Abstract
Background
Consistent evidence linking habitual sleep duration with risks of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia is lacking.
Methods
We conducted a prospective study on 7444 community-dwelling women (aged 65–80) with self-reported sleep duration, within the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study in 1995–2008. Incident MCI/dementia cases were ascertained by validated protocols. Cox models were used to adjust for multiple sociodemographic and lifestyle factors, depression, cardiovascular disease (CVD), and other clinical characteristics.
Results
We found a statistically significant (p=0.03) V-shaped association, with a higher MCI/dementia risk in women with either short (≤6 hours/night) or long (≥8 hours/night) sleep duration (vs.7 hours/night). The multicovariate-adjusted hazard for MCI/dementia was increased by 36% in short sleepers irrespective of CVD, and by 35% in long sleepers without CVD. A similar V-shaped association was found with cognitive decline.
Conclusion
In older women, habitual sleep duration predicts the future risk for cognitive impairments including dementia, independent of vascular risk factors.
Keywords: sleep duration, elderly, cognition, cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, dementia, longitudinal analysis, cohort studies
1. INTRODUCTION
Since the first experimental sleep deprivation study on humans reported in 1896 [1], considerable evidence has shown that sleep loss impairs cognitive performance [2]. However, the focus of previous clinical studies has been on short-term sleep deprivation. As insufficient sleep was increasingly recognized an important public health problem (e.g., 32% reporting ≤ 6 hours of sleep on weekdays among people aged > 60 years in a US national survey [3]), a growing attention has been directed to studying the long-term neurocognitive effects of sleep deprivation. Cross-sectional analyses of population studies on older people worldwide (e.g., in the US [4], UK [5], Finland [6], France [7], and China [8]) have revealed an inverse U-shaped or V-shaped association between cognitive function and self-reported sleep duration. Only a small number of longitudinal studies have begun to examine whether habitual short or long sleep duration increases the risk for cognitive declines [9–12] or dementia [13–15] in the elderly, but the results were mixed. Interpretation of previous findings was uncertain due to short follow-up time or lack of rigorous control of potential confounders. To examine associations of cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment (MCI)/dementia with habitual sleep duration, we conducted a longitudinal analysis based in the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS), 1995–2008.
2. METHODS
2.1 Study Population
The WHIMS [16] was an ancillary study to the Women’s Health Initiative trials of hormone therapy (WHI-HT), two large, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, clinical trials of conjugated equine estrogen treatment alone (E-alone) for women with prior hysterectomy or in combination with medroxyprogesterone acetate (E+P) for women without prior hysterectomy [17]. The WHIMS was designed to test the hypothesis that HT reduces the incidence of all-cause dementia in women aged 65 and older. Community-dwelling women were recruited during 1995–1998 from WHI-HT participants who were aged 65 to 80 years at enrollment, free of dementia defined by WHIMS protocols. After discovering an unfavorable risk-to-benefit ratio of its non-cognitive endpoints, the E+P trial was discontinued in July, 2002. The E-alone trial also ended earlier than planned in February, 2004, because of a greater risk of stroke and a lack of benefit for coronary heart disease. These decisions also ended the WHIMS trial, but annual follow-up was continued for cognitive assessment. The WHIMS study design, eligibility criteria, and recruitment procedures have been described elsewhere [16]. The current analyses are based on 7444 participants who had complete data on sleep duration at WHIMS baseline.
2.2 Study Variable
The measures of sleep disturbance in WHI cohort were developed by sleep research consultants to the WHI Behavioral Advisory Committee [18]. As part of the baseline examination, each WHI participant was asked to report “hours of sleep on a typical night during the past 4 weeks” (≤5, 6, 7, 8, 9, ≥10). Levine et al. assessed the psychometric properties of these sleep measures [19], and showed that self-reported sleep duration did not cluster with other sub-constructs of sleep disturbance, such as insomnia and sleepiness. Very good test-retest reliability was found for self-reported sleep duration (Spearman R=0.97 for same-day administration and 0.89 for 8–14 days).
2.3 Neurocognitive Outcome Variables
Our analyses included two endpoints: defined significant decline in global cognitive function, as assessed by the Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) Examination [20]; and the incidence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or probable dementia, as determined by the validated 4-phase WHIMS protocols [16, 21]. In phase 1, trained, masked and certified technicians administered the 3MS test at baseline and annually. Women who screened positively for cognitive impairment, according to education-adjusted 3MS cut-points, proceeded to more extensive neuropsychological testing (phase 2), including a modified Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) battery [22]. Participants subsequently received a detailed clinical neurological and neuropsychiatric evaluation by physicians (i.e., neurologists, geriatricians, or geriatric psychiatrists) with experience in diagnosing dementia (phase 3). Each suspected case of dementia then underwent cranial CAT scan and a series of laboratory tests to rule out possible reversible causes of cognitive decline and dementia (phase 4). Cognitive decline during the follow-up, regardless of subsequent clinical classification, was defined as loss of 3MS score by 8 units (~2 standard errors) from baseline, corresponding to a clinically significant decline [15, 23]. Following the accepted criteria [24] at WHIMS baseline, MCI was defined as poor performance (≤10th percentile in CERAD norms) on at least one CERAD test, evidence of functional impairment (but not severe enough to interfere with activities of daily living), and absence of psychiatric or other medical disorders (including probable dementia) that could explain the cognitive impairment. All clinical/testing data were then transmitted to central adjudication committee for final confirmation of dementia, based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition [25].
2.4 Measurement of Covariates
Participants completed structured questionnaires to provide baseline information on: demographics (age, race-ethnicity), socioeconomic status (SES, including education in years, family income, employment status), lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activity), and relevant clinical characteristics (use of menopausal HT, prior depression, CVD and related risk factors). Women were grouped according to body mass index (BMI, in kg/m2) categories (< 25.0 vs. 25.0–29.9 vs. ≥30.0). Hypertension was defined as antihypertensive medication or elevated blood pressure (systolic≥140 or diastolic≥90 mmHg). Treated diabetes mellitus (DM) was defined as a physician diagnosis plus oral medications or insulin therapy. History of CVD included previous coronary heart disease (myocardial infarction, coronary angioplasty, or coronary artery bypass graft), stroke, or transient ischemic attack. Good reliability and validity of both the self-reported medical histories and the physical measures have been documented [26–27]. The Burnam screening algorithm [28] was used to characterize the presence of prior depressive disorders, which increased the MCI/dementia risk [29] in WHIMS. Additional covariates included: the depressive symptoms (assessed by the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale [28]; insomnia symptoms (assessed by the well-validated WHI Insomnia Rating Scale [19, 30]); and self-rated frequency of snoring (“Did you snore”) and sleepiness (“falling asleep during quiet activities like reading, watching TV, or riding in a car”), based on a 5-point scale.
2.5 Statistical Analyses
We compared the distributions of sleep duration across different population characteristics, using chi-square tests. Follow-up time for each woman was calculated from WHI randomization (baseline) to the 3MS examination date triggering the ultimate classification of defined cognitive impairment, or the last date of completing annual cognitive assessment, whichever came first. Data on cognitive decline and incidence of MCI/dementia were analyzed separately. Because our exploratory analyses revealed a similar pattern of association between sleep duration and clinical endpoint separately for MCI and dementia, the MCI/dementia incidence was treated as a composite outcome variable, as also used in pooled analyses of WMIMS trials [21, 24]. Cox models were used to estimate hazard ratios (HRs) for each outcome associated with habitual sleep duration, adjusting for potential confounders. The assumed proportional hazard was supported by the proportionality test. Analytically, we used older women with self-reported sleep of 7 hours/night as the referent, because a growing number of sleep-health epidemiologic studies have found the lowest mortality and morbidity in people with seven hours of nightly sleep [31–32]. To obtain more precise estimates of incidence rate and the resulting HRs, small numbers of cases were pooled across the categories of more extreme sleep duration. We also stratified the effect estimates by prior histories of CVD and associated risk factors (DM, hypertension, and obesity) to assess whether these clinical attributes modified the putative neurocognitive effects of habitual sleep duration, using the tests of interaction. Several sensitivity analyses were carried out to evaluate whether the results were sensitive to use of sedatives/hypnotics/antidepressants or adjusting for concurrent depressive symptoms. Statistical analyses were performed using the SAS System for Windows, Version 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).
3. RESULTS
Distribution of Sleep Duration and Population Characteristics
Table 1 presents the population distribution of sleep duration in relation to selected baseline socio-demographics and lifestyle factors. In this cohort of 7444 older women (aged 70.1±3.8 years), 38.3% reported nightly sleep durations of 6 hours or less and 25.6% reported 8 hours or more. The proportion of short-duration sleepers (≤6 hours/night) was higher in minority women (50–57%) than that in white (36%). Older women with < high school education, family income ≤ $19,999, or alcohol use <1 drink/week were more likely to report sleep duration ≤6 hours/night, when compared to their counterparts. The percentage of extreme shorter sleep (≤5 hours/night) or longer sleep (≥9 hours/night) was higher in women with low physical activities (< 2 episodes/week), prior depression, or treated DM. We noted that the efficient randomization of HT assignment resulted in nearly identical distribution of sleep duration comparing older women in either E+P or E-alone with those in the control groups, making it unlikely to confound any putative associations with sleep duration.
Table 1.
Distributions* of Sleep Duration by Sociodemographics, Lifestyle Factors, and Clinical Characteristics in the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study Cohort, 1995–2008
Hours of sleep per night | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||
N† | ≤5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | ≥9 | p-value‡ | |
All subjects | 7444 | 696 (9.4%) | 2150 (28.9%) | 2688 (36.1%) | 1611 (21.6%) | 299 (4.0%) | |
| |||||||
Age group at screening | 0.0764 | ||||||
63–69 | 3419 | 313 (9.2%) | 924 (27%) | 1270 (37.1%) | 773 (22.6%) | 139 (4.1%) | |
70–74 | 2667 | 243 (9.1%) | 811 (30.4%) | 947 (35.5%) | 561 (21%) | 105 (3.9%) | |
≥75 | 1358 | 140 (10.3%) | 415 (30.6%) | 471 (34.7%) | 277 (20.4%) | 55 (4.1%) | |
| |||||||
Ethnicity | <.0001 | ||||||
Black or African-American | 528 | 105 (19.9%) | 196 (37.1%) | 134 (25.4%) | 72 (13.6%) | 21 (4%) | |
Hispanic White | 178 | 26 (14.6%) | 63 (35.4%) | 53 (29.8%) | 29 (16.3%) | 7 (3.9%) | |
White (not of Hispanic origin) | 6466 | 520 (8%) | 1797 (27.8%) | 2416 (37.4%) | 1471 (22.7%) | 262 (4.1%) | |
Other or Missing | 272 | 45 (16.5%) | 94 (34.6%) | 85 (31.3%) | 39 (14.3%) | 9 (3.3%) | |
| |||||||
Education | <0.0001 | ||||||
<High school | 569 | 92 (16.2%) | 169 (29.7%) | 168 (29.5%) | 113 (19.9%) | 27 (4.7%) | |
High school/GED | 1640 | 158 (9.6%) | 488 (29.8%) | 597 (36.4%) | 343 (20.9%) | 54 (3.3%) | |
School after high school | 2989 | 290 (9.7%) | 878 (29.4%) | 1052 (35.2%) | 638 (21.3%) | 131 (4.4%) | |
2989 | 290 (9.7%) | 878 (29.4%) | 1052 (35.2%) | 638 (21.3%) | 131 (4.4%) | ||
College degree or higher | 2224 | 155 (7%) | 606 (27.2%) | 863 (38.8%) | 515 (23.2%) | 85 (3.8%) | |
| |||||||
Family income | <0.0001 | ||||||
≤$ 19,999 | 1828 | 240 (13.1%) | 555 (30.4%) | 603 (33%) | 356 (19.5%) | 74 (4%) | |
$20,000 to $34,999 | 2204 | 187 (8.5%) | 615 (27.9%) | 827 (37.5%) | 484 (22%) | 91 (4.1%) | |
$35,000 to $49,999 | 1434 | 115 (8%) | 408 (28.5%) | 556 (38.8%) | 295 (20.6%) | 60 (4.2%) | |
$50,000 to $74,999 | 979 | 80 (8.2%) | 277 (28.3%) | 343 (35%) | 241 (24.6%) | 38 (3.9%) | |
≥$75,000 | 741 | 57 (7.7%) | 206 (27.8%) | 275 (37.1%) | 178 (24%) | 25 (3.4%) | |
Missing | 258 | 17 (6.6%) | 89 (34.5%) | 84 (32.6%) | 57 (22.1%) | 11 (4.3%) | |
| |||||||
Employment status | 0.0007 | ||||||
Currently employed | 1319 | 117 (8.9%) | 425 (32.2%) | 499 (37.8%) | 243 (18.4%) | 35 (2.7%) | |
Not working | 813 | 72 (8.9%) | 246 (30.3%) | 273 (33.6%) | 190 (23.4%) | 32 (3.9%) | |
Retired | 5287 | 504 (9.5%) | 1470 (27.8%) | 1908 (36.1%) | 1174 (22.2%) | 231 (4.4%) | |
| |||||||
Smoking | 0.896 | ||||||
Never smoked | 3899 | 361 (9.3%) | 1130 (29%) | 1415 (36.3%) | 846 (21.7%) | 147 (3.8%) | |
Past smoker | 2917 | 267 (9.2%) | 830 (28.5%) | 1062 (36.4%) | 635 (21.8%) | 123 (4.2%) | |
Current smoker | 524 | 54 (10.3%) | 159 (30.3%) | 177 (33.8%) | 110 (21%) | 24 (4.6%) | |
| |||||||
Alcohol consumption | <0.0001 | ||||||
Non drinker | 975 | 94 (9.6%) | 292 (29.9%) | 371 (38.1%) | 181 (18.6%) | 37 (3.8%) | |
Past drinker | 1463 | 164 (11.2%) | 417 (28.5%) | 501 (34.2%) | 310 (21.2%) | 71 (4.9%) | |
<1 drink per day | 4054 | 383 (9.4%) | 1188 (29.3%) | 1491 (36.8%) | 858 (21.2%) | 134 (3.3%) | |
>1 drink per day | 887 | 44 (5%) | 235 (26.5%) | 298 (33.6%) | 253 (28.5%) | 57 (6.4%) | |
| |||||||
Moderate or strenuous activities ≥ 20min | 0.0003 | ||||||
No activity | 4334 | 432 (10%) | 1270 (29.3%) | 1519 (35%) | 912 (21%) | 201 (4.6%) | |
Some activity | 361 | 37 (10.2%) | 92 (25.5%) | 127 (35.2%) | 82 (22.7%) | 23 (6.4%) | |
2–4 episodes/wk | 1451 | 123 (8.5%) | 416 (28.7%) | 533 (36.7%) | 338 (23.3%) | 41 (2.8%) | |
>4 episodes/wk | 1287 | 102 (7.9%) | 367 (28.5%) | 507 (39.4%) | 277 (21.5%) | 34 (2.6%) | |
| |||||||
Body mass Index (BMI) | 0.0926 | ||||||
<25 kg/m2 | 2158 | 182 (8.4%) | 609 (28.2%) | 833 (38.6%) | 453 (21%) | 81 (3.8%) | |
25–29 kg/m2 | 2695 | 250 (9.3%) | 805 (29.9%) | 950 (35.3%) | 583 (21.6%) | 107 (4%) | |
≥30 kg/m2 | 2548 | 262 (10.3%) | 722 (28.3%) | 883 (34.7%) | 571 (22.4%) | 110 (4.3%) | |
| |||||||
HT use ever | 0.3996 | ||||||
No | 4055 | 360 (8.9%) | 1170 (28.9%) | 1495 (36.9%) | 865 (21.3%) | 165 (4.1%) | |
Yes | 3387 | 336 (9.9%) | 979 (28.9%) | 1192 (35.2%) | 746 (22%) | 134 (4%) | |
| |||||||
History of Depression | <0.0001 | ||||||
No | 6669 | 584 (8.8%) | 1921 (28.8%) | 2429 (36.4%) | 1473 (22.1%) | 262 (3.9%) | |
Yes | 604 | 97 (16.1%) | 178 (29.5%) | 192 (31.8%) | 109 (18%) | 28 (4.6%) | |
| |||||||
Hypertension ever | 0.0379 | ||||||
No | 4462 | 391 (8.8%) | 1257 (28.2%) | 1657 (37.1%) | 971 (21.8%) | 186 (4.2%) | |
Yes | 2899 | 298 (10.3%) | 865 (29.8%) | 1000 (34.5%) | 627 (21.6%) | 109 (3.8%) | |
| |||||||
Treated for diabetes | 0.0397 | ||||||
No | 6946 | 636 (9.2%) | 2019 (29.1%) | 2521 (36.3%) | 1497 (21.6%) | 273 (3.9%) | |
Yes | 484 | 60 (12.4%) | 124 (25.6%) | 163 (33.7%) | 112 (23.1%) | 25 (5.2%) | |
| |||||||
High cholesterol requiring pills ever | 0.3536 | ||||||
No | 6009 | 550 (9.2%) | 1761 (29.3%) | 2169 (36.1%) | 1293 (21.5%) | 236 (3.9%) | |
Yes | 1339 | 137 (10.2%) | 361 (27%) | 483 (36.1%) | 299 (22.3%) | 59 (4.4%) | |
| |||||||
History of CVD | 0.0097 | ||||||
No | 6054 | 540 (8.9%) | 1720 (28.4%) | 2217 (36.6%) | 1335 (22.1%) | 242 (4%) | |
Yes | 1280 | 149 (11.6%) | 388 (30.3%) | 436 (34.1%) | 259 (20.2%) | 48 (3.8%) | |
| |||||||
Subcohort membership§ | |||||||
E-alone control | 1476 | 158 (10.7%) | 470 (31.8%) | 489 (33.1%) | 297 (20.1%) | 62 (4.2%) | 0.4757 |
E-alone intervention | 1461 | 169 (11.6%) | 426 (29.2%) | 482 (33%) | 322 (22%) | 62 (4.2%) | |
E+P control | 2290 | 187 (8.2%) | 647 (28.3%) | 875 (38.2%) | 493 (21.5%) | 88 (3.8%) | 0.9333 |
E+P intervention | 2217 | 182 (8.2%) | 607 (27.4%) | 842 (38%) | 499 (22.5%) | 87 (3.9%) |
The data represented the number (row %) belonging to each level of sleep duration, given for each subcategory of personal characteristics.
The total number of subjects summed up across each subcategory varies slightly because of missing values.
p-value for Chi-square test comparing the distribution of sleep duration across subcategories of each personal characteristics
E-alone: conjugated equine estrogen alone; E+P: estrogen plus progestin (medroxyprogesterone acetate)
Frequencies of Cognitive Declines and MCI/Dementia
There were 802 subjects with significant cognitive decline (i.e., 3MS decreased ≥ 8 units from baseline) and 549 new cases of MCI/dementia (368 MCI and 265 dementia including 84 progressed from MCI) during an average of 7.3–7.7 years of follow-up (Table 2). The frequencies of cognitive decline (p=0.0003) and MCI/dementia (p=0.0003) differed by habitual sleep duration, with greater cognitive decline and higher incidence of MCI/dementia noted in women reporting average sleep duration of ≤5, 6, 8 or ≥9 hours/night, as compared to women reporting sleep of 7 hours/night. Because of the relatively small numbers of events accrued in women with extreme sleep durations (e.g., 23 cases of MCI/dementia in women with sleep duration ≥9 hours/night and 65 in women with sleep duration ≤5 hours/night), we reclassified the sleep duration into three subcategories (≤6, 7, ≥8 hours/night) in the subsequent multicovariate-adjusted Cox models.
Table 2.
Incidence Rates for Having Significant Cognitive Decline and MCI/Dementia by Sleep Duration in WHIMS Cohort, 1995–2008
≥8 points decrease in 3MS† | Hours of sleep per night | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
≤ 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | ≥ 9 | |
N | 664 | 2078 | 2615 | 1562 | 286 |
Total person-years at risk | 4549.26 | 15254.74 | 19776.87 | 11548.77 | 2036.58 |
Number of significant cognitive decline | 88 | 269 | 246 | 169 | 30 |
Event rate (cases per 1000 person-years)* | 19.34 | 17.63 | 12.44 | 14.63 | 14.73 |
| |||||
MCI‡/dementia | Hours of sleep per night | ||||
| |||||
≤ 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | ≥ 9 | |
| |||||
N | 693 | 2149 | 2686 | 1610 | 298 |
Total person-years at risk | 4902.59 | 16352.33 | 21158.47 | 12387.62 | 2182.33 |
Number of MCI/dementia | 65 | 188 | 163 | 110 | 23 |
Event rate (cases per 1000 person-years)* | 13.26 | 11.50 | 7.70 | 8.88 | 10.54 |
Poisson regression with Chi-square test: p=0.0003 for the difference in 3MS decline across the category of sleep duration; p=0.0003 for the difference in MCI/dementia incidence across the category of sleep duration
3MS: Modified Mini-Mental State Examination
MCI: mild cognitive impairment
Adjusted Main Associations
Using a sleep time of 7 hours/night as the referent (Table 3), we found 35% and 22% increases in HRs for cognitive declines among women reporting sleep duration ≤ 6 and ≥8 hours/night, after adjustment for age and race/ethnicity (p<0.01). Additional adjustment for SES, lifestyles, depression, previous HT use, and conventional CVD risk factors resulted in only small changes to the estimated HR in women reporting sleep duration ≤6 hours/night, but the observed HR among women with ≥8 hours of sleep/night was attenuated. In the model accounting for multiple potential confounders (Model-V covariates in Table 3), the adjusted risk of cognitive decline significantly differed by self-reported habitual sleep pattern (p<0.01), with the HR=1.36 (95%CI: 1.14, 1.62) in short sleepers and the HR=1.18 (95%CI: 0.96, 1.45) among long sleepers. We also found 36% and 23% increases in HRs for MCI/dementia among women reporting sleep duration ≤ 6 and ≥8 hours/night (vs. 7 hours/night), after adjustment for age and race/ethnicity (p<0.01). These HR estimates were not substantially altered with further adjusting for SES, lifestyle factors, depression, and CVD-related clinical characteristics. In the model accounting for all these potential confounders (Model-V), habitual sleep duration was associated with subsequent risk for MCI/dementia (p=0.03), with the corresponding HR=1.36 (95%CI: 1.09, 1.71) in short sleepers and 1.27 (95%CI: 0.98, 1.64) among long sleepers.
Table 3.
Cox Models for Having Significant Cognitive Decline and MCI/Dementia Associated with Sleep Duration in WHIMS Cohort, 1995–2008
Hazard Ratios (95% confidence interval) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
≥8 points decrease in 3MS | Hours of sleep per night | |||
| ||||
≤ 6 | 7 | ≥ 8 | p-value | |
Crude analysis | 1.46 (1.24–1.72) | 1.00 | 1.18 (0.98–1.42) | <0.01 |
| ||||
Adjusted analyses | ||||
*Model-I | 1.35 (1.15–1.59) | 1.00 | 1.22 (1.01–1.47) | <0.01 |
†Model-II | 1.31 (1.11–1.55) | 1.00 | 1.20 (0.99–1.44) | <0.01 |
‡Model-III | 1.32 (1.12–1.56) | 1.00 | 1.20 (0.99–1.45) | <0.01 |
§Model-IV | 1.31 (1.10–1.56) | 1.00 | 1.19 (0.98–1.45) | <0.01 |
¶Model-V | 1.36 (1.14–1.62) | 1.00 | 1.18 (0.96–1.45) | <0.01 |
| ||||
MCI/dementia | Hours of sleep per night | |||
| ||||
≤ 6 | 7 | ≥ 8 | p-value | |
| ||||
Crude analysis | 1.57 (1.29–1.91) | 1.00 | 1.19 (0.95–1.50) | <0.01 |
| ||||
Adjusted analyses | ||||
*Model-I | 1.36 (1.11–1.66) | 1.00 | 1.23 (0.98–1.55) | <0.01 |
†Model-II | 1.31 (1.07–1.60) | 1.00 | 1.19 (0.94–1.50) | 0.03 |
‡Model-III | 1.31 (1.07–1.61) | 1.00 | 1.23 (0.98–1.56) | 0.03 |
§Model-IV | 1.34 (1.07–1.67) | 1.00 | 1.30 (1.01–1.67) | 0.02 |
¶Model-V | 1.36 (1.09–1.71) | 1.00 | 1.27 (0.98–1.64) | 0.03 |
Model-I: adjusted for age and race
Model-II: adjusted for age, race, and SES (education, family income, and employment status)
Model-III: adjusted for age, race, SES, and lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activities)
Model-IV: adjusted for age, race, SES, lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activities), and depression
Model-V: adjusted for age, race, SES, lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activities), depression, and other relevant clinical characteristics (previous HT use, BMI, prior CVD history, hypertension, DM, hypercholesterolemia)
All p-values were from Chi-square tests examining the difference in hazard ratio across the category of sleep duration.
Sensitivity Analyses
In sensitivity analyses, we found similar patterns of associations between habitual sleep duration and cognitive impairments. Effect estimates changed very little in further analyses after controlling for the influence of sedatives/hypnotics/antidepressants use or adjusting for concurrent depressive symptoms (CES-D, excluding sleep item). Comparing women reporting ≤6 and ≥8 hours of sleep/night to women with 7 hours of sleep/night in multicovariate-adjusted analyses (Model-V), the HRs (95%CIs) became 1.36 (1.13, 1.62) and 1.18 (0.96, 1.45) for significant cognitive decline (p<0.01) and 1.36 (1.08, 1.71) and 1.27 (0.98, 1.65) for MCI/dementia (p=0.03), after including the use of sedatives/hypnotics. Adjusting for the use of antidepressants resulted in the corresponding HRs of 1.36 (1.13, 1.62) and 1.17 (0.96, 1.44) for significant cognitive decline (p<0.01) and HRs of 1.36 (1.08, 1.70) and 1.25 (0.96, 1.62) for MCI/dementia (p=0.03). After we substituted the history of depression with “depressive symptoms,” the multivariable-adjusted HRs (95% CIs) were 1.35 (1.13, 1.61) and 1.13 (0.92, 1.38) for significant cognitive decline (p<0.01) and 1.35 (1.07, 1.70) and 1.22 (0.94, 1.60) for MCI/dementia (p=0.04), comparing women reporting ≤6 and ≥8 hours of sleep/night to women with 7 hours of sleep/night. The V-shaped associations remained robust in the additional analyses adjusting for insomnia symptoms or frequencies of snoring and sleepiness.
Effect Measure Modification by CVD/Risk Factors
There was little evidence that the adverse effects of short sleep were substantially modified by CVD/risk factors (e.g., hypertension, DM, or obesity), because the effects sizes were comparable in women with vs. without these clinical attributes (Tables 4 and 5). For instance, the HR for MCI/dementia associated with short sleep was 1.35 (1.06–1.72) in non-diabetic women and 1.39 (1.02–1.88) in those with DM. The estimates of putative adverse effects of long sleep were statistically significant without prior CVD (HR=1.32 [1.06–1.65] for cognitive decline; 1.35 [1.01–1.80] for MCI/dementia) or in non-obese women (HR=1.34 [1.05–1.72] for cognitive decline).
Table 4.
Associations (Hazard Ratios) of Significant Cognitive Decline (3MS Decreased ≥ 8 points) with Sleep Duration in WHIMS, Stratified by History of Cardiovascular Disease, Hypertension, Diabetes Mellitus, and Obesity
Cardiovascular Disease | Hypertension | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
Sleep duration per night | Yes (N=1228) |
No (N=5872) |
p-value* | Yes (N=2786) |
No (N=4338) |
p-value* |
Crude | ||||||
≤ 6 hrs | 1.63 (1.15–2.32) | 1.41 (1.17–1.70) | 1.63 (1.26–2.10) | 1.36 (1.09–1.69) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | <0.01 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.39 |
≥ 8 hrs | 0.69 (0.42–1.14) | 1.32 (1.07–1.62) | 1.17 (0.86–1.57) | 1.21 (0.94–1.54) | ||
Adjusted† | ||||||
<6 hrs | 1.61 (1.10–2.35) | 1.28 (1.05–1.57) | 1.47 (1.12–1.94) | 1.28 (1.01–1.61) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | <0.01 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.66 |
≥ 8 hrs | 0.67 (0.39–1.14) | 1.32 (1.06–1.65) | 1.19 (0.86–1.64) | 1.18 (0.91–1.54) | ||
| ||||||
Diabetes Mellitus | Obesity Status | |||||
| ||||||
Yes (N=457) |
No (N=6734) |
Obese: BMI≥30 kg/m2 (N=2482) |
Non-obese: BMI<30 kg/m2 (N=4684) |
|||
| ||||||
Crude | ||||||
≤ 6 hrs | 1.77 (1.02–3.06) | 1.42 (1.20–1.68) | 1.68 (1.26–2.24) | 1.38 (1.13–1.68) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.75 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.07 |
≥ 8 hrs | 1.36 (0.74–2.47) | 1.15 (0.94–1.40) | 0.99 (0.70–1.41) | 1.27 (1.02–1.59) | ||
Adjusted† | ||||||
≤ 6 hrs | 1.42 (0.80–2.52) | 1.35 (1.12–1.63) | 1.50 (1.11–2.05) | 1.28 (1.03–1.60) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.87 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.03 |
≥ 8 hrs | 1.07 (0.57–2.02) | 1.20 (0.97–1.49) | 0.91 (0.63–1.32) | 1.34 (1.05–1.72) |
Chi-square test p-value for interaction comparing the hazard ratios with potential effect modifier vs. hazard ratios without potential effect modifier
adjusted for age, race, SES, lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activities), depression, and other relevant clinical characteristics (previous HT use, BMI, prior CVD history, hypertension, DM, hypercholesterolemia)
Table 5.
The Associations (Hazard Ratios) of MCI/Dementia with Sleep Duration in WHIMS, Stratified by History of Cardiovascular Disease, Hypertension, Diabetes Mellitus, and Obesity
Cardiovascular Disease | Hypertension | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
Sleep duration per night | Yes (N=1278) |
No (N=6048) |
p-value* | Yes (N=2895) |
No (N=4458) |
p-value* |
Crude | ||||||
≤ 6 hrs | 1.64 (1.09–2.46) | 1.51 (1.2–1.89) | 1.47 (1.09, 1.98) | 1.61 (1.24, 2.11) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.28 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.42 |
≥ 8 hrs | 0.86 (0.50–1.47) | 1.23(0.95–1.60) | 1.32 (0.94, 1.85) | 1.09 (0.80, 1.51) | ||
Adjusted† | ||||||
<6 hrs | 1.67 (1.06–2.64) | 1.27 (0.98–1.64) | 1.35 (0.96, 1.89) | 1.39 (1.02, 1.88) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.14 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.43 |
≥ 8 hrs | 1.00 (0.56–1.78) | 1.35 (1.01–1.80) | 1.46 (1.00, 2.12) | 1.12 (0.78, 1.6) | ||
| ||||||
Diabetes Mellitus | Obesity Status | |||||
| ||||||
Yes (N=482) |
No (N=6940) |
p-value* | Obese: BMI≥30 kg/m2 (N=2546) |
Non-obese: BMI<30 kg/m2 (N=4847) |
p-value* | |
| ||||||
Crude | ||||||
≤ 6 hrs | 1.82 (0.99, 3.35) | 1.54 (1.25, 1.90) | 1.96 (1.36, 2.82) | 1.45 (1.14, 1.84) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.86 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.28 |
≥ 8 hrs | 1.37 (0.70, 2.67) | 1.16 (0.91, 1.48) | 1.22 (0.79, 1.87) | 1.20 (0.91, 1.57) | ||
Adjusted† | ||||||
≤ 6 hrs | 1.42 (0.73, 2.76) | 1.35 (1.06, 1.72) | 1.59 (1.06, 2.37) | 1.27 (0.97, 1.67) | ||
7 hrs | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.87 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.24 |
≥ 8 hrs | 1.14n(0.56, 2.33) | 1.29 (0.98, 1.71) | 1.12 (0.70, 1.79) | 1.36 (1.00, 1.86) |
Chi-square test p-value for interaction comparing the hazard ratios with potential effect modifier vs. hazard ratios without potential effect modifier
adjusted for age, race, SES, lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activities), depression, and other relevant clinical characteristics (previous HT use, BMI, prior CVD history, hypertension, DM, hypercholesterolemia)
4. DISCUSSION
During an average of more than 7-year follow-up in this group of community-dwelling, cognitively intact and generally healthy older women, both short-duration (≤6 hours/night) and long-duration (≥8 hours/night) sleepers had greater cognitive decline, and an increased risk for cognitive impairments including dementia, compared to those with self-reported sleep of 7 hours/night. These observed adverse neurocognitive effects remained after adjustment for demographics and socioeconomic status, and the associations could not be fully explained by lifestyle factors or several clinical risk factors for dementia. To our knowledge, this is the largest prospective study with the longest follow-up to demonstrate the consistent V-shaped associations linking habitual sleep duration to both cognitive decline and subsequent risk for MCI/dementia.
Our study aimed to address the data inconsistencies and methodological limitations of previous studies on the longitudinal associations between habitual sleep duration and increased risk of cognitive impairment (including dementia) and cognitive decline (Table 6). Although an adverse effect of sleep loss on MCI/dementia was suggested by the three published cohort studies [13–15], none of the reported associations with short sleep duration were statistically significant. Increased dementia risk associated with long sleep was noted in one report [13], which, however, did not differentiate night-time sleep from daytime napping, incorrectly assuming two different behavioral constructs with common antecedents and similar threats to cognitive health in the elderly [33]. Interpretation of results from these studies also suffered from the lack of rigorous control of potential confounders, such as physical activities, depressive symptoms, and CVD/risk factors. In our analyses, the V-shaped association between habitual sleep duration and MCI/dementia remained statistically significant (p=0.03) even after we accounted for sociodemographic features, lifestyle factors, depressive symptoms, and CVD-related clinical characteristics. Our study findings on cognitive decline in 3MS add to the growing evidence linking habitual sleep duration with cognitive aging in the elderly. The Nurses’ Health Study investigators did not find a statistically significant longitudinal association between self-reported sleep duration and cognitive decline [9, 12], but a V-shaped association was suggested in two other cohorts [10–11], which, however, only partially accounted for some relevant clinical characteristics and did not address the potential confounding by lifestyle factors (e.g., smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activities). In our analyses, the V-shaped association between habitual sleep duration and significant cognitive decline remained (p=0.01) even after we accounted for sociodemographic features, lifestyle factors, depression, and CVD-related clinical characteristics.
Table 6.
Summary of Longitudinal Studies Linking Sleep Duration with Cognitive Decline and Dementia
References | Study Design & Population | Measures of Sleep Duration | Neurocognitive Outcomes | Main Findings | Statistical Adjustment for Confounding | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Studies on MCI or Dementia | |||||||
| |||||||
Benito-Leon et al. [13] | 3.2-year (median) follow-up study on 3,286 participants (aged ≥65) in the Neurological Disorders in Central Spain cohort | Self-reported “total hours of sleep in a 24hour period” (nighttime sleep + daytime napping) at baseline; average daily total sleep duration grouped into five categories | Diagnosis of dementia based on DSM-IV; etiologic classification (e.g., AD); vascular dementia; dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease) based on acceptable clinical criteria | Adjusted RR (95% CI) |
|
||
All Dementia | AD | ||||||
≤5h | 1.87 (0.85, 4.15) | 2.40 (0.86, 6.66) | |||||
6h | 1.13 (0.48, 2.69) | 1.50 (0.50, 4.48) | |||||
7h | 1 [Reference] | 1 [Reference] | |||||
8h | 2.05 (0.97, 4.31) | 3.17 (1.22, 8.28) | |||||
≥9h | 2.18 (1.09, 4.37) | 2.58 (1.02, 6.50) | |||||
| |||||||
Yaffe et al. [15] | A 4.7-year (median) follow-up study on 298 women (aged 82.1 ±3.2 years) in the Study of Osteoporotic Fractures’ Sleep and Cognition Study | Home-based polysomographic measure of total sleep time, coded into tertiles |
|
Adjusted OR (95%CI) |
|
||
Sleep duration (min) | |||||||
Low (median: 269.9) | 1 [Reference] | ||||||
Mid (median: 358.2) | 0.58 (0.31, 1.09) | ||||||
High (median: 425.5) | 0.83 (0.46, 1.51) | ||||||
| |||||||
Hahn et al. [14] | 9-year follow-up of 214 Swedish adults who were aged ≥75 years at baseline and participated in the phase-II clinical examination including sleep assessment for the Kungsholmen Project | Self-reported change in sleep pattern (“a subjective feeling of reduced duration or depth of sleep compared to the subject’s own normal pattern”) from Comprehensive Psychopathological Rating Scale at baseline | Diagnosis of dementia based on DSM-IIIR; Diagnosis of AD based on NINCDS-ADRDA criteria, excluding those with MMSE≤23 at baseline or diagnosed with dementia at 3-year visit |
|
|
||
Adjusted HR (95% CI) Dementia: 1.50 (0.87, 2.59) AD: 2.77 (0.91, 3.14) | |||||||
| |||||||
Studies on Cognitive Declines | |||||||
| |||||||
Tworoger et al. [12] | 2-year study on 1,884 women aged 70–81 years, free of stroke in the in the Nurses’ Health Study, excluding antidepressants users and those with depression | Self-reported “total hours of sleep in a 24hour period” in 1986 and 2000 | Cognitive impairment: TICS score <31; Other domains: immediate/delayed recalls of East Boston Memory Test; delayed recall of TICS 10-word; categorical fluency (animals); digit span backwards test for working memory and attention | No associations between sleep duration and defined cognitive impairment or declines in cognitive performance |
|
||
| |||||||
Potvin et al. [11] | 1-year follow-up of 1,664 French-speaking adults aged 65–96 years in the Enquente sur la santé des aines (Survey on Elders’ Health) | Sleep duration subscale of the French version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index at baseline |
|
Adjusted OR (95% CI) |
|
||
General | Amnestic | ||||||
Women | |||||||
≤5 h | 1.31 (0.65, 2.65) | 1.60 (0.56, 4.61) | |||||
5–9h | [Reference] | [Reference] | |||||
≥9h | 2.10 (1.10, 4.00) | 3.70 (1.49, 9.17) | |||||
Men | |||||||
≤5h | 2.91 (1.24, 6.82) | 4.95 (1.72, 14.27) | |||||
5–9h | [Reference] | [Reference] | |||||
≥9h | 0.26 (0.03, 2.03) | 0.51 (0.06, 4.26) | |||||
No statistically significant associations for nonamnestic cognitive impairment | |||||||
| |||||||
Keage et al. [10] | 10-year follow-up of 2012 cognitively unimpaired individuals ≥65 years from the Healthy Ageing Project subcohort of the MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study | Self-reported sleep duration calculated from individual’s responses to a series of questions in the Healthy Ageing Project |
|
V-shaped association between sleep duration and cognitive declines |
|
||
Adjusted OR (95% CI) | |||||||
2-year | 10-year | ||||||
≤6.5h | 1.94 (1.01, 3.72) | 2.02 (1.17, 3.48) | |||||
6.5–8.5h | [Reference] | [Reference] | |||||
≥8.5h | 1.50 (0.72, 3.14) | 1.27 (0.64, 2.48) | |||||
| |||||||
Devore et al. [9] | 6.4-year (median) follow-up of 15,385 women ≥70 years and free of stroke and depression in the Nurses’ Health Study | Self-reported “total hours of sleep in a 24hour period” in 1986 (midlife, n=13,052) and 2000 (late life, n=15,263) | 4 assessments (each with six tests) over 6 years (1995–2001), including: global function (average score of six tests; TICS score) and verbal memory (averaging immediate/delayed recalls from the East Boston Memory Test and TICS 10-word list) | Inverse U-shaped association between late-life sleep duration and average cognition, but no association with trajectories of cognitive function over 6 years after accounting for learning effects in repeated neuropsychological assessments. |
|
Abbreviations: AD – Alzheimer’s Disease; BMI – Body Mass Index; CVD – Cardiovascular Disease; DSM-III/DSM-IV – Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd Edition/4th
Edition; HR – Hazard Ratio; HT – Hormone Therapy; MCI – Mild Cognitive Impairment; MMSE – Mini Mental State Examination; MRC–Medical Research Council; NINCDS-ARDA – National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association; OR – Odds Ratio; RR – Relative Risk; TICS – Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status.
CVD-related clinical characteristics: hypertension, diabetes mellitus, hypercholesterolemia, and/or history of cardiovascular disease.
The demonstrated consistent increase in MCI/dementia risk among short sleepers raises the questions of possible clinical mediators and underlying mechanisms. As large epidemiologic studies have linked short sleep duration with increased risks for CVD [31, 34] and metabolic disorders [35] that play a pivotal role in cognitive decline and dementia, one may consider cardiometabolic health indicators as plausible mediators linking sleep loss with pathological brain aging. However, we found that increased risk for MCI/dementia associated with sleep ≤6 hours/night remained statistically significant (HR=1.36 [1.09–1.71]) after adjusting for CVD and conventional risk factors, and such adverse effects were also present in older women without hypertension (HR=1.39 [1.02–1.88]) or DM (HR=1.35 [1.06–1.72]). One neuroimaging study on community-dwelling older adults (n=70; aged: 76.4±8.0 years) found self-reported short sleep correlated with Aβ deposition even after adjusting for CVD and BMI [36], and one night of total sleep deprivation might perturb Aβ metabolism in healthy brains [37]. Emerging data from animal models with sleep deprivation point to likely complex mechanism that may involve activation of neuroendocrine stress axis [38], neuroinflammation [39] and oxidative stress [40] in hippocampus, interruption of hippocampal neurogenesis [41], and augmentation of aging-related protein misfolding with resulting proapoptosis [42]. Together these data suggest the possibility of direct effects of long-term sleep loss on pathological brain aging and the neuropathological pathways may go beyond cerebrovascular damages.
Several important questions on mechanistic mediators may arise if our study findings are replicated to demonstrate that long sleep duration is an independent neurobehavioral risk factor for cognitive impairment. For instance, what exactly predisposes long sleepers to be more likely to develop MCI/dementia, than those usually with 7 hours of sleep? Does long sleep duration reflect a proinflammatory state [43], especially in women [44], making the affected individuals more susceptible to microglia activation [45] involved in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease? Are habitual long sleepers more likely to reset their circadian rhythms for rest-activity patterns (e.g., decreased circadian activity rhythms with less stimulating social activities) that predispose them to the neurodegenerative processes [46]? Is it possible that the longer than average duration of recumbent position increases the period with high intracranial pressure which may alter the cerebrospinal/interstitial fluid dynamic and compromise the β-amyloid clearance during sleep [47]?
The reported detrimental health effects associated with habitual sleep patterns have been criticized for either not accounting for potential confounding by co-existing depression [48] or simply representing the adverse effects of other common sleep disorders such as chronic insomnia or sleep apnea [49]. Although WHIMS participants with prior depression were more likely to have short sleep duration (Table 1) than those without depression, we did not find strong confounding by depression; adjusting for prior depression (Model-IV and V in Table 3), use of antidepressants, or concurrent depressive symptoms did not alter the observed associations. We noted that short sleepers in WHIMS did complain of more insomnia symptoms and report more frequent snoring and sleepiness (data not shown). Long sleepers in WHIMS, although also reporting frequent snoring and sleepiness, did not have more insomnia symptoms (data not shown). Considering these differences, we thus conducted additional analyses adjusting for insomnia symptoms and frequencies of snoring and sleepiness along with the covariates already included in the full model (Model V in Table 3). The V-shaped associations remained robust in these additional analyses, suggesting that the adverse neurocognitive effects of habitual short and long sleep durations are possibly independent of other sub-constructs of sleep disturbance in older women.
We recognize several limitations in our study. First, our classification of habitual sleep duration relied on self-reports. However, it has been shown that objective sleep measures have a much larger nightly variability of sleep duration than their corresponding yearly variability [50], indicating that usual sleep behavior changes little in one year, despite large short-term fluctuations. Good reproducibility of exposure classification of self-reported habitual sleep duration has been documented in large cohort studies [9, 51]. Second, although the multi-ethnic WHIMS cohort was recruited from communities, certain population characteristics may limit the generalizability of our findings. Because WHIMS participants had relatively high educational attainment, we might have underestimated the long-term neurocognitive effects associated with unhealthful sleep patterns in subpopulations of low SES that are presumably more sensitive to the detrimental health effects of sleep disturbance. Also, because WHIMS participants were all postmenopausal women aged 65 years or older, future investigations in middle-aged women and in men are needed to clarify the importance of different sleep duration during vulnerable periods and possible sex-specificity. Third, we were unable to provide reliable effect of more extreme sleep durations (e.g., ≥ 9 hours/night), due to low statistical power in such pair-wise comparisons based on a small number of events and possible attrition of older women with extreme sleep patterns. Fourth, we did not conduct analyses on dementia subtypes, also concerning the lack of statistical power. Extended WHIMS follow-up may offer an opportunity to examine the risk of Alzheimer’s disease associated with habitual sleep duration. Finally, although many potential confounders and relevant clinical characteristic were included in our analyses, we cannot rule out the possibility of unmeasured confounding or alternative explanations. For instance, extreme sleep patterns may be indicative of the presence of psychosocial attributes or environmental stressors which could lead to adverse neurocognitive outcomes by alternative pathways other than through the mediation of conventional CVD risk factors or associated lifestyle modification. Extreme sleep patterns may share common neurobiological changes [52] underlying subclinical dementia or simply reflect prodromal manifestation of subsequent cognitive impairment.
In summary, this prospective study of cognitively intact and generally healthy older women shows a consistent pattern of V-shaped associations linking habitual sleep duration to significant decline of global cognitive function and subsequent risk for developing cognitive impairment including dementia. These results do not only substantiate the important role of sleep duration in determining cognitive decline, but also support the hypothesized adverse effect of chronic sleep loss on pathological brain aging.
Research in Context.
1. Systematic review
A standard literature search (e.g., PubMed, PsycINFO) was conducted to identify original papers (focused on clinical endpoints or repeated cognitive assessment), official reports (e.g., IOM), and review articles. We found neither consistent nor convincing data on sleep duration and dementia risk. Several methodological limitations of previous research were uncovered. Whether vascular risk factors interacting with sleep duration and cognitive impairment was unclear.
2. Interpretation
We demonstrated consistent V-shaped associations linking sleep duration with cognitive decline and subsequent risk for MCI/dementia in older women. Increased risks (~30% elevation) in short sleepers (≤6 hours/night) remained after rigorous control for potential confounding, even present in those with low CVD risk. Our study results support the hypothesized adverse effect of chronic sleep loss on pathological brain aging and suggest the underlying mechanisms may go beyond cerebrovascular damages.
3. Future directions
Multidisciplinary researches are needed to better understand mechanistic mediators, substantiate the causal link, and develop behavioral intervention for optimal sleep and healthy brain aging.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful for the dedicated efforts of all investigators and staff at the WHI and WHIMS clinical centers as well as the WHI & WHIMS clinical coordinating center listed in: https://cleo.whi.org/researchers/Documents%20%20Write%20a%20Paper/WHI%20Investigator%20Short%20List.pdf.
Funding support: The WHI program is funded by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health, U.S. DHHS through contracts HHSN268201100046C, HHSN268201100001C, HHSN268201100002C, HSN268201100003C, HHSN268201100004C, and HSN271201100004C. The WHIMS was funded by Wyeth Pharmaceuticals, Inc, St. Davids, PA, and Wake Forest University. Chen was supported in part by the NIH award R21HL89422 and R01AG033078. Phillips was supported in part by NIH award DK066204 and VA HSR&D awards SHP 08-144 and IIR 07-138.
Financial disclosures: This is not an industry supported study. During the performance of this study, Robinson received research grants from Abbott, Aegerion, Astra-Zeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Daiichi-Sankyo, Hoffman la Roche, Merck, Merck Schering Plough, and Takeda. She received speaker honoraria from Merck Schering Plough and served as a consultant for Astra-Zeneca and Merck Schering Plough
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
The authors declare no conflict of interest or competing financial interests in conjunction with publishing this scientific work.
All the other authors declared no conflicts of interest, including specific financial interests and relationships and affiliations relevant to the subject of submitted manuscript.
References
- 1.Patrick GTW, Gilbert J. On the effect of loss of sleep. Psychol Rev. 1896;3:469–86. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Roth T, Costa e Silva JA, Chase MH. Sleep and cognitive (memory) function: research and clinical perspectives. Sleep Med. 2001;2:379–87. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(01)00071-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.CDC. Effect of short sleep duration on daily activities–United States, 2005–2008. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2011;60:239–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schmutte T, Harris S, Levin R, Zweig R, Katz M, Lipton R. The relation between cognitive functioning and self-reported sleep complaints in nondemented older adults: results from the Bronx aging study. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5:39–56. doi: 10.1207/s15402010bsm0501_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Akbaraly TN, Marmot MG, Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A. Change in sleep duration and cognitive function: findings from the Whitehall II Study. Sleep. 2011;34:565–73. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kronholm E, Sallinen M, Suutama T, Sulkava R, Era P, Partonen T. Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:436–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00765.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ohayon MM, Vecchierini MF. Daytime sleepiness and cognitive impairment in the elderly population. Arch Intern Med. 2002;162:201–8. doi: 10.1001/archinte.162.2.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Xu L, Jiang CQ, Lam TH, Liu B, Jin YL, Zhu T, et al. Short or long sleep duration is associated with memory impairment in older Chinese: the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Sleep. 2011;34:575–80. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Devore EE, Grodstein F, Duffy JF, Stampfer MJ, Czeisler CA, Schernhammer ES. Sleep duration in midlife and later life in relation to cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62:1073–81. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Keage HA, Banks S, Yang KL, Morgan K, Brayne C, Matthews FE. What sleep characteristics predict cognitive decline in the elderly? Sleep Med. 2012;13:886–92. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Potvin O, Lorrain D, Forget H, Dube M, Grenier S, Preville M, et al. Sleep quality and 1-year incident cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults. Sleep. 2012;35:491–9. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tworoger SS, Lee S, Schernhammer ES, Grodstein F. The association of self-reported sleep duration, difficulty sleeping, and snoring with cognitive function in older women. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20:41–8. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000201850.52707.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Benito-Leon J, Bermejo-Pareja F, Vega S, Louis ED. Total daily sleep duration and the risk of dementia: a prospective population-based study. Eur J Neurol. 2009;16:990–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hahn EA, Wang HX, Andel R, Fratiglioni L. A Change in Sleep Pattern May Predict Alzheimer Disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Yaffe K, Laffan AM, Harrison SL, Redline S, Spira AP, Ensrud KE, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women. JAMA. 2011;306:613–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shumaker SA, Reboussin BA, Espeland MA, Rapp SR, McBee WL, Dailey M, et al. The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS): a trial of the effect of estrogen therapy in preventing and slowing the progression of dementia. Control Clin Trials. 1998;19:604–21. doi: 10.1016/s0197-2456(98)00038-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Design of the Women’s Health Initiative clinical trial and observational study. The Women’s Health Initiative Study Group. Control Clin Trials. 1998;19:61–109. doi: 10.1016/s0197-2456(97)00078-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Matthews KA, Shumaker SA, Bowen DJ, Langer RD, Hunt JR, Kaplan RM, et al. Women’s health initiative. Why now? What is it? What’s new? Am Psychol. 1997;52:101–16. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.2.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Levine DW, Kaplan RM, Kripke DF, Bowen DJ, Naughton MJ, Shumaker SA. Factor structure and measurement invariance of the Women’s Health Initiative Insomnia Rating Scale. Psychol Assess. 2003;15:123–36. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48:314–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Kuller L, Rapp SR, Thal L, Lane DS, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. 2004;291:2947–58. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1989;39:1159–65. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rapp SR, Espeland MA, Shumaker SA, Henderson VW, Brunner RL, Manson JE, et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;289:2663–72. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Rapp SR, Thal L, Wallace RB, Ockene JK, et al. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;289:2651–62. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fourth. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Langer RD, White E, Lewis CE, Kotchen JM, Hendrix SL, Trevisan M. The Women’s Health Initiative Observational Study: baseline characteristics of participants and reliability of baseline measures. Ann Epidemiol. 2003;13:S107–21. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(03)00047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Heckbert SR, Kooperberg C, Safford MM, Psaty BM, Hsia J, McTiernan A, et al. Comparison of self-report, hospital discharge codes, and adjudication of cardiovascular events in the Women’s Health Initiative. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160:1152–8. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Burnam MA, Wells KB, Leake B, Landsverk J. Development of a brief screening instrument for detecting depressive disorders. Med Care. 1988;26:775–89. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198808000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Goveas JS, Espeland MA, Woods NF, Wassertheil-Smoller S, Kotchen JM. Depressive symptoms and incidence of mild cognitive impairment and probable dementia in elderly women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:57–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03233.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Levine DW, Kripke DF, Kaplan RM, Lewis MA, Naughton MJ, Bowen DJ, et al. Reliability and validity of the Women’s Health Initiative Insomnia Rating Scale. Psychol Assess. 2003;15:137–48. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Chen JC, Brunner RL, Ren H, Wassertheil-Smoller S, Larson JC, Levine DW, et al. Sleep duration and risk of ischemic stroke in postmenopausal women. Stroke. 2008;39:3185–92. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.521773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Institute of Medicine. Sleep disorders and sleep deprivation: an unmet public health problem. Washington, DC: Committee on Sleep, Medicine Research, Institute of Medicine, National Academies Press; 2006. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Vitiello MV. We have much more to learn about the relationships between napping and health in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1753–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ayas NT, White DP, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Speizer FE, Malhotra A, et al. A prospective study of sleep duration and coronary heart disease in women. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:205–9. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ayas NT, White DP, Al-Delaimy WK, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Speizer FE, et al. A prospective study of self-reported sleep duration and incident diabetes in women. Diabetes Care. 2003;26:380–4. doi: 10.2337/diacare.26.2.380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Spira AP, Gamaldo AA, An Y, Wu MN, Simonsick EM, Bilgel M, et al. Self-reported sleep and beta-amyloid deposition in community-dwelling older adults. JAMA Neurol. 2013;70:1537–43. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ooms S, Overeem S, Besse K, Rikkert MO, Verbeek M, Claassen JA. Effect of 1 Night of Total Sleep Deprivation on Cerebrospinal Fluid beta-Amyloid 42 in Healthy Middle-Aged Men: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Neurol. 2014 doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. Restricted and disrupted sleep: effects on autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12:197–210. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Zhu B, Dong Y, Xu Z, Gompf HS, Ward SA, Xue Z, et al. Sleep disturbance induces neuroinflammation and impairment of learning and memory. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;48:348–55. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Silva RH, Abilio VC, Takatsu AL, Kameda SR, Grassl C, Chehin AB, et al. Role of hippocampal oxidative stress in memory deficits induced by sleep deprivation in mice. Neuropharmacology. 2004;46:895–903. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2003.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mirescu C, Peters JD, Noiman L, Gould E. Sleep deprivation inhibits adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus by elevating glucocorticoids. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:19170–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608644103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Naidoo N, Ferber M, Master M, Zhu Y, Pack AI. Aging impairs the unfolded protein response to sleep deprivation and leads to proapoptotic signaling. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6539–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5685-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Grandner MA, Sands-Lincoln MR, Pak VM, Garland SN. Sleep duration, cardiovascular disease, and proinflammatory biomarkers. Nat Sci Sleep. 2013;5:93–107. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S31063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Miller MA, Kandala NB, Kivimaki M, Kumari M, Brunner EJ, Lowe GD, et al. Gender differences in the cross-sectional relationships between sleep duration and markers of inflammation: Whitehall II study. Sleep. 2009;32:857–64. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Block ML, Zecca L, Hong JS. Microglia-mediated neurotoxicity: uncovering the molecular mechanisms. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:57–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn2038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lim AS, Saper CB. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and dementia. Ann Neurol. 2011;70:677–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.22637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Xie L, Kang H, Xu Q, Chen MJ, Liao Y, Thiyagarajan M, et al. Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science. 2013;342:373–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1241224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mezick EJ, Hall M, Matthews KA. Are sleep and depression independent or overlapping risk factors for cardiometabolic disease? Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15:51–63. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bliwise DL, Young TB. The Parable of Parabola: What the U-Shaped Curve Can and Cannot Tell Us about Sleep. Sleep. 2007;30:1164–5. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.12.1614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Knutson KL, Rathouz PJ, Yan LL, Liu K, Lauderdale DS. Intra-individual daily and yearly variability in actigraphically recorded sleep measures: the CARDIA study. Sleep. 2007;30:793–6. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.6.793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ, White DP, Hu FB. Correlates of long sleep duration. Sleep. 2006;29:881–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.7.881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Harper DG, Stopa EG, Kuo-Leblanc V, McKee AC, Asayama K, Volicer L, et al. Dorsomedial SCN neuronal subpopulations subserve different functions in human dementia. Brain. 2008;131:1609–17. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]