Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Eur J Neurol. 2015 Jun 23;23(2):313–319. doi: 10.1111/ene.12761

Regional β-amyloid burden does not correlate with cognitive or language deficits in Alzheimer’s disease presenting as aphasia

Youngsin Jung a, Jennifer L Whitwell b, Joseph R Duffy a, Edythe A Strand a, Mary M Machulda c, Matthew L Senjem d, Clifford R Jack Jr b, Val J Lowe e, Keith A Josephs a
PMCID: PMC4689664  NIHMSID: NIHMS687701  PMID: 26101072

Abstract

Background

A subset of patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) present with early and prominent language impairment (aphasic AD). We previously reported an association between global β-amyloid burden measured on [11C] Pittsburgh compound B PET (PiB) and general cognitive impairment, but not with aphasia, in such subjects. As a follow up, we assess whether there is any association between regional β-amyloid burden, atrophy on MRI, and global cognitive impairment, aphasia, or other cognitive and functional impairment in aphasic AD.

Methods

Forty-four aphasic AD subjects that underwent PiB scanning and volumetric MRI and were determined to be positive for β-amyloid deposition were analyzed. All had completed detailed neurological, neuropsychological and language batteries. Spearman’s rank-order correlation was utilized to assess for associations.

Results

Greater visuospatial impairment was associated with increased β-amyloid burden in the primary visual cortex (p = 0.001). Although there were many trends for associations between neurocognitive and language deficits and regional β-amyloid burden, there were no strong associations that survived correction for multiple comparisons. However, neurocognitive and language impairment in these subjects strongly correlated with the degree of left lateral temporal and inferior parietal atrophy (p < 0.004).

Conclusions

The findings from this study suggest a close relation between the severity of regional atrophy and cognitive and language impairment, but argue against a strong association between regional β-amyloid burden and such deficits in aphasic AD subjects. Hence, other pathological factors may be driving the previously identified association between global β-amyloid deposition and general cognitive impairment in aphasic AD.

Keywords: Dementia, Aphasia, PET, Beta-amyloid, PiB

Introduction

Patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) most often present with loss of episodic memory, and are referred to as having amnestic AD. However, early and prominent language deficits can be the presenting symptoms of a subset of AD patients [1]. We refer to such patients as having aphasic AD to differentiate them from patients with amnestic AD. Beta-amyloid deposition is a hallmark pathological feature of AD, which occurs in a stereotypic fashion over several distinctive phases in patients with amnestic AD [2]. Similar patterns of global amyloid accumulation have been observed in patients with aphasic AD [35]. In patients with amnestic AD, higher amyloid burden has been shown to inversely correlate with cognitive function [6]. Interestingly, β-amyloid deposition may also affect cognition in AD patients presenting with aphasia. We previously found an association between global β-amyloid burden and general cognitive impairment in aphasic AD subjects [7, 8]. However, it is unclear whether this association may have been driven by other factors. We argue that if our previously identified association was genuinely related to β-amyloid then there would also be strong associations between general cognitive impairment and β-amyloid burden for specific regions. Here, we examined the relationships between regional β-amyloid burden on [11C] Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) PET, atrophy on volumetric MRI, and performances on various neurological, neuropsychological, and language metrics in a large cohort of AD patients who presented with early and prominent language impairment.

Methods

Forty-four subjects who presented to Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN with prominent language deficits without preceding memory complaints that impaired activities of daily living and had β-amyloid deposition on [11C] PiB-PET were prospectively enrolled in this study. All subjects had aphasia characterized by anomia, poor sentence repetition, and phonological errors without loss of single word comprehension or apraxia of speech. All subjects met the criteria for the logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA) [9, 10]. Each subject underwent a neurological evaluation that included the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [11], the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) [12], the Frontal Behavior Inventory (FBI) [13], and the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) [14] by a behavioral neurologist (KAJ). In addition, a language evaluation that included the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) [15], the Token Test [16], and the Boston Naming Test (BNT) [17] was performed by one of two speech pathologists (JRD & EAS). Each participant also had a neuropsychometric evaluation that included the Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) [18], the Sorting Test from the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS) [19], and the Incomplete Letters and Cube Analysis subtests from the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP) [20]. By design, all subjects had positive PiB-PET determined by a global PiB ratio cut-point of 1.5, as previously described [21]. Seven areas of interest, including Broca’s area, insular, prefrontal, parietal, medial and lateral temporal, and visual cortex, were selected. Regional PiB uptake ratios were calculated for each region as previously described [8]. Briefly, PiB-PET images were co-registered to 3D magnetisation prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) using 6 degrees-of-freedom affine registration. The automated anatomical labelling (AAL) atlas was used to calculate median PiB uptake for the regions of interest. The uptake ratios were calculated by dividing median PiB uptake in each region by median uptake in cerebellar gray matter. In addition, left lateral temporal, inferior parietal, and hippocampal atrophy on MRI was calculated as previously described [22]. The association between regional PiB uptake ratio, atrophy on MRI, and performances on neurological, neuropsychological, and language metrics was examined using Spearman’s rank-order correlation. Statistical analyses were performed using JMP software, version 9.0 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC). In order to correct for multiple comparisons, Bonferroni correction was applied to each set of comparisons among the 12 regional variables and individual neurological, neuropsychological, and language measures (Tables 35) and vice versa (Table 6) with a desired α of 0.05. Therefore, a p value < 0.004 was considered statistically significant for our regional analyses.

Table 3.

Correlations between regional amyloid burden and neurological scores

ρ value (p value)

MOCA FAB FBI CDR-SB
L prefrontal −0.30 (0.04) −0.31 (0.04) −0.03 (0.85) 0.32 (0.04)
L medial temporal −0.11 (0.47) −0.10 (0.51) −0.09 (0.57) 0.08 (0.44)
L lateral temporal −0.27 (0.08) −0.23 (0.13) −0.17 (0.28) 0.24 (0.11)
L parietal −0.21 (0.18) −0.20 (0.20) −0.14 (0.35) 0.23 (0.13)
L insula −0.21 (0.18) −0.16 (0.31) −0.25 (0.10) 0.18 (0.26)
Broca’s −0.19 (0.21) −0.11 (0.47) −0.21 (0.18) 0.16 (0.31)
Primary visual −0.40* (0.01) −0.27 (0.08) −0.17 (0.28) 0.29 (0.06)
R prefrontal −0.37* (0.01) −0.36 (0.02) −0.02 (0.92) 0.32 (0.04)
R medial temporal −0.29 (0.06) −0.30 (0.05) −0.14 (0.38) 0.19 (0.22)
R lateral temporal −0.41* (0.01) −0.37* (0.01) −0.10 (0.51) 0.33 (0.17)
R parietal −0.37* (0.01) −0.31 (0.04) −0.04 (0.81) 0.33 (0.03)
R insula −0.29 (0.05) −0.28 (0.06) −0.15 (0.34) 0.20 (0.21)
*

Trend for significance (p ≤ 0.01)

Abbreviations: L = left; R = right; MOCA = the Montreal Cognitive Assessment; FAB = the Frontal Assessment Battery; FBI = the Frontal Behavior Inventory; CDR-SB = Clinical Dementia Rating Sum of Boxes

Table 5.

Correlations between regional amyloid burden and language scores

ρ value (p value)

WAB AQ WAB repetition Token test BNT
L prefrontal −0.33 (0.03) −0.24 (0.12) −0.37* (0.01) −0.27 (0.08)
L medial temporal −0.07 (0.64) −0.03 (0.87) −0.16 (0.29) −0.06 (0.69)
L lateral temporal −0.24 (0.12) −0.13 (0.40) −0.25 (0.11) −0.28 (0.07)
L parietal −0.20 (0.18) −0.17 (0.26) −0.23 (0.14) −0.20 (0.18)
L insula −0.33 (0.03) −0.29 (0.05) −0.28 (0.06) −0.21 (0.17)
Broca’s −0.24 (0.11) −0.25 (0.11) −0.22 (0.15) −0.22 (0.15)
Primary visual −0.05 (0.74) 0.03 (0.87) −0.10 (0.51) −0.16 (0.31)
R prefrontal −0.39* (0.01) −0.30 (0.04) −0.39* (0.01) −0.31 (0.04)
R medial temporal −0.23 (0.13) −0.19 (0.21) −0.22 (0.16) −0.18 (0.24)
R lateral temporal −0.36 (0.02) −0.24 (0.12) −0.35 (0.02) −0.32 (0.04)
R parietal −0.33 (0.03) −0.26 (0.09) −0.34 (0.03) −0.29 (0.06)
R insula −0.32 (0.04) −0.31 (0.04) −0.25 (0.11) −0.18 (0.23)
*

Trend for significance (p ≤ 0.01)

Abbreviations: L = left; R = right; WAB = Western Aphasia Battery; AQ = aphasia quotient; BNT = the Boston Naming Test

Table 6.

Correlations between regional atrophy and neurocognitive and language measures

MRI volume (median [IQR])

L hippocampus L lateral temporal L inferior parietal
0.26 (0.23 – 0.27) 1.83 (1.65 – 1.96) 0.77 (0.69 – 0.84)

Correlation (ρ value [p value])

L hippocampus L lateral temporal L inferior parietal
MOCA 0.11 (0.47) 0.46** (0.002) 0.60** (<0.0001)
FAB 0.18 (0.24) 0.45** (0.002) 0.52** (0.0001)
FBI 0.22 (0.15) 0.08 (0.59) −0.06 (0.68)
CDR-SB −0.11 (0.47) −0.26 (0.09) −0.38 (0.03)
AVLT trial 1 −0.17 (0.28) 0.01 (0.95) 0.04 (0.79)
AVLT LT % retention 0.24 (0.13) 0.38* (0.01) 0.38* (0.01)
DKEFS sorting −0.09 (0.57) 0.13 (0.44) 0.26 (0.11)
VOSP cube 0.24 (0.12) 0.42* (0.01) 0.37* (0.01)
WAB AQ 0.19 (0.23) 0.44** (0.003) 0.45** (0.002)
WAB repetition −0.12 (0.43) 0.26 (0.09) 0.33 (0.03)
Token test 0.13 (0.41) 0.44** (0.003) 0.47** (0.001)
BNT 0.23 (0.13) 0.29 (0.05) 0.26 (0.08)
*

Trend for significance (p ≤ 0.01)

**

Statistically significant (p < 0.004)

Abbreviations: L = left; R = right; MOCA = the Montreal Cognitive Assessment; FAB = the Frontal Assessment Battery; FBI = the Frontal Behavior Inventory; CDR-SB = Clinical Dementia Rating Sum of Boxes; AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Test; MOANS = Mayo Clinic’s Older Americans Normative Studies; LT = long term delayed recall; DKEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; VOSP = the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery; WAB = Western Aphasia Battery; AQ = aphasia quotient; BNT = the Boston Naming Test

This study was approved by the Mayo Clinic IRB. Informed consent was obtained from the patients and their family members or significant others.

Results

The subjects included 22 men and 22 women. The demographic characteristics of the subjects are shown in Table 1. Of the 44 subjects, two were left-hand dominant, and one was ambidextrous. The median age of the participants was 68 (interquartile range [IQR], 59 – 73). The median age at disease onset was 64 (IQR, 55 – 70). The median disease duration at the time of evaluation was 3.5 years (IQR, 1.5 – 5). The median years of education were 16 (IQR, 12.5 – 18). The severity of cognitive impairment and aphasia measured by neurological, neuropsychological, and language metrics were variable in these subjects (Table 1).

Table 1.

Demographic, neurological, language, and neuropsychological characteristics

Demographics Median (interquartile range)
Gender (M/F) 22/22
Handedness (R/L/ambidextrous) 41/2/1
Age, y 68 (59 – 73)
Age at disease onset, y 64 (55 – 70)
Disease duration, y 3.5 (1.5 – 5)
Education, y 16 (12.5 – 18)

Neurological evaluation Median (interquartile range)

MOCA 16 (9 – 20.8)
FAB 12 (7.3 – 14)
FBI 10.5 (7 – 19.8)
CDR-SB 3.5 (1.5 – 6)

Neuropsychological evaluation

AVLT trial 1 MOANS 5 (2 – 7.5)
AVLT LT % retention MOANS 6 (3 – 10)
DKEFS sorting scaled score 7 (4 – 9)
VOSP cube raw score 9 (5 – 10)

Language evaluation

WAB AQ 83 (70.6 – 88.1)
WAB repetition 7.8 (7 – 8.6)
Token test 11 (5.3 – 14)
BNT 7 (3 – 11)

Abbreviations: MOCA = the Montreal Cognitive Assessment; FAB = the Frontal Assessment Battery; FBI = the Frontal Behavior Inventory; CDR-SB = Clinical Dementia Rating Sum of Boxes; AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Test; MOANS = Mayo Clinic’s Older Americans Normative Studies; LT = long term delayed recall; DKEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; VOSP = the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery; WAB = Western Aphasia Battery; AQ = aphasia quotient; BNT = the Boston Naming Test

The seven cortical regions of interest, including Broca’s area, insular, prefrontal, parietal, medial and lateral temporal, and visual cortex, displayed varying degrees of β-amyloid accumulation (Table 2). Beta-amyloid burdens were similar between the left and right hemisphere. Highest β-amyloid burdens were observed in the prefrontal and parietal cortex followed by the lateral temporal cortex, Broca’s area, insula, and primary visual cortex. The lowest β-amyloid deposition was observed in the medial temporal cortex. Global and regional amyloid burdens were positively correlated (p<0.004, data not shown). In addition, positive associations were seen between regional amyloid burdens in most areas of interest (p<0.004, data not shown) with exception to the association between medial temporal and primary visual cortex amyloid burden.

Table 2.

Regional PiB distribution

Area PiB uptake ratio
Median (interquartile range)
Left prefrontal 2.18 (2.06 – 2.41)
Left medial temporal 1.39 (1.29 – 1.44)
Left lateral temporal 2.08 (1.92 – 2.27)
Left parietal 2.19 (1.99 – 2.31)
Left insula 1.97 (1.85 – 2.09)
Broca’s 2.01 (1.87 – 2.15)
Primary visual cortex 1.76 (1.55 – 2.07)
Right prefrontal 2.18 (2.00 – 2.36)
Right medial temporal 1.38 (1.31 – 1.46)
Right lateral temporal 1.98 (1.86 – 2.21)
Right parietal 2.12 (1.99 – 2.31)
Right insula 1.90 (1.77 – 2.04)

Correlations between regional β-amyloid burden and performances on neurological, neuropsychological, and language assessments are shown in Tables 35. Global cognitive function as measured by the MoCA showed trends for associations with β-amyloid deposition in multiple cortical regions, including the right prefrontal, right lateral temporal, right parietal, and primary visual cortex. A similar trend was noted for scores on the FAB, a measure of executive function, and β-amyloid burden in the right lateral temporal cortex. However, none of the associations reached significance, including other associations between FBI and CDR and any regional β-amyloid burden in our regions of interest.

The only strong association observed on the neuropsychological measures was between visuospatial function measured by the Cube Analysis subtest of the VOSP battery and β-amyloid burden in the primary visual cortex. Short-term and long-term memory impairment measured by the AVLT and concept formation and problem-solving skills measured by DKEFS Sorting were not associated with regional β-amyloid deposition.

With our language measures, there was a trend for an association between overall aphasia severity measured by the WAB and β-amyloid burden in the right prefrontal region. In addition, aphasia severity and deficits in receptive language and auditory processing measured by the Token Test showed a trend for an inverse correlation with β-amyloid burden in the bilateral prefrontal cortex. However, none of these associations reached significance. Impaired repetition and confrontation naming measured by the WAB repetition subscores and the BNT, respectively, showed no trend for any associations with regional β-amyloid burden.

On the other hand, left lateral temporal and inferior parietal atrophy on MRI strongly correlated with impairment in global cognitive function (i.e. MoCA) and executive function (i.e. FAB), as well as aphasia severity (i.e. WAB, Token Test) (Table 6). None of these measures had significant associations with left hippocampal atrophy.

Conclusions

This study demonstrated associations between atrophy in the left lateral temporal and inferior parietal cortex, regions that are known to be affected in aphasic AD [1, 22, 23], and measures of general cognitive and language impairment, but failed to identify any convincing evidence for associations between regional β-amyloid burden and such deficits in aphasic AD subjects except for a biologically plausible association between visuospatial impairment and increased β-amyloid burden in the primary visual cortex.

There were trends for an association between degree of impairment in general cognitive function and β-amyloid burden in several cortical regions, including the prefrontal, temporal, parietal, and primary visual cortex. However, none of these associations were strong, and hence none survived our correction for multiple comparisons. The findings have implications on our previous study that showed increased global β-amyloid burden was associated with worse cognition in aphasic AD subjects [7]. A reasonable deduction, given the lack of robust findings, is that factors other than β-amyloid are the driving force behind cognitive impairment in aphasic AD subjects. Furthermore, it would not be unreasonable to consider the possibility that these other factors may have confounded our previous results. On the other hand, taking into account the findings from both studies, including the many trends identified in this study, it still remains possible that β-amyloid could partly be contributing to cognitive impairment in aphasic AD subjects. Hence, we are unable to determine whether β-amyloid is playing a definitive role in the cognitive impairment that occurs in aphasic AD subjects.

Short-term and long-term memory measures did not correlate with β-amyloid accumulation in the seven cortical areas of interest. Similarly, the overall severity of aphasia, the most prominent symptom observed in these subjects, showed a trend for an association with prefrontal β-amyloid burden without reaching significance. This is not a particularly expected association, and could have been observed by chance given the many comparisons performed. Previously, the Token test has been shown to be associated with involvement of a number of left hemisphere regions, including Broca’s area, but not the prefrontal cortex or right hemisphere regions [24]. More specific aphasia related deficits, such as impaired repetition and naming, also did not correlate with increased regional β-amyloid burden, although this was not surprising since our previous study failed to demonstrate any association between global β-amyloid burden and these deficits [7].

The confounding effects of other proteins, such as tau and TDP-43, could not be examined in this study. Several studies have shown regional discrepancy between β-amyloid deposition, cortical atrophy, and hypometabolism in AD [2527]. Similarly, we did not find clinical correlations with increased β-amyloid deposition in the parietal cortex, an area associated with atrophy and hypometabolism in lvPPA [1, 22, 28]. The accumulation of β-amyloid is thought to occur in early stages of AD [29]. Therefore, it could be that β-amyloid is deposited early before the onset of the aphasia and the cognitive deficits in aphasic AD subjects. It is possible that β-amyloid plaques then initiate downstream pathways involving these other pathologic proteins that then directly cause further insults leading to the aphasia and the later general cognitive impairment. In fact, tau pathology was shown to correlate with the degree of gray matter atrophy in AD [30], and recently TDP-43 was shown to correlate with measures of memory loss and measures of language, such as naming, as previously discussed [31, 32]. Therefore, downstream effects of β-amyloid deposition cannot be ignored in aphasic AD subjects, and serial followup and correlation with future disease progression may be helpful to inform us in this regard.

The relationship between β-amyloid and clinical phenotypes of AD remains unclear. Diffuse β-amyloid deposition has been seen in AD regardless of its phenotypes, including amnestic, aphasic, and visuospatial/perceptual AD [35]. It is also unclear whether the role of β-amyloid in aphasic AD subjects is different from its role in amnestic AD subjects. The results of this study, therefore, do not substantiate our previous finding of an association between β-amyloid burden and general cognitive impairment. Further studies that simultaneously assess for other pathologic proteins involved in AD are needed to define their roles and relationship to β-amyloid in the clinical manifestations of aphasic AD.

Table 4.

Correlations between regional amyloid burden and neuropsychological scores

ρ value (p value)

AVLT trial 1
MOANS
AVLT LT % ret
MOANS
DKEFS sorting
scaled score
VOSP cube
raw score
L prefrontal −0.03 (0.87) −0.07 (0.68) −0.23 (0.15) −0.26 (0.09)
L medial temporal −0.06 (0.70) −0.18 (0.26) −0.01 (0.94) 0.12 (0.44)
L lateral temporal 0.10 (0.51) −0.01 (0.93) −0.19 (0.26) −0.24 (0.13)
L parietal 0.12 (0.44) 0.01 (0.94) −0.13 (0.44) −0.37 (0.02)
L insula 0.04 (0.78) 0.10 (0.84) −0.05 (0.78) −0.25 (0.11)
Broca’s 0.02 (0.91) 0.12 (0.44) −0.04 (0.81) −0.26 (0.09)
Primary visual 0.18 (0.25) −0.17 (0.28) −0.24 (0.15) −0.49** (0.001)
R prefrontal −0.01 (0.93) −0.07 (0.65) −0.25 (0.12) −0.28 (0.07)
R medial temporal −0.12 (0.45) −0.10 (0.54) −0.14 (0.41) −0.09 (0.56)
R lateral temporal 0.09 (0.56) −0.15 (0.36) −0.30 (0.06) −0.32 (0.03)
R parietal 0.07 (0.68) −0.07 (0.65) −0.28 (0.09) −0.36 (0.02)
R insula −0.02 (0.89) −0.05 (0.74) −0.07 (0.70) −0.19 (0.23)
**

Statistically significant (p < 0.004)

Abbreviations: L = left; R = right; AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Test; MOANS = Mayo Clinic’s Older Americans Normative Studies; LT = long term delayed recall; DKEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; VOSP = the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery

Acknowledgement

We would like to acknowledge the Mayo Clinic Center for Translational Science Activities (CTSA) for statistical guidance.

Study funding

The study was supported by NIH grant R01DC010367.

Footnotes

Disclosure statement

There are no financial disclosures.

References

  • 1.Josephs KA, Whitwell JL, Duffy JR, et al. Progressive aphasia secondary to Alzheimer disease vs FTLD pathology. Neurology. 2008;70:25–34. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000287073.12737.35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Thal DR, Rub U, Orantes M, Braak H. Phases of A beta-deposition in the human brain and its relevance for the development of AD. Neurology. 2002;58:1791–1800. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.12.1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rabinovici GD, Jagust WJ, Furst AJ, et al. Abeta amyloid and glucose metabolism in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Annals of neurology. 2008;64:388–401. doi: 10.1002/ana.21451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Leyton CE, Villemagne VL, Savage S, et al. Subtypes of progressive aphasia: application of the International Consensus Criteria and validation using beta-amyloid imaging. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2011;134:3030–3043. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lehmann M, Ghosh PM, Madison C, et al. Diverging patterns of amyloid deposition and hypometabolism in clinical variants of probable Alzheimer's disease. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2013;136:844–858. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Villemagne VL, Pike KE, Chetelat G, et al. Longitudinal assessment of Abeta and cognition in aging and Alzheimer disease. Annals of neurology. 2011;69:181–192. doi: 10.1002/ana.22248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jung Y, Whitwell JL, Duffy JR, et al. Amyloid burden correlates with cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease presenting with aphasia. European journal of neurology : the official journal of the European Federation of Neurological Societies. 2014;21:1040–1043. doi: 10.1111/ene.12331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Whitwell JL, Lowe VJ, Duffy JR, et al. Elevated occipital beta-amyloid deposition is associated with widespread cognitive impairment in logopenic progressive aphasia. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 2013;84:1357–1364. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-305628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Josephs KA, Duffy JR, Fossett TR, et al. Fluorodeoxyglucose F18 positron emission tomography in progressive apraxia of speech and primary progressive aphasia variants. Archives of neurology. 2010;67:596–605. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76:1006–1014. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821103e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005;53:695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology. 2000;55:1621–1626. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kertesz A, Davidson W, Fox H. Frontal behavioral inventory: diagnostic criteria for frontal lobe dementia. The Canadian journal of neurological sciences Le journal canadien des sciences neurologiques. 1997;24:29–36. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100021053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 1982;140:566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kertesz A. Western aphasia battery (revised) San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp; 2007. Western aphasia battery (revised) [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wertz RT, Keith RL, Custer DD. Normal and aphasic behavior on a measure of auditory input and a measure of verbal output. Annual Convention of the American Speech and Hearing Association. 1971 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lansing AE, Ivnik RJ, Cullum CM, Randolph C. An empirically derived short form of the Boston naming test. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 1999;14:481–487. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rey A. L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Examiner's Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Warrington EK, James M. The visual object and space perception battery. Bury St Edmonds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jack CR, Jr, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML, et al. 11C PiB and structural MRI provide complementary information in imaging of Alzheimer's disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2008;131:665–680. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Madhavan A, Whitwell JL, Weigand SD, et al. FDG PET and MRI in logopenic primary progressive aphasia versus dementia of the Alzheimer's type. PloS one. 2013;8:e62471. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Annals of neurology. 2004;55:335–346. doi: 10.1002/ana.10825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Whitwell JL, Duffy JR, Strand EA, et al. Distinct regional anatomic and functional correlates of neurodegenerative apraxia of speech and aphasia: an MRI and FDG-PET study. Brain and language. 2013;125:245–252. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cohen AD, Price JC, Weissfeld LA, et al. Basal cerebral metabolism may modulate the cognitive effects of Abeta in mild cognitive impairment: an example of brain reserve. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2009;29:14770–14778. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3669-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Forsberg A, Almkvist O, Engler H, Wall A, Langstrom B, Nordberg A. High PIB retention in Alzheimer's disease is an early event with complex relationship with CSF biomarkers and functional parameters. Current Alzheimer research. 2010;7:56–66. doi: 10.2174/156720510790274446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.La Joie R, Perrotin A, Barre L, et al. Region-specific hierarchy between atrophy, hypometabolism, and beta-amyloid (Abeta) load in Alzheimer's disease dementia. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:16265–16273. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2170-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rohrer JD, Clarkson MJ, Kittus R, et al. Rates of hemispheric and lobar atrophy in the language variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD. 2012;30:407–411. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-111556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. The Lancet Neurology. 2010;9:119–128. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Whitwell JL, Josephs KA, Murray ME, et al. MRI correlates of neurofibrillary tangle pathology at autopsy: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neurology. 2008;71:743–749. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000324924.91351.7d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Josephs KA, Whitwell JL, Weigand SD, et al. TDP-43 is a key player in the clinical features associated with Alzheimer's disease. Acta neuropathologica. 2014;127:811–824. doi: 10.1007/s00401-014-1269-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Josephs KA, Murray ME, Whitwell JL, et al. Staging TDP-43 pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Acta neuropathologica. 2014;127:441–450. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1211-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES