Abstract
Research consistently reveals positive self-reported condom evaluations, yet such evaluations often do not predict condom use. Whereas positive self-reports likely reflect social norms regarding prevention of diseases and pregnancy, psychophysiological measures might better assess spontaneous condom evaluations. Here, participants completed a visual oddball task in which condoms and alcoholic beverages were infrequent targets among neutral, positive, and negative context images. Although self-reported condom evaluations were very positive, condom images presented in a negative context produced a smaller P3 than condom images presented in a neutral or positive context, suggesting that spontaneous condom evaluations were more negative than positive. The P3 elicited by alcohol images indicated positive evaluations. The findings underscore the multifaceted nature of evaluations and point to the utility of ERPs for assessing health-related attitudes.
Keywords: ERPs, P3, Condom, Alcohol, Evaluations, Self-report
Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)/acquired immune deficiency syndrome (AIDS) is an international epidemic with no known cure (approximately 40 million people worldwide currently are living with AIDS), making prevention efforts vitally important. Considering the danger of contracting sexually transmitted diseases (STDs) such as HIV through unprotected sex, understanding factors related to decision making about sexual opportunities is critical. Aside from abstinence, the recommended method of STD prevention is correct and consistent use of condoms (Pinkerton & Abramson, 1997; World Health Organization, 2003). Thus, understanding attitudes toward condoms and condom use is important in HIV prevention efforts.
Condom attitudes typically are assessed using questionnaire measures, but such measures do not always predict condom use (Albarracín, Johnson, Fishbein, & Muellerleile, 2001; Keller, 1993). Self-report measures can be fraught with validity problems, such as participants’ self-presentational concerns and difficulty accurately recalling past behavior (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Moreover, attitudes are multifaceted and complex (see Eagly & Chaiken, 1998), sometimes involving affective components (e.g., condoms decrease hedonic pleasure) and cognitive components (e.g., condoms prevent STDs) with opposing evaluative connotations. Such complexity often is not reflected in self-report measures aimed at assessing overall, general evaluations of condoms. Thus, some researchers have called for more multidimensional assessments of condom attitudes (Helweg-Larsen & Collins, 1994).
Recently, researchers have attempted to tap into different aspects of condom attitudes using reaction-time based measures such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT; see Czopp, Monteith, Zimmerman, & Lynam, 2004; Marsh, Johnson, & Scott-Sheldon, 2001. Such measures have advantages over self-reports in that they arguably reveal less controlled aspects of attitudes that unfold relatively quickly (see Nosek, 2007). However, even these measures can confound relevant cognitive and affective/motivational processes with largely irrelevant motor-related response output processes (see Ito & Cacioppo, 2007).
Fortunately, event-related brain potentials (ERPs) can provide insight into spontaneous evaluations in ways that overcome such difficulties. Numerous studies have shown that the amplitude of the P3 component is associated with evaluative categorization. Cacioppo, Crites, Berntson, and Coles (1993) were the first to show that P3 amplitude increases as a function of the evaluative distinction between an attitude object and a preceding category (see also Cacioppo, Crites, & Gardner, 1996; Cacioppo, Crites, Gardner, & Berntson, 1994). Importantly for our purposes here, P3 amplitude has been shown to vary as a function of attitude registration rather than attitude report (Crites, Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1995), a finding consistent with other data indicating that the P3 is a relatively pure measure of evaluative categorization, independent of response-related processes (see Magliero, Bashore, Coles, & Donchin, 1984). Previous work also indicates that the P3 can be useful as a tool to investigate both attitudes and knowledge that participants are unwilling to report (e.g., Allen, Iacono, & Danielson, 1992; Johnson, Barnhardt, & Zhu, 2003). Such findings suggest that the P3 can be particularly useful when assessing attitudes related to socially sensitive topics. Despite these advantages, to our knowledge ERPs previously have not been used to assess condom evaluations.
The purpose of the present research was to compare explicit, self-reported condom evaluations with P3-based, spontaneous evaluative responses to condoms. Evaluations reflected in P3 amplitude are affected both by internal, individual difference processes (e.g., Ito, Thompson, & Cacioppo, 2004) and also by experimental manipulations such as the dimension on which participants are asked to categorize targets (e.g., Crites & Cacioppo, 1996; Ito & Cacioppo, 2000). Here we use the term “spontaneous” evaluations to differentiate the process reflected in the P3 from the relatively slower and more deliberative process reflected in explicit categorizations. We used a visual oddball task in which condom images (and images of alcoholic beverages) were used as infrequent targets amid more frequent negative, neutral, and positive context images. In this paradigm, the extent to which the target P3 is enhanced relative to the valenced context in which it appears reflects the degree of evaluative difference between the target and the context (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 1993, 1994). We hypothesized that participants would report generally positive explicit attitudes toward condoms, given current cultural endorsement of condom use for health protection (Pinkerton & Abramson, 1997), but that spontaneous condom evaluations reflected in P3 amplitude would be less favorable given that condom use is infrequent (Keller, 1993). Alcohol targets were included for comparison purposes, as alcohol is another health-related attitude object associated with risky behavior that recently has been studied using implicit measures (e.g., Wiers, van Woerden, Smulders, & de Jong, 2002).
Method
Participants
Twenty-nine healthy undergraduates (19 men) enrolled in introductory psychology courses participated in partial fulfillment of course requirements. All participants had no history of neurological disorder and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
Pretest measures
Early in the semester participants completed measures of lifetime number of sexual intercourse partners and their current relationship status. Self-reported condom evaluations were measured using an item asking participants to rate condoms on a scale anchored at 1 (very bad) to 6 (very good). Participants also rated their likelihood of using a condom with someone they did not know well, using a scale ranging from 1 (definitely) to 6 (definitely not). Participants also reported their typical quantity and frequency of alcohol use (per week, based on past 3 months).
Picture Viewing Task
Participants completed a visual oddball task in which images of condoms and alcoholic beverages were infrequent targets (10% each) presented among more frequent negative (Mvalence = 3.03; Marousal = 4.87), neutral (Mvalence = 5.07; Marousal = 2.75), and positive (Mvalence = 7.60; Marousal = 4.74) context images. All context images were taken from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2001).1 Target images that did not come from the IAPS were pre-rated by 11 undergraduates (none of whom participated in the current study) using the same social affective manikin used in the IAPS. Alcohol and condom images were rated similarly in terms of both valence (Ms = 5.94 and 5.6 for alcohol and condoms, respectively) and arousal (Ms = 5.84 and 6.08, respectively).
Images were presented in the center of a monitor in sequences of five, four of which were always context images. Target images (alcohol or condoms) were shown in the fourth or fifth position within each trial sequence. Valence of context (neutral, positive, or negative) was consistent within sequences but varied randomly across sequences. Each image was presented for 250 ms, followed by a 1000-ms interstimulus interval. There were a total of 162 trial sequences (54 each of negative, neutral, and positive context), half of which included condom targets and half alcohol targets. Participants were instructed to categorize each image as positive/pleasant or negative/unpleasant by pressing one of two buttons on a response box (counterbalanced across participants). They were told that some images might not be easily categorized, but to go with their “gut instinct.” At a viewing distance of 120 cm, the images subtended a visual angle of 4.75°.
ERP Measures
Electroencephalographic (EEG) data were recorded from 28 standard scalp locations (American Encephalographic Society, 1994) using tin electrodes fixed in a stretch-lycra cap (Electro-cap International, Eaton, OH). EEG was sampled continuously at 1000 Hz and filtered online at 0.05–40 Hz. Scalp electrodes were referenced online to the right mastoid; an average mastoid reference was derived off-line. EOG was monitored from electrodes placed above and below the left eye and 2 cm lateral to the outer canthus of each eye. Impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. Ocular artifacts were corrected from the EEG signal off-line using a regression-based procedure (Semlitsch, Anderer, Schuster, & Presslich, 1986).
Results
Demographics
Fourteen participants indicated they were not in a serious relationship, 8 participants indicated they had a boyfriend or girlfriend, 3 said they were casually dating, and 5 did not indicate their relationship status. Participants indicated having from 0 to 50 lifetime sex partners (median = 3). Most participants (63%) described themselves as social drinkers; the remainder reported less frequent drinking.
Self-reported Condom Attitudes
Overt condom evaluations were uniformly quite positive. The majority of participants (77%) evaluated condoms as “good” or “very good” (M = 5.52, SD = 0.77). All participants indicated they would use condoms with casual partners.
Explicit Categorizations
Analyses of behavioral data focused on the proportion of “positive” and “negative” categorizations of each target type. An initial analysis showed that, across all target types and contexts, participants were more likely to respond “pleasant” (M = .68) than “unpleasant” (M = .28), F(1,27) = 24.67, p<.001. Thus, main analyses focused on the effects of our manipulations on the proportion of positive categorizations using a 2 (Sex of Participants) × 3 (Valence of Context; negative, neutral, positive) × 2 (Target Type; condoms, alcohol) mixed factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures on all but the first factor. This analysis showed a significant main effect of Target, F(1,27) = 12.91, p<.01, η2 = .32; participants more often categorized alcohol targets as positive (M = .71) than condoms as positive (M = .64). A significant Context effect, F(2,54) = 89.9, p<.001 (G-G adjusted), ε = .70, η2 = .77, indicated that participants were more likely to categorize targets as positive when presented in a positive (M = .83) or neutral context (M = .80) compared to a negative context (M = .42). No other effects were significant.
ERP Data
P3 amplitude
Preliminary analyses revealed a significant Coronal Location (anterior to posterior) × Lateral Location (left, midline, right) interaction, F(8,216) = 21.9, p<.001, indicating that P3 amplitude was largest at centro-parietal and parietal locations at or near the midline (see Figure 1). Thus, primary analyses were restricted to data recorded at those locations.2
A 2 (Sex) × 2 (Target Type) × 3 (Context) × 2 (Location; CP sites, P sites) × 3 (Lateral; left, midline, right) mixed factorial ANOVA with repeated measures on all but the first factor showed a main effect of Target type, F(1,27) = 23.43, p<.001, indicating that condoms elicited larger P3s (M = 14.03 μV) than alcohol (M = 10.96 μV). This effect was qualified by a Target Type × Context interaction, F(2,54) = 5.62, p<.01 (G-G adjusted), ε = .95, η2 = .172. We probed this interaction using separate linear contrasts comparing P3s elicited by condoms and alcohol across the three levels of context. For condom targets, the P3 increased linearly from negative (M = 12.86 μV) to neutral (M = 14.23 μV) to positive contexts (M = 15.0 μV), F(1,27) = 5.24, p<.05. This pattern indicates a smaller evaluative distinction between condoms and negative than between condoms and positive.3 In contrast, the P3 elicited by alcohol targets showed the opposite pattern, decreasing in a largely linear fashion from negative (M = 12.05 μV) to neutral (M = 10.58 μV) to positive (M = 10.23 μV) contexts, F(1,27) = 3.54, p = .07.
The analysis also showed a Target × Sex interaction, F(1,27) = 6.92, p<.01, η2 = .20. Whereas alcohol targets elicited similar P3s in men (M = 10.48 μV) and women (M = 11.43 μV), t(27) = 0.54, p>.50, condoms elicited marginally larger P3s in women (M = 16.18 μV) than in men (M = 11.89 μV), t(27) = 1.94, p = .06. The three-way Target × Context × Sex interaction was not significant (F<1), nor were any other effects.4
Discussion
Numerous previous reports have established the P3 as a reliable marker of spontaneous evaluative categorization (see Ito & Cacioppo, 2007). The results of this study suggest that, whereas self-reported evaluations were quite positive, spontaneous aspects of condom evaluation reflected in P3 amplitude were more negative than positive. Specifically, the size of the P3 elicited by condom targets increased linearly along with context valence. In contrast, evaluative categorization of alcohol cues appeared to be more positive than negative; P3 amplitude elicited by alcohol cues was larger in a negative than a positive context. Behaviorally, participants categorized condoms positively only 64% of the time, also suggesting less positive evaluations than indicated in the self-report data.
Given the limited sampling frame (college undergraduates), relatively small sample size, and preliminary nature of the current data, conclusions concerning the differences between self-reported and electrocortical responses to condoms seen here should be made with caution. For example, the causal mechanism(s) responsible for potential differences between spontaneous and self-report evaluations are unclear. One possibility is that positive self-reported evaluations of condoms do not reveal respondents’ true attitudes but rather reflect awareness of social norms and expectations concerning the benefits of condom use, whereas P3 amplitude provides a more “direct” measure of a person’s “true” attitude (see Crites et al., 1995).
Another plausible explanation is that attitudes toward condoms are complex, likely involving multiple components that are differentially revealed by measures at different levels of analysis. The P3 likely reflects an early, largely automatic, affect-driven component that appears to be mostly negative, perhaps based on personal experiences of decreased hedonic pleasure when using condoms (Weinstock, Lindan, Bolan, Kegeles, & Hearst, 1993), embarrassment about condom purchase or possession (Czopp et al., 2004), or other related issues that influence motivation to use condoms. Somewhat later in the processing stream, overt categorization responses likely incorporate this initial affective response with some additional information concerning the benefits of condom use that tempers the evaluative response somewhat. Finally, self-reported evaluations probably reflect both knowledge about health-protective reasons for condom use as well as prevailing social norms and concerns about self-presentation that lead to generally positive responses. To the extent that the evaluative processes reflected in the P3 can be considered similar to so-called implicit measures (see Ito & Cacioppo, 2007) and that such measures predict condom use in sexually risky situations more effectively than explicit measures do (Czopp et al., 2004; Marsh et al., 2001), the current data suggest that decisions to use condoms in risky situations are more likely based on initial (negative) affective reactions than on more reasoned cognitive responses (see Norton, Bogart, Cecil, & Pinkerton, 2005). This pattern could help to explain why, despite generally positive self-reported evaluations (e.g., Fisher, Fisher, & Rye, 1995; Gibbons, Gerrard, Blanton, & Russell, 1998), people often fail to use condoms for health protection. However, the current data do not definitively support this conclusion, nor can they differentiate between the two potential mechanisms proposed here. Future research should test these ideas directly.
In conclusion, these preliminary data highlight the potential for ERPs to measure evaluations of often complex attitude stimuli that can inform and augment other “implicit” (e.g., IAT) and explicit (self-report) attitude measures. The current data suggest that condom evaluations might differ considerably depending on levels of analysis and that a fuller understanding of attitudes toward health-related behaviors requires use of measures that tap these different levels.
Footnotes
IAPS picture numbers used in this study were: Neutral = 7002, 7004, 7090, 7233, and 6150; Negative = 9320, 9912, 9630, 9001, and 9180; Positive = 7260, 7330, 8501, 5760, and 5001.
Including data from all electrodes produces essentially the same pattern of findings but also reveals a number of higher-order interaction effects involving electrode location that are not of interest here.
Note that this same pattern emerges when participants whose self-reported evaluations were less than very positive (n = 10) are excluded from the analysis, which suggests that P3 results were not driven by “outliers” whose self-reported evaluations were relatively negative.
Inspection of Figure 1 suggests that condoms presented in negative contexts elicited slower P3 latency than did condoms presented in other contexts. Although we had no hypotheses concerning latency effects, we conducted an exploratory analysis of P3 latency using a 2 (Target) × 3 (Context) × 2 (Sex) mixed ANOVA. The Target × Context interaction was not significant, F(1,54) = 0.78, p = .46. However, a follow-up contrast showed that the P3 for condoms in a negative context (M = 591 ms) peaked more slowly than for condoms in a positive (M = 545 ms) or neutral context (M = 543 ms), F(1,27) = 6.54, p<.05.
REFERENCES
- Albarracín D, Johnson BT, Fishbein M, Muellerleile PA. Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:142–161. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen JJ, Iacono WG, Danielson KD. The identification of concealed memories using the event-related potential and implicit behavioral measures: A methodology for prediction in the face of individual differences. Psychophysiology. 1992;29:504–522. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02024.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Encephalographic Society Guideline thirteen: Guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 1994;11:111–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Crites SL, Berntson GG, Coles MGH. If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not affect the event-related brain potential? Psychological Science. 1993;4:108–112. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Crites SL, Jr., Gardner WL. Attitudes to the right: Evaluative processing is associated with lateralized late positive event-related brain potentials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1996;22:1205. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Crites SL, Jr., Gardner WL, Berntson GG. Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:115–125. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crites SL, Cacioppo JT, Gardner WL, Berntson GG. Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorization: II. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of attitude registration rather than attitude report. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;68:997–1013. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.68.6.997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crites SL, Jr., Cacioppo JT. Electrocortical differentiation of evaluative and nonevaluative categorizations. Psychological Science. 1996;7:318–321. [Google Scholar]
- Czopp AM, Monteith MJ, Zimmerman RS, Lynam DR. Implicit attitudes as potential protection from risky sex: Predicting condom use with the IAT. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 2004;26:227–236. [Google Scholar]
- Eagly AH, Chaiken S. Attitude structure and function. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. Handbook of social psychology. 4th ed Vol. 1. McGraw-Hill; New York: 1998. pp. 269–322. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher WA, Fisher JD, Rye BJ. Understanding and promoting AIDS-preventive behavior: Insights from the theory of reasoned action. Healthy Psychology. 1995;14:255–264. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.14.3.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Blanton H, Russell DW. Reasoned action and social reaction: Willingness and intention as independent predicators of health risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1164–1180. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helweg-Larsen M, Collins BE. The UCLA Multidimensional Condom Attitudes Scale: Documenting the complex determinants of condom use in college students. Health Psychology. 1994;13:224–237. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.13.3.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito TA, Cacioppo JT. Electrophysiological evidence of implicit and explicit categorization processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2000;36:660–676. [Google Scholar]
- Ito TA, Cacioppo JT. Attitudes as mental and neural states of readiness: Using physiological measures to study implicit attitudes. In: Wittenbrink B, Schwarz N, editors. Implicit measures of attitudes. Guilford Press; New York: 2007. pp. 125–158. [Google Scholar]
- Ito TA, Thompson E, Cacioppo JT. Tracking the timecourse of social perception: The effects of racial cues on event-related brain potentials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2004;30:1267–1280. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson R, Jr., Barnhardt J, Zhu J. The deceptive response: Effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity. Biological Psychology. 2003;64:217–253. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller ML. Why don’t young adults protect themselves against sexual transmission of HIV? Possible answers to a complex question. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1993;5:220–233. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-5. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida; Gainsville, FL: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Magliero A, Bashore TR, Coles MG, Donchin E. On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. Psychophysiology. 1984;21:171–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1984.tb00201.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh KL, Johnson BT, Scott-Sheldon LAJ. Heart versus reason in condom use: Implicit versus explicit attitudinal predictors of sexual behavior. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie. 2001;48:161–175. doi: 10.1026//0949-3946.48.2.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review. 1977;84:232–259. [Google Scholar]
- Norton TR, Bogart LM, Cecil H, Pinkerton SD. Primacy of affect over cognition in determining adult men’s condom-use behavior: A review. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2005;35:2493–2534. [Google Scholar]
- Nosek BA. Implicit-explicit relations. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:65–69. [Google Scholar]
- Pinkerton SD, Abramson PR. Effectiveness of condoms in preventing HIV transmission. Social Science and Medicine. 1997;44:1303–1312. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00258-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semlitsch HV, Anderer P, Schuster P, Presslich O. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology. 1986;23:695–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00696.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstock HS, Lindan C, Bolan G, Kegeles SM, Hearst N. Factors associated with condom use in a high-risk heterosexual population. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 1993;20:14–20. doi: 10.1097/00007435-199301000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, van Woerden N, Smulders FTY, de Jong PJ. Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:648–658. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization [Retrieved March 9, 2008];Condom facts and figures. 2003 Aug; from http://www.wpro.who.int/media_centre/fact_sheets/fs_200308_Condoms.htm.