Abstract
Mental health and sleep are intricately linked. This study characterized associations of psychological distress with short (≤6 hours) and long (≥9 hours) sleep duration among adults aged ≥18 years. 2013 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System data (n = 36,859) from Colorado, Minnesota, Nevada, Tennessee, and Washington included the Kessler 6 (K6) scale, which has been psychometrically validated for measuring severe psychological distress (SPD); three specifications were evaluated. Overall, 4.0% of adults reported SPD, 33.9% reported short sleep, and 7.8% reported long sleep. After adjustment, adults with SPD had 1.58 (95% CI: 1.45, 1.72) and 1.39 (95% CI: 1.08, 1.79) times higher probability of reporting short and long sleep duration, respectively. Using an ordinal measure showed a dose-response association with prevalence ratios of 1.00, 1.16, 1.38, 1.67, and 2.11 for short sleep duration. Each additional point added to the K6 scale was associated with 1.08 (95% CI: 1.07, 1.10) and 1.02 (95% CI: 1.00, 1.03) times higher probability of reporting short and long sleep duration, respectively. Some results were statistically different by gender. Any psychological distress, not only SPD, was associated with a higher probability of short sleep duration but not long sleep duration. These findings highlight the need for interventions.
1. Introduction
Two Healthy People 2020 goals are to increase public knowledge of how adequate sleep improves health and to improve mental health through prevention [1]. According to Healthy People 2020, “mental health is a state of successful performance of mental function, resulting in productive activities, fulfilling relationships with other people, and the ability to adapt to change and to cope with challenges” [1]. Previous studies suggest that the prevalence of SPD (a nonspecific measure that incorporates symptoms of anxiety, depression, and other types of psychological distress) among US adults is between 3.2 and 5.4% [2–6]. Both inadequate sleep and poor mental health are major causes of employee absenteeism and impaired work productivity [7, 8]. Since 1985, the mean age-adjusted sleep duration among US adults has declined. In 1985, 65.9% of adults reported sleeping 7 to 8 hours in a 24-hour period, compared with 62.3% in 2012 [9]. Therefore, mental health and sleep remain major public health priorities.
The association between mental health and sleep has been the focus of health research for over 100 years [10]. Sleep problems are symptoms of depression and anxiety and associated with psychosis according to the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [11]. Furthermore, studies suggest that impaired sleep may directly contribute to the development of some psychiatric disorders [12–15]. Insomnia, a disorder characterized by difficulty falling or staying asleep for example, has been shown to increase the likelihood of subsequent depression [16, 17]. On the other hand, some studies have shown bidirectional associations and other studies have shown that poor mental health may raise the risk for impaired sleep [18, 19]. In particular, two previous studies have found associations between sleep duration and serious psychological distress, as measured by the Kessler 6 (K6), which is a 6-question measure of nonspecific psychological distress developed for large, representative, health surveys and predictive of serious mental illness [13, 14]. To the best of our knowledge, no study has investigated the possible presence of dose-response associations and the functional form of the associations of psychological distress with short and long sleep duration in addition to associations of SPD among adults using the K6. Furthermore, it remains unknown whether these associations are modified by gender.
To address these gaps, we analyzed population-based data from women and men in the 2013 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS). First, the associations of SPD with short and long sleep duration are examined. Second, the associations of a five-category ordinal measure of psychological distress with short and long duration are examined. Third, associations of a scale measure of psychological distress with range 0 to 24 with short and long duration are examined. Our a priori hypotheses were that SPD would be associated with a higher probability of both short and long sleep durations. We also hypothesized that there are dose-response associations for short and long sleep duration and differences by gender.
2. Methods
2.1. Study Population
BRFSS is an annual state-based, random-digit-dialed telephone survey of noninstitutionalized, US adults aged ≥18 years in the 50 states and the District of Columbia (DC) [20]. BRFSS includes questions on sociodemographic characteristics, chronic diseases, health behaviors, and access to health care. In 2013, a new question on sleep duration was included in the core questions and five states (Colorado, Minnesota, Nevada, Tennessee, and Washington) implemented the optional BRFSS mental illness and stigma module and comprised the study sample. The cooperation rate, which is defined as the number of completed interviews divided by the number of eligible respondents who were successfully reached by an interviewer, for each of the five states was 75.9% (Colorado), 73.2% (Minnesota), 72.6% (Nevada), 72.5% (Tennessee), and 51.8% (Washington) [21]. The response rate, which is defined as the number of respondents who completed the survey as a proportion of all eligible and likely eligible persons, for each of the five states was 58.0% (Colorado), 54.3% (Minnesota), 43.7% (Nevada), 45.9% (Tennessee), and 31.1% (Washington) [21]. We analyzed available data from 36,859 US adults aged 18 years or older in five states for the variables included in this study. This study was exempt from human subjects review as the data were obtained from public-use surveillance datasets.
2.2. Independent Variables
The K6 scale is included in the optional BRFSS mental illness and stigma module and is based on the Kessler 10 scale of nonspecific psychological distress [22]. Respondents were asked “During the past 30 days, about how often did you feel… (1) nervous, (2) hopeless, (3) restless or fidgety, (4) so depressed that nothing could cheer you up, (5) everything was an effort, and (6) worthless?” Symptom frequencies are described on a 5-point Likert scale with the responses (1) all, (2) most, (3) some, (4) a little, and (5) none of the time [22, 23]. Responses are scored 4 to 0, respectively, and response scores are summed, for a total possible score range of 0 to 24. K6 scores of 13 or greater versus 12 or fewer were used as a cut-point to distinguish people with and without SPD. Additionally, the K6 scores were grouped into five categories (0, 1 to 2, 3 to 5, 6 to 10, and 11 or greater) as defined in previous studies [6, 24]. There was approximately 30%, 30%, 20%, 15%, and 5% of the population in each category.
2.3. Dependent Variables
Respondents were asked “On average, how many hours of sleep do you get in a 24-hour period?” Because the National Institutes of Health recommends 7-8 hours of sleep per day for healthy adults (http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health/health-topics/topics/sdd/howmuch), self-reported short sleep duration was defined as ≤ 6 hours of sleep in an average 24-hour period and self-reported long sleep duration was defined as ≥ 9 hours of sleep in an average 24-hour period.
2.4. Sociodemographic Characteristics
Potentially confounding sociodemographic characteristics considered for these analyses included age group (18–24, 25–34, 35–44, 45–64, or ≥ 65 years), gender (men or women), race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic white; non-Hispanic black; non-Hispanic American Indian or Alaskan Native; non-Hispanic Asian; non-Hispanic Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islanders; non-Hispanic other races; non-Hispanic multiracial; or Hispanic), marital status (married; previously married including those divorced, widowed, or separated; or never married or members of an unmarried couple), educational attainment (did not graduate high school, graduated high school or obtained the general equivalent degree, attended some college or technical school, or graduated college or technical school), annual household income (<$25,000, $25,000–$49,999, ≥$50,000, or missing), employment (employed, unemployed, homemaker/student, retired, or unable to work), and health insurance coverage (yes or no).
2.5. Statistical Analysis
All analyses were conducted using SAS-callable SUDAAN version 11.0 (Research Triangle Institute, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina) to account for complex sampling design of BRFSS. All estimates were weighted to represent the state population. Results were considered significant at P < .05. We examined differences in SPD, short sleep duration, and long sleep duration by selected sociodemographic characteristics using Chi-squared tests. Additionally, we examined differences in sleep duration by SPD and a five-category K6 measure using Chi-squared tests. Poisson regression models with robust variance were used to estimate prevalence ratios (PRs) for the probability of reporting short sleep duration or long sleep duration and the corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for all respondents with complete data and separately for women and men for three specifications of the K6: a dichotomous measure of SPD (cut-point 13), a five-category measure, and a scale measure with range 0–24 [25, 26]. We also examined quadratic terms for the scale measure of psychological distress. The multivariable adjusted regression models included the following relevant covariates: age group, race/ethnicity, marital status, educational attainment, annual household income, employment, and health insurance coverage.
3. Results
Overall, 76.3% were non-Hispanic white, 51.5% were women, and 36.7% were 45–64 years (Table 1). The prevalence of SPD was 4.0% (95% CI = 3.7, 4.4) overall and varied by gender: women were more likely to report SPD (P < .05). The prevalence of short sleep duration was 33.9% (95% CI = 33.1, 34.7) overall and varied by gender: women were less likely to report short sleep (P < .05). The prevalence of long sleep duration was 7.8% (95% CI = 7.3, 8.2) overall and varied by gender also: women were more likely to report long sleep (P < .05).
Table 1.
Characteristic | No. | Serious psychological | Short sleep duration | Long sleep duration | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
distress, % | (≤6 hours), % | (≥9 hours), % | |||||
(95% CI) | P (χ 2) | (95% CI) | P (χ 2) | (95% CI) | P (χ 2) | ||
Overall | 36,859 | 4.0 (3.7, 4.4) | 33.9 (33.1, 34.7) | 7.8 (7.3, 8.2) | |||
Gender | |||||||
Men | 15,361 | 3.1 (2.6, 3.6) | <.001 | 35.7 (34.4, 36.9) | <.001 | 6.9 (6.2, 7.5) | <.001 |
Women | 21,498 | 4.9 (4.4, 5.4) | 32.3 (31.3, 33.4) | 8.6 (8.0, 9.2) | |||
Age (years) | |||||||
18–24 | 1,707 | 3.9 (2.7, 5.1) | <.001 | 31.0 (28.1, 33.9) | <.001 | 10.8 (8.9, 12.6) | <.001 |
25–34 | 3,416 | 4.9 (3.8, 6.0) | 40.5 (38.2, 42.7) | 6.2 (5.0, 7.4) | |||
35–44 | 4,730 | 4.4 (3.6, 5.2) | 35.5 (33.5, 37.6) | 6.0 (4.9, 7.1) | |||
45–64 | 15,332 | 4.7 (4.2, 5.3) | 35.4 (34.2, 36.6) | 5.8 (5.2, 6.4) | |||
≥65 | 11,674 | 1.5 (1.2, 1.9) | 25.5 (24.2, 26.8) | 13.0 (12.0, 14.0) | |||
Race/ethnicity | |||||||
White, non-Hispanic | 31,506 | 3.9 (3.5, 4.2) | .09 | 32.3 (31.4, 33.1) | <.001 | 7.8 (7.3, 8.2) | .13 |
Black, non-Hispanic | 1,485 | 5.1 (3.3, 6.9) | 43.7 (39.8, 47.5) | 8.5 (6.3, 10.6) | |||
American Indian or Alaskan Native | 321 | 6.6 (2.9, 10.3) | 48.9 (40.0, 57.7) | 8.7 (4.7, 12.7) | |||
Asian | 629 | 2.1 (0.5, 3.7) | 36.8 (31.5, 42.1) | 4.6 (1.9, 7.3) | |||
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander | 76 | 4.3 (0.0, 9.5) | 33.9 (20.4, 47.3) | 2.8 (0.0, 6.7) | |||
Other race, non-Hispanic | 303 | 4.6 (1.1, 8.0) | 42.2 (32.4, 52.0) | 6.9 (2.2, 11.7) | |||
Multiracial, non-Hispanic | 584 | 6.0 (2.3, 9.6) | 40.5 (32.7, 48.2) | 12.7 (4.6, 20.8) | |||
Hispanic | 1,955 | 4.7 (3.3, 6.1) | 36.3 (33.3, 39.3) | 7.9 (6.1, 9.7) | |||
Marital status | |||||||
Married | 20,035 | 2.6 (2.2, 3.0) | <.001 | 31.1 (30.1, 32.1) | <.001 | 7.2 (6.6, 7.8) | .02 |
Previously married | 10,547 | 6.4 (5.6, 7.1) | 39.4 (37.8, 41.0) | 8.1 (7.4, 8.9) | |||
Never married | 6,277 | 5.3 (4.4, 6.1) | 35.8 (33.9, 37.7) | 8.7 (7.6, 9.7) | |||
Educational attainment | |||||||
Did not graduate high school | 2,026 | 9.8 (7.9, 11.7) | <.001 | 39.9 (36.8, 43.0) | <.001 | 10.6 (8.6, 12.6) | <.001 |
Graduated high school | 9,174 | 4.7 (4.0, 5.4) | 36.3 (34.7, 37.9) | 8.5 (7.6, 9.5) | |||
Some college or technical school | 11,090 | 3.7 (3.2, 4.3) | 35.1 (33.7, 36.4) | 7.1 (6.4, 7.8) | |||
Graduated college or technical school | 14,569 | 1.2 (0.9, 1.4) | 27.7 (26.6, 28.8) | 6.6 (6.0, 7.2) | |||
Annual household income | |||||||
<$25,000 | 7,991 | 9.9 (8.8, 11.0) | <.001 | 41.2 (39.4, 43.0) | <.001 | 9.0 (7.9, 10.1) | <.001 |
$25,000–$49,999 | 8,346 | 2.7 (2.1, 3.2) | 35.5 (33.8, 37.2) | 8.5 (7.4, 9.6) | |||
≥$50,000 | 15,798 | 1.0 (0.8, 1.3) | 29.8 (28.7, 30.9) | 6.0 (5.4, 6.6) | |||
Missing | 4,724 | 5.3 (4.2, 6.4) | 32.0 (29.8, 34.3) | 9.6 (8.3, 10.9) | |||
Employment | |||||||
Employed | 19,135 | 1.7 (1.4, 2.0) | <.001 | 35.0 (34.0, 36.1) | <.001 | 5.2 (4.7, 5.8) | <.001 |
Unemployed | 1,900 | 10.0 (7.8, 12.1) | 39.5 (36.1, 43.0) | 10.2 (8.2, 12.2) | |||
Homemaker/student | 2,993 | 3.9 (2.8, 5.0) | 28.2 (25.7, 30.8) | 8.4 (7.0, 9.8) | |||
Retired | 10,551 | 1.6 (1.2, 2.0) | 24.6 (23.2, 25.9) | 13.0 (11.9, 14.1) | |||
Unable to work | 2,280 | 25.4 (22.4, 28.5) | 53.5 (50.1, 57.0) | 12.8 (10.5, 15.1) | |||
Health insurance coverage | 33,112 | 3.4 (3.2, 3.9) | <.001 | 32.7 (31.9, 33.5) | <.001 | .04 |
Note. Weighted percentages; unweighted numbers. CI = confidence interval.
The proportion of persons reporting short sleep duration and long sleep duration was higher among people with SPD than those without it (Figure 1; P < .05).
The proportion of persons reporting 7-8 hours of sleep on average was lower with increasing K6 categories (Figure 2; P < .05) from 66.9% among those with a K6 score of 0 to only 27.0% among those with score of 11+.
In the crude models, SPD, the five-category measure, and the scale measure of psychological distress were associated with a higher probability of reporting short sleep duration (Table 2; P < .05). The quadratic term for the scale measure was associated with a lower probability of reporting short sleep duration (Table 2; P < .05). These associations for short sleep duration remained statistically significant after adjustment for gender, age, race/ethnicity, marital status, educational attainment, annual household income, employment, and health insurance coverage. We found statistical interactions (P < .05) for gender with the five-category measure and the scale measure of psychological distress in association with short sleep duration. This distinction is statistically, but not always practically, significant.
Table 2.
Independent variables | Short sleep duration (≤6 hours) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All | Women | Men | ||||
Crude | Adjusted | Crude | Adjusted | Crude | Adjusted | |
PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | |
Model 1: Serious psychological distress | 1.95 (1.82, 2.10) | 1.58 (1.45, 1.72) | 2.04 (1.86, 2.24) | 1.56 (1.40, 1.74) | 1.89 (1.68, 2.11) | 1.58 (1.39, 1.79) |
Model 2: K6 category | ||||||
0 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
1-2 | 1.15 (1.07, 1.23) | 1.16 (1.08, 1.24) | 1.29 (1.17, 1.42) | 1.31 (1.18, 1.44) | 1.06 (0.96, 1.17) | 1.06 (0.96, 1.17) |
3–5 | 1.41 (1.31, 1.51) | 1.38 (1.28, 1.48) | 1.55 (1.40, 1.71) | 1.53 (1.38, 1.68) | 1.33 (1.20, 1.47) | 1.28 (1.16, 1.42) |
6–10 | 1.78 (1.66, 1.92) | 1.67 (1.54, 1.80) | 2.04 (1.84, 2.25) | 1.86 (1.68, 2.07) | 1.63 (1.46, 1.82) | 1.53 (1.37, 1.70) |
11+ | 2.40 (2.22, 2.59) | 2.11 (1.94, 2.30) | 2.69 (2.42, 2.99) | 2.25 (2.00, 2.54) | 2.26 (2.02, 2.53) | 2.02 (1.79, 2.28) |
Model 3: Scale measure of psychological distress | 1.09 (1.08, 1.11) | 1.08 (1.07, 1.10) | 1.11 (1.09, 1.13) | 1.10 (1.08, 1.11) | 1.08 (1.06, 1.10) | 1.07 (1.05, 1.09) |
Scale measure of psychological distress2 | 0.998 (0.997, 0.999) | 0.998 (0.997, 0.999) | 0.997 (0.997, 0.998) | 0.997 (0.997, 0.998) | 0.998 (0.998, 0.999) | 0.999 (0.998, 1.000) |
Note. CI = confidence interval; PR = prevalence ratio.
Model 1 examined psychological distress dichotomized (serious/no serious psychological distress), Model 2 examined psychological distress categorized, and Model 3 examined the scale measure of psychological distress (0–24) and included a quadratic term for the scale measure of psychological distress. Age, gender, race/ethnicity, marital status, educational attainment, annual household income, employment, and health insurance coverage are included in all adjusted models. Gender is excluded from the stratified models.
In the crude models, SPD, the five-category measure, and the scale measure of psychological distress were associated with a higher probability of long sleep duration for all respondents (Table 3; P < .05). The quadratic term for the scale measure was not statistically significant for long sleep duration. Therefore, it was not included in the final models. After covariate adjustment, the associations for SPD and the scale measure of psychological distress remained statistically significant but not the five-category measure. We did not observe any statistically significant interactions between gender and the three specifications of the K6 in association with long sleep duration.
Table 3.
Independent variables | Long sleep duration (≥9 hours) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All | Women | Men | ||||
Crude | Adjusted | Crude | Adjusted | Crude | Adjusted | |
PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | PR (95% CI) | |
Model 1: Serious psychological distress | 1.73 (1.34, 2.22) | 1.39 (1.08, 1.79) | 1.64 (1.18, 2.27) | 1.37 (1.00, 1.86) | 1.78 (1.24, 2.55) | 1.43 (0.96, 2.16) |
Model 2: K6 category | ||||||
0 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
1-2 | 0.93 (0.80, 1.07) | 0.93 (0.80, 1.07) | 0.90 (0.75, 1.08) | 0.88 (0.74, 1.06) | 0.93 (0.74, 1.18) | 0.99 (0.78, 1.25) |
3–5 | 1.02 (0.85, 1.21) | 1.00 (0.837, 1.193) | 1.11 (0.91, 1.36) | 1.07 (0.87, 1.32) | 0.87 (0.64, 1.18) | 0.92 (0.68, 1.24) |
6–10 | 1.14 (0.94, 1.38) | 1.04 (0.852, 1.278) | 1.05 (0.84, 1.31) | 0.96 (0.76, 1.20) | 1.23 (0.88, 1.72) | 1.21 (0.86, 1.72) |
11+ | 1.52 (1.22, 1.90) | 1.22 (0.969, 1.543) | 1.52 (1.14, 2.02) | 1.25 (0.94, 1.65) | 1.43 (1.03, 1.99) | 1.21 (0.83, 1.78) |
Model 3: Scale measure of psychological distress | 1.03 (1.02, 1.04) | 1.02 (1.00, 1.03) | 1.03 (1.01, 1.05) | 1.02 (1.00, 1.03) | 1.03 (1.01, 1.05) | 1.02 (0.99, 1.04) |
Note. CI = confidence interval; PR = prevalence ratio.
Model 1 examined psychological distress dichotomized (serious/no serious psychological distress), Model 2 examined psychological distress categorized, and Model 3 examined the scale measure of psychological distress (0–24). Age, gender, race/ethnicity, marital status, educational attainment, annual household income, employment, and health insurance coverage are included in all adjusted models. Gender is excluded from the stratified models.
4. Discussion
This study using population-based data demonstrated a strong link between psychological distress and self-reported sleep duration. Furthermore, our study revealed that associations of psychological distress as measured by the K6 scale with short and long duration are not limited to a dichotomous measure of SPD and may vary by gender. The hypotheses were partially supported. We found positive linear associations and nonlinear associations for short sleep duration but only positive linear associations for long sleep duration; the higher the score on the K6 scale, the higher the probability of short and long sleep duration. Additionally, levels of psychological distress below the traditional cut-point for SPD were associated with a higher probability of short sleep duration. That is, we found dose-response associations for short sleep duration, but we did not find dose-response associations for long sleep duration.
4.1. Contributions to Extant Literature
These findings are broadly consistent with those of previous studies. In a study of 20,822 Australian young adults (aged 17–24 years) in the DRIVE study, self-reported short sleep duration was associated with prevalent psychological distress and was an independent risk factor for its persistence 1 year later [13]. In another study, among 187,091 US adults aged ≥ 18 years from seven waves of the National Health Interview Survey, participants with high psychological distress scores reported 7-8 hours of sleep less often than those in low or moderate psychological distress and were also most likely to sleep for ≤6 hours or ≥9 hours [14]. Additionally, in a study of adults aged 19–67 years in the first wave and aged 29–80 years in the second wave of the Norwegian Nord-Trøndelag Health Study, among participants not depressed, chronic insomnia increased the risk of developing depression by six times. A similar effect was found for chronic depression (without insomnia) on onset of insomnia [18].
To our knowledge, there are no previous studies that have examined the associations of various formulations of psychological distress with short and long sleep duration by gender. Interestingly, particularly at lower levels, associations with short sleep duration for the five-category measure of psychological distress were more robust among women than men in our study. Additionally, there were gender differences in the associations with short sleep duration for the scale measure of psychological distress that were statistically significant but not necessarily clinically meaningful at the individual level. Nonetheless, the observed gender differences as well as the finding that the prevalence of SPD was less among men than women are consistent with the broader epidemiologic literature [2].
There are likely at least four alternative explanations for these findings [2]. First, psychological distress might be related to gender differences in personality traits or biological components as observed with anxiety and depression [27, 28]. Second, because the K6 does not differentiate well between anxiety and depression, there may be gender differences in the types of psychological distress. Third, women may be more vulnerable or more exposed to risk factors associated with psychological distress [29, 30]. Finally, there are gender differences in the expression of emotions [31, 32]. To the extent that women and men express their psychological distress or emotions in a different way, population-based interventions are likely to be less than optimally effective if this heterogeneity is overlooked. In addition to the higher prevalence of SPD we observed, there is growing evidence that women may be more sensitive to emotions. Further research is needed to examine if these possible explanations apply to gender differences in the association of psychological distress with sleep duration. A greater understanding of gender differences in the associations between psychological distress, emotions, and sleep duration may yield more effective interventions.
The purposes and mechanisms of sleep are complex and not fully elucidated; however, advances in the neurobiology of sleep and mental illness indicate that adequate sleep is critical to optimal mental functioning, productive activities, fulfilling relationships with others, and the ability to adapt to change and to cope with challenges for both women and men [12]. Moreover, inadequate sleep impacts alertness, hormone regulation, memory formation, emotional regulation, executive function, and other aspects of behavior [12].
In addition to SPD, milder psychological distress has strong dose-response and positive linear associations with both emergency room department utilization and mortality among US adults in two previous, longitudinal studies, possibly indicating lasting effects of psychological distress [6, 24]. Based on our findings, a corollary observation is that these previously reported prospective associations with emergency room department utilization and mortality may be partially mediated by the association between psychological distress and short sleep duration. Thus, interventions that aim to promote mental health and adequate sleep may have beneficial impacts beyond those two factors.
4.2. Limitations
The findings are subject to at least four limitations. First, other sleep-related factors (e.g., insomnia, excessive daytime sleepiness, shift work, sleeping pills, and menopause) were not examined. Second, BRFSS data are collected via telephone survey and may not be representative of adults in households without phones. Third, as the data were collected from adults in five states, the generalizability of our findings may be limited. Fourth, the cross-sectional study design does not permit determining temporal sequence of psychological distress with short and long sleep duration and thus cause and effect associations cannot be determined [33].
5. Conclusion
In summary, the findings of this study suggest that milder psychological distress, in addition to SPD, was associated with a greater probability of short and long sleep duration. Additionally, some associations for short sleep duration were statistically different for women and men, but not necessarily clinically meaningful. Nevertheless, psychological distress could potentially be targeted through interventions as a way to improve the percentage of women and men who are sleeping sufficiently. Insofar as interventions promote mental health and also encourage adequate sleep, the health impacts of these interventions may extend well beyond the immediate.
Disclaimer
The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the CDC.
Conflict of Interests
There is no conflict of interests related to this paper.
Authors' Contribution
Timothy J. Cunningham generated the study concept and design, had full access to the data, analyzed the data, and drafted the paper. Anne G. Wheaton and Wayne H. Giles contributed significantly to the interpretation of the data and to editing the text.
References
- 1.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2020. 2011. https://www.healthypeople.gov/ [Google Scholar]
- 2.Drapeau A., Marchand A., Beaulieu-Prévost D. Epidemiology of psychological distress. In: L'Abate L., editor. Mental Illnesses—Understanding, Prediction and Control. chapter 5. Rijeka, Croatia: InTech; 2011. pp. 105–134. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Pratt L. A., Dey A. N., Cohen A. J. Characteristics of adults with serious psychological distress as measured by the K6 scale: United States, 2001–04. Advance data. 2007;(382):1–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Strine T. W., Dhingra S. S., Okoro C. A., et al. State-based differences in the prevalence and characteristics of untreated persons with serious psychological distress. International Journal of Public Health. 2009;54(supplement 1):9–15. doi: 10.1007/s00038-009-0001-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Reeves W. C., Strine T. W., Pratt L. A., et al. Mental illness surveillance among adults in the United States. MMWR CDC Surveillance Summaries. 2011;60(supplement 3):1–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stockbridge E. L., Wilson F. A., Pagán J. A. Psychological distress and emergency department utilization in the United states: evidence from the Medical Expenditure Panel Survey. Academic Emergency Medicine. 2014;21(5):510–519. doi: 10.1111/acem.12369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Shahly V., Berglund P. A., Coulouvrat C., et al. The associations of insomnia with costly workplace accidents and errors: results from the America insomnia survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69(10):1054–1063. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kessler R. C., Berglund P. A., Coulouvrat C., et al. Insomnia, comorbidity, and risk of injury among insured Americans: results from the America Insomnia Survey. Sleep. 2012;35(6):825–834. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ford E. S., Cunningham T. J., Croft J. B. Trends in self-reported sleep duration among us adults from 1985 to 2012. Sleep. 2015;38(5):829–832. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Palagini L., Rosenlicht N. Sleep, dreaming, and mental health: a review of historical and neurobiological perspectives. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2011;15(3):179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sutton E. L. Psychiatric disorders and sleep issues. Medical Clinics of North America. 2014;98(5):1123–1143. doi: 10.1016/j.mcna.2014.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Glozier N., Martiniuk A., Patton G., et al. Short sleep duration in prevalent and persistent psychological distress in young adults: the DRIVE study. Sleep. 2010;33(9):1139–1145. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.9.1139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Liang W., Chikritzhs T. Sleep duration and its links to psychological distress, health status, physical activity and body mass index among a large representative general population sample. International Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2013;4(1):45–51. doi: 10.4236/ijcm.2013.41010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sivertsen B., Harvey A. G., Lundervold A. J., Hysing M. Sleep problems and depression in adolescence: results from a large population-based study of Norwegian adolescents aged 16–18 years. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2014;23(8):681–689. doi: 10.1007/s00787-013-0502-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pigeon W. R., Hegel M., Unützer J., et al. Is insomnia a perpetuating factor for late-life depression in the IMPACT cohort? Sleep. 2008;31(4):481–488. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.4.481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Salo P., Sivertsen B., Oksanen T., et al. Insomnia symptoms as a predictor of incident treatment for depression: prospective cohort study of 40,791 men and women. Sleep Medicine. 2012;13(3):278–284. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sivertsen B., Salo P., Mykletun A., et al. The bidirectional association between depression and insomnia: the HUNT study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2012;74(7):758–765. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e3182648619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Morphy H., Dunn K. M., Lewis M., Boardman H. F., Croft P. R. Epidemiology of insomnia: a longitudinal study in a UK population. Sleep. 2007;30(3):274–280. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Methodologic changes in the behavioral risk factor surveillance system in 2011 and potential effects on prevalence estimates. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2012;61(22):410–413. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System, 2013 Summary Data Quality Report. Atlanta, Ga, USA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kessler R. C., Andrews G., Colpe L. J., et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32(6):959–976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kessler R. C., Barker P. R., Colpe L. J., et al. Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(2):184–189. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pratt L. A. Serious psychological distress, as measured by the K6, and mortality. Annals of Epidemiology. 2009;19(3):202–209. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Petersen M. R., Deddens J. A. A comparison of two methods for estimating prevalence ratios. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2008;8, article 9 doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-8-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zou G. A modified poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;159(7):702–706. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Parker G., Hadzi-Pavlovic D. Is the female preponderance in major depression secondary to a gender difference in specific anxiety disorders? Psychological Medicine. 2004;34(3):461–470. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703001181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Costa P. T., Jr., Terracciano A., McCrae R. R. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81(2):322–331. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cleary P. D., Mechanic D. Sex differences in psychological distress among married people. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(2):111–121. doi: 10.2307/2136638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gove W. R., Hughes M., Style C. B. Does marriage have positive effects on the psychological well-being of the individual? Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(2):122–131. doi: 10.2307/2136639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Leach L. S., Christensen H., MacKinnon A. J. Gender differences in the endorsement of symptoms for depression and anxiety: are gender-biased items responsible? Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2008;196(2):128–135. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0b013e318162aa63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Leach L. S., Christensen H., Mackinnon A. J., Windsor T. D., Butterworth P. Gender differences in depression and anxiety across the adult lifespan: the role of psychosocial mediators. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2008;43(12):983–998. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0388-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Szklo M., Nieto F. J. Epidemiology: Beyond The Basics. 2nd. Sudbury, Mass, USA: Jones and Bartlett Publishers; 2007. [Google Scholar]