Abstract
To reduce the burden of the HIV/AIDS epidemic, international donors recently began providing free antiretroviral therapy (ART) in parts of Sub-Saharan Africa. ART dramatically prolongs life and reduces infectiousness for people with HIV. This paper shows that ART availability increases work time for HIV-negative people without caretaker obligations, who do not directly benefit from the medicine. A difference-in-difference design compares people living near and far from ART, before and after treatment becomes available. Next we explore the possible reasons for this pattern. Although we cannot pinpoint the mechanism, we find that ART availability substantially reduces subjective mortality risk and improves mental health. These results show an undocumented economic consequence of the HIV/AIDS epidemic and an important externality of medical innovation. They also provide the first evidence of a link between the disease environment and mental health.
1 Introduction
The social, demographic, and economic repercussions of HIV/AIDS in Sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) have been severe. According to recent figures from UNAIDS (2010), around 33 million people in SSA have HIV and 1.8 million become infected annually. AIDS has reduced period life expectancy in Southern Africa from 62 to 48 years over the past decade and a half (UNPD 2010). However, recent improvements in access to antiretroviral therapy (ART) have profoundly changed the course of the epidemic, as well as its social and economic consequences. ART is a powerful treatment regimen that prolongs life and reduces morbidity and infectiousness for people with HIV (NIAID 2011, Smith et al. 2011, Tanser et al. 2013, Bor et al. 2013). To address the prohibitive cost of ART, a major international initiative began supplying free medicine in endemic countries in 2002. By 2005, 810,000 people began treatment under these programs. Free ART has reversed the upward mortality trend in countries with the highest HIV prevalence (Jahn et al. 2008, Bongaarts et al. 2011, Herbst et al. 2009).
Living in an HIV-endemic environment may influence economic decision-making regardless of physical illness (Conroy et al. 2013). Respondents in our sample from rural Malawi (described below) attend a median of three funerals per month, many of which are due to AIDS. They know two people who are sick with AIDS and two others who have died of AIDS in the past year. Although 6 percent of respondents have HIV, 39 percent think they might be infected and 58 percent worry about contracting the disease. By reducing life expectancy, HIV/AIDS risk may lead people to reoptimize life-cycle decisions like labor supply and education (Ben-Porath 1967, Cervellati and Sunde 2013). It may also affect economic outcomes by worsening mental health. In this setting, 30 percent of respondents report feeling depressed and 44 percent report feeling anxious in the past four weeks. 12 percent say that mental distress interferes with their activities or accomplishments.
ART clearly benefits HIV-positive recipients and their caretakers (Thirumurthy et al. 2008, McLaren 2010, Thirumurthy et al. 2012). In addition, the availability of treatment reduces health risk for HIV-negative people by lowering the probabilities of both infection and mortality conditional on infection. Lakdawalla, Malani and Reif (2014) call this benefit, which is not well-documented empirically, the insurance value of medical innovation. Studies of the benefits of medical innovation do not generally include risk-reduction value (e.g. Philipson and Jena 2006, Yin et al. 2012). Indeed, studies of the HIV-negative response to ART focus exclusively on sexual behavior (DeWalque et al. 2007, Friedman 2014) and do not examine other economic outcomes. Because of the prevalence and severity of HIV/AIDS in Sub-Saharan Africa, the risk-reduction benefit of ART may be substantial and have broad economic implications.
This study estimates the effect of ART availability on work time in rural Malawi. With support from the Global Fund, the Ministry of Health (MoH) began to offer free ART in the study area in 2008. We implement a difference-in-difference empirical strategy that compares the change in work time near and far from ART facilities, and find that ART availability increases daily work time by 38 minutes for people near an ART facility. It increases daily cultivation time by 17 minutes and other production time by 14 minutes. We exclude people with HIV and caretakers in order to isolate a robust effect for people who do not directly benefit from ART.
Our identification strategy relies on the assumption that ART proximity is uncorrelated with unobservable changes in work time. Several results support the validity of this assumption. First, ART proximity is uncorrelated with pre-interventions levels and changes in work time and demographic characteristics. ART proximity is also uncorrelated with concurrent levels and changes for thirteen measures of economic shocks and social support. Secondly, estimates are robust if we control for the interaction of Post with these variables, which suggests that correlated unobservable trends do not confound our estimates. Following Oster (2014) and Altonji, Elder and Taber (2005), we quantify the unobservable selection needed to generate our findings spuriously under a proportional selection assumption. This test suggests that unobservable selection would need to be unrealistically strong to cause a spurious result. Finally, we implement placebo tests using the proximity of non-ART clinics, roads, trading centers and schools, all of which show small and insignificant estimates. These results support the validity of our empirical approach.
The strong response of HIV-negative non-caretakers (HIV−/NCTs) suggests that ART availability may influence risk perceptions. We validate this hypothesis by showing an effect on subjective mortality risk. ART availability reduces subjective five-year mortality risk by 3 percentage points (8 percent) for HIV-negative non-caretakers within six kilometers of an ART facility. HIV infection risk and mortality risk conditional on infection both contribute to mortality risk for someone who is HIV-negative.1 We show that ART availability reduces both mortality risk components.
The impact of ART availability on work time could arise through multiple channels, and a reduced-form study cannot directly isolate causal pathways. In principle, ART availability could affect either labor supply or labor demand. The labor demand channel is not especially plausible because most respondents are subsistence farmers and because ART availability has no effect on paid employment or occupation choice. The intervention may increase labor supply by either strengthening the incentive to save or by improving mental health. Under the savings mechanism, ART availability leads people to work more in order to accumulate savings for the future. This mechanism is consistent with Baranov and Kohler’s (2014) finding that ART availability leads people to invest in child human capital.
However life-cycle reoptimization may not fully explain the work time effect. Subsistence farmers who have increased labor supply to reoptimize production should arguably adjust the use of other farm inputs such as fertilizer, land, hired labor, and farm equipment. They may also invest in livestock, which is both an agricultural product and an asset. These farm variables, which are sensitive to other economic shocks, do not respond to ART availability, suggesting that another mechanism may contribute to this pattern. In the status quo, HIV risk threatens to exacerbate depression, anxiety, and the mental disorders that reduce labor supply (Ettner et al. 1997, Hamilton et al. 1997, Kessler and Frank 1997, Berndt et al. 1998, Lim et al. 2000, Marcotte and Wilcox-Gok 2003, Patel and Kleinman 2003, Fletcher 2013). ART availability has a strong and significant effect on mental health, improving an established mental health index by 0.13 standard deviations for HIV-negative non-caretakers near an ART facility. Although the direction of causality between mental health and labor supply is ambiguous, we find that ART availability reduces perceived mental-health limitations on activities and accomplishments, which is consistent with an effect of mental health on labor supply, rather than the reverse. This result suggests a new mechanism through which the disease environment may affect economic outcomes.
In summary, this paper makes several contributions. First, we document a novel and important economic spillover of the HIV/AIDS epidemic and ART. Studies of the labor-market impact of the HIV/AIDS epidemic focus on lost productivity for people who are HIV-positive (Young 2005, Marinescu 2014). However the economic impact for HIV-negative people is likely to matter simply because most workers are HIV-negative. Our findings complement other studies of the indirect effects of HIV/AIDS on risky behavior (DeWalque et al. 2007, Friedman 2014, Baird et al. 2014, Gong 2014) and health care delivery (Wilson et al. 2014). Secondly, we provide direct evidence of the risk-reduction benefit of medical innovation. While Lakdawalla, Malani and Reif (2014) calibrate the benefit using US data on quality-of-life improvements, our estimates for mental health and subjective happiness quantify the effect of ART in a well-identified way. The large risk-reduction benefit of ART that we observe is understandable in light of the severity of the HIV/AIDS epidemic in this setting. Thirdly, we provide the first direct estimate of the effect of the disease environment on mental health. Although a literature examines how the disease environment affects productivity via physical health (Acemoglu and Johnson 2007, Bleakley 2007), the contribution of mental health has not been considered. High mortality in developing countries may contribute to underdevelopment by fostering mental disorders that hamper productivity. Finally, we show the relationship between objective and subjective mortality risk in a developing country context. Other studies of the impact of mortality risk on behavior posit but do not show an effect on mortality risk perceptions (Jayachandran and Lleras-Muney 2009, Fortson 2011).
2 Context
2.1 Setting
Malawi is a small, landlocked country in Southern Africa with a population of 15.4 million and GDP per capita of $343. The population is 85 percent rural. Most people live in remote villages and support themselves through subsistence farming of maize and other crops. In our data, 63 percent of respondents cultivate their own field and only 5 percent of respondents work for pay as the primary occupation. This setting differs from conventional labor markets in two important ways. Subsistence farmers do not interact extensively with labor or output markets, which means that work time largely reflects labor supply rather than labor demand. Secondly, people who are self-employed face fewer institutional restrictions like the forty-hour work week that dampen the labor supply elasticity. Therefore the work time response may be larger than we would expect in a formal labor market.
The HIV/AIDS epidemic is the central public health issue in Malawi. National HIV prevalence was 11 percent in 2010 (UNAIDS 2010). As of 2008, life expectancy at birth is 52.9 years (WHO 2010) and AIDS is the leading cause of adult death (AVERT 2012). Heterosexual sex is the primary mode of HIV transmission in Malawi and elsewhere in SSA.
With US$294 million from the Global Fund to Fight AIDS, Tuberculosis and Malaria, the Ministry of Health (MoH) began offering free ART through existing public health clinics in 2004. Most facilities, which were not equipped to measure CD-4 counts, based eligibility for ART on a clinical diagnosis of Stage 4 AIDS. In 2004 and 2005, the MoH only offered ART through the largest and most advanced facilities. It expanded the program and offered ART through clinics with at least one clinician and clerk in 2006. In the online appendix, Table A1 shows the characteristics of clinics throughout Malawi that received ART before May of 2006, from June of 2006 to May of 2008, and after May of 2008. The earliest ART facilities are clearly larger and more advanced, however the difference between the second and third groups is less dramatic. As we describe below, a subset of clinics in the second group provide ART to the study population.
There is no evidence that the MoH targeted ART according to HIV prevalence or other HIV indicators. The MoH had an explicit policy to maximize geographic coverage while ensuring that ART facilities could administer the program correctly (MoH 2008, Libamba et al. 2006). The 2010 distribution of clinics suggests that the MoH faithfully implemented this policy. Under targeting, we would expect demand and supply for ART to be positively correlated. However, the North Zone had the highest concentration of ART facilities and the fewest potential recipients, while the South East Zone had the lowest concentration of facilities and the most potential recipients (MoH 2010). The lack of local data on HIV prevalence would have made targeting difficult. At the time, testing was not comprehensive enough to provide geographically precise HIV prevalence information.
Rudimentary transportation infrastructure limits the access to ART facilities in rural Malawi. Primary roads are paved but secondary roads (which lead to most villages) are unimproved. Few people own cars or motorcycles and public transportation is extremely limited. To receive treatment, an ART patient must visit a facility once every two weeks for the first two months and then once per month subsequently. Pinto et al. (2013) report that in the Zomba District of Malawi, ART patients travel 1 – 2 hours each way to obtain medicine and spend an average of 7.1 hours seeking care, which may be very difficult for someone with AIDS. ART candidates cannot easily relocate closer to clinics because land is communally managed and property rental markets do not exist (Matchaya 2009).2 Patients must appear in person during the first several months of treatment but may eventually send a proxy.
2.2 Data
The Malawi Longitudinal Study of Families and Health (MLSFH) is an ongoing biennial panel survey of up to 4000 respondents in rural Malawi. The survey spans 119 villages in Rumphi, Mchinji, and Balaka Districts, which are located in the north, center, and south of the country, respectively (Kohler et al. 2014, Anglewicz et al. 2009).3 The MLSFH was not designed to be representative of rural Malawi, however sample characteristics closely match the characteristics of the rural population in the 1996 Malawi Demographic and Health Survey (DHS) (Watkins et al. 2003). We use survey rounds from 2004, 2006, 2008, and 2010, however some variables are not available in every round. Table 1 clarifies the years in which key variables are available. Work time is not available in 2008, while subjective mortality risk and mental health are not available in 2004.
Table 1.
2004 | 2006 | 2008 | 2010 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Work time | Yes | Yes | - | Yes |
Other agricultural inputs | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Subjective mortality risk | - | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Mental health | - | Yes | Yes | Yes |
HIV test results | Yes | Yes | Yes | - |
ART became available near MLSFH villages between August 2007 and March of 2008. Before this change, the median distance to an ART facility was 27 kilometers. Although several other clinics began offering ART after 2008, all of these facilities were farther from MLSFH villages than the 2008 facilities. Surveys were conducted in June and July, so that the median time between clinic openings and the 2008 interviews is seven months. This interval appears to sufficient to allow expectations and mental health to adjust (Baird et al. 2013, Okeke and Wagner 2013). This timeline enables us to examine pre-trends in labor supply and HIV prevalence but not subjective mortality risk and mental health. The absence of 2008 work time data means that we only observe one post-intervention round for work time, while we observe two post-intervention rounds for subjective mortality risk and mental health.
We use household-specific GPS coordinates to calculate each respondent’s distance by road to the nearest ART facility in 2008. As an example, the circle in Figure 1 shows the approximate locations of sample households, which are confidential. The figure also shows the actual locations of ART and non-ART clinics, markets, and roads in Mchinji District (the north-south artery through the sample area is unpaved). Figure 2 shows the kernel density of the distance to an ART facility in 2008. 18 percent of respondents live within 5 kilometers of a facility and 68 percent live within 10 kilometers. All respondents live at least 1 kilometer away. The correlation between the distance by road and the straight line distance is 0.895.
Our main regressor is the “proximity” to ART, which is defined as the inverse distance to an ART facility. This parameterization captures the non-linear impact of distance on ART access in a parsimonious way.4 Alternative parameterizations such as distance polynomials, distance bins, or a piecewise linear spline, (which appear in Section A4) either discard variation or require us to estimate additional parameters. The coefficient on ART proximity is the impact of a change in distance from infinity to zero. While distance ranges from 1 to 23 kilometers in our data, non-parametric estimates below suggest that effects are small beyond 8 kilometers. In addition, only 10 percent of respondents live within 4 kilometers of ART. Multiplying the ART proximity coefficient by 0.125, the proximity difference between 4 and 8 kilometers, yields a reasonable in-sample estimate of the impact of ART access.
MLSFH data from 2012 allow us to validate the relationship between ART proximity and ART access. Spatial proximity is a standard proxy for access to health care in Africa (Guenther et al. 2012). The 2012 MLSFH survey round (which we do not use in our main analysis because it focuses on respondents over age 45) includes 23 HIV-positive respondents, 16 of whom receive ART. ART recipients live an average of 3.5 kilometers closer to ART facilities than non-recipients, a statistically significant difference. All four respondents living with 4 kilometers of ART receive treatment, compared to 10 out of 16 living more than 8 kilometers away.
Clinics are usually located along primary roads and near trading centers. As a result, the distance to an ART facility is correlated with other geographic features. Many villages have schools, which tend to be centrally located near high-traffic areas. Respondents live a median of 4.2 kilometers from a primary road, 5.0 kilometers from a trading center, and 1.6 kilometers from a school. The correlation coefficients between the distance to ART and the distances to a road, trading center, and school are 0.17, 0.54, and 0.10 respectively.
Like other longitudinal data sets from developing countries, the MLSFH is subject to considerable attrition (Alderman et al. 2001, Thomas et al. 2012). The MLSFH loses approximately 10 percent of respondents to follow-up in between adjacent survey rounds because surveyors do not interview respondents who emigrate from sample villages. Instead, they refresh the sample with several hundred new respondents in each round (Kohler et al. 2014). Around 45 percent of one-time attriters appear in subsequent survey rounds, suggesting that they have migrated temporarily. Section Al discusses attrition in more detail. Since attrition is uncorrelated with ART proximity, it is unlikely to confound our estimates. Results are robust if we limit the sample to non-attriters.
Some estimates below restrict the sample to HIV-negative non-caretakers in order to isolate HIV risk as a mechanism. Surveyors tested respondents for HIV in 2004, 2006, and 2008 (Obare et al. 2009). The 2008 test results proxy for HIV status in 2010.5 HIV prevalence was 5.3 percent in the MLSFH in 2006, before the arrival of ART. We define a caretaker as someone whose household includes at least one adult in poor health, regardless of the specific illness. This definition is conservative because it minimizes the chance of misclassifying caretakers as non-caretakers. However it may mistakenly identify “caretakers” who are not individually responsible or whose family members have illnesses besides AIDS. This form of misclassification slightly reduces statistically power but does not otherwise threaten validity. Our sample includes 99 HIV-positive respondents and 425 caretakers.6
2.3 Measurement
To measure work time, the survey elicits the number of hours per day that respondents spend on productive activities, including farm cultivation, household production, and other economic activities.7 We define total work time as the sum of these components. This method of measuring work time is similar to other survey-based metrics, such as the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID). 89 percent of respondents are interviewed in either June or July, which coincides with the maize harvest. Work time data are available in 2004, 2006, and 2010. The change from 2004 to 2006 allows us to investigate pre-intervention trends and the change from 2006 to 2010 provides the basis for our estimates. By 2010, ART had been available for over two years.
The MLSFH measures subjective mortality risk though an innovative, interactive methodology (Delavande and Kohler 2009). After explaining the concept of probability, surveyors elicit the subjective probability that the respondent will die within the next 1, 5, or 10 years. The survey also measures subjective perceptions of HIV infection risk and HIV status. Our analysis focuses on the 5-year horizon, although results for 1 year and 10 years are similar. According to Delavande and Kohler (2009), responses “take into account basic properties of probability and vary meaningfully with observable characteristics and past experience.” When asked about the likelihood of visiting the market within two days as well as within two weeks, over 90 percent of respondents correctly provide a weakly greater probability over the longer interval. Delavande, Gine, and McKenzie (2011a, 2011b) show that responses are robust to variations in the elicitation methodology.
ART availability may reduce subjective mortality risk by reducing either HIV-positive mortality risk or HIV infection risk. The MLSFH elicits the hypothetical mortality risk for someone with HIV. The survey also measures the respondent’s perceived likelihood of contracting HIV “in the future”, which is measured on a Likert scale from 0 (no likelihood) to 3 (high likelihood).8
The survey measures mental and physical health with the “twelve-item short form” (SF-12) questionnaire in 2006, 2008, and 2010. The SF-12 includes indicators for depression, anxiety, energy, and mental-health limitations on activities and accomplishments. The MCS-12 is a summary mental health score based on a factor-analytic weighting of these responses. It ranges from 0 to 100 with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 among respondents in the United States. Higher values indicate better health. The psychiatry literature has extensively used and validated the MCS-12 (Ware et al. 1996, Kessler et al. 2002). By combining SF-12 responses with clinical diagnoses, Gill et al. (2007) show that the MCS-12 is highly predictive of depression and other mental disorders with an area under curve (AOC) of 0.92, which is comparable to other widely used indices. We analyze the depression, anxiety, and energy components of the MCS-12 individually by defining binary versions of these variables, which are originally measured on Likert scales. The binary variables indicate whether the respondent ever felt depressed and whether the respondent has always felt calm and energetic in the past four weeks. Estimates do not depend on this transformation and untransformed results are available from the authors. The survey also measures subjective well-being by asking “how satisfied are you with your life, all things considered?” on a five-point Likert scale. In 2006, 69 percent of respondents indicate that they are “somewhat satisfied” or “very satisfied”.
2.4 Baseline Characteristics and Trends Over Time
Table 2 shows summary statistics for key variables in 2006, prior to the intervention. Columns 1 – 3 focus on the full sample, including people who are HIV-positive or care for someone with HIV. Columns 4 – 6 focus on the HIV-negative non-caretaker (HIV−/NCT) sample. In Column 1, respondents are an average of 35 years old and have 5 years of education. 80 percent are married and 16 percent have a metal roof (a common wealth proxy). Respondents work an average of 8.2 hours per day, although a quarter work fewer than 6 hours and a quarter work more than 11 hours per day. They spend 32 percent of their time on cultivation, 49 percent on home production, and 19 percent on other economic activities. Respondents perceive a 39 percent risk of death within five years, which dramatically exceeds the life-table estimate of 8.5 percent for Malawians in this age cohort. They believe that HIV prevalence is 29 percent (although it is actually 6 percent) and that someone with HIV has a five-year mortality risk of 69 percent. Half of respondents perceive nonzero risk of becoming infected in the future, and 16 percent believe that this risk is “medium” or “high”. 30 percent of respondents report feeling depressed in the last four weeks and 10–11 percent say that mental health limits their activities or accomplishments.
Table 2.
Full Sample |
HIV−/NCT Sample |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean |
β̂ (ART Prox.) |
Mean |
β̂ (ART Prox.) |
|||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | |
Panel A: Demographics | ||||||
Education | 5.16 | 1.78** | 0.25 | 5.43 | 1.44* | −0.09 |
Age | 35.4 | 0.63 | 2.87 | 34.3 | 1.10 | 2.54 |
Married | 0.79 | −0.11 | 0.14 | 0.80 | −0.17 | 0.00 |
Household size | 5.24 | 0.31 | 0.95 | 5.32 | −0.20 | 0.31 |
Metal roof | 0.16 | 0.37** | 0.15 | 0.16 | 0.39** | 0.16 |
Monetary wealth (USD) | 31.2 | 31.3 | −76.7 | 31.9 | 50.5 | −95.9 |
HIV-positive | 0.05 | 0.10 | 0.57 | 0.00 | - | - |
Panel B: Work Time | ||||||
Total (hours per day) | 8.24 | −0.22 | 1.02 | 8.20 | −0.60 | 1.11 |
Farming | 2.64 | −0.10 | −0.10 | 2.67 | −0.08 | 0.67 |
Home production | 4.05 | 1.69** | 1.01 | 3.95 | 1.78** | 0.29 |
Other production | 1.54 | −1.81* | 0.11 | 1.59 | −2.30** | 0.15 |
Panel C: Mortality Risk | ||||||
Own five year | 0.39 | 0.23*** | 0.11 | 0.37 | 0.15** | −0.02 |
HIV+ five year | 0.69 | 0.24*** | 0.17*** | 0.68 | 0.24*** | 0.17** |
Infection risk | 0.73 | 0.77*** | 0.34 | 0.68 | 0.56*** | −0.11 |
HIV prevalence | 0.29 | 0.11** | 0.09 | 0.28 | 0.05 | −0.05 |
Worried about HIV | 1.57 | 0.60*** | 0.36 | 1.53 | 0.50** | −0.02 |
Panel D: Mental Health | ||||||
MCS-12 | 55.6 | −5.8*** | −2.9 | 56.2 | −5.32** | −0.90 |
Depressed in last 4 weeks | 0.30 | 0.54*** | 0.33 | 0.28 | 0.43*** | 0.21 |
Energetic in last 4 weeks | 0.57 | −0.23 | 0.02 | 0.60 | −0.17 | 0.13 |
Calm in last 4 weeks | 0.56 | −0.38*** | −0.23 | 0.59 | −0.33*** | −0.20 |
MH limits on activities | 0.10 | 0.11 | −0.02 | 0.09 | 0.10 | −0.05 |
MH limits on accomps. | 0.11 | 0.10 | 0.01 | 0.09 | 0.11 | −0.05 |
Geographic controls | - | No | Yes | - | No | Yes |
Note: we regress each variable on ART proximity using 2006 data in Columns 2, 3, 5, and 6. Columns 2 and 5 only control for region fixed effects. Columns 3 and 6 control semi-parametically for road, trading center, and school proximity.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
Columns 2 and 5 of Table 2 assess baseline balance by showing the coefficient from a regression of each variable on ART proximity with 2006 data. As we discuss above, 0.125 · β̂ approximates the in-sample difference between “near” and “far” respondents. ART proximity is uncorrelated with baseline work time. The insignificant coefficient of −0.22 implies that people near ART work around 0.03 fewer hours (< 2 fewer minutes) per day. Farm work time has a similarly flat ART proximity gradient. However people near ART spend 0.21 more hours (13 more minutes) on home production and 0.23 fewer hours (14 fewer minutes) on other production than people far from ART. This difference is statistically significant but is not economically meaningful. ART proximity is also uncorrelated with age, marital status, household size, and monetary wealth. People near ART have 0.2 additional years of schooling and are 4 percentage points more likely to have a metal roof, which are the only significant demographic differences. Finally, ART proximity is uncorrelated with HIV prevalence, which is consistent with the lack of ART targeting.
In contrast, baseline subjective mortality risk and mental health are modestly correlated with ART proximity. Mortality risk perceptions are 2.9 percentage points higher and MCS-12 scores are 0.70 points lower (0.09 standard deviations) for people near ART. This pattern seems to arise because actual and perceived HIV conditions are worse near high-traffic areas, both in the MLSFH and in nationally-representative data (Feldacker et al. 2011).9 People who witness AIDS mortality and morbidity have more extreme perceptions of the epidemic. Montgomery (2000) and Anglewicz and Kohler (2009) argue that people formulate beliefs about HIV and AIDS through observational heuristics. People may incorporate visible signals such as observed mortality and morbidity to arrive at perceptions of less observable HIV parameters like prevalence.10
In Columns 3 and 6, we assess this explanation by regressing each variable on ART proximity while controlling for the distance to a primary road, trading center, and school. We control for these distances semi-parametrically though region-specific quartile dummies for each variable. Although the estimates leave much unexplained variation (R2 values range from 0.1 to 0.2), the spatial controls greatly reduce the correlation between ART proximity and the characteristics in the table. Coefficients are insignificant for all but two variables. In particular, the coefficients for own mortality risk and the MCS-12 score are 50–53 percent smaller and are no longer significant. These results suggest that people near ART perceive greater mortality risk and have worse mental health because they also live in places with more foot traffic and therefore greater exposure to AIDS-related mortality and morbidity.
Outcomes appear to worsen for sample respondents from 2006 to 2010. From 2006 to 2010, work time declines by 119 minutes per day, subjective mortality risk increases by 4.4 percentage points, and the MCS-12 index declines by 2.6 points (0.3 standard deviations). Two key factors are likely to explain this pattern. First, 2006 outcomes may be abnormally good because of an extremely generous fertilizer subsidy that the government of Malawi offered in 2005/2006. According to Dorward et al. (2011), this program increased national maize output by 26 to 60 percent. Secondly, perceptions of the HIV/AIDS epidemic worsened over this period, which may explain these trends if mortality risk influences mental health and labor supply. The average number of observed AIDS deaths per year grew from 2.0 to 3.7 from 2006 to 2010. This pattern concurs with an 88 percent increase in MLSFH attrition due to mortality over the period (Kohler et al. 2014). The average number of acquaintances with AIDS (which is not available in 2010) grew from 2.2 to 3.4 from 2006 to 2008. During this period, perceived HIV prevalence grew from 28 to 37 percent and the share of respondents who were worried about HIV/AIDS rose from 41 to 51 percent.
3 Estimation
3.1 Empirical Strategy and Identification
This section estimates the effect of ART availability on work time. Regressions are based on a standard difference-in-difference specification with a continuous treatment variable. In the equation below, i indexes the individual, j indexes the village, r indexes the region, and t indexes the survey year.
(1) |
Proxijr is the inverse distance by road to the nearest ART facility once ART becomes available in 2008. This parameterization allows us to interpret access in terms of proximity, although we also show key results non-parametrically. 0.125 · β̂, which appears in brackets below each coefficient, is the impact of reducing ART distance from 8 kilometers to 4 kilometers, which as we have argued above, as a reasonable in-sample estimate of the impact of ART access. Non-parametric results below indicate that this distance range contributes substantially to our estimates. Postt is a dummy for the 2008 and/or 2010 survey rounds. αijr is an individual fixed effect (which replaces Proxijr) and δrt is a region-specific time fixed effect (which replaces the main effect of Postt). We cluster standard errors by village, which allows for arbitrarily-correlated errors for respondents in the same village, including respondents in the same household.
This empirical strategy relies on the assumption that ART proximity is uncorrelated with trends in the unobservable determinants of work time and other outcomes. As we discuss above, it is unlikely that policymakers targeted ART based on the local severity of the HIV/AIDS epidemic. However ART proximity could be incidentally correlated with economic shocks such as foreign aid, agricultural subsidies, or labor market conditions. We test the validity of the parallel trends assumption in two ways. First, we use data from 2004 and 2006 to test for pre-intervention trends in work time and available demographic characteristics. We cannot test for differential pre-trends for mental health and subjective mortality risk because these variables are only available in one pre-intervention round. Secondly, we investigate concurrent trends in thirteen measures of economic shocks and support.
Panel A of Figure 4 shows that total work time, our main outcome variable, does not exhibit differential pre-trends. The figure plots the ART distance gradient for work time non-parametrically by year. Although work time increases from 2004 to 2006, there is no differential change because the gradient remains flat in both years. Table 3 extends this investigation by regressing work time and demographic variables on ART proximity, 2006 ART proximity, and regional time dummies with data from 2004 and 2006. The ART proximity coefficient provides the cross-sectional gradient in 2004 and the 2006 · ART proximity coefficient provides the differential change near ART facilities. According to Column 1, which reprises Figure 4, total work time increases differentially by just 0.032 hours (< 2 minutes) near ART, with a 95-percent confidence interval of −9 to 12 minutes. Even the upper bound of 12 minutes is only 38 percent as large as our main estimate below. Panel B shows that this pre-trend is even smaller in the HIV−/NCT sample.
Table 3.
Work Time |
Educ. |
Married |
HH Size |
Metal Roof |
HIV+ |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total |
Farm |
House |
Other |
||||||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | (9) | |
Panel A: Full Sample | |||||||||
ART proximity | −0.48 (0.86) [−0.060] |
−0.99 (0.90) [−0.12] |
0.73 (0.73) [0.091] |
−0.22 (1.06) [−0.028] |
1.47 (1.15) [0.184] |
−0.17** (0.072) [−0.021] |
0.65 (0.75) [0.081] |
0.29* (0.17) [0.036] |
0.029 (0.072) [0.004] |
2006 · ART proximity | 0.26 (0.85) [0.032] |
0.89 (0.96) [0.11] |
0.96 (0.74) [0.12] |
−1.59** (0.63) [−0.20] |
0.43 (0.60) [0.053] |
−0.04 (0.08) [−0.005] |
−0.99 (0.74) [−0.12] |
0.11 (0.09) [0.014] |
0.06 (0.05) [0.008] |
Dependent variable mean Observations |
8.03 3757 |
2.50 3757 |
4.08 3757 |
1.46 3757 |
4.94 3667 |
0.86 3702 |
5.32 3061 |
0.15 3675 |
0.06 3609 |
Panel B: HIV−/NCT Sample | |||||||||
ART proximity | −0.68 (1.09) [−0.085] |
−1.32 (0.87) [−0.17] |
1.12 (0.75) [0.14] |
−0.48 (1.08) [−0.060] |
1.35 (1.29) [0.17] |
−0.21** (0.092) [−0.027] |
0.83 (0.72) [0.104] |
0.27 (0.18) [0.033] |
- |
2006 · ART proximity | 0.08 (1.16) [0.010] |
1.25 (0.85) [0.16] |
0.65 (0.87) [0.08] |
−1.82*** (0.51) [−0.23] |
0.43 (0.62) [0.053] |
−0.017 (0.096) [−0.002] |
−1.50* (0.82) [−0.19] |
0.16 (0.10) [0.020] |
- |
Dependent variable mean Observations |
8.03 2804 |
2.53 2804 |
4.01 2804 |
1.49 2804 |
5.13 2733 |
0.87 2762 |
5.29 2278 |
0.15 2742 |
- |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of the interquartile proximity difference appear in brackets. Regressions are based on data from 2004 and 2006. All regressions include region · year fixed effects.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
The rest of Table 3 investigates differential pre-trends for the components of work time and for demographic characteristics. In Columns 2 and 3, pre-trends for cultivation and home production time are small and insignificant. Column 4 shows a small but significant differential pre-trend for time spent on other economic activities. The estimate indicates that, from 2004 to 2006, people near ART differentially reduced other economic time by 0.12 hours in the full sample and by 0.08 hours in the HIV−/NCT sample. The sign of this trend is the opposite of our main estimate below but raises the possibility of differential substitution among types of labor. The rest of the table shows that there are no significant pre-trends in education or metal roof ownership. The negative pre-trend in household size is insignificant in the full sample but is marginally significant in the HIV−/NCT sample (p = 0.07). Although there is a significant level difference in marital status in 2004, there is not a significant differential trend. In Column 9, the differential HIV prevalence trend is small and insignificant (the near/far difference is 0.08), which further suggests a lack of targeting.
Next we assess whether economic conditions are correlated with ART proximity. ART availability may appear to increase work time if economic conditions differentially encourage labor near ART facilities. The MLSFH measures seven individual economic shocks, including income loss, property loss, the death of a breadwinner, divorce, poor harvest, heavy grain price fluctuations, and “other” shocks. The survey also measures the use of six forms of social support, including nutrition programs, agricultural subsidies, secondary and tertiary tuition subsidies, food-for-work programs, congregational support, and direct cash transfers. Since these variables are only available in 2008 and 2010, we cannot test for differential pre-trends. However we can assess whether ART availability is correlated with concurrent levels and changes in these variables. We regress these outcomes on ART proximity, 2010 · ART proximity and regional time dummies with 2008 and 2010 data. The ART proximity coefficient measures the cross-sectional gradient in 2008 and the 2010 · ART proximity coefficient measures the differential change from 2008 to 2010 near ART facilities.
Table 4 shows that concurrent economic shocks are not systematically correlated with ART proximity. Most estimates are economically small and statistically insignificant. According to the largest and most significant coefficient of 0.30 in Column 1, the probability of lost income increases differentially by 3.7 percentage points near ART, which is a small effect. Table 5 also shows a weak relationship between ART proximity and economic support. The coefficient on ART proximity is small and insignificant for five out of six variables while the coefficient on 2010 · ART proximity is insignificant for four out of six variables. The estimates in the table do not show a consistent pattern: for instance, people near ART receive additional congregational support but less agricultural support. In general, these results suggest that concurrent economic patterns are unlikely to confound our estimates.
Table 4.
Lost income |
Damages |
Loss of provider |
Divorce |
Poor crop |
Grain price fluctuation |
Other shocks |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | |||
Panel A: Full Sample | |||||||||
ART proximity | −0.11 (0.12) [−0.014] |
0.04 (0.09) [0.005] |
0.01 (0.11) [0.002] |
0.08 (0.07) [0.010] |
−0.21 (0.18) [−0.026] |
−0.11 (0.11) [−0.013] |
−0.01 (0.01) [−0.001] |
||
2010 · ART proximity | 0.30* (0.17) [0.037] |
−0.13 (0.09) [−0.016] |
0.02 (0.16) [0.002] |
−0.12 (0.09) [−0.014] |
0.12 (0.21) [0.015] |
−0.21 (0.15) [−0.027] |
0.16 (0.13) [0.020] |
||
Dependent variable mean Observations |
0.40 3032 |
0.12 3032 |
0.39 3032 |
0.09 3032 |
0.68 3032 |
0.73 3032 |
0.35 3032 |
||
Panel B: HIV−/NCT Sample | |||||||||
ART proximity | −0.030 (0.15) [−0.004] |
0.024 (0.093) [0.003] |
0.046 (0.11) [0.006] |
0.084 (0.088) [0.010] |
−0.23 (0.19) [−0.029] |
−0.093 (0.10) [−0.012] |
−0.007 (0.007) [−0.001] |
||
2010 · ART proximity | 0.27 (0.21) [0.034] |
−0.086 (0.11) [−0.011] |
−0.095 (0.20) [−0.012] |
−0.11 (0.13) [−0.013] |
0.20 (0.22) [0.025] |
−0.27 (0.17) [−0.034] |
0.16 (0.13) [0.020] |
||
Dependent variable mean Observations |
0.40 2280 |
0.11 2280 |
0.38 2280 |
0.09 2280 |
0.67 2280 |
0.72 2280 |
0.35 2280 |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of the interquartile proximity difference appear in brackets. Regressions are based on data from 2008 and 2010. All regressions include region · year fixed effects.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01
Table 5.
Dependent variable: | Nutrition programs |
Agro. support |
Tuition subsidies |
Food for work |
Congregation support |
Direct cash transfers |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | |
Panel A: Full Sample | ||||||
ART proximity | 0.12 (0.14) [0.015] |
0.04 (0.14) [0.005] |
0.07 (0.05) [0.009] |
−0.04 (0.12) [−0.004] |
−0.02 (0.05) [−0.003] |
0.10*** (0.04) [0.013] |
2010 · ART proximity | −0.14 (0.14) [−0.017] |
−0.31*** (0.10) [−0.039] |
−0.03 (0.071) [0.004] |
0.08 (0.12) [0.010] |
0.19** (0.08) [0.023] |
−0.070 (0.07) [−0.009] |
Dependent variable mean Observations |
0.22 3100 |
0.68 3100 |
0.02 3100 |
0.14 3100 |
0.09 3100 |
0.03 3100 |
Panel B: HIV−/NCT Sample | ||||||
ART proximity | 0.16 (0.19) [0.020] |
0.0011 (0.14) [0.000] |
0.092 (0.065) [0.012] |
−0.023 (0.11) [−0.003] |
−0.022 (0.065) [−0.003] |
0.087* (0.052) [0.011] |
2010 · ART proximity | −0.20 (0.16) [−0.025] |
−0.29** (0.12) [−0.036] |
−0.042 (0.085) [−0.005] |
0.086 (0.12) [0.011] |
0.21** (0.082) [0.027] |
−0.045 (0.052) [−0.006] |
Dependent variable mean Observations |
0.21 2333 |
0.68 2333 |
0.02 2333 |
0.13 2333 |
0.09 2333 |
0.03 2333 |
Note: village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of a change from 8 kilometers to 4 kilometers (0.125 · β̂) appear in brackets. Regressions include data from 2008 and 2010. All regressions include region · year fixed effects. Nutrition programs include free food or maize, targeted nutrition programs, and programs to feed malnourished children. Agricultural support includes inputs-for-work programs, agricultural input supply programs, and other agricultural input subsidies. Tuition subsidies apply to secondary and tertiary tuition.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
3.2 Estimates for Work Time
Table 6 shows the impact of ART availability on work time. In Panel A, which focuses on the full sample, the coefficient on ART proximity is 5.04. According to this estimate, ART availability increases daily work time differentially by 0.63 hours (38 minutes) for nearby respondents, which is 8 percent of the 2006 level. Panel A of Figure 4 illustrates this effect graphically by showing the ART distance gradient by year. Although the gradient is flat in the 2004 and 2006 pre-intervention rounds (consistent with the absence of pre-trends), it declines strikingly in ART distance in 2010, two years after ART becomes available. Panel B shows the gradient for the difference in work time between 2010 and 2006 and includes confidence intervals. ART proximity has the strongest effect in the range of 4 to 8 kilometers from a facility. By showing the effect non-parametrically, the figure also establishes that parametric assumptions are not responsible for the significant regression estimates.
Table 6.
Hours per Day |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total |
Farm |
Home |
Other |
|||||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
Panel A: Full Sample | ||||||||
2010 · ART proximity | 5.04*** (1.89) [0.63] |
5.17*** (1.80) [0.65] |
2.21* (1.18) [0.28] |
2.08* (1.20) [0.26] |
0.98 (1.05) [0.12] |
1.23 (1.10) [0.15] |
1.85* (1.03) [0.23] |
1.86* (1.01) [0.23] |
Proportional selection δ Dependent variable mean Observations |
- 7.49 5380 |
−2.23 7.49 5380 |
- 2.48 5380 |
0.86 2.48 5380 |
- 3.58 5380 |
−0.19 3.58 5380 |
- 1.44 5380 |
−4.14 1.44 5380 |
Panel B: HIV−/NCT Sample | ||||||||
2010 · ART proximity | 5.59*** (1.77) [0.70] |
5.34*** (1.62) [0.67] |
2.84*** (1.07) [0.36] |
2.74** (1.17) [0.34] |
0.79 (1.04) [0.10] |
0.89 (0.95) [0.11] |
1.96** (0.93) [0.24] |
1.71** (0.84) [0.21] |
Proportional selection δ Dependent variable mean Observations |
- 7.49 4030 |
1.11 7.49 4030 |
- 2.50 4030 |
1.27 2.50 4030 |
- 3.52 4030 |
−0.89 3.52 4030 |
- 1.46 4030 |
0.63 1.46 4030 |
Demo and economic controls | - | Yes | - | Yes | - | Yes | - | Yes |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of a change from 8 kilometers to 4 kilometers (0.125 · β̂) appear in brackets. Even columns control for Post · demographic characteristics, economic shocks, and access to social safety nets. All regressions include individual and region · time fixed effects.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
Columns 3, 5, and 7 show that people primarily respond by spending more time on farming and other economic activities, rather than household production. Nearby respondents spend 0.28 additional hours per day farming, 0.12 additional hours on home production (an insignificant result), and 0.23 additional hours on other economic activities. The magnitude of the farming time effect is consistent with the strong labor supply response to a 2005/2006 fertilizer subsidy program (Dorward et al. 2011), as well as other health interventions in developing countries (Behrman et al. 1997, Louriero 2009, Fink and Masiye 2012).11
Panel B of Table 6 focuses on the HIV−/NCT sample. Estimates in these panels closely resemble the results for the full sample in Panel A. Point estimates for total work time, cultivation time, and other production time are slightly larger (although the differences with Panel A are insignificant) for HIV-negative non-caretakers than for the full sample. In Column 1, ART availability increases daily work time by 37 minutes for nearby HIV-negative non-caretakers. Farm time and other economic time increase by 15 minutes and 12 minutes respectively. These results are striking because HIV-negative non-caretakers do not directly benefit from ART.
3.3 Robustness
Next we assess the robustness of the work time estimates. One concern is that the placement of ART may be correlated with demographic and economic determinants of work time. Table 3–Table 5 already address this issue by showing that ART proximity is not systematically correlated with observable demographic characteristics, economic shocks, or the utilization of economic support. As another test, the even columns of Table 6 estimate the impact of ART availability after controlling for trends in these variables. We include the controls flexibly by creating distinct year-specific versions of every control and interacting all of these variables with year dummies. Joint significance tests (available from the authors) show that these controls significantly impact work time. If unobservable trends are positively correlated with these variables and ART proximity, this approach should attenuate our estimates.
A comparison of the even and odd columns of Table 6 shows that estimates are not sensitive to the controls. Including the controls causes the coefficient of interest to decrease by a maximum of 15 percent. However, Oster (2014) notes that the power of this exercise hinges on the explanatory power of the controls. We implement this test more systematically by following Altonji, Elder and Taber (2005) and Oster (2014) to assess whether unobservables could plausibly explain our results under the assumption that selection on observables and unobservables is proportional. Oster derives the proportionality coefficient (δ) necessary to cause the observed treatment effect spuriously.12 A large value of δ indicates that a high degree of correlated unobservable selection is necessary to negate the observed estimate, which connotes robustness. Oster proposes δ = 1 as a benchmark at which controls and unobservables are equally correlated with treatment. In fact a lower benchmark may be reasonable if the observables explain most of the selection. Negative values of δ, which occur when the controls strengthen the treatment effect estimate, mean that unobservables must be negatively correlated with the controls to negate the estimate. Since positively correlated unobservables (e.g., unmeasured economic shocks) are the main threat to identification, negative δ’s also connote robustness.
The tables provide estimates of the proportional selection δ for regressions with controls. In Column 2, the estimate that δ = −2.23 in Panel A means that unobservable trends must cause more than twice as much selection as the controls and be negatively-correlated with the controls to negate the full-sample estimate. According to the estimate of δ = 1.11 in Panel B, unobservable trends must cause more selection than the controls to negate the HIV−/NCT estimate. Unobservable trends of this magnitude are unlikely because the controls already catalog the main economic shocks in this setting. δ estimates for the components of work time in Columns 4, 6, and 8 are similarly large or negative, although δ values are mechanically smaller for coefficients with less statistical significance.
3.4 Placebo Tests
Next we test the validity of our findings through four placebo tests. One concern is that our estimates may reflect a spatial correlation between ART and other features such as medical facilities and population centers. If differential trends near clinics confound our estimates, then regressions based on non-ART clinic proximity may yield spurious results. Similarly, if results arise because of differential trends near population centers, then regressions based on the proximity of roads, trading centers, and schools may show effects.
Table 7 shows the interactions between a 2010 dummy and ART, clinic, major road, trading center, and school proximities. Conditional on ART proximity, the 2010-clinic proximity coefficient is identified through the 68-percent subsample whose nearest clinic does not offer ART. Columns 1 – 4 show full sample estimates and Columns 5 – 8 show HIV−/NCT estimates. In Columns 1 and 5, the effects of clinic proximity, road proximity, and school proximity on work time are small and insignificant.13 The effect of trading center proximity is marginally significant but small. In contrast, the impact of ART availability conditional on these variables closely resembles the Table 6 estimate. The full-sample estimate in Column 1 is 96 percent as large in Table 6, while the HIV−/NCT estimate in Column 5 is 94 percent as large. These patterns suggest that results are due to ART access rather than proximity to other geographic features.
Table 7.
Full Sample |
HIV−/NCT Sample |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hours per Day |
Hours per Day |
|||||||
Total |
Farm |
Home |
Other |
Total |
Farm |
Home |
Other |
|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
2010 · ART proximity | 4.83** (1.97) |
1.81 (1.21) |
1.04 (1.04) |
1.97* (1.04) |
5.23*** (1.93) |
2.35** (1.14) |
0.81 (1.05) |
2.07** (0.94) |
2010 · Any clinic proximity | −0.12 (0.61) |
0.19 (0.47) |
−0.06 (0.15) |
−0.25 (0.27) |
0.03 (0.52) |
0.37 (0.43) |
−0.10 (0.20) |
−0.24 (0.25) |
2010 · Road proximity | 0.22 (0.18) |
0.14 (0.14) |
0.15 (0.11) |
−0.07 (0.10) |
0.25 (0.19) |
0.10 (0.15) |
0.21 (0.15) |
−0.05 (0.099) |
2010 · Trading center proximity | 0.47* (0.27) |
0.28* (0.15) |
0.03 (0.13) |
0.16 (0.12) |
0.45 (0.29) |
0.28* (0.15) |
0.05 (0.16) |
0.12 (0.12) |
2010 · School proximity | −0.007 (0.006) |
−0.0003 (0.009) |
−0.007 (0.007) |
−0.0001 (0.003) |
0.01 (0.009) |
0.01 (0.009) |
−0.003 (0.006) |
0.003 (0.005) |
Dependent variable mean Observations |
7.49 5380 |
2.48 5380 |
3.58 5380 |
1.44 5380 |
7.49 4066 |
2.50 4066 |
3.52 4066 |
1.46 4066 |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
Figure 5 explores this pattern graphically by plotting distance gradients to a non-ART clinic, major road, trading center, and school for the change in work time from 2006 to 2010. These plots are analogous to the ART distance gradient in Panel B of Figure 4.14 These gradients are much flatter that Figure 4, and suggest further that the impact of ART availability is not based on a correlation with the distance to these features.
4 Interpretation
4.1 Subjective Mortality Risk
The strong effect of ART availability on the work time of HIV−/NCT respondents is puzzling since these people do not directly benefit from AIDS treatment. However ART availability may affect work time by changing risk perceptions in this population. This subsection shows that ART availability reduces subjective mortality risk for HIV−/NCT respondents. In principle, ART benefits HIV-negative people by reducing HIV infection risk and mortality risk conditional on infection. Mortality risk data are available in 2006, 2008, and 2010. Without multiple pre-intervention rounds for these outcomes, we cannot investigate differential pre-trends. Table 2 shows that prior to the intervention, HIV−/NCT respondents perceived a 37 percent risk of death within five years, nearly four times as large as the life-table estimate for this age cohort in Malawi. They perceived a 68 percent mortality risk conditional on HIV infection.
For someone who is HIV-negative, subjective mortality risk depends on both infection risk and HIV-positive mortality risk. Table 8 shows the effect of ART availability on subjective mortality risk, infection risk, and HIV-positive mortality risk for the HIV−/NCT sample. As above, even columns include demographic and economic controls and provide proportional selection δ’s. We report interactions with 2008 and 2010 dummies, both of which belong to the “post” period.15 Columns 1 and 2 show estimates for own five-year mortality risk. Multiplying the coefficient by 0.11 shows that ART availability reduces subjective mortality risk differentially by 3 percentage points for nearby respondents by 2010.
Table 8.
Own Mort. Risk |
HIV+ Mort Risk |
Infection Risk |
Worried HIV |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
2008 · ART proximity | −0.13 (0.11) [−0.016] |
−0.097 (0.10) [−0.012] |
−0.14** (0.066) [−0.018] |
−0.18** (0.074) [−0.022] |
−1.09** (0.42) [−0.14] |
−0.78 (0.51) [−0.097] |
−0.52* (0.28) [−0.065] |
−0.35 (0.29) [−0.043] |
2010 · ART proximity | −0.24** (0.11) [−0.030] |
−0.20* (0.11) [−0.025] |
−0.12* (0.070) [−0.015] |
−0.15* (0.080) [−0.018] |
−0.87*** (0.27) [−0.11] |
−0.56 (0.35) [−0.069] |
−1.09** (0.44) [−0.14] |
−0.94** (0.43) [−0.12] |
Demo and economic controls | - | Yes | - | Yes | - | Yes | - | Yes |
Proportional selection δ (2008) | - | 0.42 | - | −0.55 | - | 0.42 | - | 0.21 |
Proportional selection δ (2010) | - | 0.74 | - | −0.55 | - | 0.29 | - | 0.69 |
Dependent variable mean | 0.40 | 0.40 | 0.70 | 0.70 | 0.90 | 0.90 | 1.68 | 1.68 |
Observations | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of a change from 8 kilometers to 4 kilometers (0.125 · β̂) appear in brackets. Even columns control for Post · demographic characteristics, economic shocks, and access to social safety nets.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01
Figure 6 illustrates this result graphically by plotting the ART distance gradient by year (Panel A) and for the 2006 – 2010 change (Panel B). The 2006 gradient shows higher baseline mortality risk near ART facilities, which is also evident in Table 2. As we discuss above, this pattern most likely arises because evidence of AIDS morbidity and mortality is more visible near populated areas. The figure also shows that perceived mortality risk increases over time, which is likely due to a temporal increase in observed AIDS morbidity and mortality.16 The ART distance gradient flattens between 2006 and 2008 and reverses sign by 2010, so that people near ART eventually perceive less risk than people far away. Estimates for subjective mortality risk over one-year and ten-year horizon (available from the authors) closely resemble the results in the table.
Columns 3 and 4 of Table 8 show that ART availability significantly reduces HIV-positive mortality risk and Columns 5 and 6 show that it significantly reduces perceived HIV infection risk. It is difficult to compare the magnitudes of these effects because HIV-positive mortality risk is measured on a probability scale and infection risk is measured on a Likert scale. Finally, Columns 7 and 8 show that ART availability significantly reduces the extent to which respondents “worry about HIV.” This estimate suggests a link between ART availability, subjective mortality risk, and mental health, which we explore further below.
4.2 Reasons for the Work Time Response
4.2.1 Life-Cycle Incentives
ART availability may increase HIV−/NCT work time because life expectancy influences the life-cycle incentive to save. The sign of this effect is ambiguous because additional life expectancy increases both the demand for assets and the earnings time horizon (Hazan 2009). Therefore ART availability may either lead people to supply more labor to save for the future or to supply less labor by providing a longer horizon to accumulate savings. Baranov and Kohler (2014) show that ART availability leads to an increase in expenditures on children, which is consistent with life-cycle reoptimization (Bloom et al. 2003, Ben-Porath 1967, Cervellati and Sunde 2013).
To investigate this possibility further, we explore whether people respond to ART availability on other margins. Cultivation of own land is the primary occupation for 63 percent of the sample, while paid employment is the primary occupation for just 4 percent of the sample. Nearly all respondents, including those with other primary occupations, cultivate maize. While maize is labor intensive, farmers can increase yields with fertilizer and basic mechanization. Column 3 of Table 6 shows that ART significantly increases agricultural work time. A person who reoptimizes agricultural work time should also reoptimize other farm inputs that are within his or her control.
Table 9 estimates the impact of ART availability on fertilizer use, new equipment purchases, hired labor, and seed purchases, as well as the ownership of cattle, goats, pigs, and chickens. Before proceeding, we regress these variables on economic shock and support controls to confirm that they are sensitive to economic incentives. P-values are below 0.01 in every case, indicating that these variables are sensitive to shocks (estimates available from the authors).17 Since all of these outcomes are censored at zero, we estimate Tobit models in which positive observations are expressed in logs.18 Columns 1 – 4 show that ART availability does not increase the utilization of other farm inputs. Both extensive and intensive impacts are insignificant for fertilizer, equipment, and seeds. In Column 3, respondents utilize less hired labor, which is consistent with substitution between own and hired labor inputs.19 Livestock are both a farm product and an asset. People who have reoptimized farm labor to save for the future may accumulate livestock either to increase production or to save. Columns 5 – 8 of Table 9 show that ART availability does not lead people to accumulate livestock. Columns 5 and 8 indicate that people near ART decrease cattle and chicken ownership while Columns 6 and 7 show no effect on ownership of goats or pigs. These findings suggest that life-cycle reoptimization may not fully explain the strong HIV−/NCT work time response. Nevertheless, reductions in livestock and hired labor, combined with pre-trends in other work time, raise the possibility that our results also reflect substitution behavior.
Table 9.
Fertilizer Purchases |
Equip. Purchases |
Hired Labor |
Seed Purchases |
Cattle |
Goats |
Pigs |
Chickens |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
Panel A: M.E. on pr(y > 0) | ||||||||
2008 · ART Proximity | 0.0048 (0.14) [0.000] |
0.10 (0.16) [0.012] |
−0.18* (0.11) [−0.023] |
0.16 (0.17) [0.019] |
−0.026* (0.014) [−0.003] |
0.20 (0.20) [0.024] |
−0.053 (0.17) [−0.007] |
−0.065 (0.050) [−0.008] |
2010 · ART Proximity | −0.18 (0.18) [−0.022] |
0.18 (0.14) [0.023] |
−0.44*** (0.11) [−0.055] |
0.19 (0.17) [0.024] |
−0.031* (0.018) [−0.004] |
0.19 (0.19) [0.024] |
−0.050 (0.19) [−0.006] |
−0.10* (0.060) [−0.013] |
Panel B: M.E. on E (ln y/y > 0) | ||||||||
2008 · ART Proximity | 0.047 (1.41) [0.006] |
0.78 (1.17) [0.097] |
−1.31* (0.76) [−0.16] |
1.06 (1.15) [0.13] |
−1.13* (0.61) [−0.14] |
1.10 (1.11) [0.14] |
−0.30 (0.98) [−0.038] |
−1.18 (0.88) [−0.15] |
2010 · ART Proximity | −1.76 (1.80) [−0.22] |
1.37 (1.06) [0.17] |
−3.20*** (0.77) [−0.40] |
1.31 (1.14) [0.16] |
−1.33* (0.75) [−0.17] |
1.09 (1.08) [0.14] |
−0.28 (1.10) [−0.036] |
−1.87* (1.05) [−0.23] |
Observations | 5430 | 5430 | 5430 | 5430 | 4993 | 5087 | 5014 | 5385 |
Note: clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of a change from 8 kilometers to 4 kilometers (0.125 · β̂) appear in brackets. Dependent variables are expressed in logs.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
4.2.2 General-Equilibrium Effects
Alternatively, ART availability may increase HIV−/NCT work time by increasing labor or output demand. ART recipients may demand additional goods and services, increasing economic opportunity near ART facilities. They may supply more labor, which could increase HIV-negative labor demand if labor inputs are complementary. These mechanisms are not especially plausible for two reasons. First, in a subsistence economy, most people are self-employed and consume their own output, so that markets do not heavily influence labor supply decisions on the margin. Secondly, relatively few people are affected by ART directly. While 5.3 percent of people have HIV in this setting, only 32 percent of these people qualify for ART based on their disease status. Since households have an average of 1.24 other adults who are potential caretakers, the prevalence of people directly affected by ART availability is unlikely to exceed 3.8 percent.20 A change in behavior in this small subpopulation is unlikely to have large general equilibrium effects.
To support this argument further, we estimate the impact of ART availability on occupation choice (results available from the authors). We distinguish between subsistence farming (the modal category) and all other occupations, such as wage labor market work, domestic activities, and unemployment. With a joint p-value of 0.11, subsistence farming is sensitive to economic shocks and support. However ART availability has a small and insignificant effect on occupation choice. The coefficient on ART proximity is −0.08 in 2008 and 0.09 in 2010, with p-values of 0.63 and 0.60 respectively. Although the magnitude is small, the positive 2010 coefficient suggests that, if anything, ART availability increases subsistence farming, which is incompatible with labor demand as the explanation for the impact on work time.
4.2.3 Mental Health
Finally, ART availability may increase work time by improving mental health. People who worry about HIV/AIDS are at risk of depression, anxiety, and other mental disorders. Traumatic life events such as the deaths of family members are also common in HIV-endemic set-tings.21 Mental health may affect labor supply through the disutility of effort. The marginal benefit of a unit of labor increases with effort, so that a mental health improvement increases the return to labor and the optimal labor supply (Fortin et al. 2010). Economic and psychiatric studies correlate mental health and labor market outcomes such as employment, absenteeism, and earnings (e.g. Ettner et al. 1997, Kessler and Frank 1997, Hamilton et al. 1997, Berndt et al. 1998).
Table 10 shows the impact of ART availability on mental health. Columns 1 and 2 show results for the MCS-12 score without and with demographic and economic controls. According to the 2010 estimate, ART availability increases the MCS-12 nearby respondents by 1.06 points (0.13 standard deviations), which is 28 percent of the pre-ART mental health difference between HIV-negative and HIV-positive respondents (3.74 points). MCS-I2 deviations of this magnitude are associated with tangible differences in income and household circumstances in other settings (Larson 2002, Balsa et al. 2009). A comparison of Columns 1 and 2 shows that the estimate is insensitive to the controls, although δ’s are somewhat lower than for work time or mortality risk.
Table 10.
MH limits on: |
Well- Being |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MCS-12 |
Depression |
Energy |
Calm |
Activities |
Accomps. |
|||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
2008 · ART proximity | 5.59* (2.96) [0.70] |
4.15 (3.25) [0.52] |
−0.26 (0.23) [−0.032] |
0.36** (0.15) [0.045] |
0.32* (0.16) [0.040] |
−0.12 (0.13) [−0.015] |
−0.10 (0.10) [−0.013] |
0.12 (0.19) [0.015] |
2010 · ART proximity | 8.16*** (2.55) [1.02] |
6.79** (2.69) [0.85] |
−0.44** (0.21) [−0.055] |
0.42** (0.19) [0.053] |
0.64*** (0.17) [0.080] |
−0.16 (0.10) [−0.021] |
−0.15 (0.098) [−0.019] |
0.42** (0.19) [0.052] |
Demo and economic controls | - | Yes | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Proportional selection δ (2008) | - | 0.35 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Proportional selection δ (2010) | - | 0.59 | - | - | - | - | - | - |
Dependent variable mean | 55.0 | 55.0 | 0.39 | 0.53 | 0.50 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.74 |
Observations | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 | 3663 |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. Impacts of a change from 8 kilometers to 4 kilometers (0.125 · β̂) appear in brackets. We transform the outcomes in Columns 3–5 and 8 into binary variables, as we describe in the text. All regressions include individual and region · year fixed effects. Column 2 controls for Post · demographic characteristics, economic shocks, and access to social safety nets.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
Figure 7 illustrates the identifying variation for these estimates by plotting the ART distance gradient for the MCS-12 score non-parametrically by year. Panel A shows two important patterns. Mental health is worse near eventual ART facilities in the 2006 pre-intervention round, which is also evident in Table 2. As we discuss above, greater exposure to AIDS morbidity and mortality near populated areas is the most likely explanation for this pattern. Secondly, the MCS-12 declines over time, falling by 0.3 standard deviations from 55.6 to 53.0 from 2006 to 2010. An increase in AIDS morbidity and mortality over time may explain this pattern, as we discuss in Section 4.1. The regression estimate for mental health arises because mental health falls differentially over time for people who live far from ART.
Columns 3 –7 of Table 10 show the impact on MCS-12 components, including depression, energy, anxiety (calm), and mental-health limitations on activities and accomplishments. These variables are symptomatic of poor mental health but may not predict mental disorders independently. In Columns 3 – 5, ART availability reduces the depression and anxiety measures by 5 and 6 percentage points, respectively, for nearby respondents. In Columns 6 and 7, ART availability has negative and significant effects on perceived mental-health limitations on activities and accomplishments.22 Column 8 shows a significant positive effect on subjective well-being.
The strong impact on mental health suggests that mental health may contribute to the impact of ART availability on HIV−/NCT work time. Table 11 provides more suggestive evidence of this relationship by showing the association between mortality risk, mental health, and work time. We regress mental health on subjective mortality risk in Columns 1 and 2 and work time on mental health in Columns 3 and 4. Odd columns, which omit individual fixed effects, are identified through both cross-sectional and temporal variation while even columns, which include individual fixed effects, are identified through temporal variation within individuals. Columns 1 and 2 imply that mental health is 4 – 6 points lower for someone who is certain she will die than for someone who is certain she will survive. Columns 3 and 4 indicate that a one-point increase in the MCS-12 is associated with 2 – 3 additional minutes of work per day.23 Regressions that control for demographic, economic, and safety net variables (available from the authors) lead to very similar estimates.
Table 11.
MCS-12 |
Work Time |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | |
Subjective Mortality Risk | −6.24*** (0.75) |
−3.75*** (1.05) |
- | - |
MCS-12 | - | - | 0.030*** (0.0089) |
0.056*** (0.016) |
Individual fixed effects | - | Yes | - | Yes |
Observations | 3303 | 3303 | 3303 | 3303 |
R2 | 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.18 |
Note: Village-clustered standard errors appear in parentheses. All regressions control for region · year fixed effects.
p < 0.1,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.01.
A reduced-form analysis cannot establish mechanisms definitively. Instead of an effect of mental health on work time, the increase in work time in Table 6 may improve mental health. Studies in several contexts show that economic circumstances (e.g. retirement, macroeconomic conditions, winning the lottery) affect mental health (Mandal and Roe 2008, Dave et al. 2008, Tefft 2011, Latif 2013, McInerney et al. 2013, Baird et al. 2013, Apouey and Clark 2014, Adhvaryu et al. 2014). The significant impact of ART availability on perceived mental-health limits on activities and accomplishments directly suggests an effect of mental health on labor supply. People near ART perceive that poor mental health has less of an effect on their lives.
5 Conclusion
We find that ART availability increases work time for HIV-negative non-caretakers, who do not directly benefit from AIDS treatment. Several tests show that this result is robust. Total work time and demographic characteristics do not have differential pre-intervention trends that could cause a spurious result. ART availability is not systematically correlated with observable economic shocks or measures of economic support, which minimizes the concern that ART proximity is incidentally correlated with economic determinants of work time. Estimates are robust if we control flexibly for these variables. Placebo tests for non-ART clinics, roads, trading centers, and schools show that the proximity of these features does not lead to similar results.
Next we explore the possible channels for this effect. Estimates for subjective mortality risk confirm that ART availability reduces perceived risk for people who are HIV-negative. We investigate the three primary ways through which ART availability may affect HIV−/NCT work time, including life-cycle optimization, general-equilibrium market effects, and mental health. While respondents may increase labor supply to accumulate savings, the lack of an impact on other farm activities suggests that this channel may not fully explain the response. ART availability has a large and significant effect on mental health, which suggests that this mechanism may be important.
These results lead to two broader points. Estimates suggest that ART, one of the most significant recent global health innovations, has a large risk-reduction benefit in Sub-Saharan Africa. This finding is not surprising given the prevalence and severity of HIV/AIDS. Health economists do not normally account for the risk-reduction benefit of medical innovation (Lakdawalla et al. 2014). Our results for work time and mental health direct document the large economic and welfare benefit of ART through the risk reduction channel.
Secondly, we show that the impact of the HIV/AIDS epidemic is broader than previously understood. Existing analyses of the economic consequences of HIV/AIDS focus on the health impact for HIV-positive people (Young 2005, Marinescu 2014). Studies of HIV-negative people mostly examine risky sexual behavior (DeWalque et al. 2007, Friedman 2014, Gong 2014), which is reasonable since this behavior determines transmission and HIV prevalence. By showing that HIV-negative people respond strongly to ART availability, we indirectly demonstrate that HIV/AIDS distorts behavior in important wants that microeconomic and macroeconomic analyses of HIV should acknowledge.
Finally, the estimates for mental health suggest an important potential relationship between the disease environment and economic development. Acemoglu and Johnson (2007) and Bleakley (2007) show that the disease environment affects development via physical health, but existing work does not explore the way that the disease environment may affect development via mental health. Extreme disease conditions like the HIV/AIDS epidemic create trauma, stress, and uncertainty that may hamper productivity by exacerbating poor mental health.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We received helpful suggestions from Kerwin Charles, Andrew Dorward, Erica Field, Willa Friedman, Giither Fink, Erick Gong, Kelsey Jack, Dean Karlan, Ofer Malamud, Sendhil Mullainathan, Emily Oster, Julian Reif, Rebecca Thornton, Alessandra Voena, Nicholas Wilson, and seminar participants at the University of Chicago, the University of California San Diego, and the University of Illinois at Chicago. IRBs at the University of Pennsylvania and the National Health and Science Research Committee in Malawi have approved collection of the MLSFH. The authors gratefully acknowledge the generous support for this research through National Institute of Child Health and Development (grant numbers R03HD058976, R21HD050652, R01HD044228, R01HD053781), the National Institute on Aging (grant number P30 AG12836), the Boettner Center for Pensions and Retirement Security at the University of Pennsylvania, and the National Institute of Child Health and Development Population Research Infrastructure Program (grant number R24 HD-044964).
Footnotes
Although researchers only established the impact of ART on transmission risk recently, scientists have long suspected this mechanism because ART dramatically reduces the viral load in the body. In addition, people who formulate risk perceptions according to observational heuristics may perceive an impact of ART on infection risk because they encounter fewer sick people.
Unless they happen to migrate to another MLSFH sample village, respondents who relocate closer to ART are recorded as attriters in our data set. As we discuss in Section A1, this mechanism is unlikely to confound our estimates because attrition is uncorrelated with ART proximity.
The survey began in 1998 with a survey of 1500 ever-married women aged 15 to 49 and their husbands. Surveyors selected women randomly from village-wide lists of households. The work time sample includes 1889 respondents who were present in at least one survey round and have non-missing values for the work time variables (N = 5380). Likewise, the mortality risk and mental health samples include 1764 respondents who were present in at least one round and have non-missing values for these variables (N = 3663).
Suppose that access (A) is inversely proportional to distance (D): A = k/D. An increase in distance has a larger effect on access nearby than far away because ∂A/∂D = −k/D2.
Adult HIV incidence is under 1 percent per year in the MLSFH population and throughout Malawi (Kohler et al. 2014, UNAIDS 2012). The 2008 test results should leave only a handful of undetected HIV cases by 2010.
Results for HIV-positive and caretaker respondents appear in Section A2. We do not focus on these groups because the sample sizes are small and other studies already examine the direct impacts of HIV and ART (Chandra et al. 1998, Els et al. 1999, Tostes et al. 2004, Thirumurthy et al. 2008, Thom 2009, Kuo et al. 2012, McLaren 2010, Thirumurthy et al. 2012, Okeke and Wagner 2013).
Other economic activities may include marketing work, handicraft production, carpentry, metal work, water collection for sale, charcoal or firewood preparation, alcohol production, transporting goods, and other cash activities.
The survey also measures the probability of HIV infection (ranging from 0 to 1) within one year for a hypothetical person with “normal sexual behavior.” Respondents perceive that a normal person has a 22 percent chance of contracting HIV within one year, which greatly exceeds Kohler et al.’s (2014) MLSFH incidence estimate of under 0.7 per 100 person years. Estimates using this variable (available from the authors) are equivalent to results for our primary HIV risk variable.
Figure A1 shows local polynomial regressions of actual and perceived HIV prevalence and perceived infection risk on the distance to a trading center in Balaka District. The non-zero slopes in the figure show that both actual and perceived prevalence, subjective mortality risk, and mental health are worse near trading centers. Equivalent figures for the distance to a road and school and figures that combine the three districts (available from the authors) show the same pattern.
In 2006, respondents had a median of two acquaintances with AIDS and had seen two other acquaintances die of AIDS in the past year. Figure A2 shows that respondent-observed AIDS mortality is strongly correlated with perceived infection risk despite being only weakly correlated with actual HIV prevalence. Perceived prevalence is 29 percent among respondents with no acquaintances who have recently died of AIDS, but is 38 percent among respondents with three or more dead acquaintances. These variables also predict subjective mortality risk and mental health. A similar figure for observed AIDS morbidity (available from the authors) shows the same pattern.
ART availability increases the maize yield differentially by 6 percent for nearby respondents (p = 0.02). Additional maize results are available from the authors. Because the data collection coincides with the maize harvest in June and July, these estimates do not address the potential work time response in other months. We cannot rule out that results arise through inter-seasonal labor substitution.
In Oster’s notation, X is the treatment, W1 is a vector of observable controls and W2 is a vector of unobservables. She defines δ so that . Therefore δ represents the strength of selection on unobservables relative to selection on observables. We assume conservatively for this exercise that all of the residual variation in work time may arise through unobservable selection.
One limitation the clinic proximity test is that ART and non-ART clinics also differ in other dimensions. A comparison of Columns 2 and 3 of Table A1 shows that ART clinics are more advanced than non-ART clinics, but that for the most part these facilities offer similar non-ART health services.
The plot for non-ART clinic distance in the upper left uses the subsample whose nearest clinic does not offer ART.
ART became available in designated facilities several months before the 2008 survey round. However 2010 effects may be stronger than 2008 effects due to greater exposure to treatment.
The average number of observed AIDS deaths per year grew from 2.0 to 3.7 from 2006 to 2010. This pattern concurs with an 88 percent increase in MLSFH attrition due to mortality over the period (Kohler et al. 2014). The average number of acquaintances with AIDS (which is not available in 2010) grew from 2.2 to 3.4 from 2006 to 2008. During this period, perceived HIV prevalence grew from 28 to 37 percent and the share of respondents who were “worried” about HIV/AIDS rose from 41 to 51 percent.
We also find no effect on land ownership below. However land ownership is not sensitive to economic shock and support variables, perhaps because land markets do not exist in this setting. The results in Table 9 are similar if we look only at respondents who work primarily in agriculture.
This econometric model is appropriate for log-normally distributed outcomes such as expenditures (Cameron and Trivedi 2010, p. 545). This specification omits individual fixed effects, which are incompatible with the Tobit model.
It is difficult to compare the impacts on own and hired labor because own labor is measured in hours per day and hired labor is measured as a household expenditure within the past three months. To assess the substitution between own and hired labor further, we use the Malawi Integrated Household Survey (IHS) to approximate wages in the sample districts and back out hired work time. Using our primary OLS specification, the impact of ART proximity on this measure of hired work time is −1.03, meaning that a reduction in hired labor offsets 20 percent of the increase in own total work time or 47 percent of the increase in own farm work time in Table 6.
This figure is likely to be an upper bound because HIV status is positively correlated within households and because some adult family members of HIV-positive people do not serve as caretakers.
Although studies show that HIV infection worsens mental health (Chandra et al. 1998, Els et al. 1999, Tostes et al. 2004, Thom 2009, Okeke and Wagner 2013), we are unaware of research on the mental health impact of HIV risk for people who are HIV-negative.
The impact of ART availability on work time and mental health is stronger for women than for men. Women may respond more because they face biologically higher HIV infection risk. HIV risk may also have a larger effect on female mental health because women have less control over their sexual behavior and HIV risk exposure. More detailed results by gender are available from the authors.
Taken at face value, this magnitude suggests that mental health cannot fully explain the impact of ART availability on work time in Table 6. However the MCS-12 score is a proxy variable with substantial measurement error, which may attenuate the estimates in Table 11. This issue does not interfere with Table 10 because the MCS-12 score is not a regressor.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Victoria Baranov, Email: victoria.baranov@unimelb.edu.au, University of Melbourne.
Daniel Bennett, Email: dmbennett@uchicago.edu, University of Chicago.
Hans-Peter Kohler, Email: hpkohler@pop.upenn.edu, University of Pennsylvania.
References
- Acemoglu Daron, Johnson Simon. Disease and Development: the Effect of Life Expectancy on Economic Growth. Journal of Political Economy. 2007;115(6):925–985. [Google Scholar]
- Adhvaryu Achyuta, Fenske James, Nyshadham Anant. Early Life Circumstance and Adult Mental Health. 2014 Feb; Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Alderman Harold, Behrman Jere R, Kohler Hans-Peter, Maluccio John, Watkins Susan C. Attrition in Longitudinal Household Survey Data: Some Tests for Three Developing Country Samples. Demographic Research. 2001;5(4):79–123. [Google Scholar]
- Altonji Joseph, Elder Todd, Taber Christopher. Selection on Observed and Unobserved Variables: Assessing the Effectiveness of Catholic Schools. Journal of Political Economy. 2005;113(1):151–184. [Google Scholar]
- Anglewicz Philip, Kohler Hans-Peter. Overestimating HIV Infection: the Construction and Accuracy of Subjective Probabilities of HIV Infection in Rural Malawi. Demographic Research. 2009;20:65–96. doi: 10.4054/DemRes.2009.20.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anglewicz Philip, Adams Jimi, Obare Francis, Kohler Hans-Peter, Watkins Susan. The Malawi Diffusion and Ideational Change Project 2004–06: Data Collection, Data Quality, and Analysis of Attrition. Demographic Research. 2009 May;20(21):503–540. doi: 10.4054/demres.2009.20.21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apouey Benedicte, Clark Andrew. Winning Big But Feeling No Better? The Effect of Lottery Prizes on Physical and Mental Health. Health Economics. 2014 doi: 10.1002/hec.3035. Forth-coming. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- AVERT. HIV and AIDS in Malawi. 2012 Mar; Information from http://www.avert.org/aids-malawi.htm.
- Baird Sarah, Gong Erick, Mcintosh Craig, Ozler Berk. The heterogeneous effects of HIV testing. Journal of Health Economics. 2014 Sep;37:98–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2014.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baird Sarah, de Hoop Jacobus, Ozler Berk. Income Shocks and Adolescent Mental Health. Journal of Human Resources. 2013;48(2):370–403. [Google Scholar]
- Balsa Ana, Homer Jenny, French Michael. The Health Effects of Parental Problem Drinking on Adult Children. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2009;12(2):55–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baranov Victoria, Kohler Hans-Peter. The Impact of AIDS Treatment on Savings and Human Capital Investment in Malawi. 2014 Apr; Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Behrman Jere, Foster Andrew, Rosenzweig Mark. The Dynamics of Agricultural Production and the Calorie-Income Relationship: Evidence from Pakistan. Journal of Econometrics. 1997;77:187–207. [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Porath Yoram. The Production of Human Capital and the Life Cycle of Earnings. Journal of Political Economy. 1967;75(4):352–65. [Google Scholar]
- Berndt Ernst, Finkelstein Stan, Greenberg Paul, Howland Robert, Keith Alison, Rush John, Russell James, Keller Martin. Workplace Performance Effects from Chronic Depression and Its Treatment. Journal of Health Economics. 1998;17:511–535. doi: 10.1016/s0167-6296(97)00043-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bleakley Hoyt. Disease and Development: Evidence from Hookworm Eradication in the American South. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2007 Feb;122(1):73–117. doi: 10.1162/qjec.121.1.73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloom David E, Canning David, Graham Bryan. Longevity and Life-cycle Savings. Scandinavian Journal of Economics. 2003;105(3):319–338. [Google Scholar]
- Bongaarts John P, Pelletier Francois, Gerland Patrick. Global Trends in AIDS Mortality. In: Rogers Richard G, Crimmins Eileen M., editors. International Handbooks of Population. Vol. 2. New York: Springer; 2011. pp. 171–183. [Google Scholar]
- Bor Jacob, Herbst Abraham J, Newell Marie-Louise, Bnighausen Till. Increases in Adult Life Expectancy in Rural South Africa: Valuing the Scale-Up of HIV Treatment. Science. 2013;339(6122):961–965. doi: 10.1126/science.1230413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron A. Colin, Trivedi Pravin. Microeconometrics Using Stata. revised ed. College Station, Texas: StataCorp LP; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Cervellati Matteo, Sunde Uwe. Life Expectancy, Schooling, and Lifetime Labor Supply: Theory and Evidence Revisted. Econometrica. 2013;81(5):2055–2086. [Google Scholar]
- Conroy Amy, Yeatman Sara, Dovel Kathryn. The Social Construction of AIDS During a Time of Evolving Access to Antiretroviral Therapy in Rural Malawi. Culture, Health, and Sexuality. 2013;15(8):924–937. doi: 10.1080/13691058.2013.791057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dave Dhaval, Rashad Inas, Spasojevic Jasmina. The Effects of Retirement on Physical and Mental Health Outcomes. Southern Economic Journal. 2008 Oct;75(2):497–523. [Google Scholar]
- Delavande Adeline, Kohler Hans-Peter. Subjective Expectations in the Context of HIV/AIDS in the Malawi. Demographic Research. 2009 Jun;20(31):817–874. doi: 10.4054/DemRes.2009.20.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delavande Adeline, Gine Xavier, McKenzie David. Eliciting Probabilistic Expectations in Developing Countries: How Sensitive are Answers to Variations in Elicitation Design? Journal of Applied Econometrics. 2011;26:479–497. [Google Scholar]
- Delavande Adeline, Gine Xavier, McKenzie David. Measuring Subjective Expectations in Developing Countries: a Critical Review and New Evidence. Journal of Development Economics. 2011;94:151–163. [Google Scholar]
- DeWalque Damien, Kazianga Harounan, Ove Mead. Antiretroviral Therapy Awareness and Risky Sexual Behaviors: Evidence from Mozambique. 2007 World Bank Policy Research Working Paper 5486. [Google Scholar]
- Dorward Andrew, Chirwa Ephraim, Jayne TS. The Malawi Agricultural Input Subsidy Programme: 2005–6 to 2008–9. International Journal of Agricultural SustamaUlity. 2011;9(1):232–247. [Google Scholar]
- Els Charl, Boshoff Weihann, Scott Chris, Strydom Willem, Joubert Gina, van der Ryst Elna. Psychiatric Co-morbidity in South African HIV/AIDS Patients. South African Medical Journal. 1999 Sep;89(9):992–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ettner Susan L, Frank Richard G, Kessler Ronald C. The Impact of Psychiatric Disorders on Labor Market Outcomes. Industrial and Labor Relations Review. 1997 Oct;51(3):64–81. [Google Scholar]
- Feldacker Caryl, Ennett Susan, Speizer Ilene. It’s not just who you are but where you live: An exploration of community influences on individual HIV status in rural Malawi. Social Science and Medicine. 2011;72:717–725. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fink Gunther, Masiye Felix. Health Investment and Agricultural Productivity: Evidence from a Cluster-randomized Controlled Trial Katete District. Zambia: 2012. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher Jason. Adolescent Depression and Adult Labor Market Outcomes. Southern Economic Journal. 2013 Jul;80(1):26–49. [Google Scholar]
- Fortin Bernard, Jacquemet Nicolas, Shearer Bruce. Labour Supply, Work Effort and Contract Choice: Theory and Evidence on Physicians. 2010 Sep; IZA Discussion Paper No. 5188. [Google Scholar]
- Fortson Jane. Mortality Risk and Human Capital Investment: the Impact of HIV/AIDS in Sub-Saharan Africa. Review of Economics and Statistics. 2011;93(1):1–15. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman Willa. Antiretroviral Drug Access and Behavior Change. 2014 Jan; Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Gill Sarah, Butterworth Peter, Rodgers Bryan, Mackinnon Andrew. Validity of the mental health component of the 12-item Short-Form Health Survey (MCS-12) as a measure of common mental disorders in the general population. Psychiatry Research. 2007;152:63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gong Erick. HIV Testing and Risky Sexual Behaviour. Economic Journal. 2014 Forth-coming. [Google Scholar]
- Government of Malawi Ministry of Health. Treatment of AIDS: Guidelines for the Use of Antiretroviral Therapy in Malawi. (3rd Edition) 2008 Apr; Available from www.who.int.
- Government of Malawi Ministry of Health. Quarterly HIV Programme Report: October–December 2010. 2010 Dec; Available from www.hivunitmohmw.org.
- Guenther Tanya, Sadruddin Salim, Chimuna Tiyese, Sichamba Bias, Yeboah-Antwi Kojo, Diakite Bamody, Modibo Bamadio, Swedberg Eric, March David. Beyond Distance: An Approach to Measure Effective Access to Case Management for Sick Children in Africa. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene. 2012 Nov;57(5 Suppl):77–84. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.2012.11-0747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton Vivian, Merrigan Philip, Dufresne Eric. Down and Out: Estimating the Relationship Between Mental Health and Unemployment. Health Economics. 1997 Jul;6(4):397–406. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1050(199707)6:4<397::aid-hec283>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hazan Moshe. Longevity and Lifetime Labor Supply: Evidence and Implications. Econometnca. 2009;77(6):1829–1863. [Google Scholar]
- Herbst Abraham J, Cooke Graham S, Barnighausen Till, KanyKany Angelique, Tanser Frank, Newell Marie-Louise. Adult mortality and antiretroviral treatment roll-out in rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2009;57(10):754–762. doi: 10.2471/BLT.08.058982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jahn Andreas, Floyd Sian, Cramin Amelia C, Mwaungulu Frank, Mvula Hazzie, Fipson Munthali, McGrath Nuala, Mwafilaso Johnbosco, Mwinuka Venance, Mangongo Bernard, Fine Paul EM, Zaba Basia, Glynn Judith R. Population-Level Effect of HIV on Adult Mortality and Early Evidence of Reversal After Introduction of Antiretroviral Therapy in Malawi. Lancet. 2008 May;371(9624):1603–1611. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60693-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jayachandran Seeema, Lleras-Muney Adriana. Life Expectancy and Human Capital Investments: Evidence from Maternal Mortality Declines. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2009 Feb;124(1):349–397. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler R, Andrews G, Colpe L, Hiripi E, Mroczek D, Normand S-L, Walters E, Zaslavsky M. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in nonspecific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine. 2002 Aug;32(6):959–976. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler Ronald, Frank Richard. The Impact of Psychiatric Disorders on Work Loss Days. Psychological Medicine. 1997 Jul;27(4):861–73. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797004807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler Hans-Peter, Watkins Susan, Behrman Jere, Anglewicz Philip, Kohler Iliana, Thorton Rebecca, Mkandawire James, Honde Hastings, Hawara Augustine, Chilima Ben, Bandawe Chiwoza, Mwapasa Victor. Cohort Profile: the Malawi Longitudinal Study of Families and Health (MLSFH) Forth-coming: International Journal of Epidemiology. 2014 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo Christine, Operario Don, Cluver Lucie. Depression Among Carers of AIDS-Orphaned and Other-Orphaned Children in Umlazi Township, South Africa. Global Public Health. 2012;7(3):253–269. doi: 10.1080/17441692.2011.626436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakdawalla Darius, Malani Anup, Reif Julian. The Insurance Value of Medical Innovation. 2014 Nov; Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Larson Celia. Use of the SF-12 Instrument for Measuring the Health of Homeless Persons. Health Services Research. 2002 Jun;37(3):733–50. doi: 10.1111/1475-6773.00046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latif Ehsan. The Impact of Retirement on Mental Health in Canada. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2013 Mar;16(1):35–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Libamba Edwin, Makombe Simon, Mhango Eustice, de Ascurra Teck Olga, Limbambala Eddie, Schouten Erik, Harries Anthony. Supervision, Monitoring, and Evaluation of Nationwide Scale-up of Antretroviral Therapy in Malawi. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2006 Apr;84(4):320–326. doi: 10.2471/blt.05.023952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim Debbie, Sanderson Kristy, Andrews Gavin. Lost Productivity Among Full-Time Workers with Mental Disorders. The Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2000;3(3):139–146. doi: 10.1002/mhp.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louriero Maria. Farmers’ Health and Agricultural Productivity. Agricultural Economics. 2009;40:381–388. [Google Scholar]
- Mandal Bidisha, Roe Brian, Loss Job. Retirement, and the Mental Health of Older Americans. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2008 Dec;11(4):167–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcotte David, Wilcox-Gok Virginia. Estimating Earnings Losses due to Mental Illness: a Quantile Regression Approach. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2003 Oct;6(3):123–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marinescu Ioana. HIV, Wages, and the Skill Premium. Journal of Health Economics. 2014 Sep;37:181–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2014.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matchaya Greenwell. Land Ownership Security in Malawi. African Journal of Agricultural Research. 2009 Jan;4(1):1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Mclnerney Melissa, Mellor Jennifer, Nicholas Lauren Hersch. Recession Depression: Mental Health Effects of the 2008 Stock Market Crash. Journal of Health Economics. 2013 Dec;32(6):1090–1104. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaren Zoe. The Effect of Access to AIDS Treatment on Employment Outcomes in South Africa. 2010 [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery Mark R. Perceiving Mortality Decline. Population and Development Review. 2000 Dec;26(4):795–819. [Google Scholar]
- NIAID. Treating HIV-infected People with Antiretrovirals Protects Partners from Infection: Findings Result from NIH-funded International Study. 2011 [Google Scholar]
- Obare Francis, Fleming Peter, Anglewicz Philip, Thornton Rebecca, Martinson F, Kapatuka Agatha, Poulin Michell, Watkins Susan C, Kohler Hans-Peter. Acceptance of Repeat Population-based Voluntary Counseling and Testing for HIV in Rural Malawi. Sexually Transmitted Infections. 2009;85(2):139–144. doi: 10.1136/sti.2008.030320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okeke Edward, Wagner Glenn. AIDS Treatment and Mental Health: Evidence from Uganda. Social Science and Medicine. 2013 Sep;92:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.05.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oster Emily. Unobservable Selection and Coefficient Stability: Theory and Evidence. 2014 Feb; Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Patel Vikram, Kleinman Arthur. Poverty and Common Mental Disorders in Developing Countries. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2003;81(8):609–615. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philipson Tomas, Jena Anupam. Who Benefits from New Medical Technologies? Estimates of Consumer and Producer Surpluses for HIV/AIDS Drugs. Forum for Health Economics and Policy. 2006;9(2):1–31. [Google Scholar]
- Pinto Andrew, van Lettow Monique, Rachlis Beth, Chan Adrienne, Sodhi Sumeet. Patient Costs Associated with Accessing HIV/AIDS Care in Malawi. Journal of the International AIDS Society. 2013;16:1–6. doi: 10.7448/IAS.16.1.18055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravi V. Chandra Prabha, Desai A, Subbakrishna DK. Anxiety and Depression Among HIV-infected Heterosexuals - a Report from India. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1998;45(5):401–409. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(98)00028-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith Kumi, Powers Kimberly A, Kashuba Angela DM, Cohen Myron S. HIV-1 treatment as prevention: The good, the bad, and the challenges. Current Opinion in HIV and AIDS. 2011;6(4):315–325. doi: 10.1097/COH.0b013e32834788e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanser Frank, Bnighausen Till, Grapsa Erofili, Zaidi Jaffer, Newell Marie-Louise. High Coverage of ART Associated with Decline in Risk of HIV Acquisition in Rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Science. 2013;339(6122):966–971. doi: 10.1126/science.1228160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tefft Nathan. Insights on Unemployment, Unemployment Insurance, and Mental Health. Journal of Health Economics. 2011 Mar;30(2):258–265. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2011.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thirumurthy Harsha, Zivin Joshua Graff, Goldstein Markus. The Economic Impact of AIDS Treatment: Labor Supply in Western Kenya. Journal of Human Resources. 2008;43:511–552. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thirumurthy Harsha, Goldstein Markus, Zivin Joshua Graff, Habyarimana James, Pop-Eleches Cristian. Behavioral Responses of Patients in AIDS Treatment Programs: Sexual Behavior in Kenya. Forum for Health Economics and Policy. 2012;15(2):1–29. doi: 10.1515/1558-9544.1230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thom Rita. Common Mental Disorders in People Living with HIV. The South African Journal of HIV Medicine. 2009 Oct;10(3) [Google Scholar]
- Thomas Duncan, Witoelar Firman, Frankenberg Elizabeth, Sikoki Bondan, Strauss John, Sumantri Cecep, Suriastini Wayan. Cutting the costs of attrition: Results from the Indonesia Family Life Survey. Journal of Development Economics. 2012;98(1):108–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jdeveco.2010.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tostes MA, Chalub M, Botega NJ. The Quality of Life of HIV-infected Women is Associated with Psychiatric Morbidity. AIDS Care. 2004 Feb;16(2):177–186. doi: 10.1080/09540120410001641020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- UN Population Division. World Population Prospects, the 2010 Revision: Standard (Median) Forecasts. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- UNAIDS. Global report: UN AIDS report on the global AIDS epidemic 2010. New York: World Health Organization and UNAIDS; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- UNAIDS. Global report: UNAIDS report on the global AIDS epidemic 2012. New York: World Health Organization and UNAIDS; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Ware John, Kosinki Mark, Keller Susan. A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey: Construction of Scales and Preliminary Tests of Reliability and Validity. Medical Care. 1996 Mar;34(3):220–233. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199603000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins Susan, Behrman Jere R, Kohler Hans-Peter, Zulu Eliya M. Introduction to Research on Demographic Aspects of HIV/AIDS in Rural Africa. Demographic Research. 2003;(1):1–30. Special Collection 1. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson Nicholas, Xiong Wentao, Mattson Christine. Is Sex Like Driving? HIV Prevention and Risk Compensation. Journal of Development Economics. 2014 Jan;106:78–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jdeveco.2013.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Life Tables for WHO Member States, 1990, 2000, 2010 -by Country and Region, in World Health Statistics 2010. 2010 May [Google Scholar]
- Yin Wesley, Penrod John, Maclean Ross, Lakdawalla Darius, Philipson Tomas. Value of Survival Gains in Chronic Myeloid Leukemia. American Journal of Managed Care. 2012 Nov;18(Supplement 11):S257–S264. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young Alwyn. The Gift of the Dying: the Tragedy of AIDS and the Welfare of Future African Generations. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2005 May;120(2):423–466. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.