Abstract
Several studies demonstrated that face perception is biased by the prior presentation of another face, a phenomenon termed as face-related after-effect (FAE). FAE is linked to a neural signal-reduction at occipito-temporal areas and it can be observed in the amplitude modulation of the early event-related potential (ERP) components. Recently, macaque single-cell recording studies suggested that manipulating the duration of the adaptor makes the selective adaptation of different visual motion processing steps possible. To date, however, only a few studies tested the effects of adaptor duration on the electrophysiological correlates of human face processing directly. The goal of the current study was to test the effect of adaptor duration on the image-, identity-, and generic category-specific face processing steps. To this end, in a two-alternative forced choice familiarity decision task we used five adaptor durations (ranging from 200–5000 ms) and four adaptor categories: adaptor and test were identical images—Repetition Suppression (RS); adaptor and test were different images of the Same Identity (SameID); adaptor and test images depicted Different Identities (DiffID); the adaptor was a Fourier phase-randomized image (No). Behaviorally, a strong priming effect was observed in both accuracy and response times for RS compared with both DiffID and No. The electrophysiological results suggest that rapid adaptation leads to a category-specific modulation of P100, N170, and N250. In addition, both identity and image-specific processes affected the N250 component during rapid adaptation. On the other hand, prolonged (5000 ms) adaptation enhanced, and extended category-specific adaptation processes over all tested ERP components. Additionally, prolonged adaptation led to the emergence of image-, and identity-specific modulations on the N170 and P2 components as well. In other words, there was a clear dissociation among category, identity-, and image-specific processing steps in the case of longer (3500 and 5000 ms) but not for shorter durations (< 3500 ms), reflected in the gradual reduction of N170 and enhancement of P2 in the No, DiffID, SameID, and RS conditions. Our findings imply that by manipulating adaptation duration one can dissociate the various steps of human face processing, reflected in the ERP response.
Keywords: neural adaptation, adaptation duration, face processing, event-related potentials, face after-effect
Introduction
The ability of the visual system to rapidly adjust to changing environmental conditions is one of its key characteristics (Mesik et al., 2013). Following the prolonged exposure of a visual stimulus, a subsequently viewed stimulus sometimes appears conspicuously distorted. This phenomenon, termed as adaptation-related after-effect, can affect the perception of various stimulus attributes (Müller et al., 2009). Moreover, adaptation has served as a powerful psychophysical tool in the past for demonstrating selective neural sensitivities to different stimulus dimensions from low-level stimulus features (such as contrast, orientation, spatial frequency, texture, or motion) to high-level object and face properties (Fang et al., 2006). Regarding faces, several studies have demonstrated that the way we perceive human faces can be systematically biased by a previously presented face. At the behavioral level, such face-adaptation-related after-effects are present for various dimensions or aspects of face processing—such as identity (Leopold et al., 2001), distortion (Webster and MacLin, 1999), race, expression, gender (Webster et al., 2004; Kovács et al., 2005, 2006, 2007), attractiveness (Rhodes et al., 2003), or eye-gaze direction (Schweinberger et al., 2007a). It is worth noting, however, that in some cases, repeated presentations of a given stimulus (or subsequent presentations of two stimuli from the same category) do not lead to perceptual biases (as in the case of adaptation-related after-effects) but rather to behavioral facilitations. The latter phenomenon is usually referred to as priming, typically associated with faster and/or more accurate responses. Although these two phenomena share some properties, such as their size-, viewpoint-, or position-invariances, they are clearly distinguishable regarding their behavioral consequences (Zhao and Chubb, 2001; Brooks et al., 2002; Zimmer and Kovács, 2011a). Since there are both differences and similarities at the behavioral level, the degree to which the two phenomena share the same neuronal mechanisms is under debate. It is well-known that repetition of a particular stimulus usually lowers the firing rate of the responsive neurons, reduces the BOLD responses and the MEG/EEG signals (for reviews see Grill-Spector et al., 2006; Krekelberg et al., 2006). In the case of faces, various cortical areas and structures showed decreased activation in an fMRI adaptation paradigm (fMRI-A; Grill-Spector and Malach, 2001), such as the fusiform face area (FFA, Kanwisher et al., 1997), the occipital face area (OFA, Gauthier et al., 2000), the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS, Puce et al., 1998), or the lateral occipital complex (LO or LOC, Malach et al., 1995). In electrophysiological studies, neural adaptation [or its clearest form: the so-called repetition suppression (RS)] paradigms provide a powerful technique to determine the functional properties of face-evoked ERP components and their relation to underlying neural processing modules (Eimer et al., 2011).
Regarding human face perception, electrophysiological studies have described a large, early positive (P1 or P100) and a negative (N170) wave over the occipito-temporal areas (Bentin et al., 1996; Eimer, 2000; Itier and Taylor, 2004a). Although several researchers noted that P100 also responds to faces when compared to other non-face objects (Itier and Taylor, 2004b; Herrmann et al., 2005a), most studies linked P100 rather to early pictorial encoding (Desjardins and Segalowitz, 2009). The N170 is typically regarded as a marker for the structural encoding of faces (Schweinberger, 2011). This component is larger in amplitude and shorter in latency to face stimuli than to non-face objects (Bentin et al., 1996; Rossion and Jacques, 2008) and has clear right hemisphere dominance (Bentin et al., 1996; Allison et al., 1999). The subsequent component of the ERP, the P2, is characterized by a positive-going deflection over the lateral occipito-temporal areas peaking at about 200–220 ms after stimulus onset. This component has been linked to the processing of facial relations between facial features in individual faces (Latinus and Taylor, 2006) and generally larger P2s were observed in tasks needing expertise (Stahl et al., 2008; Wiese et al., 2009). Altogether, this component might be involved in the deeper and more advanced analysis of faces when compared to earlier face-evoked components (Németh et al., 2014). In contrast to the N170, the second negative peak, the so-called N250 (or N250r), has consistently emerged as an electrophysiological correlate of face recognition (Schweinberger, 2011). This later negative component reaches its maximum between 230 and 330 ms after stimulus onset and is generated by the FFA (Eger et al., 2005).
The electrophysiological correlates of facial after-effects have been reported as early as 140–200 ms post-stimulus onset (in the time window of the N170 component). Following the initial study of Kovács et al. (2006), further works showed reduction of the N170 (or the M170 in magnetoencephalographic studies) when the adaptor image was a face when compared with noise stimuli or non-face objects (Harris and Nakayama, 2007, 2008; Kloth et al., 2010; Nemrodov and Itier, 2012). It is worth noting, however, that in most studies, this N170 adaptation to identity-congruent adaptors seems small or absent (Amihai et al., 2011). Even though the authors of previous face adaptation studies did not concentrate directly on the positive ERP components (P100 and P2), the increase of their amplitude after adaptation is evident in the grand average ERP figures of these studies (Kovács et al., 2007; Zimmer and Kovács, 2011b; Feuerriegel et al., 2015). These findings show that there are different modulating effects on the positive components (signal enhancement) when compared with the N170 as a consequence of adaptation, suggesting separate mechanisms that elicit these components and different roles that they play in face perception. It can also be hypothesized that the effect reflected on the positive components is linked to priming rather than to adaptation. The face-evoked ERP component that is more closely associated with face repetition priming is the N250 (N250r) component (Schweinberger et al., 2007b). Although a clear N250 amplitude increase was observed for repetitions of familiar faces across different images, this increase was larger for repetitions using the pixel-by-pixel same image when compared with different images of the same identity (Schweinberger et al., 2002a).
To the best of our knowledge, so far the only study testing priming and adaptation simultaneously within a single paradigm is a study by Walther et al. (2013). The authors presented pairs of stimuli, where the first stimulus was either a famous face (from three different identities) or a 50/50% morph between two famous faces and a Fourier phase-randomized image that served as control. The second, test stimulus was always a face from the morph continuum. In a 2AFC identity discrimination task the authors dissociated priming and adaptation effects based on the ambiguity of the test stimuli. They concluded that their results show that face-related adaptation and priming are both present in the same paradigm, but are never observed simultaneously for a given test stimulus. This indicates that exclusive mechanisms may underlie both adaptation and priming.
One of the important parameters affecting the mechanisms of adaptation is its timescale and this can be tested by either the manipulation of the exposition time of the adaptor or by using different adaptor-test image inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs). Determining how adaptation duration influences changes in neuronal response properties is central to the understanding whether the sensory system employs different strategies for adjusting its sensitivity on the different timescales (Patterson et al., 2013). From single cell recording results, we know that brief and prolonged adaptation can lead to qualitatively different changes in neural tuning. For example, adaptation takes place in motion direction selective neurons of macaque area MT after short but not after long exposure to their preferred motion direction (Priebe et al., 2002; Kohn and Movshon, 2003). In the case of the orientation selective neurons of V1, Patterson et al. (2013) found that adaptation with small gratings reduced responsivity and caused tuning shifts away from the adaptor grating. This effect became more pronounced with more prolonged adaptation durations. Brief and prolonged adaptation produced, however, indistinguishable effects on responsivity, but caused opposite shifts in tuning preferences in the case of large gratings. Regarding human face perception based on psychophysical data, it was shown that perceptual after-effects (namely the face identity after-effect) for simple visual attributes processed early in the cortical hierarchy increase logarithmically with adapting duration and decay exponentially with test duration (Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007). In human electrophysiological [or magnetoencephalographic (MEG)] experiments where the adaptor and test stimuli are presented in rapid succession, strong N/M170 amplitude reductions are observed for every face (or face-part) adapted condition (Harris and Nakayama, 2007, 2008; Eimer et al., 2010; Nemrodov and Itier, 2011, 2012). In one of our previous studies, we manipulated the effect of adaptation duration and stimulus position together (Kovács et al., 2007). We have found that that for shorter adaptor durations (500 ms) the adaptation effect was entirely position independent, whereas for longer durations (5000 ms), the effect was partly position invariant but partly position dependent. As it has already been mentioned, other studies varied the ISI to test the timescale of adaptation. Daelli et al. (2010) presented prototypical adaptors (S1), followed by ambiguous target objects (S2), and found aftereffects when the ISI was short (50 ms) but priming effects when the ISI was long (3100 ms). An MEG study by Harris and Nakayama (2007) also investigated the effects elicited by different ISIs (100, 200, 300, and 600 ms) and found that the M170 response to the second face decreased in a linear manner with decreasing ISI. This shows that adaptation depends on the ISI, even though this effect is short-lived as suggested by the fact that for an ISI of 800 ms there was no difference between adapted and non-adapted conditions. In a recent work, Feuerriegel et al. (2015) measured the category-specificity of adaptation over a limited range of adaptor durations (200, 500, and 1000 ms) and for two short ISIs (200 and 500 ms). Their results indicated that, not surprisingly, at the level of N170 there was no category adaptation for faces. Face adaptors led to the smallest N170 amplitudes for both target faces and chairs after 500 ms adaptor duration.
In the present study, we systematically manipulated the duration of adaptation in the commonly applied range of 200–5000 ms with different facial adaptation conditions. In a 2AFC familiarity decision task we used three different face adaptors: different identity (DiffID), different image of the same identity (SameID), and identical image of the same person (repetition suppression, RS). A Fourier phase-randomized noise image served as control (No). By using these conditions we intended to separate three major steps of face processing. The GENERIC, face category specific processes were isolated by the comparison of noise adaptation (No) with the adaptation to a face that is unrelated to the test stimulus identity (DiffID). IDENTITY-SPECIFIC effects were tested by the DiffID vs. SameID comparison while IMAGE-SPECIFIC effects were measured by the SameID vs. RS comparison. We hypothesized that by varying the duration of adaptation time, we might dissociate generic-, identity-, and image-based information processing, reflected on the face-evoked ERP components.
Materials and methods
Participants
Sixteen naïve healthy-volunteers (15 right-handed and one ambidextrous, six females, mean age = 22.69 years, ± 4.83 years SD) participated in the study. They received partial course credits for their participation and gave signed, informed consent in accordance with the Ethical Committee of the Budapest University of Technology and Economics prior to testing. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, no previous history of any neurological or ophthalmological diseases and were not under any medication. One additional participant was excluded from the behavioral and electrophysiological analysis (right-handed 22 years old male) due to an insufficient number of recognized familiar faces and a high number of recognized unfamiliar faces, which was evident in the post-testing questionnaire.
Stimuli
Grayscale images of familiar and unfamiliar faces were used, with sixteen different identities (eight female) for each category. Familiar faces were digital images of famous persons, ranging from Hollywood celebrities to politicians that Hungarian students were likely to recognize, whereas unfamiliar faces were digital images of less-known persons that were unlikely to be recognized by the participants (such as Finnish politicians or actresses from Iceland). Two different images portraying the same person were used for each identity, corresponding to a number of 32 images in total. All images were downloaded from freely available websites and converted into grayscale (8 bit) using Adobe Photoshop CS3 Extended 10.0 (Adobe Systems Inc.). Images were then cropped to reveal only the contour of the face, including hair. Stimuli subtended 3.6 × 5° of visual angle. Since previous studies have shown that early ERP components, such as P100, are sensitive to luminance (Johannes et al., 1995) and that neural processes are sensitive to luminance histogram skewness (Olman et al., 2008), stimuli were equated in luminance and their histograms were matched using the lummatch and histmatch functions of MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick Massachusetts, USA) SHINE toolbox (Willenbockel et al., 2010).
Three different images were used as adaptors: an image that was identical to the test stimulus [repetition suppression (RS) condition], a different image depicting the same identity as the test stimulus [same identity (SameID) condition], and an image depicting a same gender different identity than the test stimulus [different identity (DiffID) condition]. For the control condition, sixteen Fourier phase-randomized images were created from the face images and used as adaptors [non-adapted (No) condition].
Stimuli were presented centrally on a uniform gray background. An LG Flatron W2600 HP monitor (resolution: 1920 × 1200 pixels, refresh rate: 60 Hz) was used for stimulus presentation, while viewing distance (57 cm) was maintained constant using a chinrest. Stimulus presentation was controlled using MATLAB 2008a Psychtoolbox 3.0.9 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) and custom-made scripts.
Procedure
Subjects were instructed to perform a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) familiarity task for faces by pressing the key labeled “7” on a keyboard when the second, target face was perceived as familiar and the key labeled “8” when it was perceived as unfamiliar. A yellow fixation cross presented in the center of the screen indicated the presence of the target stimulus to the subject. Prior to the presentation of the target, a gray screen was presented for a random period of time between 500 and 700 ms, followed by the adaptor image. The duration of the adaptor image varied randomly between one of five values: 200, 1200, 2000, 3500, and 5000 ms. The adaptor was followed by a 500 ms gray screen after which the target stimulus appeared for 200 ms (Figure 1). To ensure that the subjects were also focusing on the adaptor image, a blue fixation cross directed their attention to the center of the screen during the presentation of the adaptor. Subjects were also instructed to refrain from movements during the experiment and from blinking during the presentation of the target stimulus. Each subject completed a total of 640 trials [2 (familiarity: familiar (F) vs. unfamiliar (UF)) × 4 (adaptor category: No, RS, SameID, DiffID) × 5 (adaptor duration: 200, 1200, 2000, 3500, 5000 ms) × 16 (identity)] with five breaks in between testing blocks. Adaptor categories and durations were intermixed and presented in random order. An experimental session lasted approximately 50 min.
At the end of the session participants had to answer a questionnaire that verified their familiarity with the identities used in the experiment. The questionnaire consisted of presenting each previously presented person and asking the participants to identify the familiar ones by naming them (or at least giving some correct information about them, such as occupation and nationality). In case the subject did not recognize one or more of the faces belonging to the familiar category or recognized faces belonging to the unfamiliar category, the trials containing those identities were removed from the statistical analysis for both behavioral and electrophysiological data. From the 32 faces on average 2.56 ± 2.25 faces were excluded from further analyses across participants.
Behavioral data analysis
Accuracy and response times (RTs) were recorded during the experiment. In the case of accuracy, we calculated d′-values that were analyzed with a 4 × 5 repeated measures ANOVA with adaptor category (4; No, RS, SameID, DiffID) and adaptor duration (5; 200, 1200, 2000, 3500, 5000) as within-subject factors. RTs were analyzed with a 2 × 4 × 5 repeated measures ANOVA with familiarity (2; familiarity: F vs. UF), adaptor category (4; No, RS, SameID, DiffID) and adaptor duration (5; 200, 1200, 2000, 3500, 5000 ms) as within-subject factors. All analyses involved Greenhouse—Geisser adjusted degrees of freedom for correction for non-sphericity. Post-hoc-t-statistics were performed by Fisher's LSD tests.
Electrophysiological recording and analysis
EEG acquisition and processing
Electroencephalography (EEG) data was recorded using a Brain-Amp (BrainProducts GmbH, Munich, Germany) amplifier from 60 Ag/AgCl scalp electrodes placed according to the international 10/10 electrode system (Chatrian et al., 1985) and mounted on an EC80 Easy Cap (Easycap, HerrschingBreitbrunn, Germany). Eye movements were recorded using two electrodes placed on the outer canthi of the eyes and one electrode placed on the infraorbital ridge of the right eye. All channels were referenced online to an average of the activity recorded at the two reference electrodes placed on the left and right earlobe and digitally transformed to a common averaged reference offline. The ground electrode was placed on the forehead and all input impedances were kept below 8 kΩ. EEG was digitized at 1000 Hz sampling rate (with an analog bandpass filter of 0.016–1000 Hz).
ERP data analysis
EEG data were analyzed using Brain Vision Analyzer 1.05.0002 (BrainProducts GmbH., Munich, Germany). After correcting ocular movement artifacts and digitally re-referencing to a common average, the EEG was segmented offline into 700 ms long epochs starting 100 ms prior to target stimulus onset and ending 600 ms after target stimulus onset. DC trend correction was applied, and a semi-automatic artifact rejection was implemented. Segments containing blinks, movement artifacts, and baseline drifts were rejected on the basis of visual inspection. After cleaning the EEG data, ~87% of the trials remained available for further analysis. It is worth noting, however, that the reason of removing a given segment in most cases (8% from the 13% removed trials) was that the familiarity of the given person was misjudged by the subject. EEG epochs were averaged separately for each condition and participant. Averages were band-pass filtered (Butterworth zero phase filter; 0.1–30 Hz; slope: 12 dB/oct) and baseline corrected using a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline. The peak amplitude and latency of the individually averaged ERPs were extracted using a semiautomatic detection algorithm that identified the global maxima separately for each selected channels in a specific time window as follows. P100 was defined as a main positive deflection in the 80–130 ms time window, whereas the N170 was defined as a negative component in the 135–190 ms time interval. P2 was the second positive component in the 195–250 ms time window, while N250 was defined as the second negative component at around 230–330 ms.
P100 amplitude was measured over O1 (left hemisphere, LH) and O2 (right hemisphere, RH) electrode positions (Herrmann et al., 2005a,b). For the N170, the standard posterior-occipito-temporal sites that correspond to the P7, P9, and PO9 (LH) and P8, P10, and PO10 (RH) channels were used (Wong et al., 2009). P2 amplitude was measured over O1, PO3, and PO7 (LH) and O2, PO4, and PO8 (RH) (Wang et al., 2014), while the amplitude of the N250 component was measured over P7, P9, PO9, and TP9 (LH) and P8, P10, PO10, and TP10 (RH) (Schweinberger et al., 2002b). A Five-way repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted for the amplitude values of the pooled values of the relevant electrodes with familiarity (2; F vs. UF), adaptor category (4; No, RS, SameID, DiffID), adaptation duration (5; 200, 1200, 2000, 3500, 5000 ms), hemisphere (2; LH vs. RH), and electrode (3 for N170 and P2 or 4 for N250) as within-subject factors separately for each component. (It is worth noting that for the P100 component a Four-way repeated-measures ANOVA was applied since we used only two electrodes, one for the LH and RH recordings.) The Greenhouse—Geisser correction was applied to correct for violations of sphericity, while Post-hoc-t-statistics were computed using Fisher's LSD tests.
In addition, we defined three different types of effects: ADAGENERIC as referred to the No vs. DiffID comparison; ADAIDENTITY as referred to the SameID vs. DiffID comparison; and ADAIMAGE as referred to the SameID vs. RS comparison. The magnitudes of electrophysiological adaptation effects were defined as the absolute value of the differences in amplitude that were calculated by subtracting the P100, N170, P2, and N250 amplitudes obtained during the relevant two adapted conditions. After all, we tested whether the different types of adaptations modify the ERP components differently. Therefore, a Four-way repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the electrophysiological adaptation indices with adaptation effect (3; ADAGENERIC, ADAIDENTITY, ADAIMAGE), ERP component (4; P100, N170, P2, N250), hemisphere (2; LH vs. RH), and duration (5; 200, 1200, 2000, 3500, 5000 ms) as within-subject factors. A Greenhouse—Geisser correction was applied to correct for violations of sphericity, while Post-hoc-t-statistics were computed using Fisher's LSD tests.
Results
Behavioral results
Accuracy
We observed a main effect of adaptation CONDITION on the d′-values of the behavioral performance [F(1.68, 25.22) = 7.56, p = 0.0003, ; Figure 2A]. Post-hoc comparisons suggested that it was due to a significantly worse performance (or according to the interpretation of the d′ analysis due to a significant lower sensitivity) in the case of DiffID when compared with other conditions (all ps < 0.009). Neither a main effect of DURATION [F(4, 60) = 1.47, p = 0.22, n.s.] nor a significant CONDITION × DURATION interaction [F(5.92, 88.74) = 1.53, p = 0.12, n.s.] were found.
Response times
A main effect of CONDITION was observed [F(3, 45) = 61.54, p < 0.0001, ] in a step-by-step manner: the fastest decisions were observed for RS (all ps < 0.0001) followed by SameID (all ps < 0.0001), however, no significant difference was found between No and DiffID conditions (p = 0.3). No main effect of DURATION was found [F(2.03, 30.5) = 1.19, p = 0.32], while adaptation duration modified the main effect of condition [significant CONDITION × DURATION interaction: F(12, 180) = 2.76, p = 0.002, ]. We have observed longer RT-values in the case of 5000 ms adaptation duration when compared with 200 ms (p = 0.0033) and with 3500 ms (p = 0.019) in the RS condition. In the case of the SameID condition, on the one hand, faster decisions were observed for the shortest adaptation duration (200 ms) when compared with the other durations (all ps < 0.017) except for 2000 ms duration (p = 0.31). On the other hand, smaller RT-values were observed for 2000 ms duration when compared with longer durations (p < 0.0001 for both 2000 vs. 3500 and 2000 vs. 5000 comparisons). In the case of the DiffID condition, longer RTs were measured for 1200 and 2000 ms durations when compared with the shortest (200 ms) duration (p = 0.047 and p = 0.022, respectively). Faster decisions were observed for familiar faces when compared with unfamiliar ones, suggesting a significant priming effect [main effect of FAMILIARITY: F(1, 15) = 5.27, p = 0.037, ; Figure 2B]. Although we have found that FAMILIARITY modifies the main effect of CONDITION [significant FAMILIARITY × CONDITION interaction: F(2.19, 32.84) = 12.09, p < 0.0001, ], the only relevant difference was faster decisions for RS (p < 0.0001), SameID (p = p < 0.0001), and DiffID p = 0.043) but not for No (p = 0.21) for familiar faces when compared with unfamiliar ones.
Electrophysiological results
In the subsequent ERP Results Section, we only focus on the three above-mentioned adaptation effects (GENERIC, IDENTITY-SPECIF, and IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECTS) and therefore we only discuss the main effects of CONDITION and DURATION, and the significant CONDITION × DURATION interactions in details. Any other significant main effects and interactions are presented in Tables 1, 2, respectively.
Table 1.
P100 | N170 | P2 | N250 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Significant main effects | ||||
COND | F(3, 45) = 9.83, p < 0.0001 | F(3, 45) = 47.12, p < 0.0001 | F(2.19, 32.8) = 12.63, p < 0.0001 | F(1.89, 28.33) = 11.94, p < 0.0001 |
No < SameID p = 0.0002 | No > SameID p < 0.0001 | No < SameID p = 0.0003 | No < RS p = 0.004 | |
No < RS p = 0.0001 | No > RS p < 0.0001 | No < RS p < 0.0001 | DiffID < RS p < 0.0001 | |
DiffID > RS p = 0.001 | Diff < RS p = 0.0003 | |||
HEM | F(1, 15) = 5.75, p = 0.03 | F(1, 15) = 7.58, p = 0.015 | F(1, 15) = 12.56, p = 0.003 | F(1, 15) = 4.46, p = 0.052 |
RH dominance | RH dominance | RH dominance | strong tendency for LH dominance | |
FAM | n.s. | F(1, 15) = 8.94, p = 0.009 | n.s. | F(1, 15) = 14.48, p = 0.0017 |
larger for unfamiliar faces | larger for familiar faces |
In case of negative components larger (>) means more negative.
Table 2.
P100 | N170 | P2 | N250 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Significant interactions | ||||
COND × DUR | n.s. | F(6.27, 93.99) = 3.33, p = 0.0002 | F(6.37, 95.58) = 2.4, p = 0.007 | n.s. |
No vs. SameID all ps < 0.0001 for all durations | No vs. SameID all ps < 0.025 from 1200 ms duration | |||
No vs. RS all ps < 0.012 for all durations | No vs. RS all ps < 0.0001 from 1200 ms duration | |||
DiffID vs. RS all ps < 0.03 for durations longer than 1200 ms | DiffID vs. RS all ps < 0.04 from 1200 ms duration | |||
COND × FAMIL | F(3, 45) = 3.44, p = 0.025 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. |
COND × HEM | F(3, 45) = 3.83, p = 0.016 | F(3, 45) = 5.45, p = 0.003 | F(3, 45) = 2.97, p = 0.042 stronger differences in RH | n.s. |
COND × DUR × HEM | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | F(4.67, 70.1) = 2.15, p = 0.016 |
No vs. SameID ps < 0.04 in case of longer durations (3500 and 5000 ms) in the RH (other ps > 0.2) | ||||
No vs. RS all ps < 0.03 except for 1200 ms LH (p = 0.25) | ||||
DiffID vs. RS all ps < 0.006 |
In case of negative components larger (>) means more negative.
P100
Significant GENERIC EFFECT was observed suggested by the larger P100 amplitude values in the DiffID condition when compared with the control (No) (main effect of CONDITION: F(3, 45) = 9.83, p < 0.0001, , post-hoc p < 0.0001; Figures 3A,B). Neither IDENTITY-SPECIFIC nor IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECTs were found (DiffID vs. SameID p = 0.49, SameID vs. RS p = 0.94). In the case of shorter adaptation durations (200 and 1200 ms), smaller P100s were measured when compared with longer adaptation durations [main effect of DURATION: F(4, 60) = 12.09, p < 0.0001, , post-hoc comparisons show all ps < 0.0028]. No other effects of duration were observed (all other ps > 0.71). Moreover, the main effect of condition was not modified by adaptation duration [non-significant CONDITION × DURATION interaction: F(12, 180) = 1.34, p = 0.2]. Altogether, these results suggest that the P100 reflects only generic, face category specific adaptation processes and that this effect is independent of the adaptation time.
N170
The N170 showed a significant main effect of CONDITION [F(3, 45) = 47.12, p < 0.0001, ] in a step-by-step manner (Figures 4A,B). The largest N170s were found in the case of the control (No) condition (all ps < 0.0001) followed by DiffID (all ps < 0.0035), however, no significant difference was observed between SameID and RS (p = 0.68) suggesting that at the level of N170 component, both GENERIC (p < 0.0001) and IDENTITY-SPECIFIC EFFECTs (p = 0.0035) can be observed but no IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECT (p = 0.68) is reflected on the component. Adaptation duration modified the main effect of condition [significant CONDITION × DURATION interaction: F(6.27, 93.99) = 3.33, p = 0.0002, ]. In detail, GENERIC EFFECT was observed for all durations (all ps < 0.018), IDENTITY-SPECIFIC effect was found in the cases of 1200 and 3500 ms durations (p = 0.015 and 0.003, respectively), while IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECT was only observed in the case of the longest exposure time of the adaptor (5000 ms; p = 0.038) but not for the shorter ones (all ps > 0.09).
These results suggest that the N170 adaptation effects strongly depend on the adaptation duration: while for short adaptation durations the N170 reflects only generic, category specific effects, prolonging the adaptation duration leads to the emergence of identity and image specific adaptation effects as well.
P2
Although there was a strong tendency for both GENERIC EFFECT (namely larger P2s in the case of DiffID when compared with No, p = 0.054) and IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECT (larger P2s in the case of RS when compared with SameID, p = 0.053) and a somewhat weaker tendency for IDENTITY-SPECIFIC EFFECT (namely smaller P2s in the case of DiffID when compared with SameID, p = 0.065), neither of the comparisons reached the level of significance even though a significant main effect of CONDITION was observed [F(2.19, 32.8) = 12.63, p < 0.0001, ] (Figures 5A,B). Another strong tendency was observed in the case of adaptation duration [main effect of DURATION: F(4, 60) = 2.43, p = 0.057, ], suggesting slightly larger P2s in both 1200 and 2000 ms durations when compared with the shortest (200 ms) duration. The main effect of condition, however, was strongly modified by adaptation duration [significant CONDITION × DURATION interaction: F(6.37, 95.58) = 2.4, p = 0.007, ]. In the case of the shortest adaptation duration, there were no significant effects reflected on the amplitude of the component (all ps > 0.3). In the case of longer durations (either 3500 or 5000 ms long exposures) all three effects were observed (GENERIC EFFECT: ps < 0.035, IDENTITY-SPECIFIC EFFECT ps < 0.009, and IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECT ps < 0.019).
N250
Supporting prior studies of N250r (Schweinberger et al., 2002a, 2004; Kaufmann et al., 2009), the most negative N250s were observed in the RS condition when compared with all other conditions [main effect of CONDITION: F(1.89, 28.33) = 11.94, p < 0.0001, , all ps < 0.0041]. We also observed less negative N250s for DiffID than for any other conditions (all ps < 0.012). N250s evoked by target faces were larger for the longest adaptation duration (5000 ms) when compared with the shorter durations [main effect of DURATION: F(2.44, 36.66) = 3.02, p = 0.025, , post-hoc comparisons: all ps < 0.035]. Although the statistical investigation of the CONDITION × DURATION interaction did not reach the level of significance [F(12, 180) = 1.63, p = 0.09], significant three-way CONDITION × DURATION × HEMISPHERE interaction [F(4.67, 70.1) = 2.15, p = 0.016, ] was found, suggesting that in the case of 1200 ms duration neither GENERIC (p = 0.11) nor IMAGE-SPECIFIC EFFECTs (p = 0.44) were observed (Figures 6A,B) over the right hemisphere. It was also the case for the two longest durations (3500 and 5000 ms) in the right hemisphere in the case of the IDENTITY-SPECIFIC EFFECT (all p = 0.18 for 3500 ms and p = 0.35 for 5000 ms). Since every other relevant comparisons were significantly different (all ps < 0.04) we can further emphasize the role of N250 in image and identity specific stimulus encoding.
By running an analysis based on different types of adaptation effects, we suggest that altogether larger adaptation effects were observed for GENERIC when compared with IDENTITY-SPECIFIC adaptation [main effect of ADAPTATION EFFECT: F(2, 30) = 3.97, p = 0.03, , post-hoc comparison: p = 0.01]. There was a weak tendency that ADAIMAGE was slightly larger than ADAIDENTITY (p = 0.066). Although no main effect of DURATION was observed [F(4, 60) = 0.83, p = 0.51], the significant ADAPTATION EFFECT × DURATION interaction suggested that the ADAGENERIC effects were the most pronounced for the 2000 and 3500 ms adaptation durations [F(8, 120) = 2.21, p = 0.03, , post-hoc comparison: all ps < 0.042]. Interestingly, ADAGENERIC was the strongest in the case of 2000 ms duration, while at this duration the ADAIDENTITY was the smallest one.
Discussion
The goal of the present work was to study the behavioral and electrophysiological effects of systematically varying adaptation duration and whether this variation leads to a differentiation between the various stages of face processing, such as the generic category coding vs. processing of identity and image specific information. By using a large range of adaptation times we expected to find a differentiation between generic-, identity-, and image-specific processes.
The behavioral results indicated a better performance and faster decisions for the SameID and RS conditions when compared with the DiffID condition, corresponding to a strong priming effect in these cases (Ellis et al., 1987; Roberts and Bruce, 1989; Johnston and Barry, 2001). It is worth noting that the best performance and fastest decisions were observed in the case of RS when compared with the control (No) condition when the exposition time of the adaptor reached 1200 ms. In a recent study, Walther et al. (2013) found that the ambiguity of the stimuli regarding their classification plays an important role in differentiating between priming and adaptation-related after-effects with priming effects mostly found for unambiguous ones. This priming effect [namely in cases where the face adaptor image belonged to the same identity (SameID or RS) vs. to another identity (DiffID)] was to be expected, since the stimuli used in our experiment were unambiguous to the expected decision of the participants (as we used original photos without manipulating the familiarity information within the given face by morphing or other techniques that would have made the task more difficult). In the case of the speed of decision, we have found faster RTs for familiar faces, which is in line with earlier findings (Tong and Nakayama, 1999) that showed that familiar faces are processed faster than unfamiliar ones.
In the case of the P100 component, larger amplitudes were observed for all face-adapted conditions when compared with control (No) and the main effect of condition suggests a generic adaptation effect (ADAGENERIC) already at this level. This general signal enhancement is in line with our previous findings (Kovács et al., 2005, 2006, 2007; Zimmer and Kovács, 2011b), suggesting that there can be different modulating effects on the early P100 when compared to the later N170 as a consequence of adaptation, emphasizing separate mechanisms eliciting these components as well as their different roles in face perception.
All face adapted conditions caused a signal reduction of the N170 component when compared with noise adaptation (No). This general effect might be related to face detection and can be observed already after the shortest adaptation duration (200 ms). Recently, Feuerriegel et al. (2015) varied the exposition time of the adaptor stimulus systematically from 200 to 1000 ms and found no generic adaptation effect on the N170. These conflicting results can be explained by the fact that Feuerriegel et al. (2015) compared faces against another object category (chairs) while in the present study a uniform noise pattern was used as control condition. From the results of rapid adaptation studies where the authors tested the category-specificity of the adaptation effect (comparing ERPs evoked by face-adapted faces vs. non-face object adapted faces), we know that 200 ms duration is not enough to produce category-specific adaptation effects within the time-window of the N170 component (Nemrodov and Itier, 2012). It is worth noting, however, that Tian et al. (2015) have emphasized that the length of the ISI plays also an important role in the strength of the adaptation effect. In a rapid adaptation paradigm the authors have found both within-category (face adapted faces) and cross-category (house adapted faces) adaptation effects but only in the case of relatively shorter (~450 ms) ISI while in the case of longer ISI (~850 ms) only face-sensitive adaptation effects were observed. Therefore, it is possible that (at least at the shortest adaptation times) the N170 is also sensitive to the presentation of an object shape or Gestalt, a hypothesis requiring further tests. Moreover, since both SameID and RS adaptors are related to the test stimulus, it is possible that the bilateral reduction of the N170 component is also related to the processing of identity, a conclusion also supported by earlier findings (Jacques and Rossion, 2006; Jacques et al., 2007; Caharel et al., 2009). It is worth noting, however, that this additional signal reduction when compared to the generic effect is present only for the longest adaptation duration (5000 ms). This result also supports the claims of Nemrodov and Itier (2012) regarding the validity of rapid adaptation paradigms in testing category specific processes.
Regarding the P2 component, in the case of the shortest duration, there was no significant difference between conditions. However, as adaptation duration reached 1200 ms a step-by-step differentiation was found among No, DiffID, SameID, and RS conditions in an ascending manner for both hemispheres. This means that for adaptation durations longer than ~1000 ms the P2 component reflected generic, identity-specific and image-specific effects as well. Whereas the P100 and N170 components are clearly linked to special and well-defined stages of face processing, this does not seem to hold for P2. Some studies linked this component to task difficulty (Philiastides et al., 2006) while others emphasized the role of P2 in face-related tasks for which we have expertise (such as in case of own race effect, Stahl et al., 2008). P2 enhancement was also found recently investigating the effect of added noise (Bankó et al., 2011, 2013; Németh et al., 2014). Even though the LO/LOC is believed to be an object-selective area, some studies have found that its caudal part is also responsive to faces. Indeed, Nagy et al. (2012) and Hermann et al. (2015) have found bidirectional connections between the LO and the face-selective OFA-FFA complex, with faces modulating the LO-FFA connection and objects modulating the LO-OFA connection. Moreover, it is known that the neural generator of both P100 and P2 is the LOC (for review, see Schendan and Lucia, 2010). Therefore, one would expect a similar behavior of these two components—namely a large and pronounced enhancement for repeated stimulus presentations (RS). Since the largest alteration was measured between RS and control (No) and considering that LOC is involved in the generation of this component one could interpret this signal enlargement as a re-entrant loop from the OFA-FFA complex to the LOC. It is worth noting, however, that there are studies emphasizing other brain regions that can be involved in the generation of the early visual components, such as P100. Studies using less complex visual stimuli (isoluminant checkerboard stimuli) differentiated an early and a late phase of the P100 component that were localized to sources in dorsal extrastriate cortex of the middle occipital gyrus, and to sources in the ventral extrastriate cortex of the fusiform gyrus, respectively (Di Russo et al., 2001, 2005). Other source localization studies that used human face stimuli also have mentioned the fusiform gyrus as the neural generator of the P100 component (Herrmann et al., 2005b).
Regarding N250, not surprisingly, familiar faces evoked larger N250s, supporting prior results (Begleiter et al., 1995; Schweinberger et al., 1995; Pfüzte et al., 2002). At the level of this component generic-, identity-, and image-specific effects were found equally for the shortest and longest durations. These findings show that all observed adaptation effects (generic, identity-specific, and image-specific) happen in parallel and not independent from one another in this time-window.
Our results show that on the one hand, the observed face-evoked ERP components reflect generic, identity-specific, and image-specific adaptation effects that are modulated by adaptation duration in a way that in the case of the shortest duration (commonly applied in rapid adaptation paradigms) only the earlier P100 and N170 components reflect generic adaptation effects (for summary see our model on Figure 7). On the other hand generic, identity-, and image-specific effects can equally be observed on the amplitude modulation of the P2 and N250 components. In the case of longer durations however, generic effects have already been measurable from the earliest face-evoked responses onwards while identity-, and image-specific effects are present beyond the N170 component.
In conclusion, the current study is the first to investigate the relationship between systematically varying the adaptor duration in a broad range and the behavioral and electrophysiological responses elicited by the test stimuli after adapting different processing stages. Our results suggest that after long-term adaptation generic, identity-specific, and image-specific effects are reflected equally and earlier on the ERP components when compared to short-term adaptation conditions. In the case of rapid adaptation, these effects are delayed and can be observed only on the N250 component. Altogether we can summarize the consequences of our findings as follows: (1) While prior results rather suggested a sequential processing of faces, starting with a generic face categorization at about 150 ms (within the time-window of the N170 component), and identification/recognition being associated with three later components at around 250 ms (within the time-window of the N250 component), ours is the first study that clearly shows that with long-term adaptation these processes can all be observed from early components onwards and they are parallel to each other. (2) Our results suggest that in order to see the identity-specific processing steps completely, one needs to apply several seconds-long adaptation. (3) Our results also support those single-cell recording studies that suggest that short- and long-term adaptation have entirely different neural mechanisms (Priebe et al., 2002; Kohn and Movshon, 2003; Patterson et al., 2013).
Author contributions
Designed the experiment: MZ, KN, GK; data acquisition: MZ, AZ, KN; data analyses: MZ, AZ, GK; interpretation of the data: MZ, AZ, GK; provided materials: MZ, AZ, KN, GK; wrote the article: MZ, AZ, GK; proofed/revised the article: MZ, AZ, KN, GK.
Funding
This work was supported by a Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grant (KO 3918/1-2; 2-1).
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Dorottya Bencze for her help in running the experiments.
References
- Allison T., Puce A., Spencer D. D., McCarthy G. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cereb. Cortex 9, 415–430. 10.1093/cercor/9.5.415 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amihai I., Deouell L. Y., Bentin S. (2011). Neural adaptation is related to face repetition irrespective of identity: a reappraisal of the N170 effect. Exp. Brain Res. 209, 193–204. 10.1007/s00221-011-2546-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bankó E. M., Gál V., Körtvélyes J., Kovács G., Vidnyánszky Z. (2011). Dissociating the effect of noise on sensory processing and overall decision difficulty. J. Neurosci. 31, 2663–2674. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2725-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bankó É. M., Körtvélyes J., Weiss B., Vidnyánszky Z. (2013). How the visual cortex handles stimulus noise: insights from amblyopia. PLoS ONE 8:e66583. 10.1371/journal.pone.0066583 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begleiter H., Porjesz B., Wang W. (1995). Event-related brain potentials differentiate priming and recognition to familiar and unfamiliar faces. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 94, 41–49. 10.1016/0013-4694(94)00240-L [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentin S., Allison T., Puce A., Perez E., McCarthy G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 551–565. 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436. 10.1163/156856897X00357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks B. E., Rosielle L. J., Cooper E. E. (2002). The priming of face recognition after metric transformations. Perception 31, 297–313. 10.1068/p3283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caharel S., d'Arripe O., Ramon M., Jacques C., Rossion B. (2009). Early adaptation to repeated unfamiliar faces across viewpoint changes in the right hemisphere: evidence from the N170 ERP component. Neuropsychologia 47, 639–643. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chatrian G. E., Wirch A. L., Edwards K. H., Turella G. S., Kaufman M. A., Snyder J. M. (1985). Cochlear summating potential to broadband clicks detected from the human external auditory meatus. A study of subjects with normal hearing for age. Ear Hear. 6, 130–138. 10.1097/00003446-198505000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daelli V., van Rijsbergen N. J., Treves A. (2010). How recent experience affects the perception of ambiguous objects. Brain Res. 1322, 81–91. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desjardins J., Segalowitz S. J. (2009). Deconstructing the P1 and N170 face-effect using independent component analysis. Psychophysiology 46, S24 Available online at: https://www.brocku.ca/psychology/people/Desjardins%202009%20SPR%20poster.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Di Russo F., Martínez A., Sereno M. I., Pitzalis S., Hillyard S. A. (2001). Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potentials. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 95–111. 10.1002/hbm.10010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Russo F., Pitzalis S., Spitoni G., Aprile T., Patria F., Spinelli D., et al. (2005). Identification of the neural sources of the pattern-reversal VEP. Neuroimage 24, 874–886. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eger E., Schweinberger S. R., Dolan R. J., Henson R. N. (2005). Familiarity enhances invariance of face representations in human ventral visual cortex: fMRI evidence. Neuroimage 26, 1128–1139. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M. (2000). The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. Neuroreport 11, 2319–2324. 10.1097/00001756-200007140-00050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M., Gosling A., Nicholas S., Kiss M. (2011). The N170 component and its links to configural face processing: a rapid adaptation study. Brain Res. 1376, 76–87. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eimer M., Kiss M., Nicholas S. (2010). Response profile of the face-sensitive N170 component: a rapid adaptation study. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2442–2452. 10.1093/cercor/bhp312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis A. W., Young A. W., Flude B. M., Hay D. C. (1987). Repetition priming of face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 39, 193–210. 10.1080/14640748708401784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fang F., Murray S. O., He S. (2006). Duration-dependent fMRI adaptation and distributed viewer-centered face representation in human visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1402–1411. 10.1093/cercor/bhl053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feuerriegel D., Churches O. F., Keage H. A. D. (2015). Is neural adaptation of the N170 category-specific? Effects of adaptor stimulus duration and interstimulus interval. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 96, 8–15. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauthier I., Skudlarski P., Gore J. C., Anderson A. W. (2000). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 191–197. 10.1038/72140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill-Spector K., Henson R., Martin A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 14–23. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill-Spector K., Malach R. (2001). fMRI-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 107, 293–321. 10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00019-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris A., Nakayama K. (2007). Rapid face-selective adaptation of an early extrastriate component in MEG. Cereb. Cortex 17, 63–70. 10.1093/cercor/bhj124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris A., Nakayama K. (2008). Rapid adaptation of the m170 response: importance of face parts. Cereb. Cortex 18, 467–476. 10.1093/cercor/bhm078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermann P., Bankó É. M., Gál V., Vidnyánszky Z. (2015). Neural basis of identity information extraction from noisy face images. J. Neurosci. 35, 7165–7173. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3572-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann M. J., Ehlis A. C., Ellgring H., Fallgatter A. J. (2005a). Early stages (P100) of face perception in humans as measured with event-related potentials (ERPs). J. Neural Trans. 112, 1073–1081. 10.1007/s00702-004-0250-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann M. J., Ehlis A. C., Muehlberger A., Fallgatter A. J. (2005b). Source localization of early stages of face processing. Brain Topogr. 18, 77–85. 10.1007/s10548-005-0277-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itier R. J., Taylor M. J. (2004a). N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cereb. Cortex 14, 132–142. 10.1093/cercor/bhg111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itier R. J., Taylor M. J. (2004b). Effects of repetition learning on upright, inverted and contrast-reversed face processing using ERPs. Neuroimage 21, 1518–1532. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacques C., d'Arripe O., Rossion B. (2007). The time course of the inversion effect during individual face discrimination. J. Vis. 7:8. 10.1167/7.8.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacques C., Rossion B. (2006). The speed of individual face categorization. Psychol. Sci. 17, 485–492. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01733.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johannes S., Münte T. F., Heinze H. J., Amngun G. R. (1995). Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing. Cogn. Brain Res. 2, 189–205. 10.1016/0926-6410(95)90008-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston R. A., Barry C. (2001). Best face forward: similarity effects in repetition priming of face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 54, 383–396. 10.1080/713755976 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N., McDermott J., Chun M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufmann J. M., Schweinberger S. R., Burton A. M. (2009). N250 ERP correlates of the acquisition of face representations across different images. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 625–641. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kloth N., Schweinberger S. R., Kovács G. (2010). Neural correlates of generic versus gender-specific face adaptation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2345–2356. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohn A., Movshon J. A. (2003). Neuronal adaptation to visual motion in area MT of the macaques. Neuron 39, 681–691. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00438-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács G., Zimmer M., Bankó E., Harza I., Antal A., Vidnyánszky Z. (2006). Electrophysiological correlates of visual adaptation to faces and body parts in humans. Cereb. Cortex 16, 742–753. 10.1093/cercor/bhj020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács G., Zimmer M., Harza I., Antal A., Vidnyánszky Z. (2005). Position-specificity of facial adaptation. Neuroreport 16, 1945–1949. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000187635.76127.bc [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács G., Zimmer M., Harza I., Vidnyánszky Z. (2007). Adaptation duration affects the spatial selectivity of facial aftereffects. Vision Res. 47, 3141–3149. 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krekelberg B., Boynton G. M., van Wezel R. J. (2006). Adaptation: from single cells to BOLD signals. Trends Neurosci. 29, 250–256. 10.1016/j.tins.2006.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latinus M., Taylor M. J. (2006). Face processing stages: impact of difficulty and the separation of effects. Brain Res. 1123, 179–187. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.09.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leopold D. A., O'Toole A. J., Vetter T., Blanz V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 89–94. 10.1038/82947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leopold D. A., Rhodes G., Müller K. M., Jeffery L. (2005). The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 897–904. 10.1098/rspb.2004.3022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malach R., Repras J. B., Benson R. R., Kwong K. K., Jiang H., Kennedy W. A., et al. (1995). Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 8135–8139. 10.1073/pnas.92.18.8135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesik J., Bao M., Engel S. A. (2013). Spontaneous recovery of motion and face aftereffects. Vision Res. 89, 72–78. 10.1016/j.visres.2013.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller K. M., Schillinger F., Do D. H., Leopold D. A. (2009). Dissociable perceptual effects of visual adaptation. PLoS ONE 4:e6183. 10.1371/journal.pone.0006183 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagy K., Greenlee M. W., Kovács G. (2012). The lateral occipital cortex in the face perception network: an effective connectivity study. Front. Psychol. 3:141. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Németh K., Kovács P., Vakli P., Kovács G., Zimmer M. (2014). Phase noise reveals early category-specific modulation of the event-related potentials. Front. Psychol. 5:367. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00367 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nemrodov D., Itier R. J. (2011). The role of eyes in early face processing: a rapid adaptation study of the inversion effect. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 783–798. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02033.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nemrodov D., Itier R. J. (2012). Is the rapid adaptation paradigm too rapid? Implications for face and object processing. Neuroimage 61, 812–822. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olman C., Boyaci H., Fang F., Doerschner K. (2008). V1 responses to different types of luminance histogram contrast [Abstract]. J. Vis. 8, 345a 10.1167/8.6.345 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson C. A., Wissig S. C., Kohn A. (2013). Distinct effects of brief and prolonged adaptation on orientation tuning in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 532–543. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3345-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli D. G. (1997). The Video Toolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437–442. 10.1163/156856897X00366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfüzte E. M., Sommer W., Schweinberger S. R. (2002). Age-related slowing in face and name recognition: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Psychol. Aging 17, 140–160. 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philiastides M. G., Ratcliff R., Sajda P. (2006). Neural representation of task difficulty and decision making during perceptual categorization: a timing diagram. J. Neurosci. 26, 8965–8975. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1655-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Priebe N. J., Churchland M. M., Lisberger S. G. (2002). Constraints on the source of short-term motion adaptation in macaque area MT. I. The role of input and intrinsic mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 354–369. 10.1152/jn.00852.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puce A., Allison T., Bentin S., Gore J. C., McCarthy G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. J. Neurosci. 18, 2188–2199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes G., Jeffery L., Clifford C. W., Leopold D. A. (2007). The timecourse of higher-level face aftereffects. Vision Res. 47, 2291–2296. 10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes G., Jeffery L., Watson T. L., Clifford C. W., Nakayama K. (2003). Fitting the mind to the world: face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects. Psychol. Sci. 14, 558–566. 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1465.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts T., Bruce V. (1989). Repetition priming of face recognition in a serial choice reaction-time task. Br. J. Psychol. 80, 201–211. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02314.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossion B., Jacques C. (2008). Does physical interstimulus variance account for early electrophysiological face sensitive responses in the human brain? Ten lessons on the N170. Neuroimage 39, 1959–1979. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schendan H. E., Lucia L. C. (2010). Object-sensitive activity reflects earlier perceptual and later cognitive processing of visual objects between 95 and 500 ms. Brain Res. 6, 124–141. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R. (2011). Covert recognition and the neural system for face processing, in Oxford Handbook of Face Perception, eds Calder A. J., Rhodes G., Johnson M. H., Haxby J. V. (Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 345–366. [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R., Huddy V., Burton A. M. (2004). N250r: a face-selective brain response to stimulus repetition. Neuroreport 15, 1501–1505. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000131675.00319.42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R., Kaufmann J. M., Moratti S., Keil A., Burton A. M. (2007b). Brain responses to repetitions of human and animal faces, inverted faces, and objects – an MEG study. Brain Res. 1184, 226–233. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R., Kloth N., Jenkins R. (2007a). Are you looking at me? Neural correlates of gaze adaptation. Neuroreport 18, 693–696. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280c1e2d2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R., Pfütze E.-M., Sommer W. (1995). Repetition priming and associative priming of face recognition: evidence from event-related potentials. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 21, 722–736. 10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.722 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R., Pickering E. C., Burton A. M., Kaufmann J. M. (2002b). Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition. Neuropsychologia 40, 2057–2073. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00050-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinberger S. R., Pickering E. C., Jentzsch I., Burton A. M., Kaufmann J. M. (2002a). Event-related brain potential evidence for a response of inferior temporal cortex to familiar face repetitions. Cogn. Brain Res. 14, 398–409. 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00142-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stahl J., Wiese H., Schweinberger S. R. (2008). Expertise and own-race bias in face processing: an event-related potentials study. Neuroreport 19, 583–587. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f97b4d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tian T., Feng X., Feng C., Gu R., Luo Y.-J. (2015). When rapid adaptation paradigm is not too rapid: evidence of face-sensitive N170 adaptation effects. Biol. Psychol. 109, 53–60. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tong F., Nakayama K. (1999). Robust representations for faces: evidence from visual search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 25, 1016–1035. 10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.1016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walther C., Schweinberger S. R., Kaiser D., Kovács G. (2013). Neural correlates of priming and adaptation in familiar face perception. Cortex 49, 1963–1977. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang W., Miao D., Zhao L. (2014). Visual MMN elicited by orientation changes of faces. J. Integr. Neurosci. 13, 485–495. 10.1142/S0219635214500137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webster M. A., Kaping D., Mizokami Y., Duhamel P. (2004). Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature 428, 557–561. 10.1038/nature02420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webster M. A., MacLin O. H. (1999). Figural aftereffects in the perception of faces. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 647–653. 10.3758/BF03212974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiese H., Stahl J., Schweinberger S. R. (2009). Configural processing of other-race faces is delayed but not decreased. Biol. Psychol. 81, 103–109. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willenbockel V., Sadr J., Fiset D., Horne G. O., Gosselin F., Tanaka J. W. (2010). Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 671–684. 10.3758/BRM.42.3.671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong S., Baltuch G. H., Jaggi J. L., Danish S. F. (2009). Functional localization and visualization of the subthalamic nucleus from microelectrode recordings acquired during DBS surgery with unsupervised machine learning. J. Neural Eng. 6:026006. 10.1088/1741-2560/6/2/026006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao L., Chubb C. (2001). The size-tuning of the face-distortion after-effect. Vision Res. 41, 2979–2994. 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00202-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmer M., Kovács G. (2011a). Position-specificity of adaptation-related face aftereffects. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 586–595. 10.1098/rstb.2010.0265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmer M., Kovács G. (2011b). Electrophysiological correlates of face distortion after-effects. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 64, 533–544. 10.1080/17470218.2010.501964 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]