Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2015 Oct 15;4:1073. [Version 1] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.7198.1

Leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume: a meta-analysis

Gustav Nilsonne 1,2,a, Sandra Tamm 1,2, Kristoffer N T Månsson 3,4, Torbjörn Åkerstedt 1,2, Mats Lekander 1,2
PMCID: PMC4670011  PMID: 26674112

Abstract

Leukocyte telomere length has been shown to correlate to hippocampus volume, but effect estimates differ in magnitude and are not uniformly positive. This study aimed primarily to investigate the relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus gray matter volume by meta-analysis and secondarily to investigate possible effect moderators. Five studies were included with a total of 2107 participants, of which 1960 were contributed by one single influential study. A random-effects meta-analysis estimated the effect to r = 0.12 [95% CI -0.13, 0.37] in the presence of heterogeneity and a subjectively estimated moderate to high risk of bias. There was no evidence that apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype was an effect moderator, nor that the ratio of leukocyte telomerase activity to telomere length was a better predictor than leukocyte telomere length for hippocampus volume. This meta-analysis, while not proving a positive relationship, also is not able to disprove the earlier finding of a positive correlation in the one large study included in analyses. We propose that a relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocamus volume may be mediated by transmigrating monocytes which differentiate into microglia in the brain parenchyma.

Keywords: Telomeres, Morphometry, Hippocampus, Microglia

Introduction

Background

Telomeres are protective DNA-protein complexes at the ends of eukaryotic chromosomes 1. In cells with limited proliferative capacity, telomeres shorten with each cell division 2. Thus, telomere length in non-stem cells is a marker for biological aging. Germ cells, stem cells, and some cancer cells are able to maintain telomere length by expressing the enzyme telomerase reverse transcriptase 1, 2. Shorter leukocyte telomere length has been linked to adverse health outcomes including cancer 3, cardiovascular disease 4, and psychiatric disorders 5, as well as poor self-rated health outcomes such as sleep quality and daytime functioning 6. Recently, the association between leukocyte telomere length and brain morphology has been investigated by several research groups. The hippocampus has been a particular area of interest, possibly because it is a particularly plastic brain region, capable of neurogenesis 7, 8 and volume increases e.g. in spatial learning 9, 10, but also especially afflicted by atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease 11. The relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume has however not been fully clarified by investigations to date.

Two possible moderators of a relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume have been proposed. Wikgren et al. 12 proposed apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype as a moderator, while Jacobs et al. 13 proposed telomerase activity as a moderator, suggesting that the ratio of telomerase activity to telomere length is a better predictor than either measure alone.

Aims

This meta-analysis aimed primarily to estimate the correlation between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus gray matter volume. Secondary aims were to investigate the possible moderating effect of APOE genotype on this correlation and to investigate whether the ratio of leukocyte telomerase to telomere length is a better predictor of hippocampus volume than either measure alone. We discuss possible mechanisms for the putative association between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume and point to avenues for further inquiry.

Methods

PubMed ( www.pubmed.gov) was searched on 2015-09-22 using the search string ”telomer* hippocampus” with no limitations by date nor language. Results were reviewed by title and abstract. Studies reporting data on the association between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume in humans were included. Search results were reviewed independently by one investigator (GN) and by a committee of three investigators (KM, TÅ, and ML), reaching the same sample selection. Data were extracted by one investigator (GN) and checked for accuracy by another investigator (ST). The following variables were coded: number of participants and their sex distribution and age, observed correlation between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume, analysis covariates, and whether apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype or telomerase activity were also analysed. Figure 1 shows data inclusion. We analysed data with R version 3.2.0 14 with the metafor 15, psych 16, and pwr 17 packages. A random-effects model was fitted due to heterogeneity between studies. A sensitivity analysis investigating the effect of excluding one study was performed post-hoc. The meta-analysis protocol was not predefined, and hence not registered. To compare telomere length, telomerase activity, and their ratio, data were estimated from published scatterplots using the freely available WebPlotDigitizer ( http://arohatgi.info/WebPlotDigitizer/) version 3.8 18.

Figure 1. Data inclusion flowchart.

Figure 1.

One study (Jacobs et al. 13) reported results for right and left hippocampus separately. Partial r 2 values for the relationship between hippocampus volume and leukocyte telomere length were however identical for both sides ( r 2 = 0.16) and this value was used. The same study reported data only for one subset of participants (APOE 4 non-carriers). All code and data needed to reproduce the analyses in this paper are available at 19, as is the list of PubMed search results.

Results

Included studies

Five studies were included, of which two each contained two different participant cohorts, with a total of 2107 participants ( Table 1). Several possible sources of heterogeneity were identified in sampling strategies and analysis methods. Grodstein et al. 20 included a sub-group of patients with minimal cognitive impairment, and Wolkowitz et al. 21 included a cohort with major depressive disorder. All studies determined relative average telomere length by PCR as the ratio of telomeres to single genes (T/S ratio). Two studies (Grodstein et al. 20 and King et al. 22) used the T/S ratio in statistical analyses, whereas the other three studies (Wikgren et al. 12, Jacobs et al. 13, and Wolkowitz et al. 21) determined the average telomere base pair length and used that for statistical analyses. One study (King et al. 22) log-transformed telomere length data prior to analysis in order to better approximate a normal distribution. Three studies (Grodstein et al. 20, Jacobs et al. 13, and Wikgren et al. 12) did not use a log-transformation, and one study (Wolkowitz et al. 21) reported that some variables were log-transformed but did not say whether telomere length was one of them.

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.

APOE = Apolipoprotein E genotype, investigated (+) or not investigated (-); TA = telomerase activity, investigated (+) or not investigated (-); 3 & 4 = APOE alleles; MDD = Major Depressive Disorder; a = Age was not given for imaging subsample, age for whole healthy subsample substituted; b = Median and interquartile range.

Study and group Year Ref. n Female, n Age, mean (SD) Covariates APOE TA
Grodstein 2008 20 26 26 (100%) 79.2 (2.2) a age, education - -
Wikgren 3/ 3 2012 12 29 18 (62%) 62.1 (8.5) age, body size + -
Wikgren 4 2012 12 28 18 (64%) 61.1 (8.5) age, body size + -
King 2014 22 1960 1153 (59%) 50 (42 – 58) b age, sex, race/ethnicity + -
Wolkowitz MDD 2015 21 19 12 (63%) 37.8 (12.0) age, sex - +
Wolkowitz control 2015 21 17 11 (59%) 34.9 (9.6) age, sex - +
Jacobs non- 4 2015 13 28 28 (100%) 58.0 (4.7) age, education, BMI + +

Hippocampus volume was determined using magnetic resonance imaging in all included studies. King et al. 22, Jacobs et al. 13, and Wolkowitz et al. 21 used FreeSurfer ( http://freesurfer.net/) for automated parcellation. Wikgren et al. 12 used manual tracing and did not specify whether the person performing the tracing was blind to other participant outcomes at the time. Grodstein et al. 20 did not specify how hippocampus volumes were determined. Covariates for analyses differed between studies ( Table 1). No study fractionated leukocytes nor analysed the relative contributions of different leukocyte subsets.

Correlation of leukocyte telomere length to hippocampus volume, quantitative synthesis

A random-effects meta-analysis of all 7 datasets yielded a summary estimate of r = 0.12 [95% CI -0.13, 0.37], p = 0.35, Figure 2, indicating a positive direction of the relationship between telomere length and hippocampus volume, but failing to show a significant difference from 0. A test for heterogeneity was significant ( τ 2 = 0.086, I 2 = 0.84, H 2 = 6.25, Q df=6 = 28.1, p < 0.0001), the Q-Q plot indicated deviation from normality, and the trim-and fill-method to test sensitivity for publication bias imputed two additional studies and yielded an estimate of r = 0.01 [95% CI -0.23, 0.25], p = 0.96, calling into question whether model assumptions were satisfied and suggesting that the effect estimate may be an overestimation due to publication bias. In spite of the small number of studies, we explored meta-regression as a means to address study heterogeneity, using mean age and fraction of female participants as independent variables. Neither was significantly associated (age: r 2 = 0.00, p = 1.00; sex: r 2 = 0.23, p = 0.14).

Figure 2. Forest plot.

Figure 2.

Diamonds at bottom show estimates for a random-effects model including all studies and an estimate when excluding one study as a sensitivity analysis.

The observed deviation from normality was mostly due to the 4/ 4-positive cohort from Wikgren 2012 12 ( Figure 3). In order to investigate the sensitivity of the meta-analysis to this particular study, the analysis was performed again without it. The meta-analytic estimate was now r = 0.23 [95% CI 0.05, 0.40], p = 0.01, without significant heterogeneity, although power to detect heterogeneity was now very limited ( τ 2 = 0.017, I 2 = 0.48, H 2 = 1.93, Q df=4 = 7.9, p = 0.09), and with adequate normality as judged by inspection of the Q-Q plot ( Figure 3). The trim-and fill-method to test sensitivity for publication bias imputed two additional studies and yielded an effect estimate of r = 0.12 [95% CI -0.07, 0.31], p = 0.22 ( Figure 3).

Figure 3. Funnel plots and Q-Q plots.

Figure 3.

Funnel plots show that the sample was right-skewed. Trim-and-fill analysis imputed two additional studies (unfilled circles) both in the full sample and in the reduced sensitivity analysis sample. Q-Q plots show that in the full sample, there was deviation from linearity due to one study. When this study was removed in the sensitivity analysis, linearity was acceptable.

Risk of bias was assessed qualitatively and by inspection of funnel plots and Q-Q plots. Two of the included studies reported results only for subsets of participants, without indicating that these sub-group analyses were specified on beforehand. Grodstein et al. 20 reported results only for healthy participants and participants with minimal cognitive impairment (MCI), leaving out 5 participants with dementia. Jacobs et al. 13 reported results only for APOE 4 non-carriers, leaving out 19 APOE 4 carriers. Only one study (Wolkowitz et al. 21) showed negative results. None of the studies reported blinding of experimenters carrying out laboratory nor statistical analyses. All studies except King et al. 22 reported fewer than 30 participants. With 30 participants and using the effect estimate of r = 0.12 found in the main analysis, power would be 0.10. The largest effect estimate in this paper, observed in the sensitivity analysis, was r = 0.23, for which 30 participants would yield a power of 0.23. The observed low power among the included studies is not remarkable compared to the neuroscience field in general 23. In combination with the preponderance of observed significant associations, it gives rise to a suspicion of bias. Funnel plots showed that the sample was right-skewed, although the small number of studies limits interpretation. The risk of bias was subjectively estimated to be moderate to high.

Correlation of leukocyte telomere length to hippocampus volume, qualitative synthesis

A qualitative synthesis takes as point of departure the large study by King et al. 22, since it contributes most of the participants (1960 of 2107) in this meta-analysis. This study was based on the Dallas Heart Study 2 cohort, a sample of 3401 participants examined in 2007–2009 and intended to be representative of the adult population of Dallas County, Texas. 2082 of these participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging and the final sample contained 1960 individuals. 48 brain regions were investigated using FreeSurfer. A positive direction of the association between leukocyte telomere length and brain volume was found for all 46 parenchymatous brain regions, whereas a negative direction of the association was found for the lateral and inferior lateral ventricles. Associations were significant at α = 0.05 in 27 regions after correcting for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate method. The strongest association was found for the precuneus ( r = 0.13), followed by the thalamus ( r = 0.11), and then the hippocampus, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, inferior parietal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, inferior temporal cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex (all r = 0.10). Leukocyte telomere length predicted both total cerebral volume ( r = 0.12) and total cortical volume ( r = 0.11). The relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume was moderated by age and was strongest in older participants. Authors reported that only marginal effects were seen when adjusting for hypertension, obesity, diabetes mellitus, and smoking status. The final model adjusted for age, sex, and ethnicity. Hippocampal and cortical volumes were found in an overlapping sample to correlate to cognitive performance using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment 24, supporting the validity of these volumetric measurements.

Demographic and clinical variables associated to leukocyte telomere length in the Dallas Heart Study 2 cohort have been reported in a previous paper including 3155 participants 25. With telomere length categorized into tertiles, there was a negative correlation to monocyte fraction (short tertile: 6.88% (SD 2.21), middle tertile: 6.75% (SD 2.10), long tertile: 6.53% (SD 2.03), p adj = 0.03), but not to other leukocyte fractions. Leukocyte telomere length also correlated positively to education and income, even when adjusting for age, gender and ethnicity. Since education and income were not analysed in King et al. 22, they may represent unknown confounders or effect mediators.

In summary, the association between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume found by King et al. 22 gains credence from the large sample and the population-based sampling strategy. The consistent association found across parenchymatous brain regions further supports the finding of an effect in the hippocampus. The previously reported finding that leukocyte subsets were mostly stable across telomere length tertiles in the Dallas Heart Study 2 is reassuring, in that it suggests that apparent differences in telomere length were not due to differences in cell type composition, although such confounding cannot be fully ruled out by this analysis.

The other included studies besides King et al. 22 all had samples for which results could be estimated of n < 30. While samples were small, reported effects were in the positive direction with the exception of one sub-group (from Wikgren et al. 12), and estimated effects were larger than in King et al. 22 with the exception of one further sub-group (from Wolkowitz et al. 21). This pattern of results is expected in the presence of bias.

Qualitatively, the overall pattern of results favors a positive association between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume.

Effect of APOE genotype

The effect of APOE genotype on the relationship between telomere length and hippocampus volume, first proposed by Wikgren et al. 12, has been subsequently investigated by King et al. 22 and by Jacobs et al. 13. Wikgren et al. 12 reported a different effect magnitude in two groups ( 3/ 3 homozygotes: r = -0.52; 4 carriers: r = 0.13), but not an interaction test which would tell whether that difference was statistically significant. In contrast, Jacobs et al. 13 reported a positive association ( r = 0.40) only among 4-noncarriers. Jacobs et al. 13 also did not test whether the difference between groups was statistically significant. King et al. 22 reported no significant effect of APOE 2, 3 nor 4 genotype in 1409 participants for whom APOE data were available. It was not possible to investigate these results quantitatively as effect sizes were not reported. In summary, none of the included studies provide positive evidence for a moderating effect of APOE genotype on the association between telomere length and hippocampus volume.

Leukocyte telomerase/telomere length ratio as predictor

Two studies with four datasets investigated leukocyte telomerase activity in addition to leukocyte telomere length as predictors for hippocampus volume. Jacobs et al. 13 proposed that the leukocyte telomerase/telomere length ratio is a better predictor for hippocampus volume than either measure alone, but without formally testing whether the ratio yielded a higher correlation. We decided to investigate this proposition. Since it was necessary to perform a test for dependent correlations, and the correlations between telomere length and telomerase activity were not given, we extracted data from scatterplots presented in both papers and calculated the required correlations from the estimated data. Comparisons by inspection of original plots and plots of estimated data showed satisfactory data extraction accuracy and can be viewed at 19. Pairwise estimates of hippocampus volume correlated well ( r’s = 0.99-1.00), further supporting successful data extraction.

In Jacobs et al. 13, telomere length and telomerase activity did not correlate (left: r = -0.19 [95% CI -0.19, 0.53], p = 0.32; right: r = -0.14 [95% CI -0.49, 0.25], p = 0.48; Note that while these measures have no intrinsic laterality, data were extracted separately for the right and left sides, and the difference between sides is likely due to estimation error). The ratio of telomerase activity to telomere length was a better predictor for hippocampus volume than telomerase activity alone for the right side but not the left. On neither side was the ratio a better predictor than telomere length ( Table 2). In Wolkowitz et al. 21, telomere length and telomerase activity did not correlate (healthy controls: r = 0.09 [95% CI -0.44, 0.58], p = 0.75; patients with major depressive disorder: r = 0.14 [95% CI -0.34, 0.56], p = 0.57). The ratio of telomerase activity to telomere length was not a better predictor than telomerase activity nor telomere length in either group ( Table 2). It is notable that correlations between telomerase activity and hippocampus volume differed in direction between the two studies ( Table 2).

Table 2. Comparisons of the ratio of telomerase to telomere length as predictor of hippocampus volume to either measure alone.

Data from Jacobs et al. 13 were estimated from published scatterplots showing only the APOE 4-negative participant subset, separately for right and left hippocampus, and are adjusted for age. Further adjustments for education and BMI were not possible because data were not available. Data from Wolkowitz et al. 21 were estimated from published scatterplots and are not adjusted for any covariates. Adjustments for age and sex were not possible because data were not available. TA = telomerase activity; TL = telomere length; HV = hippocampus volume; Ratio = ratio of telomere activity to telomere length; MDD = major depressive disorder; a = effect direction favors telomere length or activity over ratio.

Study and group n TL and HV
r [95% CI]
TA and HV
r [95% CI]
Ratio and HV
r [95% CI]
Ratio vs TA
p
Ratio vs TL
p
Jacobs left side 28 0.48 [0.13, 0.72] -0.33 [-0.62, 0.05] -0.37 [-0.65, -0.00] 0.17 0.59 a
Jacobs right side 28 0.56 [0.23, 0.77] -0.28 [-0.59, 0.11] -0.35 [-0.64, 0.03] 0.005 0.30 a
Wolkowitz MDD 19 0.07 [-0.40, 0.51] 0.58 [0.17, 0.82] -0.41 [-0.73, 0.05] 0.49 a 0.27
Wolkowitz control 16 0.09 [-0.44, 0.57] 0.18 [-0.36, 0.64] -0.00 [-0.51, 0.51] 0.56 a 0.81 a

Discussion

Main findings and interpretation

We investigated the correlation between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume. In the main analysis, we found a correlation of r = 0.12 with considerable uncertainty and not significantly different from 0. Studies were heterogeneous and the exclusion, as a sensitivity analysis, of one study (Wikgren et al. 12) shifted the estimate to r = 0.23, which was significantly different from 0. Trim-and-fill analyses to investigate publication bias yielded imputations which brought the estimated effect down to r = 0.01 in the full sample and r = 0.12 in the sample excluding one study, neither of which was significantly different from 0.

A meta-analysis does not necessarily yield better estimates than a single high-powered study, mainly because a high-powered study can be expected to have low bias, and if smaller studies with more bias are included in a meta-analysis, then the resulting estimate may be more biased and hence less accurate than that of the single study 26. Therefore, while this meta-analysis failed to corroborate a positive association, we argue that it does not provide compelling evidence to reject the positive estimate from the large study by King et al. 22, which contributed 1960 out of 2107 participants.

We found no evidence for APOE genotype as an effect moderator between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume. The ratio of telomerase activity to telomere length was a better predictor than telomerase activity on one side in Jacobs et al. 13 but not in either subsample in Wolkowitz et al. 21. We conclude that there is no evidence that APOE genotype moderates the relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume, while the evidence that the telomerase activity to telomere length ratio predicts hippocampus volume better than telomerase activity is inconclusive.

Limitations

An important limitation of this meta-analysis is that none of the included studies fractionated leukocytes before analysis, meaning that any change in the leukocyte cell type composition may confound results. Hippocampal atrophy has been linked to outcomes that are also associated to changes in leukocyte subset composition, such as inflammation and sleep pattern changes 27, 28. Therefore, leukocyte cell type composition is a possibly important and uninvestigated confounder.

Furthermore, this meta-analysis was limited by the small number of included studies, and by the relatively small sample sizes in all studies except one. Studies were heterogeneous and the risk of bias was subjectively judged to be moderate to high. Since none of the studies published their data in a format conducive to easy reuse, some analyses were precluded. Estimation of data from published scatterplots gives rise to measurement error, which, while probably small, could affect the results. Participant samples were predominantly middle-aged and all included more women than men, limiting external validity.

Potential mechanisms linking leukocyte telomere length to hippocampus volume

Different models have been proposed to explain the relationship between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume. King et al. 22 have proposed, as one possibility among others, that longer telomeres are a surrogate marker for higher telomerase activity, causing greater proliferative capacity and more cell proliferation ( Figure 4, model 1). Wikgren et al. 12, on the other hand, have proposed that longer telomeres indicate fewer cell divisions have taken place, consistent with fewer cells and a smaller tissue volume ( Figure 4, model 2). These two models make contradictory predictions, and if one accepts a positive correlation between leukocyte telomere volume and hippocampus volume, then a process according to model 2 cannot constitute a dominating contribution to the observed effect. The positive correlation between leukocyte telomerase activity and telomere length predicted by model 1 was however not demonstrated in the present datasets.

Figure 4. Two models proposed in earlier literature for relationships between telomere length and hippocampus volume.

Figure 4.

A missing link in models 1 and 2 is the relationship between circulating leukocytes and brain parenchyma, as regards telomere length and cell turnover. Leukocyte telomere length represents an average over a large number of white blood cells of different types 6, 29. While peripheral leukocytes are a heterogeneous cell population, they share a histogenetic origin in the hematopoietic stem and progenitor cells of the bone marrow. By contrast, neurons and macroglia (oligodendrocytes, and astrocytes) all derive from neuronal stem cells. Neurogenesis continues to a limited extent in adulthood, especially in the hippocampus and subventricular zone 7, 8, 30. Microglia, unlike neurons and macroglia, are derived from circulating mononuclear cells which originate in early fetal life from yolk sac hematopoietic stem cells, and later from mesodermally derived hematopoietic stem cells which in adults locate chiefly in the bone marrow 47. Whether these two classes of hematopoietic stem cells have the same or different origin is not known 31. There is controversy about the extent to which mononuclear cell transmigration and microglial differentiation continues in adult life. Studies using bone marrow transplantation in mice have found that 0 – 25% of microglia derived from the transplanted bone marrow after 4 – 15 weeks 3238. These findings have been interpreted as evidence both for and against a substantial contribution of bone marrow-derived cells to the microglial cell population in adulthood. In particular, it is not yet clear to what extent injury to the blood-brain barrier (induced, for example, by radiation) facilitates transmigration, and to what extent bone marrow-derived microglia remain in the brain after the resolution of local inflammation. To date, only one study has investigated the correlation between telomere length in leukocytes and in human postmortem brain tissue, and it found a moderate correlation of r = 0.42 in a study including 29 patients, all with Alzheimer’s disease 39.

Experiments using knock-out mouse models have shown that monocyte recruitment to the brain depends on monocyte chemoattractant protein-1/chemokine (C-C motif) ligand 2/small inducible cytokine A2 (MCP-1/CCL2) signalling trough the CCR2 receptor and that monocyte recruitment to the brain increases in peripheral inflammation 40. MCP-1 knock-out mice had less microglial recruitment to the hippocampus and more neurogenesis following cranial irradiation 41. Activated microglia can secrete neurotoxic factors including proinflammatory cytokines and reactive oxygen species (ROS), and microglial activation has been proposed as an important pathophysiological process in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s dementias 27, 42. Leukocyte telomere length has however not been conclusively linked to Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s dementias in humans 4345. Zhou et al. 46 found that telomerase inhibition caused impaired hippocampal neurogenesis, while overexpression of telomerase reverse transcriptase (TERT) led to increased hippocampal neurogenesis.

It is of course possible that an association between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume could arise from factors determining both outcomes independently ( Figure 5, model 3). King et al. 22 speculate that chronic inflammation may be an important determinant. Wolkowitz et al. 21 add that oxidative stress or endogenous hormonal regulation might influence telomerase activity and cell turnover in distant tissues such as leukocytes and brain. Lindqvist et al. 5 similarly point to inflammation, oxidative stress, and cortisol signalling as possible mechanisms. None of these possible determinants are however clearly supported by empirical data at the present time. Model 3 can be seen as a reformulation of reasoning from these aforementioned papers. Alternatively, we propose speculatively that microglia are a mechanistic link between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume ( Figure 5, model 4). According to this model, hippocampus volume is affected by microglial telomere length by processes related to microglial proliferation and activation. Possibly, longer telomeres could prevent pathological activation of microglia which causes neuronal damage. This model is highly tentative, but it does have the important advantage of providing a possible mechanism by which leukocyte telomere length can affect the cell turnover kinetics of neurons and macroglia, even though they have completely different histogenesis. One observation consistent with the proposed model is that monocytes were the only leukocyte subset correlated to leukocyte telomere length in the Dallas Heart 2 study, even though the effect was small 25. More research is needed to verify or falsify testable predictions arising from this model.

Figure 5. Two models proposed here for relationships between telomere length and hippocampus volume.

Figure 5.

Conclusions

The high-powered study by King et al. 22 estimated a positive correlation of r = 0.10 between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume. The present analysis does not provide compelling evidence against this finding, while it also does not provide absolute evidence in favor. The main meta-analytic effect estimate was r = 0.12, with estimates ranging from r = 0.00 to r = 0.23 after trim-and-fill imputation and in sensitivity analyses. We found no support for a moderating effect of APOE genotype and inconclusive evidence for using the telomerase activity/telomere length ratio as a predictor for hippocampus volume instead of telomere length.

More research in this area is needed to answer several outstanding questions. Differential analyses of white blood cells will be important in order to verify that apparent differences in leukocyte telomere length are not in fact differences in leukocyte cell type composition. Studies comparing telomere lengths in peripheral blood cells and in neurons, macroglia, and microglia will help elucidate the possible mechanistic link due to monocyte-microglial differentiation. Longitudinal designs will allow investigation of whether telomere shortening precedes brain atrophy, or vice versa. Further study of the relationships between leukocyte telomere length, brain volume, and environmental, physiological, and behavioral determinants such as inflammation, sleep and endocrine signalling will help elucidate possible mechanistic factors. Multivariate predictive modelling will be useful to investigate putative diagnostic and/or prognostic properties of telomere length measures. Studies with large samples and open publication of data are greatly to be desired.

Data availability

ZENODO: Hippocampus-volume-telomere-length: Release for publication, doi: 10.5281/zenodo.31771 19

Funding Statement

The authors declare that no grants were involved in supporting this work.

[version 1; referees: 2 approved]

References

  • 1. Blackburn EH: Telomeres and telomerase: their mechanisms of action and the effects of altering their functions. FEBS Lett. 2005;579(4):859–862. 10.1016/j.febslet.2004.11.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Chan SR, Blackburn EH: Telomeres and telomerase. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1441):109–121. 10.1098/rstb.2003.1370 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Artandi SE, DePinho RA: Telomeres and telomerase in cancer. Carcinogenesis. 2010;31(1):9–18. 10.1093/carcin/bgp268 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Fyhrquist F, Saijonmaa O, Strandberg T: The roles of senescence and telomere shortening in cardiovascular disease. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2013;10(5):274–283. 10.1038/nrcardio.2013.30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Lindqvist D, Epel ES, Mellon SH, et al. : Psychiatric disorders and leukocyte telomere length: Underlying mechanisms linking mental illness with cellular aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:333–364. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Prather AA, Gurfein B, Moran P, et al. : Tired telomeres: Poor global sleep quality, perceived stress, and telomere length in immune cell subsets in obese men and women. Brain Behav Immun. 2015;47:155–162. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Eriksson PS, Perfilieva E, Björk-Eriksson T, et al. : Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med. 1998;4(11):1313–1317. 10.1038/3305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Deng W, Aimone JB, Gage FH: New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(5):339–350. 10.1038/nrn2822 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, et al. : Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(8):4398–4403. 10.1073/pnas.070039597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Colgin LL, Moser EI, Moser MB: Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31(9):469–477. 10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, et al. : The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263–269. 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Wikgren M, Karlsson T, Lind J, et al. : Longer leukocyte telomere length is associated with smaller hippocampal volume among non-demented APOE ε3/ε3 subjects. PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e34292. 10.1371/journal.pone.0034292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Jacobs EG, Epel ES, Lin J, et al. : Relationship between leukocyte telomere length, telomerase activity, and hippocampal volume in early aging. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(7):921–923. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.870 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. R Core Team: R: A language and environment for statistical computing.2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Viechtbauer W: Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1–48. 10.18637/jss.v036.i03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Revelle W: psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, R package version 1.5.4.2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Champely S: pwr: Basic Functions for Power Analysis. R package version 1.1-2.2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Rohatgi A: WebPlotDigitizer.2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Nilsonne G: Hippocampus-volume-telomere-length: Release for publication.2015. Data Source [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 20. Grodstein F, van Oijen M, Irizarry MC, et al. : Shorter telomeres may mark early risk of dementia: preliminary analysis of 62 participants from the nurses’ health study. PLoS One. 2008;3(2):e1590. 10.1371/journal.pone.0001590 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Wolkowitz OM, Mellon SH, Lindqvist D, et al. : PBMC telomerase activity, but not leukocyte telomere length, correlates with hippocampal volume in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2015;232(1):58–64. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.01.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. King KS, Kozlitina J, Rosenberg RN, et al. : Effect of leukocyte telomere length on total and regional brain volumes in a large population-based cohort. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(10):1247–1254. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1926 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, et al. : Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(5):365–376. 10.1038/nrn3475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Gupta M, King KS, Srinivasa R, et al. : Association of 3.0-T brain magnetic resonance imaging biomarkers with cognitive function in the Dallas Heart Study. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(2):170–175. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.3418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Kozlitina J, Garcia CK: Red blood cell size is inversely associated with leukocyte telomere length in a large multi-ethnic population. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e51046. 10.1371/journal.pone.0051046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Nuijten MB, van Assen MALM, Veldkamp CLS, et al. : The replication paradox: combining studies can decrease accuracy of effect size estimates. Rev Gen Psychol. 2015;19(2):172–182. 10.1037/gpr0000034 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Mandrekar-Colucci S, Landreth GE: Microglia and inflammation in Alzheimer’s disease. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2010;9(2):156–167. 10.2174/187152710791012071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Mullington JM, Simpson NS, Meier-Ewert HK, et al. : Sleep loss and inflammation. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;24(5):775–784. 10.1016/j.beem.2010.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Armanios M, Blackburn EH: The telomere syndromes. Nature reviews Genetics. 2012;13(10):693–704. 10.1038/nrg3246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Lee SW, Clemenson GD, Gage FH: New neurons in an aged brain. Behav Brain Res. 2012;227(2):497–507. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Ueno H, Weissman IL: The origin and fate of yolk sac hematopoiesis: application of chimera analyses to developmental studies. Int J Dev Biol. 2010;54(6–7):1019–1031. 10.1387/ijdb.093039hu [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Priller J, Flügel A, Wehner T, et al. : Targeting gene-modified hematopoietic cells to the central nervous system: use of green fluorescent protein uncovers microglial engraftment. Nat Med. 2001;7(12):1356–1361. 10.1038/nm1201-1356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Mildner A, Schmidt H, Nitsche M, et al. : Microglia in the adult brain arise from Ly-6c hiccr2 + monocytes only under defined host conditions. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(12):1544–1553. 10.1038/nn2015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Ginhoux F, Greter M, Leboeuf M, et al. : Fate mapping analysis reveals that adult microglia derive from primitive macrophages. Science. 2010;330(6005):841–845. 10.1126/science.1194637 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Ajami B, Bennett JL, Krieger C, et al. : Infiltrating monocytes trigger EAE progression, but do not contribute to the resident microglia pool. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(9):1142–1149. 10.1038/nn.2887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Capotondo A, Milazzo R, Politi LS, et al. : Brain conditioning is instrumental for successful microglia reconstitution following hematopoietic stem cell transplantation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(37):15018–15023. 10.1073/pnas.1205858109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Burrell K, Hill RP, Zadeh G: High-resolution in-vivo analysis of normal brain response to cranial irradiation. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e38366. 10.1371/journal.pone.0038366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Okonogi N, Nakamura K, Suzuki Y, et al. : Cranial irradiation induces bone marrow-derived microglia in adult mouse brain tissue. J Radiat Res. 2014;55(4):713–719. 10.1093/jrr/rru015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Lukens JN, Van Deerlin V, Clark CM, et al. : Comparisons of telomere lengths in peripheral blood and cerebellum in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5(6):463–469. 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.05.666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. D’Mello C, Le T, Swain MG: Cerebral microglia recruit monocytes into the brain in response to tumor necrosis factoralpha signaling during peripheral organ inflammation. J Neurosc. 2009;29(7):2089–2102. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3567-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Lee SW, Haditsch U, Cord BJ, et al. : Absence of CCL2 is sufficient to restore hippocampal neurogenesis following cranial irradiation. Brain Behav Immun. 2013;30:33–44. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Lull ME, Block ML: Microglial activation and chronic neurodegeneration. Neurotherapeutics. 2010;7(4):354–365. 10.1016/j.nurt.2010.05.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Eitan E, Hutchison ER, Mattson MP: Telomere shortening in neurological disorders: an abundance of unanswered questions. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37(5):256–263. 10.1016/j.tins.2014.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Franco S, Blasco MA, Siedlak SL, et al. : Telomeres and telomerase in Alzheimer’s disease: epiphenomena or a new focus for therapeutic strategy? Alzheimers Dement. 2006;2(3):164–168. 10.1016/j.jalz.2006.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Thomas P, O’ Callaghan NJ, Fenech M: Telomere length in white blood cells, buccal cells and brain tissue and its variation with ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Mech Ageing Dev. 2008;129(4):183–190. 10.1016/j.mad.2007.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Zhou QG, Hu Y, Wu DL, et al. : Hippocampal telomerase is involved in the modulation of depressive behaviors. J Neurosci. 2011;31(34):12258–12269. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0805-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Rezaie P, Male D: Colonisation of the developing human brain and spinal cord by microglia: a review. Microsc Res Tech. 1999;45(6):359–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2015 Nov 24. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.7754.r10997

Referee response for version 1

Owen M Wolkowitz 1

This is a well done study with appropriate analyses.

It is timely and of interest.

It is limited (as appropriately acknowledged by authors) by the small number of studies and the large impact of one study (King).

There is a very good discussion of different theoretical models.

It would be worth also highlighting in the abstract their analyses of telomerase itself, not just the TL/telomerase ratio.

They present a secondary analysis without Wikgren. This was a post-hoc decision, but this is acknowledged, and the main findings of the meta-analysis include that study.

The only real issue:

  • There should have more discussion of the role of moderators (although this information was not available across all studies), e.g.,  age, sex, illness (medical and psychiatric), medications, education, SES, as well as MRI field strength, etc. Indeed, the authors reported that only marginal effects were seen when adjusting for hypertension, obesity, diabetes mellitus, and smoking status. This should be given more emphasis.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2015 Nov 2. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.7754.r10826

Referee response for version 1

Kevin S King 1

The current systematic review and meta-analysis of works evaluating the possible link between leukocyte telomere length and hippocampus volume is robust and thorough. It provides a useful quantitative analysis of the current research data and also evaluates potential theories on underlying mechanisms. My reviews are often more lengthy but I think this present work is comprehensive and I really do not think I have a lot to add. 

We were intrigued by existing studies but were worried about potential for bias in existing studies. A particular cause for concern was the small sample size of the studies and relatively modest size of the association being studied. This article gives concrete analysis to support this and determined the power of studies with a sample size of 30 to show an association was only 10%. As mentioned in this rather comprehensive work, some of the work stated that their findings were hypothesis generating and the associations tested were not stipulated at the outset but rather emerged in subgroup analysis. Thanks is still owed to these smaller studies for pioneering the ideas underlying this work. We were rather fortunate to have as a partner the Dallas Heart Study which made our work possible and are grateful for their support.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Nilsonne G: Hippocampus-volume-telomere-length: Release for publication.2015. Data Source [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Data Availability Statement

    ZENODO: Hippocampus-volume-telomere-length: Release for publication, doi: 10.5281/zenodo.31771 19


    Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

    RESOURCES