Abstract
Working memory (WM) is strongly influenced by attention. In visual working-memory tasks, recall performance can be improved by an attention-guiding cue presented before encoding (precue) or during maintenance (retrocue). Although precues and retrocues recruit a similar fronto-parietal control network, the two are likely to exhibit some processing differences, since precues invite anticipation of upcoming information, while retrocues may guide prioritisation, protection, and selection of information already in mind. Here we explored the behavioral and electrophysiological differences between precueing and retrocueing in a new visual working-memory task designed to permit a direct comparison between cueing conditions. We found marked differences in event-related potential (ERP) profiles between the precue and retrocue conditions. In line with precues primarily generating an anticipatory shift of attention toward the location of an upcoming item, we found a robust lateralization in late cue-evoked potentials associated with target anticipation. Retrocues elicited a different pattern of ERPs that was compatible with an early selection mechanism, but not with stimulus anticipation. In contrast to the distinct ERP patterns, alpha band (8-14 Hz) lateralization was indistinguishable between cue types (reflecting, in both conditions, the location of the cued item). We speculate that whereas alpha-band lateralization after a precue is likely to enable anticipatory attention, lateralization after a retrocue may instead enable the controlled spatiotopic access to recently encoded visual information.
Keywords: Attention: Visual, Memory: working memory, Alpha Oscillations, EEG, Event-related potential
Introduction
Visual working memory (WM) is known to be malleable by selective attention (Oberauer, 2002; Awh et al., 2006; Postle, 2006; Gazzaley and Nobre, 2011; Nobre and Stokes, 2011). In partial-report paradigms, the presentation of a selective cue preceding the onset of the memory array (‘precue’) effectively reduces memory load, leading to improved performance. Importantly, selective cues presented after encoding ('retrocues', Griffin and Nobre, 2003; Landman et al., 2003; Pertzov et al., 2012), during the maintenance delay, can also improve performance. As with precues, retrocues increase the probability of accurate recall (Murray et al., 2011; 2013; Williams et al., 2012), possibly by shifting the cued item into a more robust representational format (Sligte et al., 2008; Oberauer, 2013; Rerko and Oberauer, 2013; Rerko et al., 2014). Prioritization in WM may rely on similar mechanisms as in anticipatory attention (Chun, 2011, Chun et al., 2011). In order to chart the possible commonalities and differences in anticipatory and retrospective shifts of attention, we compared the dynamics of spatial attention mechanisms triggered by precues and retrocues using electroencephalography (EEG).
Research on the neural effects of anticipatory spatial cues has revealed a number of event-related potentials (ERPs) that reflect the orienting of attention in space. Broadly, they have been grouped into early potentials related to processing the cue and shifting attentional focus (see Methods for an introduction of the early potentials used in this study), and later potentials related to anticipation. Among later potentials (beyond 500 ms after cue onset), the late directing attention-related positivity (or LDAP, Harter et al., 1989; Hopf and Mangun, 2000; Eimer et al., 2003; Murray et al., 2011) has been linked to anticipatory processing of the upcoming task-relevant stimulus. Similarly, the lateralization of alpha-band oscillations has been hypothesized to regulate visual excitability in specific regions of cortex so that anticipated information at an attended location can receive prioritized processing (Foxe et al., 1998; Worden et al., 2000; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010, but see Pfurtscheller et al., 1996). One possibility is that directed alpha-band lateralization leads to the preferential routing of attended visual input into higher-level areas through changes in excitability of the underlying neural population (Jensen et al., 2012).
Compared to anticipatory cueing, the neural consequences of retrocueing are much less explored. The behavioral effects of retrocues are generally of comparable magnitude as those of precues, and retrocues activate a similar fronto-parietal control network (Nobre et al., 2004; Chun et al., 2011). Moreover, retrocues share some of the early ERP markers of precues (Griffin and Nobre, 2003), and retrospective cues have been shown to modulate activity in visual regions involved in the encoding of WM information (Lepsien and Nobre, 2006; Sligte et al., 2009; Kuo et al., 2011; Munneke et al., 2012; Kuo et al., 2014). Nevertheless, there may still be substantial differences in the attention-related neural mechanisms triggered by retrocues compared to precues.
In contrast to the effects on control networks and ERPs, the effect of retrocues on neural oscillations, and on posterior alpha oscillations in particular, is unknown. During WM maintenance, a number of studies have shown posterior alpha power increases (Klimesch et al., 1999; Jensen et al., 2002; Haegens et al., 2010; Spitzer and Blankenburg, 2012). The increase in posterior alpha power during WM delays has been interpreted as an active inhibitory process designed to limit interference from distracting visual information (Sauseng et al., 2009; Bonnefond and Jensen, 2012). Anticipatory cueing studies typically require the selection of a visual stimulus. In contrast, studies finding WM-related alpha-power increases either presented non-visual material for rehearsal (such as vibrotactile stimuli, which do not require processing in visual cortex, Haegens et al., 2010; Spitzer and Blankenburg, 2012), or they employed a verbal WM task (Sternberg, 1966), which requires the rehearsal of letter strings. In the latter task, once visual information has been transformed into a phonological code, visual cortex presumably becomes task-irrelevant. It is unclear whether alpha power also increases in a task encouraging storage of information as a visual code, and how retrocues would then modulate alpha power.
We were interested in testing whether comparing the oscillatory responses to anticipatory and retrospective shifts of attention can arbitrate between different models of how retrocues improve WM. The improvement could be achieved, (1) by maintaining spatial attention towards the cued location, possibly in anticipation of the probe. In this case, retrospective cues would lead to a contralateral decrease in alpha power, similar to what is observed after an anticipatory cue and possibly reflecting probe anticipation. Alternatively, (2) it could arise through protection from interference, by inhibiting currently irrelevant locations. This should lead to a relative increase in contralateral alpha power in order to limit interference. Finally, (3) transient access to the cued item and recoding into a preferred representational format (such as the ‘focus of attention’, see Oberauer, 2013, Cowan, 2000) may be the cause of retrocueing benefits. In this case, a temporary decrease in alpha power might suffice to allow access to areas storing cued information before it is transformed into a different representational state.
We examined this question using a novel task design that allowed us to address these possibilities. In the past, task design has complicated direct comparisons of the neural mechanisms involved in precue and retrocue processing, since many studies have not controlled for some important nuisance factors. For one, precues have tended to appear earlier in the trial sequence (before presentation of the memory array) than retrocues (after presentation of the memory array, see Griffin and Nobre, 2003; Nobre et al., 2004). Since the two cue types have appeared at different times within a trial sequence, serial position in the trial complicates the interpretation of any differences. In addition to aiding retrieval, retrocues may have conferred a behavioral advantage by increasing anticipatory attention towards the probe. This effect could contribute to the activity increase in fronto-parietal control regions that is seen both in precueing and retrocueing. In the current study, we adapted the task design to compare modulatory mechanisms evoked by precues and retrocues. The current design equates all aspects of stimulus presentation between cueing conditions, so that only the nature of the attention shift (anticipatory or retrospective) differs between cue types. A spatial cue, presented at the same serial position in every trial, could increase the relevance of either, (a), one of two items presented in a previous array (in the retrocue condition), or, (b), one of two memory items from an upcoming second array (precue).
We analyzed ERPs linked to attentional selection (see Methods) and to sustained anticipatory attention (LDAP). When target information was already accessible to attentional selection (i.e., after retrocues, but not precues), we examined early potentials linked to attentional target selection and alpha-band lateralisation in response to the different cue types. We predicted that early potentials might be similar for prospective and retrospective cues, reflecting attentional orienting to a cued spatial location. In contrast, we predicted that the LDAP and alpha lateralization would show differences between conditions, since they have been associated primarily with stimulus anticipation. Prospective cues, where relevant information is still forthcoming, might lead to a stronger LDAP and alpha lateralization than retrospective cues, where relevant information has already been retrieved and must be protected from upcoming interference. We hoped to use these results to inform different theories of prioritization in WM, and about the possible functions of alpha oscillations more generally.
Our results revealed some unexpected effects, shedding light on common as well as distinct mechanisms of anticipatory and retrospective attention shifts. We found ERP responses to be largely in line with our hypotheses: We observed early lateralized ERP responses to both cue types, while an EEG marker of anticipatory attention (LDAP) robustly differentiated between retro- and prospective cues. In addition, early stimulus-evoked processing (P1) was attenuated after a retrocue and amplified after a precue, especially contralateral to the cued side. Surprisingly, this occurred even though the spatiotopic response of alpha synchronization was identical in anticipatory and retrospective attention to WM items. The unexpected alpha-band response to retrocues favor the interpretation that alpha-band lateralization during retrocueing may reflect access to spatiotopically stored information, rather than perceptual anticipatory attention.
Methods
Participants
We tested 24 healthy volunteers between the ages of 18 and 35 on a sequential VSTM task (Fig. 1a). Data from six participants contained too many eye movements or blinks to be analyzed (more than 50% of trials), leaving data from 18 participants (age range 19-33 years, 11 female) in the final analysis.
Figure 1.
Task Design and Behavioral Results. (A) Example Trial Structure. Each trial began with the onset of a fixation cross, followed by the display of a memory array consisting of two oriented bars. Participants remembered the orientation of each bar. After a 800 ms delay, a cue appeared. Informative cues could point to a quadrant that had contained an item in the first array (retrocues), or to a previously unoccupied quadrant (precues). The cued item was probed in 70% of trials. Uninformative (neutral) cues gave no information about which item would be cued. Following the cue, a second array appeared, with two items in previously unoccupied quadrants. After a further delay, a probe stimulus appeared at the location of one of the previous stimuli. The probe appeared at the same location, but was rotated clockwise or counter-clockwise (relative to the probed memory item). The magnitude of rotation changed from trial to trial and ranged from 2 to 39°. Participants judged the direction of rotation, and received visual feedback in the form of a green (correct response) or red (incorrect) fixation dot during the inter-trial interval (not shown). (B) Behavioral Performance. Each panel shows the proportion of clockwise responses as a function of the magnitude and direction of the relative probe rotation (angular change), separately for valid, neutral, and invalid cues (the invalid condition corresponds to trials probing an uncued item from the respective array, irrespective of whether the cue pointed to the other item in the same array, or one of the items in the other array). When array 1 was probed (retrocue trials, left panel), valid cues significantly improved accuracy, primarily by reducing guess rate (i.e. the offset between the asymptote of the sigmoid response function and 0 or 1). Memory precision (i.e., the slope of the function) was not significantly affected by cue validity. Precue trials (right panel) showed better performance overall, but the same effect of cue validity. Here, costs of invalid cues are also visible. Both plots show mean responses across all 18 observers. Error bars indicate s.e.m.
Precision Working-Memory Task
We collected EEG data while participants performed a precision visual working-memory task (see Bays and Husain, 2008). The task was modified to include the presentation of a spatial cue in between two serially presented memory arrays (see Fig. 1a). Participants viewed two arrays, each containing two peripherally presented oriented bars, separated by a spatial or neutral cue, and were subsequently required to judge whether a probe appearing at a previously occupied location was rotated clockwise or counter-clockwise. Each trial contained a total of four memory items, each presented in a separate quadrant of the screen. Spatial retrocues (towards a location in the first array) and precues (towards a location in the upcoming second array) indicated the item that was likely to be probed (with a validity of 70%). On invalidly cued trials, one of the other three items was probed (each with 33% likelihood).
Each trial began with a fixation period (400 ms), followed by memory array 1 (presented for 200 ms). Each memory array consisted of two oriented bars (length 1.5° visual angle, width 0.2°, with a small disc of 0.6° diameter at the bar’s center to denote the point of rotation). In each array, stimuli were centered in diagonally opposite quadrants of the screen (i.e., either the top left and bottom right quadrants, or the top right and bottom left quadrants), at 5° eccentricity from fixation. After an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 800 ms, a cue appeared (200 ms). Neutral cues (a dark gray square of side length 0.4° at fixation) were uninformative about which item would be probed. Spatial cues (a thin white border of 0.1° along two adjacent edges of the gray cue square, forming an arrow pointing to one of the four quadrants) indicated the location of the item that would be probed (with a validity of 70%). If the cued item had already been presented in array 1, the cue acted as a retrocue. Alternatively, the cue acted as a precue if it indicated an item to be presented in array 2. Memory array 2 appeared (also for 200 ms) after cue offset and another ISI of 800 ms. To prevent spatial overlap across arrays, the two stimuli were presented in the two quadrants that were unoccupied in array 1. After a further ISI of 800 ms, a probe stimulus appeared (for 200 ms) at the same location as one of the four memory items. The probe was an oriented bar similar to the item at that location, but rotated relative to the original orientation. Participants made a binary response indicating whether the probe was rotated clockwise or counter-clockwise, relative to the item at the probed location. We varied the rotation angle between memory and probe angles between 2° and 39°. Rotation angles were binned into three categories: easy (rotation angles uniformly distributed between 21 and 39°), medium (11-21°), and hard (2-11°). The bins of the small and medium angles were narrower than the large angle bin to improve estimation of memory precision (see Behavioral Analysis) and to encourage participants to remember stimuli as accurately as possible. Responses were made using a mouse with a trackball that could be rotated clockwise or counter-clockwise. Participants were explicitly encouraged to make use of cues to improve their accuracy. Reaction time was not emphasized. Inter-trial intervals were uniformly jittered between 1.25 and 1.75 seconds, with an average duration of 1.5 seconds.
Participants completed the experiment in one session after two practice blocks. The task consisted of ten 7-minute blocks, each comprising 64 trials, for a total of 640 trials. Of these, 400 trials were cued (200 retrocue, 200 precue), and 240 trials were neutral (with 120 probing array 1, and 120 probing array 2), meaning that a cue was presented on 62.5% of trials. Of the 400 cued trials, 280 trials (140 retrocue, 140 precue) were validly cued. Participants sat in a dimly lit booth at a distance of 74 cm from the monitor (22 inch Samsung SyncMaster 2233; resolution: 1680 * 1050 pixels; refresh rate: 60Hz; screen width: 47cm). A chin rest was used to help participants keep their heads still. Binocular gaze locations were continuously recorded with a desktop mount video-based eye tracker at 500 Hz (EyeLink 1000, SR Research, Ontario, Canada), allowing for offline detection of blinks and saccades. All trials in which gaze deviated from the central fixation dot by more than 1.5° were discarded. Participants were instructed to fixate on the center of the screen at all times and not to blink during trials. Eyetracker data from three participants were too noisy for analysis. In these cases we relied on the electro-oculogram data for blink and saccade detection (see below).
Behavioral Analysis
In a first step, we used a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA on response accuracy to test the effects of: array probed (array 1, array 2), cue validity (valid, neutral, invalid), and trial difficulty (easy (21-39° rotation angle), medium (11-21°), hard (2-9°)). In a second step, we made use of the variable trial difficulty to test the effects of cue validity and array on the likelihood of correct recall and the precision of memory. We assumed that observers would respond at random on trials where they had no memory of the probed item, while on remembered trials they would respond with Gaussian variability. Therefore we used a mixture model (Zhang and Luck, 2008), consisting of a uniform distribution (to account for the proportion of random guesses) and a von Mises distribution (to account for the precision of remembered responses). We fit a model with two free parameters:
(Equation 1) |
where P(correct∣Δ) is the probability of a correct response given a probe that is rotated Δ degrees relative to the remembered stimulus, vonMises is the cumulative von Mises distribution (i.e. the circular analogue of the normal distribution, which can be evaluated numerically) with mean μ (fixed at 0) and precision κ (kappa, proportional to the inverse of the standard deviation), evaluated at Δ. pGuess determines asymptotic performance (i.e. the guess rate, or proportion of random responses). Estimates of memory precision (kappa) and guess rate (pGuess) were then compared across conditions using a repeated-measures ANOVA with factors cue validity and array probed.
EEG Acquisition
The EEG was recorded continuously with NuAmp amplifiers (Neuroscan Inc., Albany, USA) from 40 Ag/AgCl electrodes. Electrodes were positioned according to a subset of the 10-10 international system (AEEGS, 1991). Recordings were taken from the following electrodes: Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, Pz, POz, Oz, FP1/2, F3/4, F5/6, FC3/4, FC5/6, C3/4, C5/6, CP3/4, CP5/6, P3/4, P5/6, PO3/4, PO7/8, O1/2. Blinks and eye movements were monitored by deriving bipolar recordings from an electrode placed below the right eye and FP2 (vertical electrooculogram, VEOG) and from electrodes placed near the outer canthi of each eye (horizontal EOG, HEOG). The electrode in position AFz was used as the ground. The right mastoid was used as the active reference. Recordings were also taken from the left mastoid to derive an average-mastoid reference off-line. Electrical impedance was kept below 5 kΩ and activity was filtered with a low-pass filter of 300 Hz. The analogue-to-digital sampling rate of the brain activity was set at 1000 Hz and data were recorded continuously for the entire experiment.
EEG Processing
Processing and EEG analyses were done offline, using the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) and custom Matlab scripts for preprocessing. First, the EEG was re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoid, and bandpass-filtered between 0.1 and 40 Hz (using a second-order Butterworth filter). We bandpass-filtered the HEOG and VEOG data between 1 and 40 Hz, and smoothed the filtered timeseries with a sliding boxcar window of 11 ms length to remove brief spikes that were not related to eye movements. Next, the EEG and EOG were segmented into epochs ranging from 1600 ms before the cue (600 ms before the first stimulus array) to 3000 ms after the cue (1000 ms after the onset of the probe stimulus). We automatically rejected trials with EOG amplitudes outside ±75μV surrounding array 1 (−200 to +400 ms), the cue (−200 to +1600 ms), array 2 (−1200 to +600 ms), or the probe (−200 to +600 ms). We used eyetracking data (see above) and the HEOG to eliminate trials with saccades to the stimuli. Finally, all epochs were visually inspected for any further artifacts. For ERP analyses of cue- and array-locked potentials, we baseline-corrected the data using a short interval before the onset of the event of interest (200 ms before cue onset and 50 ms before onset of array 2). The shorter baseline period before onset of array 2 was chosen to minimize any potential effect of late, slow potentials evoked by the cue. When calculating lateralization measures, on trials with a right-hand cue, we flipped the channel topography along the midline to obtain an index of lateralization (contra- versus ipsilateral to the cue). On neutral trials, we flipped the topography if the probed item was on the right.
ERP Analyses
One of the earliest potentials showing cue-dependent lateralization is the early directing attention negativity (or EDAN, Harter et al., 1989). It has been interpreted as a consequence of attentional orienting towards a relevant spatial location (Harter et al., 1989; Yamaguchi et al., 1994; Hopf and Mangun, 2000; Nobre et al., 2000; Murray et al., 2011), though in some cases it may also reflect the selection of relevant features of the cue stimulus itself (van Velzen and Eimer, 2003). The latter interpretation of the EDAN parallels that of another lateralized potential – the posterior contralateral N2 (N2pc, Eimer 1996, or posterior contralateral negativity – PCN, e.g., Töllner et al., 2012). The N2pc (or PCN) is elicited when a target item is identified and selected from a spatially distributed array (Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Eimer, 1996; Woodman and Luck, 1999, 2003; Hopf et al., 2002). Much like the EDAN, it consists of an accentuated negative potential over posterior contralateral electrodes. The EDAN is usually succeeded in time by the anterior directing attention negativity (ADAN, Eimer et al., 2002; Praamstra et al., 2005; Green and MacDonald, 2006), which is another lateralized component that has been associated with the controlled deployment of spatial attention (e.g., Van Velzen and Eimer, 2003). This component is usually observed over frontal sensors, peaking approximately between 300 and 500 ms after a cue (e.g., Kelly et al., 2009).
We averaged trials separately for the three conditions of interest (retrocue, precue, or neutral), and used 3×2 ANOVAs with factors cue type (retrocue, precue, neutral) and side (contralateral, ipsilateral) to test for the effects of cueing and of lateralization in a number of potentials, defined a priori based on previous literature testing anticipatory cueing effects (Hopf and Mangun, 2000; Nobre et al., 2000; Murray et al., 2011). Relative to the cue, we tested the N1 (150-180 ms after cue onset, at PO7/8), the N2 (240-300 ms, PO7/8, i.e. the N2pc or PCN), the early directed attention-related negativity, or EDAN (250-350 ms, PO7/8, O1/2, Mangun, 1994; Hopf and Mangun, 2000), the anterior directed attention-related negativity, or ADAN (350-500 ms, FC3/4 and C3/4), and the late directed attention-related positivity, or LDAP (750-1000 ms, PO7/8 and O1/2). On neutral trials, lateralization was calculated relative to the side of the probed item.
Relative to array 2, we were interested in the early visual response of the P1 (60-140 ms after array onset, tested at PO7/8 and O1/2). We also tested for more persistent effects on encoding by examining the amplitude of the N1 (150-180 ms) and P2 (300-400 ms) potentials, as well as lateralization in the N2 window (N2pc, 240-300 ms, PO7/8).
Analysis of Alpha Power Lateralization
To test for the effects of cues on desynchronization in the alpha band (8-14 Hz) and in surrounding frequencies, we decomposed the broadband signal over 6 occipital-parietal sensors (O1/2, PO7/8, P5/6) using Hanning tapers (window width 5 cycles), estimated at 0.5 Hz steps between 5 and 20 Hz (using custom Matlab scripts and the Fieldtrip Toolbox, Oostenveld et al., 2011). After log transforming (10*log10) the power at each frequency, we baseline corrected single-trial data by subtracting the average power in the 200 ms preceding the onset of array 1 (i.e., 1200 ms before cue onset). We then averaged power across bins in the alpha range (8-14 Hz), and averaged separately across contralateral and ipsilateral sensors (relative to the cued side on cue trials, or relative to the probed side on neutral trials) at each time point after cue onset. We then calculated contra- and ipsilateral alpha power, averaged between 500 and 1000 ms after cue onset (i.e., up to the onset of array 2), and tested for effects of lateralization and of cue type in a 2×3 ANOVA.
Quadrant-Specific Analysis of Alpha Power
The preceding analysis only tested for lateralization of alpha power following a cue to the left or to the right. However, cues were directed at a specific quadrant, warranting a more detailed analysis of the topographical distribution of alpha power. Specifically, we reasoned that the pattern of alpha desynchronization might be specific to the cued quadrant, rather than the entire hemifield. In this task, the spatial specificity of alpha desynchronization could carry additional importance because the uncued quadrant (of the cued hemifield) always contained a (previously seen or an upcoming) distractor. To test this hypothesis, we used an encoding model to estimate the topographical pattern of alpha power desynchronization following visual stimulation in each quadrant, and applied this model to the cue-induced alpha-power topographies to estimate the specificity of desynchronization for the cued quadrant. Conceptually, this analysis is identical to encoding models that have been used to estimate information about the orientation, color, or shape of a stimulus, using fMRI (Brouwer and Heeger, 2009; 2011; Naselaris et al., 2011; Serences and Saproo, 2012) or EEG (Garcia et al., 2013).
For each trial in the experiment (i.e., the test trial), we used a general linear model (fit to all other trials in the experiment, i.e., the training trials) to estimate the topographical distribution of alpha power evoked by a probe presented in each of the four quadrants (using neutral and valid cue trials together and averaging over 8 to 14 Hz and 200 to 800 ms after probe onset, when the effect was strongest). We solved the GLM via ordinary least squares:
(Equation 2) |
where C1 is the design matrix (4 regressors × no. of training trials, with each regressor set to 1 for all trials with a probe in the corresponding quadrant, and set to 0 otherwise), B1 is the training data set (probe-evoked alpha power at 35 EEG sensors × no. of training trials), and W is the estimated weight matrix (35 sensors × 4 regressors/quadrants). Given that the absolute power in a given frequency band might have differed across sensors, variability in power across the topography could have decreased the sensitivity of this analysis. However, a control analysis using a fifth, constant regressor to model out the mean at each sensor yielded nearly identical results. Therefore, differences in power between sensors were ignored in the subsequent analyses.
We used W to estimate the quadrant-wise activation in the test trial, B2 (35 sensors × 1 trial):
(Equation 3) |
where C2 is the ‘activation’ (in arbitrary units) in each quadrant. For each trial, we then sorted this activation relative to the cued quadrant. This procedure was repeated (using the same weight matrix W) for each time point in the trial and each frequency (between 5 and 20 Hz) before moving to the next iteration in the leave-one-out procedure. We then used a 2×4 ANOVA with factors cue type (precue, retrocue) and quadrant (cued, same hemifield, opposite hemifield, and diagonally opposed) to test for cue-evoked quadrant-specific activation after the onset of array 1, the cue, array 2, and the probe. Further, we classified the cued quadrant on each trial, time point, and frequency (using a simple voting procedure that assigned a quadrant label according to the regressor with the highest activation level) to test the accuracy of the model. We compared classification accuracy to chance (25%) using one-sample t-tests. To assess the effects of cueing, we also used a window-of-interest approach by averaging the classification accuracy from 8 to 14 Hz and 500 to 1000 ms after cue onset.
Results
As a point of reference, Table 1 provides a schematic summary of all results described below.
Table 1.
Summary of Behavioral and Neural Cueing Effects. + indicates a significant effect, ns indicates no effect, and red./rev. indicates a significant reduction or reversal of an effect, respectively (marginally significant effects in parentheses).
Behavior | ERPs (cue) | Alpha-Band Effects | ERPs (arra 2) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Recall Rate | Precision | N1 / N2pc | EDAN | ADAN | LDAP | Lateralization | Quadrant-based Desynchronization | P1 | N2pc | P2 | |
Retrocue | + | ns | + | + | + | ns (rev.) | + | + | red. | ns (rev.) | red. |
Precue | + | ns | ns | + | ns | + | + | + | + | ns | + |
Behavioral Results
We conducted a 3×2×3 ANOVA (Fig. 1b) on accuracy with factors cue validity (valid, invalid, neutral), array probed (array 1, array 2), and probe difficulty (hard, medium, easy). We found significant main effects of cue validity (F2,34=36.660, p=3.3*10−9), array (F1,17=64.076, p=3.6*10−7), and difficulty (F2,34=76.028, p=2.8*10−13); as well as significant interactions between cue validity and array (F2,34=7.389, p =0.002), cue type and difficulty (F4,68=7.663, p=3.70*10−5), and array and difficulty (F2,34=3.599, p=0.038). The three-way interaction between cue validity, array, and difficulty was also significant (F4,68=3.882, p=.007).
Given the significant three-way interaction, we conducted post-hoc analyses to test whether both precues (array 2 probed) and retrocues (array 1 probed) individually affected accuracy. For trials on which array 1 was probed (‘retrocue’ trials), we found significant effects of cue validity (F2,34=17.77, p=5.1*10−6) and difficulty (F2,34=20.40, p=1.5*10−6), along with a significant interaction (F4,68=3.50, p=0.012). This confirmed that: (a) retrocues improved accuracy and that (b) the cueing benefit was largest on the easiest trials (t-test comparing valid retrocues with neutral trials for easy probes: t17=6.51, p = 5.4*10−6, Cohen’s d=1.82, medium probes: t17=4.08, p=7.8*10−4, d=1.33, difficult probes: t17=1.49, p=0.153, d=0.49). Compared to neutral cues, invalid retrocues (that is, trials probing a non-cued item from array 1) did not decrease performance (t-test neutral against invalid cues, t17<1, p>0.40 for all three difficulties). However, this null-result does not imply that invalid retrocueing carries no cost, as accuracies were sufficiently low (neutral: 57.7%, invalid: 55.7%) to have produced a floor effect. We found a similar (but stronger, given the three-way interaction) pattern of results when array 2 was probed (‘precue’ trials; effect of cue validity: F2,34=37.72, p=2.3*10−9, effect of difficulty: F2,34=57.97, p=1.1*10−11, interaction: F4,68=8.75, p=9*10−6). In addition to the graded increase in effect size on valid trials (in line with the interaction between cue validity and difficulty, valid cues showed a larger benefit on easier trials, i.e., easy: t17=7.53, p=8.19*10−7, d=0.94, medium: t17=3.87, p=0.001, d=0.83, difficult: t17=2.79, p=0.006, d=0.70), here we also saw significant costs of invalid cues (i.e., trials probing a non-cued item from array 2) on easy and medium difficulty trials (invalid vs. neutral, easy: t17=−4.13, p=0.0007, d=0.99, medium: t17=−5.76, p=2.3*10−5, d=1.79, hard: t17=−0.71, p=0.488, d=0.22). These results confirm our behavioral hypotheses that both valid precues and retrocues significantly improve accuracy, in spite of the baseline accuracy differences between arrays 1 and 2. In other words, retrocues (to items in array 1) improve memory in spite of interference (from items in array 2).
We used a model-based approach to dissociate whether cueing affected the precision or recall rate of remembered items. We found that cueing affected only the guess rate (F1,17=7.81, p=0.002), with no significant effect on precision (kappa, p=0.987), indicating that cues primarily improved the likelihood of recalling an item, but not its fidelity. The probed array also affected guess rate (F2,34=17.4, p<0.001), but not precision (p=0.705). However, there was a significant interaction between cue validity and probed array on precision (F2,34=3.676, p=0.036). The interaction reflected a significant difference in precision on valid trials probing either the first or second array (t17=2.635, p=0.0174) that was not present on neutral or invalid trials (p=0.380 and p=0.445, respectively). Thus, validly cued items presented in the second array were more likely to be recalled, and they were recalled with higher precision, than items in array 1. Given these differences in performance between arrays, we investigated the cueing effect separately for array 1 (retrocues) and for array 2 (precues). Retrocue validity significantly improved guess rate (F2.34=3.98, p=0.028, t-test valid vs neutral: t17=3.43, p=0.003), but not precision (p=0.256). Similarly, precues affected only guess rate (F2.34=7.255, p=0.002, t-test valid vs neutral: t17=4.17, p=0.0006) but not precision (p=0.152).
Event-related Responses to the Cue
We next examined cue-evoked lateralization of the ERP as a marker of spatially directed attention to previous or upcoming information (Fig. 2). We examined the well-known ERP markers of directed attention: EDAN, ADAN, and LDAP. In addition, we tested for modulation of the N2pc (or PCN) related to spatiotopic mechanisms of target selection.
Figure 2.
ERP Responses to the Cue. (A). Grand average potential timecourses taken from visual sensors (O1/2, PO7/8, see top left topography in B for locations). Visual ERPs showed modulations by both retrocues (top panel) and precues (middle panel), lateralizing in response to the cued side (contralateral sensors: dark lines, ipsilateral: gray lines). Neutral cues did not elicit any lateralization. The three shaded areas indicate time ranges used to calculate topographies and bar plots in (B) and (C). (B) Average lateralization topographies at times of interest in the N1 (left column), EDAN (middle column), and LDAP (right column) ranges. Topographies show difference between potentials after left cues and right cues (Left minus Right). (C) Bar plots showing mean potentials at times and sensors (contralateral, C, and ipsilateral, I) of interest. Error bars indicate s.e.m.
In general, we found that both retrocues and precues modulated markers of attentional orienting. The EDAN was present after both retrocues and precues (t17=−7.06, p<0.0001 for retrocues, t17=−2.23, p=0.04 for precues, p=0.28 for neutral), although it was significantly stronger after a retrocue (side*cue type interaction F2,34=12.33, p<0.001). The intermediate stage ADAN was not strongly modulated by either cue type (t17=−2.33, p<0.05 for retrocues, t17=−1.44, p=0.16 for precues, p>0.90 for neutral, side*cue type interaction p>0.20). In contrast, the LDAP, immediately preceding the onset of the second array (250 ms to 0 ms pre-stimulus), showed lateralization for both retrocues and precues, but in opposite directions. While precues showed a modest relative contralateral positivity (t17=2.35, p=0.031), retrocues exhibited a relative contralateral negativity (t17=−2.417, p=0.027), resulting in a significant interaction between side and cue type in the interval from 750-1000 ms (F2,34=11.17, p=0.004). In addition, we saw a main effect of cue type (F1,17=10.07, p=0.006). This main effect was driven by more negative potentials after precues: while retrocue and neutral trials did not differ (F1,17=1.01, p=0.33), there was a difference in potential between precue and neutral trials (F1,17=11.04, p=0.004). The difference between precues and retrocues was strongest over ipsilateral sensors (t17=4.21, p=0.0006), with only a trend for a difference at contralateral sites (t17=2.02, p=0.06).
Returning to the earliest processing stage, we also found attentional orienting effects that were specific to retrocues. The N1 was significantly lateralized after a retrocue (paired t-test contra- vs. ipsilateral sensors, t17=−4.39, p=0.0004), but not after precues or neutral cues (p>0.38 for both, side*cue type (retro/precue) interaction F2,34=4.45, p<0.05). Likewise, the N2pc was modulated only after a retrocue (t17=−4.03, p=0.0009, p>0.20 for precues and neutral, side*cue type interaction F2,34=6.60, p<0.05).
Event-related Responses to Array 2
Regarding the response to the second stimulus array (Fig. 3), we predicted that both items would be attended on neutral trials, the cued item would be attended on precue trials, and no items would be attended on retrocue trials. We found that this prediction was borne out in the magnitude and lateralization of the P1 response, occurring 60-140 ms after stimulus onset. The ANOVA revealed a main effect of cue type (F2,34=6.74, p=0.003) and a significant interaction with side (F2,34=7.56, p=0.002), indicating that cue types affected P1 magnitude differentially at contra- and ipsilateral sensors. While precues led to the expected contralateral P1 enhancement (paired t-test, t17=2.51, p=0.022), retrocues attenuated the P1 contralateral to the cued side (t17=−3.06, p=0.007). Neutral trials did not show any lateralization (t17=0.41, p=0.689). Overall, the P1 was smaller on retrocue trials than on precue or neutral trials (t17=−3.03, p=0.008 for retrocue vs. precue, t17=−3.01, p=0.008 for retrocue vs. neutral), with no significant difference between precue and neutral P1 (t17=0.94, p=0.363, precue-neutral). In sum, the P1 amplitude reduction is in line with the hypothesis that retrocues suppress processing of the second array. Further, the reduction of the P1 contralateral to the cue indicates an additional, selective suppression in processing of new items in the retrocued hemifield.
Figure 3.
ERP Responses to Array 2. (A). Grand average potential timecourses taken from visual sensors (O1/2, PO7/8, see top left topography in B). After retrocues, visual responses (P1 and P2) were attenuated, and P1 was lower at contralateral (dark lines) compared to ipsilateral sites (gray lines). Conversely, precues showed a contralateral increase in P1 amplitude. Shaded areas indicate the time range used to calculate topographies and bar plots in (B) and (C). (B) Average lateralization topographies at times of interest in the P1 (left column) and P2 (right column) ranges. Topographies show difference between potentials after left cues and right cues (Left minus Right) for the P1, and average potential for the P2. (C) Bar plots showing mean potentials at times and sensors (contralateral, C, and ipsilateral, I) of interest. Sensors of interest (P1: O1/2, PO7/8, P2: P3/4/5/6, CP3/4/5/6) are indicated on the topographies in the top row. Error bars indicate s.e.m.
We tested how these effects carried forward by analyzing the P1-N1 amplitude difference. We again saw a main effect of cue type (F2,34=9.33, p=0.001) that appeared to be driven by a reduction of the P1-N1 amplitude on retrocued trials (t-test compared to neutral, t17=−3.67, p=0.002, t17=−4.48, p=0.0003 on contralateral and ipsilateral sensors, respectively). The P1-N1 amplitude was not lateralized in either cue condition (p>0.30 for retrocues and precues). N1 amplitude itself (150-180 ms) was not modulated by cue type (F2,34=1.19, p=0.318) or by side (F2,34=0.12, p=0.730), with no interaction (F2,34=1.72, p=0.194).
Next, we examined potentials in the N2 range (240-300 ms, PO7/8). We saw no strong evidence for lateralization: while there was a main effect of cue type (F2,34=10.349, p=0.002), there was no main effect of cued side (F1,17=2.692, p=0.119) and no interaction between side and cue type (F2,34=1.400, p=0.260). A paired t-test between contra- and ipsilateral potentials after precues showed no significant lateralization (t17=−0.289, p=0.776). After retrocues, following the inverted P1 effect, there was a trend towards a contralateral negativity (t17=−2.043, p=0.054).
Finally, the P2 (over PO7/8 and O1/2), occurring between 300 and 400ms after onset of the array, also showed a main effect of cue type (F2,34=8.11, p=0.001). P2 magnitude over more central-parietal sensors (P3/4/5/6, CP3/4/5/6) showed the same pattern (F2,34=6.08, p=0.006). In both cases, the magnitude was highest for neutral trials, followed by precue trials. There was no significant P2 lateralization in the central-parietal region of interest (F1,17=1.04, p=0.323 over central-parietal cortex), but we did see a main effect of cue side over visual cortex (F1,17=5.10, p=0.037). The effect over visual cortex (PO7/8, O1/2) did not correspond with significant lateralization in any one cue condition (contra minus ipsi difference, t17=−1.59, p=0.129, t17=−1.20, p=0.247, t17=−1.11, p=0.284, for retrocue, precue, and neutral trials, respectively), and no significant interaction (F2,34=0.16, p=0.850).
Cue Effects on Alpha Power
Overall, the onset of pre-, retro-, and neutral cues led to a temporary desynchronization in the alpha band for roughly 400 ms (see timecourses in Fig. 4b). Between 500 ms and 1000 ms after the cue (leading up to array 2), alpha power gradually rose again. On spatial cue trials, this rebound coincided with a divergence in power between contralateral and ipsilateral sensors (Fig. 4b, main effect of side, F1,17=13.66, p=0.002), leading to a significant lateralization both on retrocue trials (t17=−3.19, p=0.005) and precue trials (t17=−3.19, p=0.005), but not on neutral trials (t17=0.08, p=0.93, interaction side*cue type: F2,34=3.39, p=0.045). While the relative decrease in contralateral alpha power after a precue confirmed our hypothesis, we did not expect to see the same pattern after a retrocue (since contralateral alpha could have increased to inhibit processing of the upcoming stimulus in array 2, in order to protect the cued item). This similarity of alpha lateralization stands in contrast to the marked differences between cue types found in the ERP during the same time period (i.e., the LDAP in the 500 ms leading up to array 2).
Figure 4.
Cue-evoked changes in posterior alpha power (8-14 Hz) after spatial retrocues and precues. (A) Topographic distribution of alpha power. Top row shows effects of retrocues after the cue (i.e., preceding the onset of array 2), and before the onset of the probe. Bottom row shows effects of precues in the same time range. Colors indicate power change (relative to prestimulus baseline, in decibels). (B) Alpha power timecourses at posterior sensors (O1/2, PO7/8, P5/6) between cue onset and probe onset. Each plot contains timecourses of contralateral (dark lines) and ipsilateral (light gray) power, separated by cue condition (retrocue/neutral/precue). Black lines at bottom indicate time points of significant lateralization (contralateral minus ipsilateral, p<0.05). The bottom right plot shows lateralization (contra-ipsi) timecourses for the three cue conditions. Green and red lines show time points of significant lateralization for retrocues and precues, respectively. Shaded areas indicate s.e.m.
Cue-related lateralization was quenched by the onset of the second stimulus array, but then re-emerged in the 250 ms immediately before probe onset (main effect of side, F1,17=9.336, p=0.007), between 1750 and 2000 ms after the cue. Again, both cue types were significantly lateralized during this pre-probe interval (retrocues: t17=−2.36, p=0.03, precues: t17=−2.81, p=0.01, neutral: t17=−0.59, p=0.56). On precue trials, this effect emerged earlier, beginning 500 ms before onset of the probe (lateralization 500-250 ms before probe onset, t17=−3.01, p=0.008, p>0.70 for retrocue and neutral trials). For both cue types, the cue-induced alpha-band lateralization was restricted to parietal-occipital sensors (Fig. 4a).
We additionally examined whether cued lateralization (as measured by alpha-band lateralization or the LDAP) related to behavioral improvements after retrocues or precues. We used the model-estimated cueing benefit (i.e. the difference in recall rate between cueing and neutral conditions) to test for Pearson correlations (across participants) with LDAP magnitude and the magnitude of cue-evoked alpha lateralization (contralateral minus ipsilateral power). We found a significant correlation between alpha-band lateralization after retrocues and the difference in recall rate between retrocues and neutral trials (r=−0.474, p=0.0469), indicating that when alpha power was more lateralized (more negative), the improvement in recall rate (after retrocues) was greater. However, this correlation was only significant at an uncorrected statistical level, and we found no other correlations with behavior (all uncorrected p>0.078). This result should therefore be interpreted with some caution, particularly since our sample size (N=18) may have been sufficient to show robust within-subject statistics, but fairly low for between-subject correlation.
Quadrant-specific Activation in the Alpha Band
The topographical distribution of alpha power in response to the probe (8-14 Hz, 200-800 ms after onset) reliably distinguished the spatial location of the probe stimulus in all three cue conditions (see Fig. 5a, dashed windows after probe onset, classification accuracy against chance level: p<0.001 for neutral (t17=5.93), valid precue (t17=5.74), and valid retrocue trials (t17=4.28)). Using a leave-one-out procedure, we used this time (200-800 ms after probe onset) and frequency (8-14 Hz) window to train quadrant-specific topographies (Fig. 5a) that were then applied to the test trial at all time points and frequencies (5-20 Hz). Quadrant classification after cue onset (Fig 5c, 500 to 1000 ms after cue onset, 8-14 Hz, see also dashed windows after cue onset in Fig. 5b) was significant on both retrocue (t17=2.41, p=0.028) and precue trials (t17=2.84, p=0.011), but not on neutral trials (t17=−0.06, p=0.953). In addition, on precue trials we were able to decode the cued quadrant in the period leading up to the probe (1500 to 2000 ms after cue onset, t17=3.64, p=0.002, p>0.50 on retrocue and neutral trials). The latter effect accompanies the earlier re-emergence of alpha lateralisation after array 2 on precue trials (Fig. 4e). The analysis described below, examining quadrant-specific activation, may elucidate why, on retrocue trials, alpha lateralization did seem to appear just before probe onset (1750 to 2000 ms after the cue, Fig. 4b,f), even though we were not able to classify the cued quadrant in this epoch using the decoding approach.
Figure 5.
Quadrant Decoding using Alpha Band Power (t-test vs. chance, 25%). For each trial, we calculated the 8-14 Hz topographies evoked by probe onset on all other trials, separately for probes presented in the four screen quadrants. (A) shows the topographies, averaged across all trials and all participants. For illustration, the average topography has been subtracted from each quadrant-specific topography. The topography of the left-out test trial was then calculated at each frequency and time point in the trial, and regressed against the four template topographies. The stimulus quadrant corresponding to the best-fitting topography was voted to have evoked the pattern on the test trial, and was compared to the actually cued quadrant (or probed quadrant on neutral trials) to calculate classification accuracy. (B) shows t-statistic maps of classification accuracy (one-sample t-tests against 25%, computed at each time point and frequency), smoothed using a 3-point moving average. Dashed windows after cue onset in (B) correspond to the data shown in (C). (C) Bar plots of classification accuracy per condition, averaged across 8-14 Hz, 500-1000 ms after cue onset. For both cue types, quadrant was classified above chance (*: p<0.05, ns: not significant).
To examine the quadrant specificity of the analysis further, we separated the activation in the cued quadrant and the three uncued quadrants (in the same hemifield, the opposite hemifield, and the diagonally opposite corner) on retrocue and precue trials (Fig. 6).
Figure 6.
Quadrant-specific Activation in the Alpha Band. (A) Retrocues elicit quadrant-specific shifts in alpha power. Each of the four time-frequency plots shows the correspondence of average topographies with the probe-evoked pattern, separately for each time point (from onset of array 1 to 1 s after the probe) and frequency (5-20 Hz). The top left plot shows correlations with topographies evoked by a probe in the cued quadrant, with the other three plots showing correlations with topographies evoked by probes in the three uncued quadrants (separately for probes in the cued hemifield, and in the two quadrants of the opposite hemifield). Color indicates correlation strength (in arbitrary units), with warmer colors indicating a better fit. (B) Quadrant-specific topographic patterns in response to precues.
At a basic level, our analysis revealed predicted quadrant-specific patterns during periods of visual stimulation. In other words, the activation timecourses followed the stimulus sequence. On retrocue trials (Fig. 6a), activation in the two quadrants in which stimuli had appeared in array 1 (i.e., the cued and diagonally opposed quadrants) preceded cue onsets (when stimuli were presented in these locations), while the two remaining quadrants showed activity after onset of the second array (when stimuli were presented there). Conversely, precue trials showed the opposite activation timecourse (Fig. 6b). Here, activation in response to the second array emerged in the cued and, to a lesser extent, diagonally opposed quadrants, while the remaining quadrants showed activation preceding the cue. These timecourses indicate that our analysis was sensitive enough to detect the stimulus sequence in each trial type. Further, they suggest why retrocued quadrants could not be decoded leading up to the probe: since the stimuli in the second array led to alpha band activation in the uncued quadrants, they may have counteracted the spatial specificity of the retrocue-based activation in the cued quadrant (without necessarily reducing the more basic lateralization effect).
We again saw reliable activation in the cued quadrant after probe onset and after cue onset (500-1000 ms) that was limited to the alpha band. Importantly, for the post-cue epoch (500-1000 ms), we saw a main effect of quadrant (F3,51=5.83, p=0.002), but no effect of cue type (precue/retrocue, F1,17=0.243, p=0.63) and no interaction (F3,51=0.216, p=0.89). When we restricted our analysis to the two quadrants within the cued hemifield, we saw the same pattern (main effect of quadrant: F1,17=10.220, p=0.005; main effect of cue type: F1,17=0.002, p=0.97; interaction: F1,17=0.521, p=0.48). The robust activation difference between quadrants of the same hemifield confirms that shifts in alpha power were indeed specific to a cued quadrant, rather than a cued hemifield. In addition, our analysis shows that both precues and retrocues led to indistinguishable quadrant-specific reductions in alpha power.
Discussion
We used a novel task in which a cue occurring within an equivalent temporal and spatial context triggers either anticipatory or retrospective shifts of attention during WM. Our main finding is that retrospective attention shifts lead to a relative contralateral decrease in posterior alpha-band power. Importantly, cued alpha-lateralisation was temporary rather than sustained, potentially constraining theories of how top-down prioritization operates in WM. The findings point to a mechanism of transient access to visually selective populations while selected WM information is transformed into a more robust representational state. Before discussing this mechanism, we will lay out the consequences of retrospective attention shifts on behavior and on ERP markers to constrain the interpretation of the alpha-band results.
Behavioral Impact of Cueing in Working Memory
Cues conferred behavioral benefits for both precued and retrocued items (Griffin and Nobre, 2003; Landman et al., 2003) by increasing the likelihood of recalling the correct item (also see Murray et al., 2011; 2013). The precision of memory representations was not modulated by either cue type. Prior studies using free recall of probed features have found that precues significantly improve both the rate of recall and precision (e.g., Bays et al., 2011; Gorgoraptis et al., 2011; Zokaei et al., 2011). The likelihood of correct recall seems to be robustly improved by both precues and retrocues, irrespective of the mode of assessment. By contrast, the forced-choice design of the current study may not have been sensitive enough to measure cue-evoked changes in precision.
While the behavioral improvement due to precues and retrocues was similar, we found that overall performance was significantly higher for items from the second array. Presumably, performance for array 1 was lower because of the interfering effect of array 2 (Zanto and Gazzaley, 2009). This asymmetry in difficulty may have induced a difference in motivation that could explain some of the discrepancies in the ERP results: since performance was comparatively high on pre-cue trials, participants may have used the cues less (or at least differently) and still achieved a high level of performance. Nonetheless, precues induced both accuracy benefits and costs (on infrequent invalid trials), demonstrating that observers were using them to their advantage.
ERP Markers Point to Processing Differences between Retrospective and Prospective Attention Shifts
Within the first 500 ms of cue processing, both prospective and retrospective cues led to the lateralization of potentials that have been associated with shifts of spatial attention. Retrocues had a strong effect on the EDAN (250-350 ms after the cue), and also modulated the ADAN (350-500 ms), indicating a rapid refocusing of attention (Kiss et al., 2008; Brignani et al., 2009). In addition, retrocues may also have prompted the rapid selection of the cued target from WM (Kuo et al., 2009), as indicated by even earlier lateralizations in the N1 (150-180 ms) and N2 (240-300 ms, i.e. N2pc) ranges.
Lateralization may have occurred more quickly after retrocues than precues because retrocues allowed for immediate selection from the contents of WM. Other studies have found variability in the time course of selection from WM, with some seeing markers of access in the N2 time range (Kuo et al., 2009; Astle et al., 2010; Leszczyński et al., 2012), while others found that similar, but slightly later components responded to cues directing attention in WM (Dell'Acqua et al., 2010; Eimer and Kiss, 2010). In the current study, the time pressure to complete selection before the onset of the second array (and the interference this likely caused) may have encouraged the relatively early shifts induced by retrocues.
Similar to retrocues, precues also showed a significant lateralization in the EDAN, in line with previous findings (Harter et al., 1989; Yamaguchi et al., 1994; Hopf and Mangun, 2000; Nobre et al., 2000; van Velzen and Eimer, 2003). However, the magnitude of the EDAN was significantly larger after retrocues than precues. Precues may have shown attenuated (and delayed) lateralization in this study for a number of reasons. First, observers may have used them less, since performance was already better on precue trials (see above). Second, a precue in the current study required more operations than in typical studies of anticipatory attention, since participants first had to parse whether the cue pertained to the previous or the upcoming array, and possibly needed to suppress information about the first array before shifting attention.
Interestingly, ERP responses to precues and retrocues began to diverge between 500 and 1000 ms after the cue, leading up to the onset of the second array. While precues evoked the typical lateralized anticipatory response (LDAP), retrocues did not, and even led to a brief but statistically significant reversal of this potential. The LDAP has been associated with the top-down deployment of anticipatory spatial attention, which should have improved encoding of relevant information on precue trials. On retrocue trials, none of the upcoming information was relevant (since the cued information had already been presented), and thus one may have expected no anticipatory lateralization at all. The reversal of the LDAP may indicate a shift of attention away from the location of the retrocued item to reduce interference from the upcoming array. The transient nature of the effect (and the relatively modest statistical significance) may alternatively indicate that the LDAP did not play a large role in guiding attention in this task, or that this late potential peaked at different timepoints in different participants, leading to a temporal smearing (and consequent reduction) of the effect. Another possibility is that this late negativity reflects contralateral delay activity (CDA), a sustained negative potential often observed in delay periods of WM tasks (Vogel and Machizawa, 2004). In the current study, this negativity emerged only late after the cue, and was not sustained. This makes it unlikely to reflect persistent maintenance of the cued information in WM, but this interpretation cannot be ruled out entirely.
We found that the visually evoked responses to the second memory array were also differentially modulated by precues and retrocues. Precues again led to the expected enhancement of processing, as indicated by an increase in P1 amplitude contralateral to the cued stimulus (Hillyard et al., 1998). In contrast, retrocues led to an overall reduction in P1 amplitude, and to a reduced P1 contralateral to the cued side. Similarly, the N2 also showed a main effect of cue type (although there was no strong evidence for lateralization in this interval, possibly because selection had already begun earlier). The slightly later P2 response was also smallest after retrocues, with increasing amplitudes on precue and neutral trials. This pattern points to a reduction in processing of the second array after retrocues, possibly to protect WM contents by reducing interference. The increasing amplitude of the P2 may also have reflected increased encoding demands after the second array on precue and neutral trials.
Alpha-band Lateralization May Support Different Operations After Prospective and Retrospective Attention Shifts
The main focus of our study lay on how alpha-band (8-14 Hz) oscillations respond to retrocues and precues. Importantly, we expected the alpha-band responses to provide information about the neural mechanism of prioritization during WM maintenance. After both precues and retrocues, we found a contralateral, quadrant-specific desynchronization of posterior alpha-band power. Under both precues and retrocues, the quadrant-specific biasing of alpha power appears to reflect desynchronization over areas representing the cued quadrant, rather than increased synchronization in irrelevant areas. We draw this conclusion from the cross-comparison of the quadrant-specific, multivariate pattern evoked by the probe stimulus (which most likely led to a desynchronization in the alpha band). After retrocues, we found strong cross-generalization only in the interval between the cue and array 2.
On precue trials, the alpha-band lateralization coincided with other markers of increased excitability (the LDAP and an increased P1 amplitude to the subsequent array). Therefore, the pre-cue alpha-band effects likely supported sustained attention towards the anticipated location of the cued stimulus. On retrocue trials, the alpha-band effect was not compatible with sustained anticipation. The ERP response to the second, irrelevant array was attenuated (especially contralateral to the cue). Also, lateralization was not sustained throughout the trial: the appearance of array 2 eliminated the lateralization, and it only re-emerged immediately before probe onset.
Our results could help arbitrate among several theories of how prioritization acts in WM. First, the results do not support a pure inhibition mechanism: under an inhibition account, alpha-band power over irrelevant cortex would increase so that prioritized WM information is protected from interference (Sauseng et al., 2009). Several studies have argued that alpha power over currently task-irrelevant cortex increases in order to inhibit processing of distracting stimuli that could otherwise interfere with the contents of WM (Klimesch et al., 1999; Jensen et al., 2002; Haegens et al., 2010; Bonnefond and Jensen, 2012). We may have expected to see a similar effect after a retrocue: once the cued item has been selected for protection, alpha power could have increased, especially contralateral to the cued side, in order to reduce interference from the second array. Although we observed evidence for inhibition of the second array, we observed a contradictory pattern of alpha lateralisation: a relative contralateral desychronization.
A second possible mechanism of WM prioritization is sustained attention (Awh et al., 2006; Chun, 2011; Chun et al., 2011). A sustained attention account would predict that retrocues improve memory by persistently increasing excitability at the anticipated probe location. This should have led to a sustained desynchronization contralateral to the cued (and eventually probed) location. However, we observed only a transient desynchronization, such that lateralization did not immediately re-emerge after processing of the second array. Moreover, alpha lateralisation did not lead to an overall increase in excitability (as measured by the response to array 2). A recent study showing contralateral alpha-band desynchronization after a retrocue (Poch et al., 2014) could be compatible with sustained attention. However, their design and analysis did not allow a distinction between the sustained attention account and a more transient mechanism. The authors did not examine the time course of desynchronization, and so were not able to test whether lateralization was sustained or transient. Perhaps more importantly, there were no intervening events between cue and probe onset, making it difficult to dissociate sustained attention to the cued item from anticipatory attention towards the probe stimulus.
Putative Role for Spatiotopic Alpha Shifts in Item-Specific Access
Our results appear to be most consistent with the idea that retrocues elicit short-term increases in activity in spatiotopic visual cortex. This could give control areas (such as prefrontal cortex) access to the population storing the cued visual features in order to move them into a more robust representational state (Sligte et al., 2008; Olivers et al., 2011; Lewis-Peacock and Postle, 2012; Lewis-Peacock et al., 2012; Oberauer, 2013; Chatham et al., 2014). Alpha-band desynchronization could either reflect increased local activity or communication between visual cortex and top-down control areas.
Recent studies into the mechanistic effects of alpha-band desynchronization have demonstrated that a decrease in alpha power primarily coincides with a gain in neural excitability. Alpha desynchronization has been shown to increase baseline (Bollimunta et al., 2011) and stimulus-evoked (Haegens et al., 2011) firing rates, the likelihood of perceiving two stimuli instead of one (Lange et al., 2013), and, to some extent, the amplitude of the stimulus-evoked EEG response (P1, Rajagovindan and Ding, 2011). We speculate that, after a retrocue, the selective desynchronization in the alpha band results in a temporary spatiotopic increase in excitability for the cued item. The increased excitability presumably translates into enhanced retention for the cued feature information. This could occur by making the activity in the sensory area more resilient to decay (reducing the chances of sudden loss of feature information, Murray et al., 2013), or by involving control regions themselves in the representation of the cued feature. Alternatively, the observed alpha-band effects could be a consequence of increased local neural activity, which would lead to decreased alpha power specifically at the activated site. In either case, once the cued information has been accessed, there is no longer a need for sustained alpha lateralization, and it can return to baseline levels.
Methods capable of assessing local activity (either invasively or using gamma oscillations as a proxy) may be necessary to arbitrate between the two putative consequences of alpha lateralization outlined here. In addition, future studies will need to use longer delay periods to identify conclusively the temporal dynamics of alpha-mediated memory access when there is less time pressure from an imminent distractor array. Finally, neurodisruptive techniques (such as transcranial magnetic stimulation, TMS) might also provide evidence of the functional role of alpha oscillations in the current task. For instance, TMS may affect only a cued item, and only after alpha synchronization has shifted to prioritize it. Zokaei and colleagues (2014) recently demonstrated that TMS to visual cortex disrupted the precision of WM items only after they had been cued. This specific disruption for preferred items might depend on a prior reduction in alpha power that sufficiently increases excitability, allowing TMS to have a larger effect. Examining the temporal dynamics of WM disruption could help arbitrate between the mechanisms outlined above.
Beyond the implications for WM, our results show that retrospective attention tasks may be used to illuminate the role of alpha oscillations in top-down prioritization separately from their role in anticipation. Even though the alpha-band lateralization and the LDAP occur in the same time window, they appear to give different results after retrocues. The discrepancy might have occurred because the two markers reflect different operations that are decoupled in retrocueing, but not in precueing. Specifically, alpha lateralization after retrocues may not reflect anticipatory spatial attention shifts. This is particularly surprising in light of findings that, in anticipatory cueing tasks, alpha lateralization is correlated with ERP markers of anticipation (such as the LDAP, see Kelly et al., 2009) and with contralateral delay activity (see Mazaheri and Jensen, 2008; van Dijk et al., 2010). Additionally, alpha lateralization usually predicts subsequent visual processing (Rajagovindan and Ding, 2011) and visual excitability (Romei et al., 2008a; 2008b).
After retrocues, alpha-band lateralization and LDAP may reflect different neural operations, such as, for example, access to the cued part of sensory cortex (alpha-band desynchronization) vs. facilitation of sensory processing of upcoming stimuli (LDAP). In sum, studies using retrospective attention shifts may prove fruitful in distinguishing the role of alpha oscillations in top-down attention from its role in anticipation.
Conclusion
We conclude that retrospective shifts of attention to items in working memory trigger spatiotopic access to information, reflected in early ERP markers of spatial attention shifts and lateralization of alpha-band power over visual cortex. Even though alpha-band lateralization was similar after precues and retrocues, the size and lateralization of visual responses evoked by a WM array after the cue depended on the cue type. Our task design and the difference in ERPs to the second array make alternative mechanisms of retrocueing less probable: neither sustained protection via distractor inhibition, nor sustained spatial attention, could easily account for our pattern of results. Nonetheless, the proposed transient spatiotopic access may occur on its own, or in combination with a prospective shift of attention in anticipation of a probe stimulus. We speculate that the access operation may help shift prioritized information into a more robust representational format (Cowan, 2000), thus increasing the likelihood of correct recall. The successful ability to use top-down attentional control to prioritize relevant information is thus a crucial aspect of intelligent cognition and behavior.
References
- AEEGS American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;8:200–202. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akam T, Kullmann DM. Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:111–122. doi: 10.1038/nrn3668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Astle DE, Nobre AC, Scerif G. Subliminally Presented and Stored Objects Capture Spatial Attention. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:3567–3571. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5701-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Awh E, Vogel EK, Oh SH. Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience. 2006;139:201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bays PM, Gorgoraptis N, Wee N, Marshall L, Husain M. Temporal dynamics of encoding, storage, and reallocation of visual working memory. Journal of Vision. 2011;11:6–6. doi: 10.1167/11.10.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bays PM, Husain M. Dynamic Shifts of Limited Working Memory Resources in Human Vision. Science. 2008;321:851–854. doi: 10.1126/science.1158023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollimunta A, Mo J, Schroeder CE, Ding M. Neuronal Mechanisms and Attentional Modulation of Corticothalamic Alpha Oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4935–4943. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5580-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnefond M, Jensen O. Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against Anticipated Distracters. Current Biology. 2012;22:1969–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brignani D, Lepsien J, Rushworth MFS, Nobre AC. The Timing of Neural Activity during Shifts of Spatial Attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:2369–2383. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brouwer GJ, Heeger DJ. Decoding and Reconstructing Color from Responses in Human Visual Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:13992–14003. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3577-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brouwer GJ, Heeger DJ. Cross-orientation suppression in human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011 doi: 10.1152/jn.00540.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chun MM. Visual working memory as visual attention sustained internally over time. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1407–1409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chun MM, Golomb JD, Turk-Browne NB. A Taxonomy of External and Internal Attention. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:73–101. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci. 2000;24:87–114. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01003922. discussion114–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dell'Acqua R, Sessa P, Toffanin P, Luria R, Jolicœur P. Orienting attention to objects in visual short-term memory. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:419–428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edin F, Klingberg T, Johansson P, McNab F, Tegner J, Compte A. Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:6802–6807. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901894106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M. The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1996;99:225–234. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M, Velzen JV, Driver J. Cross-Modal Interactions between Audition, Touch, and Vision in Endogenous Spatial Attention: ERP Evidence on Preparatory States and Sensory Modulations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:254–271. doi: 10.1162/089892902317236885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M, Forster B, van Velzen J. Anterior and posterior attentional control systems use different spatial reference frames: ERP evidence from covert tactile-spatial orienting. Psychophysiol. 2003;40:924–933. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M, Kiss M. An electrophysiological measure of access to representations in visual working memory. Psychophysiol. 2010;47:197–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00879.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ester EF, Serences JT, Awh E. Spatially Global Representations in Human Primary Visual Cortex during Working Memory Maintenance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:15258–15265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foxe JJ, Simpson GV, Ahlfors SP. Parieto-occipital ~10 Hz activity reflects anticipatory state of visual attention mechanisms. Neuroreport. 1998;9:3929–3933. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199812010-00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia JO, Srinivasan R, Serences JT. Near-Real-Time Feature-Selective Modulations in Human Cortex. Current Biology. 2013;23:515–522. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gazzaley A, Nobre AC. Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;16:129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorgoraptis N, Catalao RFG, Bays PM, Husain M. Dynamic Updating of Working Memory Resources for Visual Objects. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8502–8511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0208-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green JJ, McDonald JJ. An event-related potential study of supramodal attentional control and crossmodal attention effects. Psychophysiol. 2006;43:161–171. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00394.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffin IC, Nobre AC. Orienting attention to locations in internal representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15:1176–1194. doi: 10.1162/089892903322598139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens S, Nacher V, Luna R, Romo R, Jensen O. α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:19377–19382. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117190108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens S, Osipova D, Oostenveld R, Jensen O. Somatosensory working memory performance in humans depends on both engagement and disengagement of regions in a distributed network. Human Brain Mapping. 2010;31(1):26–35. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison SA, Tong F. Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature. 2009;458:632–635. doi: 10.1038/nature07832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harter MR, Miller SL, Price NJ, LaLonde ME, Keyes AL. Neural Processes Involved in Directing Attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1989;1:223–237. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.3.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillyard SA, Vogel EK, Luck SJ. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1998;353:1257–1270. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopf JM, Mangun GR. Shifting visual attention in space: an electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2000 doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00313-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopf JM, Boelmans K, Schoenfeld AM, Heinze HJ, Luck SJ. How does attention attenuate target–distractor interference in vision? Cognitive Brain Research. 2002;15:17–29. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00213-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen O, Bonnefond M, VanRullen R. An oscillatory mechanism for prioritizing salient unattended stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:200–206. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen O, Gelfand J, Kounios J, Lisman JE. Oscillations in the Alpha Band (9-12 Hz) Increase with Memory Load during Retention in a Short-term Memory Task. Cerebral Cortex. 2002;12:877–882. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:1–8. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly SP, Gomez-Ramirez M, Foxe JJ. The strength of anticipatory spatial biasing predicts target discrimination at attended locations: a high-density EEG study. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30:2224–2234. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06980.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiss M, Jolicœur P, Dell'Acqua R, Eimer M. Attentional capture by visual singletons is mediated by top-down task set: New evidence from the N2pc component. Psychophysiol. 2008;45:1013–1024. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00700.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Schwaiger J, Auinger P, Winkler T. “Paradoxical” alpha synchronization in a memory task. Cognitive Brain Research. 1999;7:493–501. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(98)00056-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo B-C, Stokes MG, Murray AM, Nobre AC. Attention Biases Visual Activity in Visual STM. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014 doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00577. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo BC, Rao A, Lepsien J, Nobre AC. Searching for Targets within the Spatial Layout of Visual Short-Term Memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8032–8038. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0952-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo BC, Yeh YY, Chen A, D'Esposito M. Functional connectivity during top-down modulation of visual short-term memory representations. Neuropsychologia. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landman R, Spekreijse H, Lamme VAF. Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research. 2003;43:149–164. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00402-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange J, Oostenveld R, Fries P. Reduced Occipital Alpha Power Indexes Enhanced Excitability Rather than Improved Visual Perception. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:3212–3220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3755-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lepsien J, Nobre AC. Attentional Modulation of Object Representations in Working Memory. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;17:2072–2083. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leszczyński M, Myers NE, Akyürek EG, Schubö A. Recoding between two types of stm representation revealed by the dynamics of memory search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;24:653–663. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis-Peacock JA, Drysdale AT, Oberauer K, Postle BR. Neural Evidence for a Distinction between Short-term Memory and the Focus of Attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;24:61–79. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis-Peacock JA, Postle BR. Decoding the internal focus of attention. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luck SJ, Hillyard SA. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiol. 1994;31:291–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02218.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mangun GR. Cognitive electrophysiology. Birkhäuser Boston; Boston, MA: 1994. Orienting Attention in the Visual Fields: An Electrophysiological Analysis; pp. 81–101. [Google Scholar]
- Mazaheri A, Jensen O. Asymmetric Amplitude Modulations of Brain Oscillations Generate Slow Evoked Responses. J Neurosci. 2008 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1631-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munneke J, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. Shifting Attention within Memory Representations Involves Early Visual Areas. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e35528. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray AM, Nobre AC, Clark IA, Cravo AM, Stokes MG. Attention Restores Discrete Items to Visual Short-Term Memory. Psychological Science. 2013 doi: 10.1177/0956797612457782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray AM, Nobre AC, Stokes MG. Markers of preparatory attention predict visual short-term memory performance. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1458–1465. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naselaris T, Kay KN, Nishimoto S, Gallant JL. Encoding and decoding in fMRI. NeuroImage. 2011;56:400–410. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nobre AC, Coull JT, Maquet P, Frith CD, Vandenberghe R, Mesulam MM. Orienting Attention to Locations in Perceptual Versus Mental Representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:363–373. doi: 10.1162/089892904322926700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nobre AC, Sebestyen GN, Miniussi C. The dynamics of shifting visuospatial attention revealed by event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:964–974. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00015-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nobre AC, Stokes MG. Attention and short-term memory: crossroads. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1391–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K. Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2002;28:411–421. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K. The focus of attention in working memory—from metaphors to mechanisms. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K, Kliegl R. A formal model of capacity limits in working memory. Journal of Memory and Language. 2006;55:601–626. [Google Scholar]
- Olivers CNL, Peters J, Houtkamp R, Roelfsema PR. Different states in visual working memory: when it guides attention and when it does not. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:327–334. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience. 2011;2011:1–9. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasternak T, Greenlee MW. Working memory in primate sensory systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pertzov Y, Bays PM, Joseph S, Husain M. Rapid Forgetting Prevented by Retrospective Attention Cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2012 doi: 10.1037/a0030947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Stancák A, Jr., Neuper C. Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band — an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: A review. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1996;24:39–46. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00066-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poch C, Campo P, Barnes GR. Modulation of alpha and gamma oscillations related to retrospectively orienting attention within working memory. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014 doi: 10.1111/ejn.12589. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postle BR. Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience. 2006;139:23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Praamstra P, Boutsen L, Humphreys GW. Frontoparietal control of spatial attention and motor intention in human EEG. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:764–774. doi: 10.1152/jn.01052.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajagovindan R, Ding M. From prestimulus alpha oscillation to visual-evoked response: an inverted-U function and its attentional modulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:1379–1394. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rerko L, Oberauer K. Focused, unfocused, and defocused information in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2013;39:1075–1096. doi: 10.1037/a0031172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rerko L, Souza AS, Oberauer K. Retro-cue benefits in working memory without sustained focal attention. Memory & Cognition. 2014:1–17. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0392-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romei V, Brodbeck V, Michel C, Amedi A, Pascual-Leone A, Thut G. Spontaneous Fluctuations in Posterior α-Band EEG Activity Reflect Variability in Excitability of Human Visual Areas. Cortex. 2008a doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romei V, Rihs T, Brodbeck V, Thut G. Resting electroencephalogram alpha-power over posterior sites indexes baseline visual cortex excitability. Neuroreport. 2008b;19:203–208. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f454c4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Heise KF, Gruber WR, Holz E, Karim AA, Glennon M, Gerloff C, Birbaumer N, Hummel FC. Brain Oscillatory Substrates of Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity. Current Biology. 2009;19:1846–1852. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serences JT, Saproo S. Computational advances towards linking BOLD and behavior. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:435–446. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sligte IG, Scholte HS, Lamme VAF. Are There Multiple Visual Short-Term Memory Stores? PLoS One. 2008;3(2):e1699. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sligte IG, Scholte HS, Lamme VAF. V4 Activity Predicts the Strength of Visual Short-Term Memory Representations. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:7432–7438. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0784-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer B, Blankenburg F. Supramodal Parametric Working Memory Processing in Humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:3287–3295. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5280-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg S. High-speed scanning in human memory. Science. 1966;153:652–654. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3736.652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Töllner T, Müller HJ, Zehetleitner M. Top-Down Dimensional Weight Set Determines the Capture of Visual Attention: Evidence from the PCN Component. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22:1554–1563. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Dijk H, Van Der Werf J, Mazaheri A, Medendorp WP, Jensen O. Modulations in oscillatory activity with amplitude asymmetry can produce cognitively relevant event-related responses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:900–905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908821107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Velzen J, Eimer M. Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events. Psychophysiol. 2003;40:827–831. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams M, Hong SW, Kang MS, Carlisle NB, Woodman GF. The benefit of forgetting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2012;20:348–355. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0354-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodman GF, Luck SJ. Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature. 1999;400:867–869. doi: 10.1038/23698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodman GF, Luck SJ. Serial deployment of attention during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2003;29:121–138. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.29.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Worden MS, Foxe JJ, Wang N, Simpson GV. Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20:1–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-j0002.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamaguchi S, Tsuchiya H, Kobayashi S. Electrooencephalographic activity associated with shifts of visuospatial attention. Brain. 1994;117:553–562. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.3.553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Neural Suppression of Irrelevant Information Underlies Optimal Working Memory Performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:3059–3066. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4621-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang W, Luck SJ. Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature. 2008;453:233–235. doi: 10.1038/nature06860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zokaei N, Gorgoraptis N, Bahrami B, Bays PM, Husain M. Precision of working memory for visual motion sequences and transparent motion surfaces. Journal of Vision. 2011;11:2–2. doi: 10.1167/11.14.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zokaei N, Manohar S, Husain M, Feredoes E. Causal Evidence for a Privileged Working Memory State in Early Visual Cortex. J Neurosci. 2014;34:158–162. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2899-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]