Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Dec 14.
Published in final edited form as: Eur J Public Health. 2014 Aug 26;25(1):115–121. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cku136

What is the effect of unemployment on all-cause mortality? A cohort study using propensity score matching

Tom Clemens 1, Frank Popham 2, Paul Boyle 3
PMCID: PMC4677456  EMSID: EMS66332  PMID: 25161201

Abstract

Background

There is a strong association between unemployment and mortality but whether this relationship is causal remains debated. This study utilises population level administrative data from Scotland within a propensity score framework to explore whether the association between unemployment and mortality may be causal.

Methods

The study examined a sample of working men and women aged 25 to 54 in 1991. Subsequent employment status in 2001 was observed (in work or unemployed) and the relative all-cause mortality risk of unemployment between 2001 and 2010 was estimated. To account for potential selection into unemployment of those in poor health, a propensity score matching approach was used. Matching variables were observed prior to unemployment and included health status up to the year of unemployment (hospital admissions and self-reported limiting long term illness) as well as measures of socio-economic position.

Results

Unemployment was associated with a significant all-cause mortality risk relative to employment for men (hazard ratio 1.85 95% CI 1.33-2.55). This effect was robust to controlling for prior health and socio-demographic characteristics. Effects for women were smaller and statistically insignificant (HR 1.51 95% CI 0.68-3.37).

Conclusion

For men, the findings support the notion that the often observed association between unemployment and mortality may contain a significant causal component though for women there is less support for this conclusion. However, female employment status, as recorded in the census, is more complex than for men and may have served to under-estimate any mortality effect of unemployment. Future work should examine this issue further.

Keywords: Unemployment, mortality, Scotland, causality, matching, health-selection, propensity score

Introduction

An extensive literature has documented and described the strong association between labour market disadvantage and health & mortality. (1-5). However, it remains difficult to determine if the link between unemployment and mortality is a causal relationship because poor health (health selection) is a risk factor for both unemployment and mortality. (6) Furthermore, unemployment is more likely to occur among individuals from poorer socio-economic backgrounds and it may be that the deleterious health effects associated with poverty and disadvantage prior to unemployment may be responsible for the increase in mortality risk rather than any effects caused by the unemployment itself. (6)

As labour market status cannot be randomised, most studies rely on observational data where adequate analytical control for confounding is difficult. (6) Solutions include using ‘wear-off periods’ during which mortality events are ignored for a period subsequent to baseline observation allowing health selection effects to diminish. (7) However, the effectiveness of this approach in studies of the unemployed has been questioned. (7) Alternatively, use is made of natural experiments such as, for example, instances of mass redundancies following large-scale company downsizing or collapse, (8,9) or comparison of the relationship during periods of recession with periods of economic prosperity when the prevalence of unemployment is lower. (10,11) The rationale underpinning these types of studies is that poor health is less likely (during recession / factory closure) to be the reason for job loss and is more likely to be ‘randomly distributed’ across employment groups. (8-11) In general, this body of evidence casts doubt on or lowers the effect. (12)

The importance of correctly specifying the timing of potentially confounding events relative to the unemployment event appears to have been neglected in previous studies. For example, many studies rely on self-reported health measures that are recorded simultaneously with economic activity rather than more detailed historical health data allowing adjustment for events that occur prior to unemployment. (1,2,13) The timing of health events is particularly important in order to avoid the problem of ‘over-adjustment’ for incidents of poor health that occur after unemployment which may introduce a biasing effect towards the null hypothesis. (14) In the absence of a true randomised design, this study addresses these issues by using propensity score matching to mimic the randomisation of unemployment in an observational dataset (15). In order to do so, we assume that there is minimal unmeasured confounding through the use of a longitudinal linkage study, which links between census, hospital admissions and mortality registry data to provide a large and nationally representative data sample that contains of number of contextual and potentially confounding variables. The aim of the paper is to test for a causal relationship between unemployment and mortality.

Methods

Data and sample

The Scottish Longitudinal Study (SLS) consists of linked 1991 and 2001 national census records for a 5.3% sample of the Scottish population and contains both socio-demographic and self-reported health information. Importantly, it contains information on current employment status (in both 1991 and 2001) and the length of the current spell (2001 census only). (16) For this study, linkages were made to vital events registry data, including death registrations (from National Records of Scotland vital events) and event based hospital admission records (available for the period 1980 onwards) from the Scottish Morbidity Records (SMR). The SMR records a range of information pertaining to a hospital admission including date of admission and ICD coded diagnoses. Figure one provides details of the initial sample selection which was restricted to individuals aged 25-54 in 1991 in order to capture pre-retirement age individuals in 2001 and to remove economically inactive and unemployed in 1991, those untraced at the 2001 census and cases missing information for any of the baseline variables. The resulting sample size was 29,923 for men and 22,339 for women.

Study Design

We used propensity score matching to mimic the experimental randomisation of unemployment. The ‘treatment group’ was defined as individuals who had moved from employment in 1991 to be unemployed in 2001 with the ‘control group’ defined as individuals who were in work in both 1991 and 2001. The propensity score (i.e. the predicted probability of unemployment in 2001) was estimated separately for men and women as a function of known confounders of the unemployment-mortality relationship including socio-demographic and self-reported health risk factors (from the 1991 census) and from ICD coded psychiatric & hospital admissions and cancer registrations from 1980 onwards (from the SMR). Details of these confounding variables and full sample distributions by treatment status are provided in table one. Given that unemployment was likely to have occurred before census day in 2001, socio-demographic and health variables from the 2001 census were not used to predict the propensity score in order to minimise the potential for over-adjustment for events occurring after unemployment.

Table 1. Characteristics of full samples of men and women in terms of covariates predicting unemployment in 2001 and standardised differences between 2001 unemployed and in work groups.

Covariates Men (N = 29,923)
Women (N = 22,339)
Unemployed In work Stan. Diff Unemployed In Work Stan. Diff
Health Variables (from SMR and 1991 census)
Self-reported limiting long-term illness (in 1991) %
 No 96.8 98.1 −0.083 95.9 98.4 −0.151
 Yes 3.2 1.9 −0.083 4.1 1.6 0.151
% of individuals with at least one hospital admission (from SMR) for activity limiting or disabling conditions in following disease categories during period 1980 up to 2001 or the year last worked
 2 Neoplasms 4.4 6.2 −0.080 18.5 16.2 0.061
 3 Blood and immune mechanism <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 4 Endocrine, nutritional and metabolic <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 5 Mental and behavioural 6.1 2.0 0.209 4.8 1.9 0.162
 6 Nervous system 1.6 1.3 0.025 1.4 1.7 −0.024
 7 Eye and adnexa <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model
 9 Circulatory system 2.7 3.0 −0.018 1.4 1.3 0.009
 10 Respiratory system <1 <1 <0.1 1.6 <1 <0.1
 11 Digestive system <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 14 Genitourinary system <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model
 17 Congenital conditions <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 20 External causes of morbidity 15.4 12.8 0.075 8.5 5.9 0.101
Socio-demographic variables (from 1991 census)
Housing Tenure (in 1991) %
 Owner occupied 53.6 73.8 −0.430 59.5 73.1 −0.291
 Privately Rented 6.3 5.9 0.017 6.9 4.7 0.094
 Social Housing 39.7 20.0 0.441 33.6 22.2 0.256
 Communal Establishment <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model
Educational Attainment (in 1991) %
 None 86.3 75.5 0.277 81.9 73.3 0.207
 Other higher qualifications (non-degree) 7.6 10.4 −0.098 10.1 15.6 −0.165
 First degree and higher degree 4.4 12.3 −0.289 6.0 9.3 −0.124
 Not stated 1.7 1.9 −0.015 2.1 1.8 0.022
Mean age (in 1991) 39.1 37.4 0.620 36.8 37.0 −0.086
Marital status (in 1991) %
 Married (first marriage) 60.7 73.1 −0.266 57.2 71.0 −0.291
 Single 23.8 16.8 0.175 22.7 15.2 0.192
 Remarried 8.0 5.8 0.087 7.3 5.7 0.065
 Divorced 6.9 3.9 0.133 11.7 6.9 0.166
 Widowed <1 <1 <0.1 1.1 1.3 <0.1
Deprivation quintiles (in 1991) %
 Least deprived Quintile 16 23.9 −0.199 15.6 22.0 −0.164
 2nd 22.2 29.6 −0.170 30.4 28.5 0.042
 3rd 20.6 21.6 −0.025 20.8 22.5 −0.041
 4th 22.9 15.5 0.189 19.2 16.4 0.073
 Most Deprived Quintile 18.3 9.4 0.260 14.0 10.5 0.107
Social Class (in 1991) %
 Professional Occupations 3.8 7.8 −0.172 1.4 2.5 −0.080
 Managerial and Technical Occupations 22.7 30.0 −0.166 24.3 32.8 −0.189
 Skilled Non-manual Occupations 9.1 10.6 −0.050 35.2 36.9 −0.035
 Skilled Manual Occupations 36.6 33.2 0.071 9.2 6.3 0.109
 Partly Skilled Occupations 20.9 14.0 0.183 21.5 13.4 0.215
 Unskilled Occupations 6.2 3.4 0.131 8.5 8.1 0.014
 Armed Forces <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model

To avoid low numbers, cells with very low proportions are rounded up to one.

Source: Scottish Longitudinal Study

Lynch et al. identify ICD coded conditions that are activity limiting or disabling and only these codes were used from the SMR events when predicting the propensity score. (17) Because the SMR data is event based and because the 2001 census contains information about the year of last employment for individuals who are out of work, we were able to differentiate between health events occurring before from those occurring after that year. Thus, for the unemployed, only SMR events occurring before this year were used to predict the propensity score whereas the full available records up to 2001 were used for individuals who were working in 2001. Aggregation of the hospital admission information is detailed in Figure one.

Figure 1. Sample selection criteria (corresponding sample size in brackets) and outline of covariates used to estimate the propensity score for unemployment in 2001.

Figure 1

The propensity score was then used to pair unemployed with in work individuals to form the matched sample. A number of matching algorithms exist to do this but simulation studies have suggested that matching one treatment case with the closest single control case (rather than two or three) optimises the trade-off between bias reduction and sampling variability. (18) Once matched, control cases were removed from the ‘pot’ to prevent them being matched to more than one treated case. In order to ensure that all matched pairs were adequately similar, an additional restriction, known as caliper matching, was imposed to ensure that the propensity scores of control cases lay within an interval of 0.01 of the propensity score of their matched treated case. All treated cases without an appropriate matched control were excluded from the matched sample along with all of the control cases that were not required to provide a match. To assess whether the matched sample was balanced, distributions of the variables used to predict the propensity score were compared. Balance across variables was assessed using standardised differences which calculate differences in the prevalence of each level of each variable in units of the pooled standard deviation. Standardised differences of roughly 0.1 or less are considered negligible for the purposes of determining balance. (19,20)

To determine the sensitivity of the results when using different propensity score approaches, mortality risks were also calculated using the sub-classification method. This involves calculating quintiles of the entire propensity score distribution and estimating mortality hazard ratios separately within each of these quintiles, which in effect calculates risks separately amongst individuals with similar probability of unemployment. (21) These estimates are reported individually and then combined and weighted appropriately to produce an overall effect. (22) The results from both approaches were compared.

The period of mortality follow-up started from the 2001 census day (29th April 2001) to the end of 2010. Embarkations from the study during the follow-up period due to migration were identified and censored. Cox proportional hazards models were used to estimate the relative mortality risk of unemployment and were conducted separately for men and women.

Results

4% and just under 2.5% of men and women respectively in the initial sample were unemployed in 2001. Death rates in the follow-up period (2001-2010) for unemployed and in work amongst men were 9% and 3.7% and amongst women 3.4% and 2.3% respectively. Means and proportions of the variables used to predict the propensity score and standardised differences between the in-work and those unemployed were examined (Table 1). For both men and women, imbalances (standardised difference > +/− 0.1) were noted for all of the socio-demographic variables and, in terms of health, for hospitalisation for mental and behavioural illness. The degree of imbalance is also illustrated in Figure one of the supplementary material and shows that, in terms of the distribution of the overall propensity scores, the unemployed and the in work were relatively similar for both men and women.

Standardised differences for all variables were less than 0.1 for men in the matched sample indicating balance with respect to these variables (table 2). For women, the degree of balance was broadly similar although mean age was slightly higher in the control group. As the direction of this small difference was likely to result in only a fractional increase in bias towards the null hypothesis, it was considered ignorable. Thus, within the matched sample, the transition into unemployment in 2001 was considered independent of these variables.

Table 2. Characteristics of case matched samples of men and women in terms of covariates predicting unemployment in 2001 and standardised differences between 2001 unemployed and in work groups.

Covariates Men
Women
Unemployed In work Stan. Diff Unemployed In Work Stan. Diff
Health Variables (from SMR and 1991 census)
Self-reported limiting long-term illness (in 1991) %
 No 96.8 97.3 −0.030 95.9 96.8 −0.048
 Yes 3.2 2.7 0.030 4.1 3.2 −0.048
% of individuals with at least one hospital admission (from SMR) for activity limiting or disabling conditions in following disease categories during period 1980 up to 2001 or the year last worked
 2 Neoplasms 4.4 3.7 0.036 18.5 19.7 −0.031
 3 Blood and immune mechanism <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 4 Endocrine, nutritional and metabolic <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 5 Mental and behavioural 5.9 4.7 0.054 4.8 5.5 −0.032
 6 Nervous system 1.6 1.1 0.043 1.4 <1 <0.1
 7 Eye and adnexa <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model
 9 Circulatory system 2.7 2.7 0.000 1.4 1.2 0.018
 10 Respiratory system <1 <1 <0.1 1.6 1.1 0.043
 11 Digestive system <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 14 Genitourinary system <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model
 17 Congenital conditions <1 <1 <0.1 <1 <1 <0.1
 20 External causes of morbidity and mortality 15.4 14.8 0.017 8.5 6.9 0.060
Socio-demographic variables (from 1991 census)
Housing Tenure (in 1991) %
 Owner occupied 53.7 52.7 0.020 59.5 58.8 0.014
 Privately Rented 6.3 4.5 0.080 6.9 6.0 0.037
 Social Housing 39.6 42.9 −0.067 33.6 35.2 −0.034
 Communal Establishment <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model
Educational Attainment (in 1991) %
 None 86.3 87.8 −0.045 81.9 83.3 −0.037
 Other higher qualifications (non-degree) 7.6 7.3 0.011 10.1 8.7 0.048
 First degree and higher degree 4.4 3.6 0.041 6.0 6.0 0.000
 Not stated 1.7 1.3 0.033 2.1 2.1 0.000
Mean age (in 1991) 39.1 39.1 0.02 36.8 37.0 0.102
Marital status (in 1991) %
 Married (first marriage) 60.8 62.5 −0.035 57.2 59.5 −0.047
 Single 23.6 22.7 0.021 22.7 22.7 0.000
 Remarried 8.0 8.0 0.000 7.3 7.8 −0.019
 Divorced 6.9 5.8 0.045 11.7 9.4 0.075
 Widowed <1 1.1 <0.1 1.1 <1 <0.1
Deprivation quintiles (in 1991) %
 Least deprived Quintile 16.0 15.4 0.016 15.6 13.5 0.060
 2nd 22.2 22.0 0.005 30.4 31.1 −0.015
  3rd 20.7 19.8 0.022 20.8 20.6 0.005
 4th 22.9 22.7 0.005 19.2 19.5 −0.008
 Most Deprived Quintile 18.2 20.1 −0.048 14.0 15.3 −0.037
Social Class (in 1991) %
 Professional Occupations 3.8 3.2 0.033 1.4 1.8 −0.032
 Managerial and Technical Occupations 22.7 23.2 −0.012 24.3 23.3 0.023
 Skilled Non-manual Occupations 9.1 8.7 0.014 35.2 36.6 −0.029
 Skilled Manual Occupations 36.7 36.7 0.000 9.2 8.5 0.025
 Partly Skilled Occupations 20.9 21.7 −0.020 21.5 22.9 −0.034
 Unskilled Occupations 6.0 6.1 −0/004 8.5 6.9 0.060
 Armed Forces <1 <1 <0.1 Not in model

To avoid low numbers, cells with very low proportions are rounded up to one.

Source: Scottish Longitudinal Study

Table two shows results from the mortality follow-up analyses, estimated from both the full sample sub-classification approach and the restricted case matched sample. A weighted average of the unadjusted sub-class estimates showed effects (2.55 for men and 1.53 for women) which were considerably higher than those acquired from the case matched sample for men though similar for women (1.85 for men and 1.51 for women). The adjusted coefficients in the sub-classification models were more comparable to the matched sample analysis which indicated either potential residual confounding in the unadjusted subclass models (particularly for men) or the fact that stratification on the propensity score (without adjustment) often results in estimates biased away from the null hypothesis in analyses of time-to-event outcomes. (23) All of the coefficients for men were statistically significant (p < 0.01) and showed at least an 85% excess mortality risk in the period 2001-2010 for the unemployed relative to those in work in 2001. For women, the findings suggested a 50% increase in the risk of mortality but none of these are significant at p<0.05 or p<0.01.

Discussion

Main findings

This study examined the effect of unemployment for mortality with an analysis which attempted to mimic a randomised experiment and captured the timing of confounding effects through the use of observational longitudinal data. After matching based on health and other confounding variables the findings showed an 85% and 50% increase in the risk of mortality for men and women respectively who were registered as unemployed ten years from baseline compared to those who remained in employment. Although the effect for men was statistically significant (p<0.01) the effect for women was not.

Limitations

There are limitations with the analysis. If there were unmeasured differences between the unemployed and in work that also relate to mortality, our effect estimate will not be free of bias as the matching approach will not be able to take account of the unmeasured confounding. Given that that the study was able to control for a wide range of known confounders it could be argued that considerable residual confounding was less likely but this remains a possibility.

For those who were unemployed in 2001, the year in which the unemployment spell began was used as the censoring variable for hospital admissions with any events occurring after this date considered as possible outcomes of unemployment and ignored. However, for those who were in work in 2001, hospital admissions information for the entire period between 1980 and 2001 were included in the analysis. This may give the appearance of a comparatively higher prevalence of serious health events in some members of the in work group as a result of a lengthier ‘at risk’ period and therefore is a possible source of bias.

The use of census data to capture labour market participation presents two important limitations. Firstly, it represents a snapshot of the population on that particular census day and will contain both short and long term unemployed but with an oversampling of the latter. (24) Further over-sampling of the long term unemployed would be expected due to the period of economic boom in 2001 when unemployment was identified. In our sample of unemployed, 31% and 36% of men and women respectively had worked within 16 weeks of the census date (29th April 2001) and 64% and 67% within 64weeks. This left 36% and 33% of the samples in a spell of unemployment longer than 64 weeks. Compared to national labour market statistics, these figures appear to confirm under-representation of the short-term unemployed. (25) The inherent difference and the implications for subsequent effect estimates of census based measures of exposure compared to exposure based on length and number of spells has been reflected on in more detail previously elsewhere. (24)

One possible effect of using unemployment measured on one particular day as a measure of exposure might have been to underestimate subsequent hazard ratios. The control group of unexposed might have contained a large number of individuals who experienced previous spells of unemployment and the mixing of exposed and unexposed individuals in the control group in this way is likely to have artificially diluted the resulting effect estimates. It would be of interest in future studies to explore the impact of length of unemployment on mortality.

Secondly, it is widely asserted that female labour market participation differs compared to men in terms of reduced labour market attachment and greater involvement in household responsibilities such as looking after a family. (5,26-29) In census data this may lead to underestimation of the level of unemployment amongst women who may not as readily acknowledge themselves as unemployed in the census compared to men and may instead choose alternative census categorisations such as “looking after home” or “other”. (30) As a result, the lower sample unemployment rate that is shown in table one for women may in fact hide the true level of unemployment in the sample which excludes these alternative categorisations. This introduces two possible problems. Firstly, the significantly lower numbers of unemployed women means that the hazard ratios were estimated with far less precision than those for men making it harder to reject the null hypothesis. Secondly, our hazard ratios could have been biased upwards or downwards because it is difficult to determine whether or not this misclassification was greater amongst women who were more or less vulnerable to the health effects of unemployment. Finally, a general limitation of the study is the relatively small sample size which precluded analysis of cause specific mortality.

Interpretation

Though initial studies on the subject tended to support the theory that unemployment is independently related to mortality they were often lacking adequate control for health. (2) Conversely, studies using both quasi-experimental methods & natural experiments and those with direct control for health have found less evidence for, or have downplayed, the effect size. (8,11,12) A meta-analysis of the unemployment and mortality literature found average ‘age and additional covariate adjusted’ mortality hazard ratios that were similar to our findings at 1.78 and 1.37 for unemployed or out of work men and women respectively. (5) However, a direct comparison with these average values is difficult as it obscures considerable heterogeneity between studies in terms of research design, availability of adjustment covariates and coding of unemployment status. For example, studies that consider all out of work individuals reported hazard ratios that were around 50% higher compared to studies who restricted their analysis to individuals actively seeking work. Similarly, studies that did not adjust for age were around 16% higher compared to those that did and those that adjust for more than one measure of socio-economic status were reduced by 13% when compared to studies with only one or no measure of socio-economic status. Given, that the present study adjusts for age as well as a range of both individual and area socio-economic status variables and uses a well-defined measure of unemployment which excludes the economically inactive, we might have expected the effects sizes to be considerably smaller than the average sizes observed in this meta-analysis.

There are other features of our study (not considered in the meta-analysis) which might also lead us to expect, a priori, more conservative effect sizes. For example, our baseline sample was observed in 1991 during a period of recession, when health related selection has been suggested to be less likely. (10,11) Furthermore, the analysis was restricted to individuals who were in employment in 1991 which is likely to result, instead, in the selection of individuals who are relatively advantaged in terms of labour market success. This is due to the fact that they have managed to retain employment at a time when the overall unemployment rate and therefore likelihood of unemployment is higher.

One possible explanation for the higher than expected effect sizes observed in this study could be that many previous studies may have miss-specified confounding effects by ignoring the timing of them relative to unemployment. For example, intermediate events that occur after unemployment are unlikely to have caused that unemployment and adjusting for them as if they are confounding rather than mediating effects is likely to result in a bias towards the null hypothesis. (14) Longitudinal data combined with information about when an individual was last in work is therefore an important feature of this study. Another possible explanation is that the effect of unemployment varies between countries, perhaps reflecting differences in the extent of state or welfare support. (31,32) The UK is traditionally less generous in its provision of welfare state support when compared to, for example, many Scandinavian countries and this may also contribute to a worse health effect of unemployment. (33)

The findings for women cannot be interpreted as straightforwardly as for men. On the one hand the null findings for women may indicate that women suffer less from the negative effects of unemployment when compared to men. Support for this explanation can be found in work that argues that women are less tied to work and income generation (34-36) as well as meta-analysis evidence that highlights a consistently higher risk of mortality associated with unemployment for men than for women. However, in contrast, qualitative evidence suggests that women suffer similar feelings of isolation, loneliness and boredom during unemployment casting doubt on the notion that women are less affected. (37) Moreover, other evidence points to the fact that women’s participation in the labour market has and continues to change rapidly to the point that comparisons between men and women show increasingly less marked differences. (38) This evidence, in conjunction with the limitations associated with using census data to capture female labour market participation may cast doubt on the notion that women are less susceptible to the effects of unemployment than men. In light of these changing patterns, future work should continue to focus on the relatively neglected question of the health effects of labour market position amongst women, perhaps through the use of data, where available, that better captures the details and complexities of women’s labour market participation.

Conclusion

This study provides strong evidence that, for men at least, unemployment is independently associated with an elevated all-cause mortality risk. To date, it is the only study of unemployment and mortality in the UK that has utilised information about hospital admissions prior to unemployment to adjust for health selection rather than relying solely on census based self-reported health measures.

Supplementary Material

Suppl

Table 3. Mortality risks of unemployment relative to employment during follow-up period 2001-2010.

Sample Men Women

Sub-classification on the propensity score across full sample Cox hazard ratio (C.I. < .05)
Unadjusted models Adjusted models Unadjusted models Adjusted models
  Quintile 1 2.91* (0.92-9.14) 2.37ns (0.74-7.60) 1.53ns (0.21-11.02) 1.49ns (0.20-10.88)
  Quintile 2 1.95* (0.92-4.16) 1.89ns (0.88-4.06) 0.70ns (0.10-5.03) 0.59ns (0.08-4.27)
  Quintile 3 2.64*** (1.70-4.09) 2.48*** (1.58-3.87) 0.56ns (0 .08-4.05) 0.44ns (0.06-3.22)
  Quintile 4 1.60** (1.02-2.53) 1.63** (1.03-2.58) 1.36ns (0.43-4.28) 1.30ns (0.41-4.12)
  Quintile 5 1 89*** (1.40-2.55) 1 92*** (1.41-2.60) 2.15** (1.09-4.24) 2.14** (1.06-4.32)
  Weighted average effect estimate 2.55*** (2.08-3.12) 1 97*** (1.60-2.42) 1.53ns (0.91-2.55) 1.41ns (0.84-2.37)

Case matched restricted sample (no adjusted models) 1.85*** (1.33-2.55) 1.51ns (0.68-3.37)

ns (not significant)

*

(p<.10)

**

(p<.05)

***

(p<.01)

Hazard ratios show the mortality effect of unemployment relative to being in work. Unadjusted models contain no additional adjustment and adjusted models include adjustment for all of the covariates in tables two and three that were used to predict the propensity score.

Source: Scottish Longitudinal Study

Key points.

  • Unemployment is strongly associated with mortality, however, prior health and other characteristics can confound the association casting doubt on the extent to which the association is evidence of a causal pathway.

  • Using a novel research design with a focus on the timing of confounding effects relative to unemployment, this study finds a strong and significant excess risk of mortality associated with unemployment.

  • The study extends previous observational evidence and, assuming that there is minimal unobserved confounding, suggests support for a causal explanation for the association between unemployment and mortality.

Acknowledgements

The help provided by staff of the Longitudinal Studies Centre - Scotland (LSCS) is acknowledged. The LSCS is supported by the ESRC/JISC, the Scottish Funding Council, the Chief Scientist’s Office and the Scottish Government. The authors alone are responsible for the interpretation of the data. Census output is Crown copyright and is reproduced with the permission of the Controller of HMSO and the Queen’s Printer for Scotland. Tom Clemens was funded by the ESRC (PTA-031-2006-00514) during completion of this work and Frank Popham is funded by the Medical Research Council (MC_A540_5TK10).

Funding: Economic and Social Research Council Studentship for Tom Clemens’ PhD.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Statement

None declared.

Contributor Information

Tom Clemens, School of GeoSciences, The University of Edinburgh, Institute of Geography, Drummond Street, Edinburgh EH8 9XP.

Frank Popham, MRC/CSO Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow, 4 Lilybank Gardens, Glasgow, G12 8RZ.

Paul Boyle, School of Geography & Geosciences, Irvine Building, University of St Andrews, North Street, St Andrews, Fife, Scotland, KY16 9AL.

References

  • 1.Iversen L, Andersen O, Andersen PK, Christoffersen K, Keiding N. Unemployment and mortality in Denmark, 1970-80. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 1987;295(6603):879–84. doi: 10.1136/bmj.295.6603.879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Moser KA, Goldblatt PO, Fox AJ, Jones DR. Unemployment and mortality: comparison of the 1971 and 1981 longitudinal study census samples. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 1987;294(6564):86–90. doi: 10.1136/bmj.294.6564.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Martikainen PT. Unemployment and mortality among Finnish men, 1981-5. Bmj. 1990;301(6749):407–11. doi: 10.1136/bmj.301.6749.407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bethune A. Unemployment and mortality. In: Drever F, Whitehead M, editors. Health inequalities. Office for national statistics; London: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Roelfs DJ, Shor E, Davidson KW, Schwartz JE. Losing life and livelihood: A systematic review and meta-analysis of unemployment and all-cause mortality. Social Science & Medicine. 2011;72(6):840–54. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bartley M, Ferrie J, Montgomery SM. Health and labour market disadvantage: unemployment, non-employment, and job insecurity. In: Marmot M, Wilkinson RG, editors. Social Determinants of Health. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2006. pp. 78–96. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Clemens T, Boyle P, Popham F. Unemployment, mortality and the problem of healthrelated selection: Evidence from the Scottish and England & Wales (ONS) Longitudinal Studies. Hsq. 2009;43(1):7–13. doi: 10.1057/hsq.2009.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Steenland K, Pinkerton LE. Mortality Patterns following Downsizing at Pan American World Airways. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(1):1–6. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Martikainen P, Maki N, Jantti M. The effects of unemployment on mortality following workplace downsizing and workplace closure: a register-based follow-up study of Finnish men and women during economic boom and recession. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;165(9) doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Martikainen PT, Valkonen T. The effects of differential unemployment rate increases of occupation groups on changes in mortality. American Journal of Public Health. 1998;88(12):1859–61. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.12.1859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Martikainen PT, Valkonen T. Excess mortality of unemployed men and women during a period of rapidly increasing unemployment. Lancet. 1996;348(9032):909–12. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)03291-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lundin A, Lundberg I, Hallsten L, Ottosson J, Hemmingsson T. Unemployment and mortality—a longitudinal prospective study on selection and causation in 49321 Swedish middle-aged men. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2010;64(01) doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.079269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gerdtham UG, Johannesson M. Absolute income, relative income, income inequality, and mortality. Journal of Human Resources. 2004;39(1) [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Schisterman EF, Cole SR, Platt RW. Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass) 2009;20(4) doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181a819a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Austin PC. An Introduction to Propensity Score Methods for Reducing the Effects of Confounding in Observational Studies. Multivariate Behav Res. 2011 May;46(3):399–424. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2011.568786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Boyle PJ, Feijten P, Feng Z, Hattersley L, Huang Z, Nolan J, et al. Cohort Profile: The Scottish Longitudinal Study (SLS) International journal of epidemiology. 2009;38(2) doi: 10.1093/ije/dyn087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lynch C, Holman CDJ, Moorin RE. Use of Western Australian linked hospital morbidity and mortality data to explore theories of compression, expansion and dynamic equilibrium. Australian Health Review. 2007;31(4) doi: 10.1071/ah070571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Austin PC. Statistical Criteria for Selecting the Optimal Number of Untreated Subjects Matched to Each Treated Subject When Using Many-to-One Matching on the Propensity Score. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;172:1092–7. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Normand S-LT, Landrum MB, Guadagnoli E, Ayanian JZ, Ryan TJ, Cleary PD, et al. Validating recommendations for coronary angiography following acute myocardial infarction in the elderly: a matched analysis using propensity scores. Journal of clinical epidemiology. 2001;54(4):387–98. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00321-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Austin PC. Balance diagnostics for comparing the distribution of baseline covariates between treatment groups in propensity-score matched samples. Statistics in Medicine. 2009;28(25):3083–107. doi: 10.1002/sim.3697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB. Reducing bias in observational studies using subclassification on the propensity score. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1984:516–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Wiley; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Austin PC. The performance of different propensity score methods for estimating marginal hazard ratios. Statistics in Medicine. 2013;32(16):2837–49. doi: 10.1002/sim.5705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bartley M, Ferrie J. Do we need to worry about the health effects of unemployment? Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2010;64(01) doi: 10.1136/jech.2009.089797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Begum N. Characteristics of the short-term and long-term unemployed. Labour market trends. 2004;112(4):139–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Weatherall R, Joshi H, Macran S. Double burden or double blessing? Employment, motherhood and mortality in the longitudinal study of England and Wales. Social Science & Medicine. 1994;38(2):285–97. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90398-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rubery J, Fagan C, Maier F. Occupational Segregation, Discrimination and Equal Opportunity. In: Schimd G, O’Reilly J, Schomann K, editors. International handbook of labour market policy and evaluation. Edward Elgar Publishing; Cheltenham: 1996. pp. 431–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gonzalo MT, Saarela J. Gender differences in exit rates from unemployment: evidence from a local Finnish labour market. Finnish Economic Papers. 2000;13(2):129–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bivand P. Who are the “economically inactive”. Centre for Economic and Social Inclusion; London: 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Burström B, Holland P, Diderichsen F, Whitehead M. Winners and losers in flexible labor markets: the fate of women with chronic illness in contrasting policy environments--Sweden and Britain. International Journal of Health Services. 2003;33(2):199–217. doi: 10.2190/UTC5-P2FJ-BTBA-0E3V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bambra C. Work, Worklessness, and the Political Economy of Health. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bambra C, Eikemo TA. Welfare state regimes, unemployment and health: a comparative study of the relationship between unemployment and self-reported health in 23 European countries. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2009;63(2):92–8. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.077354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Esping-Andersen G. The three worlds of welfare capitalism. Polity Pr. 1990 [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hakim C. Grateful slaves and self-made women: fact and fantasy in women’s work orientations. European Sociological Review. 1991;7(2):101–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Paul K, Moser K. Unemployment impairs mental health: Meta-analyses. Journal of Vocational Behavior. 2009;74(3):264–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Strandh M, Hammarström A, Nilsson K, Nordenmark M, Russel H. Unemployment, gender and mental health: the role of the gender regime. Sociology of Health & Illness [Internet] 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2012.01517.x. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9566.2012.01517.x. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 37.Grant L, Price C, Buckner L. Connecting Women With the Labour Market. Centre for Social Inclusion, Sheffield Hallam University; Sheffield: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Beatty C, Fothergill S, Houston D, Powell R, Sissons P. A gendered theory of employment, unemployment, and sickness. Environment and Planning C: Government & Policy. 2009;27(6):958–74. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Suppl

RESOURCES