Skip to main content
Dose-Response logoLink to Dose-Response
. 2015 May 4;13(1):dose-response.14-030.Ulsh. doi: 10.2203/dose-response.14-030.Ulsh

Are Risks From Medical Imaging Still too Small to Be Observed or Nonexistent?

Brant A Ulsh 1,
PMCID: PMC4674180  PMID: 26673121

Abstract

Several radiation-related professional societies have concluded that carcinogenic risks associated with doses below 50-100 mSv are either too small to be detected, or are nonexistent. This is especially important in the context of doses from medical imaging. Radiation exposure to the public from medical imaging procedures is rising around the world, primarily due to increased utilization of computed tomography. Professional societies and advisory bodies consistently recommend against multiplying small doses by large populations to predict excess radiation-induced cancers, in large part because of the potential for sensational claims of health impacts which do not adequately take the associated uncertainties into account. Nonetheless, numerous articles have predicted thousands of future cancers as a result of CT scanning, and this has generated considerable concern among patients and parents. In addition, some authors claim that we now have direct epidemiological evidence of carcinogenic risks from medical imaging. This paper critically examines such claims, and concludes that the evidence cited does not provide direct evidence of low-dose carcinogenicity. These claims themselves have adverse public health impacts by frightening the public away from medically justified exams. It is time for the medical and scientific communities to be more assertive in responding to sensational claims of health risks.

Keywords: medical imaging, linear no-threshold hypothesis, radiation risk, atomic bomb survivors, computed tomography

INTRODUCTION

It has recently been claimed that we now have direct evidence of carcinogenic risks associated with medical imaging (Brenner and Hall 2012), and rarely a week goes by without the publication of a study [e.g. (Berrington de Gonzalez et al. 2009; Smith-Bindman et al. 2009; Miglioretti et al. 2013)] or newspaper article (Redberg and Smith-Bindman 2014) warning of thousands of future cancer cases caused by medical imaging. How much credence should patients, parents, and healthcare practitioners put in these claims?

Referring physicians are called upon to ensure that there is a clinical need for the information to be provided by any imaging exam involving exposure to ionizing radiation, (Brody et al. 2007; IAEA 2009; Rehani 2012; Esz 2013). In the international radiation protection framework, this process is formally known as “justification”. In order to make a sound justification decision, an accurate understanding of both the benefits and risks of the exam are required. There is little disagreement that medical imaging exams should be appropriately justified, and it is widely recognized that the benefits almost always outweigh any risks of imaging exams for medically indicated exams, except in the case of redundant exams (Valentin 2001; IAEA 2009). For example, repeat exams because of poor record-keeping or lack of ready access to patient records are difficult to justify. Similarly, imaging exams for the purpose of defensive medicine are not in the interest of the patient, and do not satisfy the justification requirement. The determination of when medical imaging exams involving ionizing radiation are justified, by balancing observed benefits versus projected risks [e.g. (Berrington de Gonzalez et al. 2008)], is directly dependent on how risks are estimated. If risks are overestimated, then access to medical imaging may be inappropriately denied or discouraged in patient populations where it could be of benefit. Conversely, if risks are underestimated, then medical imaging may expose patient populations to radiation when there is little diagnostic benefit.

Due to increased utilization primarily of computed tomography (CT), ionizing radiation exposure to the general population from medical diagnostic imaging procedures is on the rise in the United States (Wiest et al. 2002; Fazel et al. 2009; NCRP 2009; Sodickson et al. 2009; Larson et al. 2011), and around the world (Shrimpton et al. 2006; Aroua et al. 2010; Etard et al. 2010; Tabeie et al. 2013). Medical doses now represent the largest source of population dose in the United States. The collective medical dose to the public has been increasing broadly, but at varying rates across different sectors of the population, with the largest growth rate occurring among older patients (Smith-Bindman et al. 2008; Fazel et al. 2009; Smith-Bindman et al. 2012).

The increasing population dose, combined with widely publicized instances of accidental patient overexposures (Bogdanich 2009; Zarembo 2009) and unjustified exposures (Oikarinen et al. 2009), has generated concern among the public, regulatory authorities (Bogdanich 2010; FDA 2010), and in the scientific community (Brenner and Hall 2007; The Joint Commission 2011). Increasing doses from diagnostic medical imaging among pediatric patients (Brenner et al. 2001a; Larson et al. 2011) has generated special concern. Adding to the public alarm, articles have recently appeared in the peer-reviewed scientific literature purporting to directly observe increased cancer risks among populations exposed to low radiation doses, and several other articles have been published predicting excess cancer incidence from diagnostic imaging. There is, however, disagreement on how to estimate radiation risks, and how these estimates should be communicated to patients and the public. The purpose of this paper is to critically examine the studies most frequently cited as providing direct evidence of carcinogenicity associated with doses characteristic of diagnostic imaging, and to consider the most prudent expression of current knowledge of radiation risks associated with diagnostic imaging from a public health perspective.

WHAT ARE THE RISKS OF DIAGNOSTIC IMAGING EXAMS?

As previously discussed (Ulsh 2012), radio-epidemiologic studies traditionally start with the null hypothesis that there is no association between exposure to ionizing radiation and cancer risk (Rothman and Greenland 1998). Should the evidence prove sufficient to reject the null hypothesis in favor of an alternative hypothesis (e.g. radiation exposure increases the incidence of cancer in linear proportion to dose), the alternative is accepted in place of the null. If a particular study fails to observe a statistically significant change in cancer risk associated with radiation dose, the null stands i.e. the study fails to support the hypothesis that radiation exposure is causally related to cancer risk. In the event that a radio-epidemiology study fails to observe increases in cancer risk, it is critically important that the following mistakes in interpretation be avoided:

  1. It should not be concluded that there is absolutely no relationship between radiation exposure and cancer risk. It is well known that radiation is a weak carcinogen, and radio-epidemiology studies almost always struggle with the problem of lack of statistical power to detect a very small effect hypothetically associated with low radiation doses (Land 1980). Absence of evidence is not evidence of absence (UNSCEAR 2000; ICRP 2005).

  2. It should not be concluded, based on the preceding argument, that it is prudent to simply assume some undetectably small risk exists in the absence of evidence to support such an assumption. In particular, such an assumption violates logical principals (Walton 1999) and the scientific method (Popper 1959) by inappropriately shifting the burden of proof from the alternative hypothesis [e.g. low doses of radiation increase cancer risk in linear proportion to dose, all the way down to zero dose, as predicted by the linear, no-threshold (LNT) theory], to the null hypothesis (low doses of radiation have no effect on cancer risk). The prudence, or lack thereof, of such an assumption as a matter of radiation protection policy has no bearing on the success or failure of the scientific hypotheses predicted by the LNT theory.

The acceptance or rejection of the LNT theory is also not dependent on the presentation of other, putatively superior, alternative hypotheses (e.g. hormesis, linear threshold models). In other words, it is inappropriate to argue, for example, that since there is no demonstrably superior model (a very debatable argument in itself), we might as well continue to assume a LNT relationship. There is either statistically significant evidence of increases in cancer risk associated with low doses of radiation, or there is not, regardless of the success or failure of other alternative hypotheses. What radiation protection measures should be taken in the absence of evidence of increases in cancer risk from low radiation doses is an important and subjective question, and depends on many non-scientific factors including the public health and financial consequences of various courses of action. However, it is inappropriate for such subjective considerations to enter into the objective evaluation of the hypotheses predicted by the LNT as a scientific theory.

The linear, no-threshold model is recommended by many expert advisory bodies for setting radiation protection regulations (NCRP 2001; ICRP 2005; NRCNA 2005), but most of these same agencies explicitly recommend against using the LNT to predict risks for individual patients, or to predict future cancer increases from low doses multiplied by large populations. It has been argued that the LNT is the most suitable model for regulatory and public health policy applications (Martin 2005). Conversely it has also been argued that continued reliance on the LNT for radiation protection and risk assessment is unjustified (Cuttler 2009). Suffice it to say, there is no scientific consensus on this issue. In a 2002 random survey of American and European subscribers to the journal Science, a majority of the respondents believed a sublinear threshold model more accurately describes low-dose radiation risks than does the LNT (Jenkins-Smith et al. 2009), though a plurality favor radiation protection standards based on LNT (Silva et al. 2007). There remains significant debate about whether low doses in general (Brenner et al. 2003; Dauer et al. 2010; Ulsh 2010, 2012), and doses from diagnostic imaging in particular (Brenner and Elliston 2004; Brenner and Hall 2007; Scott et al. 2008; Jaffurs and Denny 2009; Laskey et al. 2010; Einstein 2012), increase cancer risk. There is considerable evidence of thresholds and adaptive/hormetic dose responses in radiation biological studies (Luckey 1980, 1991; Redpath 2006; Scott et al. 2008; Ait-Ali et al. 2010; Kuefner et al. 2010; Munley et al. 2011; Phan and Boreham 2011; Phan et al. 2012; Ma et al. 2013; Antosh et al. 2014). It has been specifically argued that this evidence implies that the risks associated with diagnostic imaging procedures are overestimated by the LNT (Redpath 2006).

Numerous professional societies and expert advisory bodies have recommended against quantitatively estimating risks from low doses of radiation (Table 1). For example, the Health Physics Society has concluded,

“In accordance with current knowledge of radiation health risks, the Health Physics Society recommends against quantitative estimation of health risks below an individual dose of 5 rem in one year or a lifetime dose of 10 rem above that received from natural sources… Estimation of health risk associated with radiation doses that are of similar magnitude as those received from natural sources should be strictly qualitative and encompass a range of hypothetical health outcomes, including the possibility of no adverse health effects at such low levels...below 5–10 rem (which includes occupational and environmental exposures), risks of health effects are either too small to be observed or are nonexistent” (HPS 2010).

Table 1.

Professional society and expert advisory body statements on risks of low radiation doses

Expert body/professional society Statement
American Association of Physicists in Medicine “Discussion of risks related to radiation dose from medical imaging procedures should be accompanied by acknowledgement of the benefits of the procedures. Risks of medical imaging at effective doses below 50 mSv for single procedures or 100 mSv for multiple procedures over short time periods are too low to be detectable and may be nonexistent. Predictions of hypothetical cancer incidence and deaths in patient populations exposed to such low doses are highly speculative and should be discouraged. These predictions are harmful because they lead to sensationalistic articles in the public media that cause some patients and parents to refuse medical imaging procedures, placing them at substantial risk by not receiving the clinical benefits of the prescribed procedures” (AAPM 2011).
Australasian Radiation Protection Society “There is insufficient epidemiological evidence to establish a dose-effect relationship for effective doses of less than a few tens of millisieverts in a year above the background level of exposure”.
“…no inference may be drawn concerning the risk to health or risk of fatality of an individual from an effective dose below 10 mSv in a year. For individual doses less than some tens of millisieverts in a year, risk inferences are unreliable and carry a large uncertainty that includes the possibility of zero risk”. (ARPS 2008)
Health Physics Society “In accordance with current knowledge of radiation health risks, the Health Physics Society recommends against quantitative estimation of health risks below an individual dose of 5 rem in one year or a lifetime dose of 10 rem above that received from natural sources. Doses from natural background radiation in the United States average about 0.3 rem per year. A dose of 5 rem will be accumulated in the first 17 years of life and about 25 rem in a lifetime of 80 years. Estimation of health risk associated with radiation doses that are of similar magnitude as those received from natural sources should be strictly qualitative and encompass a range of hypothetical health outcomes, including the possibility of no adverse health effects at such low levels”.
“There is substantial and convincing scientific evidence for health risks following highdose exposures. However, below 5–10 rem (which includes occupational and environmental exposures), risks of health effects are either too small to be observed or are nonexistent” (HPS 2010).
International Commission on Radiation Protection “Collective effective dose is an instrument for optimisation, for comparing radiological technologies and protection procedures. Collective effective dose is not intended as a tool for epidemiological studies, and it is inappropriate to use it in risk projections. This is because the assumptions implicit in the calculation of collective effective dose (e.g., when applying the LNT model) conceal large biological and statistical uncertainties. Specifically, the computation of cancer deaths based on collective effective doses involving trivial exposures to large populations is not reasonable and should be avoided. Such computations based on collective effective dose were never intended, are biologically and statistically very uncertain, presuppose a number of caveats that tend not to be repeated when estimates are quoted out of context, and are an incorrect use of this protection quantity” (ICRP 2007).
Society for Pediatric Radiology “To prevent misconceptions and public alarm, it is important to realize that the radiation used in CT scans has not been proven to cause cancer during a child’s lifetime. The very small risk of cancer from radiation exposure is an estimate and is based on information and statistics that are debatable” (SPR 2001).
United Nations Scientific Committee on the Effects of Atomic Radiation “In general, increases in the incidence of health effects in populations cannot be attributed reliably to chronic exposure to radiation at levels that are typical of the global average background levels of radiation. … the Scientific Committee does not recommend multiplying very low doses by large numbers of individuals to estimate numbers of radiation-induced health effects within a population exposed to incremental doses at levels equivalent to or lower than natural background levels” (UNSCEAR 2012)

Similarly, and more directly focused on doses from medical imaging, the American Association of Physicists in Medicine (AAPM) has stated,

“Risks of medical imaging at effective doses below 50 mSv for single procedures or 100 mSv for multiple procedures over short time periods are too low to be detectable and may be nonexistent. Predictions of hypothetical cancer incidence and deaths in patient populations exposed to such low doses are highly speculative and should be discouraged. These predictions are harmful because they lead to sensationalistic articles in the public media that cause some patients and parents to refuse medical imaging procedures, placing them at substantial risk by not receiving the clinical benefits of the prescribed procedures” (AAPM 2011).

Nonetheless, several review and editorial articles have been published claiming that we now have direct evidence of carcinogenic risk from doses characteristic of medical imaging.

The typical adult dose from a chest CT exam series is approximately 7 mGy, and the typical dose from a CT head series (Figure 1) is approximately 2 mGy (Mettler et al. 2008a). The corresponding doses for pediatric patients are approximately 3.0 mGy for a body CT, and 3.7 mGy for a head CT [values estimated from effective dose data for youngest and smallest patients shown in (Huda and Vance 2007)]. Is it true that “we have now passed a watershed in our field where it is no longer tenable to claim that CT risks are ““too low to be detectable and may be non-existent”” (Brenner and Hall 2012)? The studies cited in support of this assertion include the Japanese atomic bomb survivors, the Oxford Study of Childhood Cancer (OSCC), and new epidemiologic studies conducted in Great Britain and in Australia, each of which is considered below.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1.

A CT head scan.

LIFE-SPAN STUDY OF THE ATOMIC BOMB SURVIVORS

Some authors have claimed that the Life Span Study (LSS) of the Japanese atomic bomb survivors provides direct evidence of increased cancer risks in the range of doses typical of CT scans (Brenner et al. 2003; Hall 2003; Brenner and Hall 2012). Brenner and Hall (2012) cited the most recently published solid cancer incidence data (Preston et al. 2007) and asserted “about 30,000 atomic bomb survivors who were located several miles from the bomb epicenter did indeed receive organ doses comparable with those from a few CT scans and did show a significant increase in cancer risk”. Preston et al. (2007) stated,

“Of the 105,000 members of the LSS included in the current analyses, about 35,000 received doses between 5 and 200 mGy. This large group of low- to moderate-dose survivors provides adequate statistical power to make direct inferences about cancer risks at low doses.”

Elsewhere Brenner et al. (2003) pose the question, “What is the lowest dose of x- or γ-radiation for which convincing evidence of significantly elevated cancer risks in humans is available?” Do the atomic bomb survivor data really directly demonstrate significant excess risks in the range of typical CT exams? It is difficult to answer this question from Preston et al. (2007), because the authors never directly state the lowest dose where statistically significant increased risks are directly observed. They did state, “There was a statistically significant dose response in the range of 0–0.15 Gy”, and they reported “The data were consistent with a linear dose response over the 0- to 2-Gy range”. It is not entirely clear whether this can be interpreted to mean that the risks in the range of 0 to <0.15 Gy were not statistically significantly increased, but a visual inspection of their data (Figure 2) would appear to support such an interpretation. Although Preston et al. (2007) did not present confidence intervals on the individual ERR data points, their graph of the ERR for all solid cancer incidence reveals that several of the data points below 0.2 Gy have central estimate values that are approximately zero (Figure 2). If this interpretation is correct, and the ERR from zero to approximately <150 mGy are not statistically significantly elevated, it means that the LSS study does not support the assertion that even doses more than 20 times higher than an adult chest CT have been shown to be associated with increased cancer risks. This conclusion is consistent with assessments by the Health Physics Society (HPS 2010) and the American Association of Physicists in Medicine (AAPM 2011) (Table 1) which Drs. Brenner and Hall question (Hall 2003; Brenner and Hall 2012).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2.

Excess relative risk (ERR) for all solid cancer incidence in relation to radiation exposure among the atomic bomb survivors, 1958-1998. The thick solid line is the fitted linear gender-averaged ERR dose response at age 70 after exposure at age 30 based on data in the 0- to 2-Gy dose range. The points are non-parametric estimates of the ERR in dose categories. The thick dashed line is a nonparametric smooth of the category-specific estimates and the thin dashed lines are one standard error above and below this smooth. Figure reproduced from (Preston et al. 2007) with permission. Red oval added to emphasize low dose region discussed in text.

In support of the assertion that the LSS data show direct evidence of low dose risks, Hall (2002) presents a graph reportedly showing ERR for cancer mortality in A-bomb survivors from 1950-1990 over the range of 0 to approximately 150 mSv (his Figure 6). The point of Hall’s figure, as described in the text, was to show,

“radiation-induced cancer risk estimates are now available from individuals exposed over 50 years ago to doses comparable to those currently involved in helical CT. No theories, no extrapolations, no models are involved” (Hall 2002).

The latest LSS mortality data contradict Hall’s conclusion that this data directly demonstrate risks from low radiation doses. Figure 3 [which is reproduced from Figure 4 of (Ozasa et al. 2012b, 2012a)] shows the updated version of the mortality data considered by (Hall 2003). Four of the five lowest data points (over the same low dose range 0 to <150 mGy) have central estimates of excess relative risk slightly above zero and one has a central estimate below zero, but none of them are statistically significantly elevated. With no theories, no assumptions, and no extrapolations, cancer mortality risks from doses even more than 20 times higher than the typical adult chest CT exam are not statistically significantly elevated. This does not support the conclusion of Hall (2003) that the LSS data provide direct evidence of low-dose cancer risk. In fact, Ozasa et al. (2012b, 2012a) concluded “the estimated lowest dose range with a significant ERR for all solid cancer was 0 to 0.20 Gy”. This means that even doses almost 30 times higher than the dose received from a typical adult chest CT exam series and approximately 100 times higher than the dose from a typical adult head CT series did not carry statistically significantly increased solid cancer risks. On this basis, claims that the atomic bomb survivor data provide direct evidence of increased cancer risks from doses in the range of a typical single, or even several, CT exams are not supported.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3.

Excess relative risk (ERR) for all solid cancer mortality in relation to radiation exposure among the atomic bomb survivors, 1950-2003. The black circles represent ERR and 95% CI for the dose categories, together with trend estimates based on linear (L) with 95% CI (dotted lines) and linear-quadratic (LQ) models using the full dose range, and LQ model for the data restricted to dose <2 Gy. Figure reproduced from (Ozasa et al. 2012b) with permission. Red oval added to emphasize low dose region discussed in text.

But there is more to the story. The latest LSS incidence and mortality data are also consistent with a nonzero dose threshold. Preston et al. (2007) state that the best estimate of a threshold is 0.04 Gy with an upper 90% confidence interval of 0.085 Gy. They note that the linear-threshold model was not statistically significantly superior to a linear no-threshold model, however they did not discuss whether the converse is also true (i.e. a linear model is not statistically significantly superior to a linear-threshold model). If neither model is statistically significantly superior to the other, then a proper interpretation is that the data are consistent with both models and the data are insufficient to exclude one model in favor of the other. With regard to the mortality data, Ozasa et al. (2012b) concluded that “a formal dose-threshold analysis indicated no threshold; i.e., zero dose was the best estimate of the threshold”. So is this the piece of atomic bomb survivor evidence that shows significant risks from doses typical of CT exams? A close look at how Ozasa et al. (2012b) conducted their threshold analysis reveals that the answer is again, no. According to Doss (2012, 2013), the functional form used by Ozasa et al. (2012b) allowed only positive values, but one of the data points had a negative central estimate value, and eight of the ten lowest dose data points had 95% confidence intervals that included negative values (Figure 3). Alternative analyses that did not use a functional form, but instead used the data to determine the shape of the fit, demonstrated that a nonzero threshold could not, in fact, be excluded.

Even after six decades studying the atomic bomb survivors, other important questions remain. In particular, both the latest mortality (Ozasa et al. 2012b) and incidence (Preston et al. 2007) data continue to assume a constant neutron relative biological effectiveness (RBE) value of 10. However, there is strong indication that this value is significantly underestimated, perhaps by as much as a factor of 10, both in the epidemiological data (Kellerer et al. 2006; Walsh 2013), and in chromosome aberration data (Sasaki et al. 2006; Sasaki et al. 2008).

This is not a trivial issue, as the evidence suggests that cancer risks could be reduced by a factor of 0.7 if a more appropriate neutron RBE value were adopted (Sasaki et al. 2008). A constant neutron RBE of 10 is not consistent with the latest ICRP guidance on the subject (ICRP 2003b), and use of the RBE values recommended by ICRP would result in a significant decrease in the risk estimate for gamma radiation (Ruhm and Walsh 2007). In addition, chromosome aberration data suggests that the neutron RBE is higher at lower neutron doses. This would induce more curvature in the dose-response – in other words, more departure from linearity. There is also evidence of curvature in the cancer mortality data (Walsh et al. 2004a), and it appears to be increasing with time (Ozasa et al. 2012b). Intuitively, this might suggest that the degree of risk overestimation in the current LSS reports is even more significant at lower doses. Some caution is in order here, as these conclusions are based on publicly available grouped LSS data. Confidence in these conclusions would be strengthened if they were based on analyses of individual LSS data (Ruhm and Walsh 2007).

In light of the neutron RBE issue, the LSS’s use of the colon (a deep organ with a relatively high degree of neutron shielding by other tissues, compared to shallower organs) as a target organ for the combined all solid cancer category is especially problematic. In addition to the neutron RBE issue, there are very real differences in the etiology of cancers and radiosensitivity in different organs. For example, there is strong evidence of a relationship between high doses of radiation and cancers of the female breast, colon, esophagus, liver, lung, ovary, salivary gland, stomach, and urinary bladder, but there is no consistent evidence of a similar relationship for Hodgkin’s lymphoma, non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma, multiple myeloma or cancers of the gall bladder, larynx, male genitalia, oral cavity, pancreas, pharynx, prostate, skin (squamous cell carcinoma and malignant melanoma), small intestine, and uterus (Ulsh 2012). There is even evidence of differences between cancer types in the latest LSS cancer incidence data (Preston et al. 2007), where the reported excess relative risk values estimated at 1 Gy (assuming neutron RBE=10, and a LNT model) for cancers of the rectum, gallbladder, lung, non-melanoma skin, breast, uterus and “other” were significantly different than that reported for all solid cancers combined. Even when higher neutron RBE values are used, there are indications that there are differences between specific organ ERR estimates and the all solid cancer combined estimate, though the differences shrink as neutron RBE is increased (Walsh et al. 2004b). The obvious advantage of considering all solid cancers together is that it provides a larger number of cancer cases compared to any individual site, and therefore increases the putative statistical power to detect an effect. However, real differences in the types of cancers being combined would compromise the appropriateness of the all solid cancer combined category.

OXFORD STUDY OF CHILDHOOD CANCERS

Another study cited as providing direct evidence of increased carcinogenic risks in the range of doses typical of diagnostic imaging is the Oxford Study of Childhood Cancers (OSCC), which purported to have observed increased risks after cumulative doses as low as 10 mSv [(Doll and Wakeford 1997), but see also (Boice and Miller 1999)]. It has been claimed that, “the Oxford Study of Childhood Cancers was able to detect a significant increase in paediatric cancer risk for a mean dose of only 6 mGy” (Brenner 2014). In contrast to these plain, unreserved assertions, there is a great deal of debate about the OSCC. Some authors support the view that the OSCC provides evidence of increased carcinogenic risks from in utero exposures (Doll and Wakeford 1997), while others have raised serious questions about the study (Boice and Miller 1999; ICRP 2003a; Brent 2013).

The ICRP conducted a detailed analysis of the OSCC (ICRP 2003a). They made no claims that the OSCC provided direct and conclusive evidence of carcinogenic risks at very low doses. Indeed, they noted several weaknesses and limitations of the OSCC, including:

Selection biases

The OSCC studied only fatal cancers, while current standards of epidemiologic practice are to use incident cases so as to avoid possible biases from selection factors related to fatality. The ICRP noted “the potential for such biases probably increased with calendar time because treatment of childhood cancers, especially childhood leukaemia, has become more and more successful” (ICRP 2003a). The ICRP also noted the high losses of study subjects in the OSCC, and concluded “Study losses of this magnitude are generally considered to be unacceptable by present day standards for epidemiologic studies” (ICRP 2003a).

Information biases

The OSCC relied on interviews with mothers of pediatric study subjects conducted by doctors and nurses from local health departments to determine the number of x-ray exams subjects were exposed to in utero. In contrast, modern case-control studies use specially-trained interviewers. The interviewers were also most likely aware of the status of the interview subjects as cases or controls, which raises concerns about possible interviewer bias. There is evidence of this bias, as the ICRP stated, “In both of the OSCC methodological studies, the cases showed slightly more over-reporting and slightly less under-reporting of abdominal irradiation, which would produce a bias” (ICRP 2003a). There are serious questions about the accuracy of the interviewees’ memory, and the ICRP concluded “…there may be substantial inaccuracy in reporting, and there is a suggestion in the OSCC study that differential recall of x-ray procedures could have biased the results”, and “…only 66% of reported prenatal x rays were actually confirmed” (ICRP 2003a).

Uncertainties in dose estimates

There is significant uncertainty in the dose estimates used in the OSCC. Sources of this uncertainty include:

  • reliance on medical personnel’s memories when medical records did not indicate the number of films per x-ray exam (which was common pre-1960);

  • despite the fact that a woman may have had multiple x-ray exams throughout her pregnancy, details of only the first exam were considered in the dose-response analysis in early reports, and later reports are unclear on how this issue was treated;

  • fluoroscopic exams (which deliver very high doses relative to typical abdominal film x-rays) were not included in dose estimates;

  • doses from non-abdominal x-rays were not included, in spite of the fact that these could deliver significant abdominal doses because the ovary was in the direct beam in over 50% of chest x-rays.

On the issue of uncertainty in dose estimates, the ICRP concluded,

“…a major limitation of this dataset is the uncertainties about in-utero doses, as discussed in Chapter 3. Dose uncertainties have not been taken into account adequately by any investigator, and it seems unlikely that sufficient historical dosimetric data are available to characterise uncertainties or achieve more accurate dose estimates” (ICRP 2003a).

Comparison with other studies

Other studies of pediatric populations exposed to very low doses failed to observe increased risks (Hammer et al. 2009; Hammer et al. 2011). A systematic review of numerous other studies failed to find any associations (Schulze-Rath et al. 2008). Similarly, reviews of epidemiological studies of adult and pediatric populations exposed to medical diagnostic or therapeutic procedures found inconsistent results, with some studies reporting associations between number of procedures and subsequent cancer, and some studies failing to observe such associations (Ron 2003). Specific studies of pediatric populations more frequently reported positive dose-responses (Ron 2002). All of these studies examined planar and projection radiography studies, and while earlier protocols tended to result in higher doses relative to modern protocols, in general they all deliver significantly lower doses than CT exams. Another point that must be remembered is that these are studies of populations with underlying medical conditions, and the impact of these conditions on risk estimates must be considered before generalizing to the population at large.

The ICRP also noted a discrepancy between case-control studies (like the OSCC), which are subject to recall bias and some of which report significant increases in risk, and cohort studies which are not subject to recall bias, and which have consistently failed to observe increased risks. On this subject, the ICRP stated, “…most of the case–control studies in Table 8.5 had the potential for recall bias just as the OSCC study did” and “One notable feature of the data in Table 8.6 is the low RRs for cohort studies of in-utero diagnostic radiation effects” (ICRP 2003a). The ICRP noted that while the confidence intervals of risk estimates from cohort studies are wide enough to be compatible with those of the OSCC, “the cohort studies provide limited evidence at best for increased risk associated with in-utero radiation exposure” (ICRP 2003a). Brent noted that of 35 case-control studies studying childhood cancer risks in children of women who received prenatal diagnostic x-ray exams, 25 of them failed to observe statistically significantly increased relative risks, and none of the 17 cohort studies of similar populations observed statistically significantly increased relative risks (Brent 2013).

Similarly, the ICRP compared the risk estimates of the OSCC with the results from the LSS studies, and noted, “Although the observed number of cancer deaths from in-utero irradiation and the number expected from the OSCC-based risk estimate may be marginally statistically compatible, the support for the OSCC-based risk projection is weak at best” (ICRP 2003a). They also stated, “By way of contrast, the in-utero risk estimate calculated from collective dose and person-years for the Japanese atomic bomb cohort (Section 8.8.4) yielded a negative risk estimate that is statistically incompatible with a positive association” (ICRP 2003a). It should be noted that since the ICRP published their analysis in 2003, a study of the solid cancer incidence in atomic bomb survivors exposed in utero has been published (Preston et al. 2008). That study concluded that in contrast to the OSCC, “The absolute risks among those [atomic bomb survivors] exposed in utero are therefore likely to be considerably lower than simple projections based on studies of childhood cancers in other in utero–exposed populations (which have been estimated to be approximately 6% per Sv by age 15 [in the OSCC]) and may be lower than absolute risks among those exposed early in life” (explanatory text in italics added). (Preston et al. 2008) did caution, “additional follow-up of this cohort is necessary before definitive conclusions can be made about the nature of the risks for those exposed in utero.”

In contrast to other studies, the OSCC observed similar risks for hematopoietic cancers and for solid cancers, and it has been suggested that this may be a sign of bias (Boice and Miller 1999). The ICRP agreed, concluding, “Although the arguments fall short of being definitive because of the combination of biological and statistical uncertainties involved, they raise a serious question of whether the great consistency in elevated RRs, including embryonal tumours and lymphomas, may be due to biases in the OSCC study rather than a causal association”(ICRP 2003a).

In summary, the ICRP concluded, “there are several methodological weaknesses in the OSCC dataset” (ICRP 2003a). They stated that these weaknesses do not completely invalidate the study, but they may have a quantitative impact on the results. In light of these weaknesses and limitations, assertions that the OSCC provides incontrovertible direct evidence of carcinogenic risks from doses typical of medical imaging are overstated.

DO NEW EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDIES DIRECTLY OBSERVE RISKS?

It has been claimed that a recent study conducted in the UK (Pearce et al. 2012) is “the first epidemiological study to show an excess incidence of leukaemia and brain cancer in children and adolescents who had received CT scans”, and “the new data confirm that the cancer risk associated with the radiation from a CT scan is very small, but not zero” (Hall and Brenner 2012). However, significant limitations of the (Pearce et al. 2012) study have been identified (Cohen 2013), including (1) individual doses were not directly assessed, but rather “typical” doses were assumed; (2) doses applied were for adults and assumed no decrease for pediatric patients, even though this is the standard of care; (3) the reason for the CT was not considered, and it is possible that the underlying condition indicating the CT has associated cancer susceptibility. One of the strongest associations (Pearce et al. 2012) observed was for gliomas, but they did not control for prior head injury. Head injuries are a common reason for head CT in children (Maguire et al. 2009), and head injury may be associated with brain tumors. Brenner and Hall cited a single study (Nygren et al. 2001) to assert that there were no obvious confounders for brain tumors. The single study Brenner and Hall cited failed to observe an association between prior traumatic head injury and primary brain tumors among more than 311,000 Swedish patients. But the possibility of confounding cannot be dismissed on the basis of one study. There is a very real possibility of reverse causation (i.e. an underlying medical condition associated with brain cancer may be the reason for the scan)(UNSCEAR 2013). The epidemiological evidence of an association between head injury and subsequent brain tumors is mixed (Inskip et al. 1995) with some studies reporting an association (Hochberg et al. 1984; Preston-Martin et al. 1989; Hu et al. 1998; Preston-Martin et al. 1998; Monteiro et al. 2006; Chen et al. 2012) and some not (Parker and Kernohan 1931; Choi et al. 1970; Annegers et al. 1979; Carpenter et al. 1987; Schlehofer et al. 1992; Zampieri et al. 1994; Inskip et al. 1998; Wrensch et al. 2000; Rutherford and Wlodarczyk 2009). But formal criteria for establishing causality between brain tumors and prior traumatic injury have been published (Salvati et al. 2004), and there are many confirmed case reports in the literature of gliomas forming at the exact location of a prior brain injury, often verified by CT or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)(Schmitt 1983; Troost and Tulleken 1984; Di Trapani et al. 1996; Sabel et al. 1999; Henry and Rajshekhar 2000; Magnavita et al. 2003; Moorthy and Rajshekhar 2004; Salvati et al. 2004; Anselmi et al. 2006; Zhou and Liu 2010). This partial list of references is by no means meant to be exhaustive – it is simply intended to demonstrate that there are numerous examples of confirmed case reports relating gliomas to prior trauma. There are also several case reports between previous trauma and subsequent development of meningioma (Sakai et al. 1981; Rodrigues et al. 2006; Francois et al. 2010; Kizilay et al. 2010), which is another of the primary brain tumor types observed by (Pearce et al. 2012). There is epidemiological evidence of this relationship as well, (Preston-Martin et al. 1998), including dose response relationships (Phillips et al. 2002). Past epidemiological evidence in large part shows an association between radiation exposure and brain tumors only after high doses characteristic of radiotherapy (UNSCEAR 2006; Mettler and Upton 2008). No consistent association has been evident at the low doses typical of modern diagnostic imaging. The possibility that the relationships observed by (Pearce et al. 2012) are a result of underlying medical conditions cannot be discounted in the absence of proper controls. For example, where CT scans were given to persons with head injuries, matching controls would have been persons with head injuries that did not receive CT scans. In light of this limitation, and the others described by (Cohen 2013), citation of this study in support of projections of increased numbers of cancer among the general population (Miglioretti et al. 2013), and as definitive vindication of prior controversial claims of cancer risks associated with CT (Brenner and Hall 2012) is unconvincing.

Similarly, the (Mathews et al. 2013) study has been characterized as “compelling data on the magnitude of the cancer risk attributable to ionizing radiation” from CT scans (Sodickson 2013). However several limitations in the Mathews et al. (2013) study have been identified (Walsh et al. 2013), including: (1) the misclassification of patients with unknown exposures at the start of the study as unexposed, which the authors assert would have a small impact, but no supporting evidence for this assertion was provided; (2) omission of doses from missed exams; (3) omission of doses from retakes. There are several puzzling results, including (1) dissimilarity in leukemia patters compared to the A-bomb survivors; (2) excess risks for all solid cancers except brain cancer in the first four years after exposure; (3) significant excesses of melanoma and Hodgkin’s lymphoma, which is at odds with other more powerful studies; and (4) the lack of any excess breast cancer.

UNSCEAR recently reviewed the epidemiologic evidence on the sensitivity of pediatric subjects relative to adults (UNSCEAR 2013), and concluded:

  • For 25% of the cancer types, children appear to be more sensitive than adults;

  • Children appear to have the same radiosensitivity as adults for 15% of cancer types;

  • For 10% of the cancer types, children appear to be less sensitive than adults;

  • For 20% of cancer types, no conclusion can be drawn about the sensitivity of children relative to adults because the evidence is too weak; and

  • For about 30% of cancer types there is only a weak relationship or no relationship at all to radiation exposure.

NEW ARTICLES – PREDICTING EXCESS CANCER INCIDENCE

It is obvious that the advice of numerous professional societies and expert advisory bodies against multiplying very low doses by large populations to estimate radiation-induced health effects using the LNT (Table 1) is frequently contravened even in the scientific literature (Pauwels and Bourguignon 2011). Much of the public alarm regarding doses from diagnostic imaging procedures has been the result of newspaper articles and articles in the scientific literature predicting excess radiation-induced cancers from these procedures. These studies are frequently based on LNT calculations of risk. There are numerous examples of these kinds of articles generating the sensational claims the AAPM warned against.

An article in the January 30, 2014 edition of the New York Times was alarmingly titled, “We Are Giving Ourselves Cancer”, and was accompanied by a provocative cartoon of a doctor holding an x-ray film, and wearing a gas mask and military helmet (Redberg and Smith-Bindman 2014). The authors only mentioned in passing that medical imaging saves lives, and quickly moved on to the many sensational claims asserting that CT scans are killing patients that formed the main message of the article. Readers were provided no balancing perspective, and no indication that the claims in this article were far outside the scientific mainstream.

Another well-known example was a paper appearing in the American Journal of Roentgenology (AJR) in 2001 which predicted an increase in cancer from CT scans in pediatric patients (Brenner et al. 2001a). This paper garnered an enormous amount of attention in the popular press, including an article in USA Today (Sternberg 2001), which generated public alarm about the doses and putative risks associated with imaging. The AJR paper multiplied the low estimated doses from CT scans in pediatric patients by population CT utilization data and risk estimates derived from the Japanese atomic bomb survivors to predict an excess of cancer deaths in US children. In this instance, even though the original AJR paper discussed uncertainties associated with the excess cancer predictions, the USA Today article omitted any mention of uncertainties, and stated “Each year, about 1.6 million children in the USA get CT scans to the head and abdomen — and about 1,500 of those will die later in life of radiation-induced cancer” (Sternberg 2001). The USA Today article was severely criticized in the scientific literature as grossly sensational and a misrepresentation of the AJR paper (Donnelly and Frush 2001; Sane 2001). However the authors of the AJR paper have described the press coverage (including the USA Today article) as “quite responsible” (Brenner et al. 2001b) and “reasonably balanced reporting” (Brenner et al. 2001c). The Society of Pediatric Radiology (SPR) apparently disagreed, stating, “…the SPR considers the recent press coverage to be unbalanced and potentially dangerous to the health and welfare of children” (SPR 2001). The SPR also stated,

“To prevent misconceptions and public alarm, it is important to realize that the radiation used in CT scans has not been proven to cause cancer during a child’s lifetime. The very small risk of cancer from radiation exposure is an estimate and is based on information and statistics that are debatable” (SPR 2001).

The SPR’s position has also been explicitly criticized (Brenner et al. 2001c). Brenner et al. (2001c) asserted “small individual cancer risk must be multiplied by a large (and increasing) population of children undergoing CT examination…” to gain a public health perspective, and further, “even a very small individual radiation risk, when multiplied by such a large (and increasing) number of children, is likely to produce a significant long-term public-health issue.” These statements are in direct contradiction to the advice offered by the The American Association of Physicists in Medicine, the Australasian Radiation Protection Society, the French Academies of Science and Medicine, the Health Physics Society, the ICRP, the International Organization for Medical Physics, the Society for Pediatric Radiology, and UNSCEAR, (Table 1). Such predictions frighten the public away from medically indicated exams (Boutis et al. 2013; Fletcher et al. 2013), and on that basis are damaging to public health. Almost 35 years ago, Lauriston Taylor, past president of the Health Physics Society and the first Chairman of the National Council on Radiation Protection & Measurements (NCRP), condemned this practice, stating:

“An equally mischievous use of the numbers game is that of calculating the number of people who will die as a result of having been subjected to diagnostic X-ray procedures. An example of such calculations are those based on a literal application of the linear, non-threshold, dose-effect relationship, treating the concept as a fact rather than a theory. ... These are deeply immoral uses of our scientific knowledge.” (Taylor 1980)

In yet another example, another study used risk estimates provided by the Biological Effects of Ionizing Radiation (BEIR) VII Committee (NRCNA 2005), combined with preliminary population CT utilization data used by the NCRP (Mettler et al. 2008b), to project an additional 29,000 cancers in the US population per year (Berrington de Gonzalez et al. 2009). A similar, frequently cited study examined the frequency of 11 common CT exams among 1119 patients at San Francisco area medical facilities, estimated the effective doses of the exams, multiplied by BEIR VII risk coefficients, and concluded, “An estimated 1 in 270 women who underwent CT coronary angiography at age 40 years will develop cancer from that CT scan (1 in 600 men)” (Smith-Bindman et al. 2009). A more cautiously worded study of CT coronary angiography calculated radiation risks from this procedure, but also discussed limitations (including the application LNT as recommended by BEIR VII) at some length (Einstein et al. 2007). Another recent study has predicted that the 4 million pediatric CT scans performed in the US each year will eventually result in 4870 future cancers (Miglioretti et al. 2013). Similarly, it has been predicted that radiogenic cancers from diagnostic imaging will account for 0.6 to 3% of total cancers in 15 countries around the world (Berrington de Gonzalez and Darby 2004). Excess cancers in specific populations at a sub-national level have also been predicted using similar methodologies, and the predicted excesses are usually in the range of a few percent of total expected cancers (Sodickson et al. 2009; Dijkstra et al. 2010; Khong et al. 2013).

These projections calculate theoretical, rather than empirically observed, risks (NRCNA 2012) and such estimates have been criticized on a number of bases, including inappropriate averaging of doses received by a segment of the population (e.g. medical patients receiving CT) over the entire population (McCollough et al. 2009; Pauwels and Bourguignon 2011). In doing so, they assume that the population being scanned has the same risk profile as the population at large. There is evidence that this is incorrect, and in fact, the fraction of the population undergoing, for example CT scans, is at significantly higher risk of death, presumably from their underlying medical conditions (Zondervan et al. 2011). Failure to take this into account can result in significant overestimates of radiation risk (Brenner et al. 2011). In addition, these studies have been criticized for frequently using effective dose (designed for radiological protection purposes) for risk estimation (for which effective dose is inappropriate), and for applying risks calculated for Japanese atomic bomb survivors to an American public exposed to fractionated medical exposures, often with underlying medical conditions, using the LNT hypothesis (Hendee and O’Connor 2012).

There are real public health consequences of these sensational predictions. A recent study observed that when parents of children presenting with isolated head injuries in an emergency department were advised that a CT scan was indicated, but may increase their child’s risk of cancer by 1 in 10,000 [based on (Pearce et al. 2012)], willingness to proceed with the scan was reduced by over 20% (Boutis et al. 2013). There is also anecdotal evidence from professionals in the imaging field that articles in the popular press about radiation risks from medical imaging are generating concern among patients (Esz 2013; Fletcher et al. 2013).

REAL BENEFITS VS. HYPOTHETICAL RISKS

It is important to clarify that none of the analyses provided in this paper prove an absolute absence of risks from CT exams. Absence of evidence is not the same as evidence of absence. However absence of evidence certainly should not be interpreted as evidence of increased risks either (Ulsh 2012). To be consistent with the logical underpinnings of the scientific method, the burden of proof must be born by those asserting an effect (Walton 1999; Ulsh 2012). It is frequently argued that it is prudent to assume some small risk from low radiation doses, even if it cannot be directly demonstrated (ICRP 2007). This argument would only be compelling if there was no economic, or more importantly, public health cost of assuming an undetectable risk. This is certainly not the case for medical imaging. Assuming a risk where none can be demonstrated frightens patients and parents away from medically indicated exams (Boutis et al. 2013), and distorts the justification process. This entails real public health costs.

So what are physicians to do? What advice should they offer their patients on diagnostic medical imaging? The following recommendations have been offered for ordering physicians (Huda 2009):

Only tests that will affect patient management should be ordered

This case can be made on the basis of most productively allocating limited resources – there is no need to resort to exaggerated estimates of risks which distort the justification process.

Non-ionizing imaging modalities should be considered, especially for pediatric patients

This just makes sense in the interest of prudence, as long as the same information can be obtained with non-ionizing modalities (Muratore 2012). However, a consideration of the very real risks of non-ionizing imaging modalities (e.g. sedation, often required for MRI in young children) must be a part of this decision (Brody and Guillerman 2014).

Tests should only be ordered where there are potential patient benefits

Again, this case can be made on the basis of most productively allocating limited resources.

Optimize radiation doses to be as low as reasonably achievable while maintaining diagnostic value

As explained by McCollough, “It’s analogous to taking an aspirin. Taking a whole bottle all at once is risky, but taking two pills isn’t. Still, if only one pill will get rid of my headache, I’ll use just one. The radiation exposure from a CT scan isn’t dangerous, but if we can use an even lower amount, without compromising the exam, that’s what we’ll do” (Mayo Clinic 2013). However, we must also be mindful of sacrificing diagnostic utility in the interest of reducing doses for which the risk is already too small to detect or nonexistent. As pointed out by (Brody and Guillerman 2014), dose minimization is not the same as diagnostic optimization, and the two can conflict.

The scientific and medical communities have too often been caught flat-footed by sensational claims of future cancers which frighten the public. As Sir Winston Churchill once famously said, “A lie gets halfway around the world before the truth has a chance to get its pants on”. The time for timid and heavily caveated responses to these dangerous claims is well past. The message we should be sending to the public is that while trivial risks cannot be absolutely excluded, the best evidence we have fails to indicate any significant risks from the low doses associated with medical imaging (Ulsh 2010). Any risks that are so small that we cannot detect them are hypothetical, but the benefits of medical imaging are real and substantial. It is time to speak out forcefully and directly about the safety and real benefits of diagnostic medical imaging.

REFERENCES

  1. AAPM. 2011. AAPM position statement on radiation risks from medical imaging procedures. Available at http://www.aapm.org/org/policies/details.asp?id=318&type=PP&current=true
  2. Ait-Ali L, Andreassi MG, Foffa I, Spadoni I, Vano E, Picano E. 2010. Cumulative patient effective dose and acute radiation-induced chromosomal DNA damage in children with congenital heart disease. Heart 96:269–274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Annegers JF, Laws ER, Kurland LT, Grabow JD. 1979. Head trauma and subsequent brain-tumors. Neurosurgery 4:203–206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anselmi E, Vallisa D, Berte R, Vanzo C, Cavanna L. 2006. Post-traumatic glioma: Report of two cases. Tumori 92:175–177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Antosh M, Fox D, Hasselbacher T, Lanou R, Neretti N, Cooper LN. 2014. Drosophila melanogaster show a threshold effect in response to radiation. Dose-Response pre-press: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aroua A, Aubert B, Back C, Biernaux M, Einarsson G, Frank A, Friberg EG, Griebel J, Hart D, Jarvinen H, Leitz W, Maru K, Nekolla EA, Olerud HM, Tenkanen-Rautakoski P, Trueb P, Valero M, de Waard I, Waltenberg H, Ziliukas J. 2010. Collective doses from medical exposures: an inter-comparison of the “TOP 20” radiological examinations based on the EC guidelines RP 154, in: Third European IRPA Congress, Radiation and Nuclear Safety Authority, Helsinki, FIN, pp. 229–238 [Google Scholar]
  7. ARPS. 2008. Low dose radiation. Available at http://www.arps.org.au/?q=content/low-dose-radiation
  8. Berrington de Gonzalez A, Darby S. 2004. Risk of cancer from diagnostic X-rays: estimates for the UK and 14 other countries. Lancet 363:345–351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berrington de Gonzalez A, Kim KP, Berg CD. 2008. Low-dose lung computed tomography screening before age 55: estimates of the mortality reduction required to outweigh the radiation-induced cancer risk. J Med Screening 15:153–158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berrington de Gonzalez A, Mahesh M, Kim KP, Bhargavan M, Lewis R, Mettler F, Land C. 2009. Projected cancer risks from computed tomographic scans performed in the United States in 2007. Arch Intern Med 169:2071–2077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bogdanich W. 2009. Radiation overdoses point up dangers of CT scans, in: New York Times, New York Times, New York, NY, pp. A13 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bogdanich W. 2010. At hearing on radiation, calls for better oversight, in: New York Times, New York Times, New York, NY, pp. A12 [Google Scholar]
  13. Boice JD, Miller RW. 1999. Childhood and adult cancer after intrauterine exposure to ionizing radiation. Teratology 59:227–233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Boutis K, Cogollo W, Fischer J, Freedman SB, Ben David G, Thomas KE. 2013. Parental knowledge of potential cancer risks from exposure to computed tomography. Pediatrics [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brenner DJ. 2014. What we know and what we don’t know about cancer risks associated with radiation doses from radiological imaging. Br J Radiol [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brenner DJ, Doll R, Goodhead DT, Hall EJ, Land CE, Little JB, Lubin JH, Preston DL, Preston RJ, Puskin JS, Ron E, Sachs RK, Samet JM, Setlow RB, Zaider M. 2003. Cancer risks attributable to low doses of ionizing radiation: assessing what we really know. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:13761–13766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brenner DJ, Elliston CD. 2004. Estimated radiation risks potentially associated with full-body CT screening. Radiology 232:735–738 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brenner DJ, Elliston CD, Hall EJ, Berdon WE. 2001a. Estimated risks of radiation-induced fatal cancer from pediatric CT. Am J Roentgenol 176:289–296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brenner DJ, Elliston CD, Hall EJ, Berdon WE. 2001b. Estimates of the cancer risks from pediatric CT radiation are not merely theoretical: comment on “point/counterpoint: in x-ray computed tomography, technique factors should be selected appropriate to patient size. Against the proposition”. Med Phys 28:2387–2388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brenner DJ, Elliston CD, Hall EJ, Berdon WE. 2001c. Response to the statement by The Society for Pediatric Radiology on radiation risks from pediatric CT scans. Pediatr Radiol 31:389–391 [Google Scholar]
  21. Brenner DJ, Hall EJ. 2007. Computed tomography—an increasing source of radiation exposure. N Engl J Med 357:2277–2284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Brenner DJ, Hall EJ. 2012. Cancer risks from CT scans: now we have data, what next? Radiology 265:330–331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Brenner DJ, Shuryak I, Einstein AJ. 2011. Impact of reduced patient life expectancy on potential cancer risks from radiologic imaging. Radiology 261:193–198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Brent RL. 2013. Carcinogenic risks of prenatal ionizing radiation. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Brody AS, Frush DP, Huda W, Brent RL. American Academy of Pediatrics Section on R 2007. Radiation risk to children from computed tomography. Pediatrics 120:677–682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Brody AS, Guillerman RP. 2014. Don’t let radiation scare trump patient care: 10 ways you can harm your patients by fear of radiation-induced cancer from diagnostic imaging. Thorax [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Carpenter AV, Flanders WD, Frome EL, Cole P, Fry SA. 1987. Brain cancer and nonoccupational risk-factors - a case-control study among workers at 2 nuclear-facilities. American Journal of Public Health 77:1180–1182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Chen YH, Keller JJ, Kang JH, Lin HC. 2012. Association between traumatic brain injury and the subsequent risk of brain cancer. Journal of Neurotrauma 29:1328–1333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Choi NW, Schuman LM, Gullen WH. 1970. Epidemiology of primary central nervous system neoplasma .2. case-control study. American Journal of Epidemiology 91:467–485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cohen M. 2013. Cancer risks from CT radiation: is there a dose threshold? J Am Coll Radiol 10:817–819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cuttler JM. 2009. Commentary on using LNT for radiation protection and risk assessment. Dose-Response 8:378–383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dauer LT, Brooks AL, Hoel DG, Morgan WF, Stram D, Tran P. 2010. Review and evaluation of updated research on the health effects associated with low-dose ionising radiation. Radiat Prot Dosimetry 140:103–136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Di Trapani G, Carnevale A, Scerrati M, Colosimo C, Vaccario ML, Mei D. 1996. Post-traumatic malignant glioma. Report of a case. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences 17:283–286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dijkstra H, Groen J, Bongaerts F, Van der Jagt E, Greuter M. 2010. Radiation-induced cancer risk from recurrent CT scanning, in: Third European IRPA Congress, Radiation and Nuclear Safety Authority, Finland, Helsinki, FIN, pp. 275–283 [Google Scholar]
  35. Doll R, Wakeford R. 1997. Risk of childhood cancer from fetal irradiation. Br J Radiol 70:130–139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Donnelly LF, Frush DP. 2001. Fallout from recent articles on radiation dose and pediatric CT. Pediatr Radiol 31:388; discussion 389–391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Doss M. 2012. Evidence supporting radiation hormesis in atomic bomb survivor cancer mortality data. Dose-Response 10:584–592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Doss M. 2013. Linear no-threshold model vs. radiation hormesis. Dose-Response 11:480–497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Einstein AJ. 2012. Effects of radiation exposure from cardiac imaging: how good are the data? J Am Coll Cardiol 59:553–565 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Einstein AJ, Henzlova MJ, Rajagopalan S. 2007. Estimating risk of cancer associated with radiation exposure from 64-slice computed tomography coronary angiography. JAMA 298:317–323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Esz J. Personal communication 2013.
  42. Etard C, Aubert B, Sinno-Tellier S. 2010. Medical exposure of the French population in 2007, in: Third European IRPA Congress, Radiation and Nuclear Safety Authority, Finland, Helsinki, FIN, pp. 239–243 [Google Scholar]
  43. Fazel R, Krumholz HM, Wang Y, Ross JS, Chen J, Ting HH, Shah ND, Nasir K, Einstein AJ, Nallamothu BK. 2009. Exposure to low-dose ionizing radiation from medical imaging procedures. N Engl J Med 361:849–857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. FDA. 2010. Initiative to reduce unnecessary radiation exposure from medical imaging. Available at http://www.fda.gov/Radiation-EmittingProducts/RadiationSafety/RadiationDoseReduction/default.htm
  45. Fletcher JG, Kofler JM, Coburn JA, Bruining DH, McCollough CH. 2013. Perspective on radiation risk in CT imaging. Abdominal Imaging 38:22–31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Francois P, N’ Dri D, Bergemer-Fouquet AM, Ben Ismail M, Papagiannaki C, Cottier JP, Jan M. 2010. Post-traumatic meningioma: three case reports of this rare condition and a review of the literature. Acta Neurochir (Wien) 152:1755–17560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hall EJ. 2002. Lessons we have learned from our children: cancer risks from diagnostic radiology. Pediatr Radiol 32:700–706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hall EJ. 2003. Reply. Pediatric Radiology 33:815–817 [Google Scholar]
  49. Hall EJ, Brenner DJ. 2012. Cancer risks from diagnostic radiology: the impact of new epidemiological data. British Journal of Radiology 85:E1316–E1317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hammer GP, Seidenbusch MC, Regulla DF, Spix C, Zeeb H, Schneider K, Blettner M. 2011. Childhood cancer risk from conventional radiographic examinations for selected referral criteria: results from a large cohort study. Am J Roentgenol 197:217–223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hammer GP, Seidenbusch MC, Schneider K, Regulla DF, Zeeb H, Spix C, Blettner M. 2009. A cohort study of childhood cancer incidence after postnatal diagnostic X-ray exposure. Radiat Res 171:504–512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hendee WR, O’Connor MK. 2012. Radiation risks of medical imaging: separating fact from fantasy. Radiology 264:312–321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Henry PT, Rajshekhar V. 2000. Post-traumatic malignant glioma: case report and review of the literature. Br J of Neurosurgery 14:64–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hochberg F, Toniolo P, Cole P. 1984. Head trauma and seizures as risk-factors of glioblastoma. Neurology 34:1511–1514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. HPS. 2010. Radiation risk in perspective: position paper of the Health Physics Society. Available at http://hps.org/documents/risk_ps010-2.pdf
  56. Hu JF, Johnson KC, Mao Y, Guo LH, Zhao XG, Jia XY, Bi DZ, Huang GR, Liu RZ. 1998. Risk factors for glioma in adults: A case-control study in northeast China. Cancer Detection and Prevention 22:100–108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Huda W. 2009. What ER radiologists need to know about radiation risks. Emerg Radiol 16:335–341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Huda W, Vance A. 2007. Patient radiation doses from adult and pediatric CT. Am J Roentgenol 188:540–546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. IAEA. 2009. Report of a consultation on justification of patient exposures in medical imaging. Radiat Prot Dosimetry 135:137–144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. ICRP. 2003a. ICRP Publication 90: Biological effects after prenatal irradiation (embryo and fetus). Ann ICRP 33:1–200 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. ICRP. 2003b. Relative biological effectiveness (RBE), quality factor (Q), and radiation weighting factor (wR): ICRP Publication 92. Ann ICRP 33:1–121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. ICRP. 2005. Low-dose extrapolation of radiation-related cancer risk. Vienna [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. ICRP. 2007. The 2007 Recommendations of the International Commission on Radiological Protectio. ICRP publication 103. Ann ICRP 37:1–332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Inskip PD, Linet MS, Heineman EF. 1995. Etiology of brain tumors in adults. Epidemiologic Reviews 17:382–414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Inskip PD, Mellemkjaer L, Gridley G, Olsen JH. 1998. Incidence of intracranial tumors following hospitalization for head injuries (Denmark). Cancer Causes & Control 9:109–116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Jaffurs D, Denny A. 2009. Diagnostic pediatric computed tomographic scans of the head: actual dosage versus estimated risk. Plast Reconstr Surg 124:1254–1260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Jenkins-Smith HC, Silva CL, Murray C. 2009. Beliefs about radiation: scientists, the public and public policy. Health Phys 97:519–527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kellerer AM, Ruhm W, Walsh L. 2006. Indications of the neutron effect contribution in the solid cancer data of the A-bomb survivors. Health Phys 90:554–564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Khong PL, Ringertz H, Donoghue V, Frush D, Rehani M, Appelgate K, Sanchez R. 2013. ICRP publication 121: radiological protection in paediatric diagnostic and interventional radiology. Ann ICRP 42:1–63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kizilay Z, Bolat E, Acar F, Yalcin N, Cirak B, Coskun ME. 2010. Post-traumatic meningioma. Pam Med J 3:106–108 [Google Scholar]
  71. Kuefner MA, Grudzenski S, Hamann J, Achenbach S, Lell M, Anders K, Schwab SA, Haberle L, Lobrich M, Uder M. 2010. Effect of CT scan protocols on x-ray-induced DNA double-strand breaks in blood lymphocytes of patients undergoing coronary CT angiography. Eur Radiol 20:2917–2924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Land CE. 1980. Estimating cancer risks from low doses of ionizing radiation. Science 209:1197–1203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Larson DB, Johnson LW, Schnell BM, Goske MJ, Salisbury SR, Forman HP. 2011. Rising use of CT in child visits to the emergency department in the United States, 1995-2008. Radiology 259:793–801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Laskey WK, Feinendegen LE, Neumann RD, Dilsizian V. 2010. Low-level ionizing radiation from noninvasive cardiac imaging: can we extrapolate estimated risks from epidemiologic data to the clinical setting? JACC Cardiovasc Imaging 3:517–524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Luckey TD.1980. Hormesis with Ionizing Radiation. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL [Google Scholar]
  76. Luckey TD.1991. Radiation Hormesis. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL [Google Scholar]
  77. Ma S, Kong B, Liu B, Liu X. 2013. Biological effects of low-dose radiation from computed tomography scanning. Int J Radiat Biol 89:326–333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Magnavita N, Placentino RA, Mei D, Ferraro D, Di Trapani G. 2003. Occupational head injury and subsequent glioma. Neurological Sciences 24:31–33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Maguire JL, Boutis K, Uleryk EM, Laupacis A, Parkin PC. 2009. Should a head-injured child receive a head CT scan? A systematic review of clinical prediction rules. Pediatrics 124:e145–154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Martin CJ. 2005. The LNT model provides the best approach for practical implementation of radiation protection. Br J Radiol 78:14–16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mathews JD, Forsythe AV, Brady Z, Butler MW, Goergen SK, Byrnes GB, Giles GG, Wallace AB, Anderson PR, Guiver TA, McGale P, Cain TM, Dowty JG, Bickerstaffe AC, Darby SC. 2013. Cancer risk in 680 000 people exposed to computed tomography scans in childhood or adolescence: data linkage study of 11 million Australians. Brit Med J 346:f2360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mayo Clinic. 2013. Reducing radiation exposure one image at a time, in: Discovery’s Edge, Mayo Clinic’s Online Research Magazine, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, [Google Scholar]
  83. McCollough CH, Guimaraes L, Fletcher JG. 2009. In defense of body CT. Am J Roentgenol 193:28–39 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mettler FA, Jr, Huda W, Yoshizumi TT, Mahesh M. 2008a. Effective doses in radiology and diagnostic nuclear medicine: a catalog. Radiology 248:254–263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mettler FA, Jr, Thomadsen BR, Bhargavan M, Gilley DB, Gray JE, Lipoti JA, McCrohan J, Yoshizumi TT, Mahesh M. 2008b. Medical radiation exposure in the U.S. in 2006: preliminary results. Health Phys 95:502–507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mettler FAJ, Upton AC. 2008. Carcinogenesis of specific sites. In: Medical Effects of Ionizing Radiation, pp 117–282. W.B. Saunders Company/Elsevier, Philadelphia, PA [Google Scholar]
  87. Miglioretti DL, Johnson E, Williams A, Greenlee RT, Weinmann S, Solberg LI, Feigelson HS, Roblin D, Flynn MJ, Vanneman N, Smith-Bindman R. 2013. The use of computed tomography in pediatrics and the associated radiation exposure and estimated cancer risk. JAMA Pediatr 1–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Monteiro GTR, Pereira RA, Koifman RJ, Koifman S. 2006. Head injury and brain tumours in adults: A case-control study in Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. European Journal of Cancer 42:917–921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Moorthy RK, Rajshekhar V. 2004. Development of glioblastoma multiforme following traumatic cerebral contusion: Case report and review of literature. Surgical Neurology 61:180–184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Munley MT, Moore JE, Walb MC, Isom SP, Olson JD, Zora JG, Kock ND, Wheeler KT, Miller MS. 2011. Cancer-prone mice expressing the Ki-rasG12C gene show increased lung carcinogenesis after CT screening exposures. Radiat Res 176:842–848 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Muratore CS.2012. Pediatric abdominal CT scans: do it correctly. Better yet, don’t do it at all. J Surg Res [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. NCRP. 2001. Evaluation of the Linear-Nonthreshold Dose-Response Model for Ionizing Radiation. 7910 Woodmont Avenue, Suite 800, Bethesda, MD 30814
  93. NCRP. 2009. Ionising Radiation Exposure of the Population of the United States. NCRP Report No. 160. 7910 Woodmont Avenue, Suite 800, Bethesda, MD 30814
  94. NRCNA. 2005. Health risks from exposure to low levels of ionizing rdiation: BEIR VII Phase 2. Washington, DC [PubMed]
  95. NRCNA. 2012. Tracking radiation exposure from medical diagnostic procedures: workshop report. National Academies Press, Washington, DC: [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Nygren C, Adami J, Ye W, Bellocco R, af Geijerstam JL, Borg J, Nyren O. 2001. Primary brain tumors following traumatic brain injury - a population-based cohort study in Sweden. Cancer Causes & Control 12:733–737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Oikarinen H, Merilainen S, Paakko E, Karttunen A, Nieminen MT, Tervonen O. 2009. Unjustified CT examinations in young patients. Eur Radiol 19:1161–1165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Ozasa K, Shimizu Y, Suyama A, Kasagi F, Soda M, Grant EJ, Sakata R, Sugiyama H, Kodama K. 2012a. Errata. Radiat Res 179:e0040–e0041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Ozasa K, Shimizu Y, Suyama A, Kasagi F, Soda M, Grant EJ, Sakata R, Sugiyama H, Kodama K. 2012b. Studies of the mortality of atomic bomb survivors, Report 14, 1950-2003: an overview of cancer and noncancer diseases. Radiat Res 177:229–243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Parker HL, Kernohan JW. 1931. The relation of injury and glioma of the brain. Journal of the American Medical Association 97:535–540 [Google Scholar]
  101. Pauwels EK, Bourguignon M. 2011. Cancer induction caused by radiation due to computed tomography: a critical note. Acta Radiol 52:767–773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Pearce MS, Salotti JA, Little MP, McHugh K, Lee C, Kim KP, Howe NL, Ronckers CM, Rajaraman P, Craft AW, Parker L, Berrington de Gonzalez A. 2012. Radiation exposure from CT scans in childhood and subsequent risk of leukaemia and brain tumours: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet 380:499–505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Phan N, Boreham DR. 2011. Health effects from low dose occupational and medical radiation exposure and the role of adaptive response. Health Phys 100:286–287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Phan N, De Lisio M, Parise G, Boreham DR. 2012. Biological effects and adaptive response from single and repeated computed tomography scans in reticulocytes and bone marrow of C57BL/6 mice. Radiat Res 177:164–175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Phillips LE, Koepsell TD, van Belle G, Kukull WA, Gehrels JA, Longstreth WT., Jr 2002. History of head trauma and risk of intracranial meningioma: population-based case-control study. Neurology 58:1849–1852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Popper KR.1959. The Logic of Scientific Discovery. Routledge Classics, New York, NY [Google Scholar]
  107. Preston DL, Cullings H, Suyama A, Funamoto S, Nishi N, Soda M, Mabuchi K, Kodama K, Kasagi F, Shore RE. 2008. Solid cancer incidence in atomic bomb survivors exposed in utero or as young children. Journal of the National Cancer Institute 100:428–436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Preston DL, Ron E, Tokuoka S, Funamoto S, Nishi N, Soda M, Mabuchi K, Kodama K. 2007. Solid cancer incidence in atomic bomb survivors: 1958-1998. Radiat Res 168:1–64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Preston-Martin S, Mack W, Henderson BE. 1989. Risk-factors for gliomas and meningiomas in males in Los Angeles county. Cancer Research 49:6137–6143 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Preston-Martin S, Pogoda JM, Schlehofer B, Blettner M, Howe GR, Ryan P, Menegoz F, Giles GG, Rodvall Y, Choi NW, Little J, Arslan A. 1998. An international case-control study of adult glioma and meningioma: the role of head trauma. International Journal of Epidemiology 27:579–586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Redberg RF, Smith-Bindman R. 2014. We are giving ourselves cancer, in: The New York Times, New York, NY, pp. A27 [Google Scholar]
  112. Redpath JL. 2006. Health risks of low photon energy imaging. Radiat Prot Dosimetry 122:528–533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Rehani MM. 2012. ICRP and IAEA actions on radiation protection in computed tomography. Ann ICRP 41:154–160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Rodrigues D, Scoones D, Bradey N, Aziz F, Kane P. 2006. De novo meningioma arising at a previous burr hole site. Br J Neurosurg 20:233–235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Ron E.2002. Let’s not relive the past: a review of cancer risk after diagnostic or therapeutic irradiation. Pediatr Radiol 32:739–744; discussion 751-734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Ron E. 2003. Cancer risks from medical radiation. Health Phys 85:47–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Rothman KJ, Greenland S. 1998. Modern Epidemiology. 2nd ed, Lippincot Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia, PA [Google Scholar]
  118. Ruhm W, Walsh L. 2007. Current risk estimates based on the A-bomb survivors data - A discussion in terms of the ICRP recommendations on the neutron weighting factor. Radiation Protection Dosimetry 126:423–431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Rutherford GW, Wlodarczyk RC. 2009. Distant sequelae of traumatic brain injury: premature mortality and intracranial neoplasms. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 24:468–474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Sabel M, Felsberg J, Messing-Junger M, Neuen-Jacob E, Piek J. 1999. Glioblastoma multiforme at the site of metal splinter injury: a coincidence? Case report. J Neurosurg 91:1041–1044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Sakai N, Ando T, Yamada H, Ikeda T, Shimokawa K. 1981. Meningioma associated with subdural hematoma - - report of a case and review of 15 reported cases. Neurol Med Chir (Tokyo) 21:329–336 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Salvati M, Caroli E, Rocchi G, Frati A, Brogna C, Orlando ER. 2004. Post-traumatic glioma. Report of four cases and review of the literature. Tumori 90:416–419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sane SM.2001. Reports on CT scans in children overstated potential risks. AAP News 18:183 [Google Scholar]
  124. Sasaki MS, Endo S, Ejima Y, Saito I, Okamura K, Oka Y, Hoshi M. 2006. Effective dose of A-bomb radiation in Hiroshima and Nagasaki as assessed by chromosomal effectiveness of spectrum energy photons and neutrons. Radiation and Environmental Biophysics 45:79–91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Sasaki MS, Nomura T, Ejima Y, Utsumi H, Endo S, Saito I, Itoh T, Hoshi M. 2008. Experimental derivation of relative biological effectiveness of A-bomb neutrons in Hiroshima and Nagasaki and implications for risk assessment. Radiat Res 170:101–117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Schlehofer B, Blettner M, Becker N, Martinsohn C, Wahrendorf J. 1992. Medical risk-factors and the development of brain-tumors. Cancer 69:2541–2547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Schmitt HP. 1983. Trauma and tumor - malignant glioma in the area of a traumatic lesion of the brain with metal scales. Fortschritte Der Neurologie Psychiatrie 51:227–231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Schulze-Rath R, Hammer GP, Blettner M. 2008. Are pre- or postnatal diagnostic X-rays a risk factor for childhood cancer? A systematic review. Radiat Environ Biophys 47:301–312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Scott BR, Sanders CL, Mitchel REJ, Boreham DR. 2008. CT scans may reduce rather than increase the risk of cancer. J American Physicians and Surgeons 13:8–11 [Google Scholar]
  130. Shrimpton PC, Hillier MC, Lewis MA, Dunn M. 2006. National survey of doses from CT in the UK: 2003. Br J Radiol 79:968–980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Silva CL, Jenkins-Smith HC, Barke RP. 2007. Reconciling scientists’ beliefs about radiation risks and social norms: explaining preferred radiation protection standards. Risk Anal 27:755–773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Smith-Bindman R, Lipson J, Marcus R, Kim KP, Mahesh M, Gould R, Berrington de Gonzalez A, Miglioretti DL. 2009. Radiation dose associated with common computed tomography examinations and the associated lifetime attributable risk of cancer. Arch Intern Med 169:2078–2086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Smith-Bindman R, Miglioretti DL, Johnson E, Lee C, Feigelson HS, Flynn M, Greenlee RT, Kruger RL, Hornbrook MC, Roblin D, Solberg LI, Vanneman N, Weinmann S, Williams AE. 2012. Use of diagnostic imaging studies and associated radiation exposure for patients enrolled in large integrated health care systems, 1996-2010. JAMA 307:2400–2409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Smith-Bindman R, Miglioretti DL, Larson EB. 2008. Rising use of diagnostic medical imaging in a large integrated health system. Health Aff (Millwood) 27:1491–1502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Sodickson A. 2013. CT radiation risks coming into clearer focus. British Medical Journal 346:1–2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Sodickson A, Baeyens PF, Andriole KP, Prevedello LM, Nawfel RD, Hanson R, Khorasani R. 2009. Recurrent CT, cumulative radiation exposure, and associated radiation-induced cancer risks from CT of adults. Radiology 251:175–184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. SPR. 2001. Risks and benefits in pediatric CT. Pediatric Radiology 31:387–387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Sternberg S.2001. CT scans in children linked to cancer, in: USA Today, June 19
  139. Tabeie F, Mohammadi H, Asli IN. 2013. Population radiation dose from diagnostic nuclear medicine procedures in the Tehran population in 1999-2003: striking changes in only one decade. Health Phys 104:127–131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Taylor LS. 1980. Some nonscientific influences on radiation protection standards and practice. The 1980 Sievert Lecture. Health Phys 39:851–874 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. The Joint Commission. 2011. Sentinel Event Alert Issue 47. Sentinel Event Alert 1–4 [Google Scholar]
  142. Troost D, Tulleken CAF. 1984. Malignant glioma after bombshell injury. Clinical Neuropathology 3:139–142 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Ulsh BA. 2010. Checking the foundation: recent radiobiology and the linear no-threshold theory. Health Phys 99:747–758 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Ulsh BA. 2012. The new radiobiology: returning to our roots. Dose Response 10:593–609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. UNSCEAR. 2000. Sources and effects of ionizing radiation. New York, NY
  146. UNSCEAR. 2006. Effects of Ionizing Radiation. Report to the General Assembly, with Scientific Annexes. New York, NY [Google Scholar]
  147. UNSCEAR. 2012. Report of the United Nations Scientific Committee on the Effects of Atomic Radiation Fifty-ninth Session (21-25 May 2012). New York, NY
  148. UNSCEAR. 2013. Biological mechanisms of radiation ations at low doss. New York, NY
  149. Valentin J. 2001. Radiation and your patient: A guide for medical practitioners: ICRP Supporting Guidance 2. Annals of the ICRP 31:1–52 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Walsh L. 2013. Neutron relative biological effectiveness for solid cancer incidence in the Japanese A-bomb survivors: an analysis considering the degree of independent effects from gamma-ray and neutron absorbed doses with hierarchical partitioning. Radiation and Environmental Biophysics 52:29–36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Walsh L, Ruhm W, Kellerer AM. 2004a. Cancer risk estimates for gamma-rays with regard to organ-specific doses - Part 1: All solid cancers combined. Radiation and Environmental Biophysics 43:145–151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Walsh L, Ruhm W, Kellerer AM. 2004b. Cancer risk estimates for gamma-rays with regard to organ-specific doses - Part II: Site-specific solid cancers. Radiation and Environmental Biophysics 43:225–231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Walsh L, Shore R, Auvinen A, Jung T, Wakeford R. 2013. Re: Cancer risk in 680,000 people exposed to computed tomography scans in childhood or adolescence: data linkage study of 11 million Australians. Br Med J [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Walton D. 1999. The appeal to ignorance, or argumentum ad ignorantiam. Argumentation 13:367–377 [Google Scholar]
  155. Wiest PW, Locken JA, Heintz PH, Mettler FA., Jr 2002. CT scanning: a major source of radiation exposure. Semin Ultrasound CT MR 23:402–410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wrensch M, Miike R, Lee M, Neuhaus J. 2000. Are prior head injuries or diagnostic X-rays associated with glioma in adults? The effects of control selection bias. Neuroepidemiology 19:234–244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Zampieri P, Meneghini F, Grigoletto F, Gerosa M, Licata C, Casentini L, Longatti PL, Padoan A, Mingrino S. 1994. Risk-factors for cerebral glioma in adults - a case-control study in an italian population. Journal of Neuro-Oncology 19:61–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Zarembo A.2009. Cedars-Sinai radiation overdoses went unseen at several points, in: Los Angeles Times, Los Angeles Times, Los Angeles, CA [Google Scholar]
  159. Zhou B, Liu WG. 2010. Post-traumatic glioma: Report of one case and review of the literature. International Journal of Medical Sciences 7:248–250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Zondervan RL, Hahn PF, Sadow CA, Liu B, Lee SI. 2011. Frequent body CT scanning of young adults: indications, outcomes, and risk for radiation-induced cancer. J Am Coll Radiol 8:501–507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dose-Response are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES