effects observed here are correlational, and that causation cannot be inferred. Therefore the possibility of reverse-causation, or indeed bi-directionality, cannot be disregarded. For instance, high caffeine consumption may be a cause of high stress, anxiety, and depression, but suffering from such conditions may also lead towards the high consumption of caffeinated products as a coping strategy. Furthermore, it is possible that the effects observed here are attributable to personality characteristics associated with caffeine users, rather than to their use of caffeine. Future research should therefore aim to conduct intervention studies in order to investigate the nature of these relationships further.

Conclusions

The current study has presented results that suggest caffeine consumption may be associated with stress, anxiety, and depression in secondary school children, though the effect on stress disappeared after additional dietary, demographic, and lifestyle variance was controlled for statistically. The effects observed also appeared to differ between males and females. Though caffeine consumption was associated with anxiety in males at the multivariate level, no such observation was made in females. Furthermore, though the effects relating to depression occurred in both sexes, the threshold at which they appeared was lower in males than it was in females.

Initial analyses of individual caffeine sources implied that coffee may have been responsible for the effects observed in relation to total caffeine intake, but further investigations suggested this not to have been the case, and that they were likely attributable to caffeine consumption in general rather than to any particular source. The study also identified very high caffeine intake (>1000 mg/w) to be a risk factor associated with anxiety and depression, although effects were sometimes detected at lower doses. These findings may therefore be a concern for public health and school policy, and should be considered an important area for further investigation.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the contribution of The Waterloo Foundation for funding the research. In addition, the authors wish to express their gratitude for the on-going support and collaboration with Pool Academy, Penrice Community College, and Treviglas Community College, as well as to thank each of the teachers and pupils who made the research possible.

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The current research was supported by a grant from The Waterloo Foundation (grant number: 503692).

References

  1. Adan A, Prat G, Fabbri M, et al. (2008) Early effects of caffeinated and decaffeinated coffee on subjective state and gender differences. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 1698–1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akbaraly TN. (2009) Dietary pattern and depressive symptoms in middle age. Br J Psychiatry 195: 408–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arria AM, Bugbee BA, Caldeira KM, et al. (2014) Evidence and knowledge gaps for the association between energy drink use and high-risk behaviors among adolescents and young adults. Nutr Rev 72: 87–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barone JJ, Roberts HR. (1996) Caffeine consumption. Food Chem Toxicol 34: 119–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Birmaher B, Ryan N, Williamson D, et al. (1996) Childhood and adolescent depression: A review of the past 10 years. Part II. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35: 1575–1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boulenger J-P, Uhde TW, Wolff EA, III, et al. (1984) Increased sensitivity to caffeine in patients with panic disorders. Preliminary evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41: 1067–1071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Branum AM, Rossen LM, Schoendorf KC. (2014) Trends in caffeine intake among US children and adolescents. Pediatrics 133: 386–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brice CF, Smith AP. (2002) Factors associated with caffeine consumption. Int J Food Sci Nutr 53: 55–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bruce M, Scott N, Shine P, et al. (1992) Anxiogenic effects of caffeine in patients with anxiety disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49: 867–869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Calamaro CJ, Mason TBA, Ratcliffe SJ. (2009) Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics 123: e1005–e1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Caykoylu A, Ekinci O, Kuloglu M. (2008) Improvement from treatment-resistant schizoaffective disorder, manic type after stopping heavy caffeine intake: A case report. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 1349–1350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Charney DS, Heninger GR, Jatlow PI. (1985) Increased anxiogenic effects of caffeine in panic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42: 233–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chelben J, Piccone-Sapir A, Lanco L, et al. (2008) Effects of amino acid energy drinks leading to hospitalization in individuals with mental illness. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 30: 187–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cysneiros RM, Farkas D, Harmatz JS, et al. (2007) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamic interactions between zolpidem and caffeine. Clin Pharmacol Ther 82: 54–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Doherty M, Smith PM. (2004) Effects of caffeine ingestion on exercise testing: A meta-analysis. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab 14: 626–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dratcu L, Grandison A, McKay G, et al. (2007) Clozapine-resistant psychosis, smoking, and caffeine: Managing the neglected effects of substances that our patients consume every day. Am J Ther 14: 314–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ferré S. (2008) An update in the mechanisms of the psychostimulant effects of caffeine. J Neurochem 105: 1067–1079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fleming JE, Offord DR. (1990) Epidemiology of childhood depressive disorders: A critical review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 29: 571–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fulkerson JA, Sherwood NE, Perry CL, et al. (2004) Depressive symptoms and adolescent eating and health behaviors: A multifaceted view in a population-based sample. Prevent Med 38: 865–875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gilliland K, Andress D. (1981) Ad lib caffeine consumption, symptoms of caffeinism, and academic performance. Am J Psychiatry 138: 512–514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Greden JF. (1974) Anxiety or caffeinism: A diagnostic dilemma. Am J Psychiatry 131: 1089–1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Haskell CF, Kennedy DO, Wesnes KA, et al. (2005) Cognitive and mood improvements of caffeine in habitual consumers and habitual non-consumers of caffeine. Psychopharmacology 179: 813–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Higdon JV, Frei B. (2006) Coffee and health: A review of recent human research. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 46: 101–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. James JE, Bruce MS, Lader MH, et al. (1989). Self-report reliability and symptomatology of habitual caffeine consumption. Br J Clin Pharmacol 27: 507–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. James JE, Kristjánsson AL, Sigfúsdóttir ID. (2011) Adolescent substance use, sleep, and academic achievement: Evidence of harm due to caffeine. J Adolesc 34: 665–673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kaplan GB, Greenblatt DJ, Ehrenberg BL, et al. (1997) Dose-dependent pharmacokinetics and psychomotor effects of caffeine in humans. J Clin Pharmacol 37: 693–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kendler KS, Myers J, Gardner CO. (2006) Caffeine intake, toxicity and dependence and lifetime risk for psychiatric and substance use disorders: An epidemiologic and co-twin control analysis. Psychol Med 36: 1717–1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Khantzian EJ. (1997) The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harv Rev Psychiatry 4: 231–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kristjánsson AL, Sigfúsdóttir ID, Allegrante JP, et al. (2011) Adolescent caffeine consumption, daytime sleepiness, and anger. J Caffeine Res 1: 75–82. [Google Scholar]
  30. Kristjánsson AL, Sigfúsdóttir ID, Frost SS, et al. (2013) Adolescent caffeine consumption and self-reported violence and conduct disorder. J Youth Adolesc 42: 1053–1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lara DR. (2010) Caffeine, mental health, and psychiatric disorders. J Alzheimers Dis 20(Suppl. 1): S239–S248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lazarus RS. (1993) Coping theory and research: Past, present, and future. Psychosom Med 55: 234–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lee MA, Flegel P, Greden JF, et al. (1988) Anxiogenic effects of caffeine on panic and depressed patients. Am J Psychiatry 145: 632–635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lieberman HR, Tharion WJ, Shukitt-Hale B, et al. (2002) Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training. Psychopharmacology 164: 250–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lieberman HR, Wurtman RJ, Emde GG, et al. (1987) The effects of low doses of caffeine on human performance and mood. Psychopharmacology 92: 308–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lorist M, Tops MM. (2003) Caffeine, fatigue and cognition. Brain Cogn 53: 82–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Luebbe AM, Bell DJ. (2009) Mountain Dew or mountain don’t?: A pilot investigation of caffeine use parameters and relations to depression and anxiety symptoms in 5th- and 10th-grade students. J School Health 79: 380–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Malinauskas BM, Aeby VG, Overton RF, et al. (2007) A survey of energy drink consumption patterns among college students. Nutr J 6(35). Available at: www.nutritionj.com/content/6/1/35 (accessed 27 May 2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mandel H. (2002) Update on caffeine consumption, disposition and action. Food Chem Toxicol 40: 1231–1234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McLellan TM, Lieberman HR. (2012) Do energy drinks contain active components other than caffeine? Nutr Rev 70: 730–744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Meadows-Oliver M, Ryan-Krause P. (2007) Powering up with sports and energy drinks. J Pediatr Health Care 21: 413–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Miller KE. (2008a) Energy drinks, race, and problem behaviors among college students. J Adolesc Health 43: 490–497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Miller KE. (2008b) Wired: Energy drinks, jock identity, masculine norms, and risk taking. J Am Coll Health 56: 481–490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nardi AE, Lopes FL, Freire RC, et al. (2009) Panic disorder and social anxiety disorder subtypes in a caffeine challenge test. Psychiatry Res 169: 149–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nawrot P, Jordan S, Eastwood J, et al. (2003) Effects of caffeine on human health. Food Addit Contam 20: 1–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ogawa N, Ueki H. (2003) Secondary mania caused by caffeine. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 25: 138–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pettit ML, DeBarr KA. (2011) Perceived stress, energy drink consumption, and academic performance among college students. J Am Coll Health 59: 335–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pollak CP, Bright D. (2003) Caffeine consumption and weekly sleep patterns in US seventh-, eighth-, and ninth-graders. Pediatrics 111: 42–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Reissig CJ, Strain EC, Griffiths RR. (2009) Caffeinated energy drinks - a growing problem. Drug Alcohol Depend 99: 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Richards G, Smith AP. (2015) Risk factors for, and effects of, stress, anxiety, and depression in adolescents. Br J Educ Soc Behav Sci 10: 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  51. Richards G, Malthouse A, Smith AP. (2015b) The diet and behaviour scale (DABS): Testing a new measure of food and drink consumption in a cohort of secondary school children from the south west of England. J Food Res 4: 148–161. [Google Scholar]
  52. Richards G, Millward N, Evans P, et al. (2015a) Acute effects of energy drinks on behavioural sanctions in secondary school children: A preliminary study. J Food Res 4: 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  53. Riksen NP, Rongen GA, Smits P. (2009) Acute and long-term cardiovascular effects of coffee: Implications for coronary heart disease. Pharmacol Ther 121: 185–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ríos JL, Betancourt J, Pagán I, et al. (2013) Caffeinated-beverage consumption and its association with socio-demographic characteristics and self-perceived academic stress in first and second year students at the University of Puerto Rico Medical Sciences Campus (UPR-MSC). Puerto Rico Health Sci J 32: 95–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Roberts RE, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. (1995) Symptoms of DSM-III-R major depression in adolescence: Evidence from an epidemiological survey. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 34: 1608–1617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Scott NR, Chakrabotry J, Marks V. (1989) Caffeine consumption in the United Kingdom: A retrospective survey. Food Sci Nutr 42: 183–191. [Google Scholar]
  57. Seifert SM, Schaechter JL, Hershorin ER, et al. (2011) Health effects of energy drinks on children, adolescents, and young adults. Pediatrics 127: 511–528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sharma V. (2010) Red bull and mania. German J Psychiatry 13: 178–180. [Google Scholar]
  59. Shuttleworth I. (1995) The relationship between social deprivation, as measured by individual free school meal eligibility, and educational attainment at GCSE in Northern Ireland: A preliminary investigation. Br Educ Res J 21: 487–504. [Google Scholar]
  60. Smit HJ, Rogers PJ. (2002) Effects of energy drinks on mood and mental performance: Critical methodology. Food Qual Prefer 13: 317–326. [Google Scholar]
  61. Smith AP. (2009a) Effects of caffeine in chewing gum on mood and attention. Hum Psychopharmacol 24: 239–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Smith AP. (2009b) Caffeine, cognitive failures and health in a non-working community sample. Hum Psychopharmacol 24: 29–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Smith AP, Sturgess W, Gallagher J. (1999) Effects of a low dose of caffeine given in different drinks on mood and performance. Hum Psychopharmacol 14: 473–482. [Google Scholar]
  64. Temple JL, Ziegler AM. (2011) Gender differences in subjective and physiological responses to caffeine and the role of steroid hormones. J Caffeine Res 1: 41–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Temple JL, Bulkley AM, Briatico L, et al. (2009) Sex differences in reinforcing value of caffeinated beverages in adolescents. Behav Pharmacol 20: 731–741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Thoits PA. (1995) Stress, coping, and social support processes: Where are we? What next? J Health Soc Behav 35: 53–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Tondo L, Rudas N. (1991) The course of a seasonal bipolar disorder influenced by caffeine. J Affect Disord 22: 249–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Williams G. (2014) Researching and developing mental health and well-being assessment tools for supporting employers and employees in Wales. PhD Thesis, Cardiff University, UK. [Google Scholar]
  69. Williams G. (2015) Assessing multiple factors of well-being using single-item measures. In: Sharples S, Shorrock S, Waterson P. (eds) Contemporary Ergonomics and Human Factors 2015. London: Taylor & Francis, pp. 291–298. [Google Scholar]
  70. Williams G, Smith AP. (2012) A holistic approach to stress and well-being. Part 6: The Wellbeing Process Questionnaire (WPQ Short Form). Occup Health Work 9: 29–31. [Google Scholar]
  71. Williams G, Smith AP. (2013) Measuring wellbeing in the workplace: Single item scales of depression and anxiety. In: Anderson M. (ed) Contemporary Ergonomics and Human Factors 2013. London: Taylor & Francis, pp. 87–94. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES