Skip to main content
World Journal of Psychiatry logoLink to World Journal of Psychiatry
. 2015 Dec 22;5(4):404–411. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v5.i4.404

Comprehensive neurocognitive assessment of patients with anorexia nervosa

Andrea Phillipou 1,2,3,4,5,6,7, Caroline Gurvich 1,2,3,4,5,6,7, David Jonathan Castle 1,2,3,4,5,6,7, Larry Allen Abel 1,2,3,4,5,6,7, Susan Lee Rossell 1,2,3,4,5,6,7
PMCID: PMC4694554  PMID: 26740932

Abstract

AIM: To utilise a comprehensive cognitive battery to gain a better understanding of cognitive performance in anorexia nervosa (AN).

METHODS: Twenty-six individuals with AN and 27 healthy control participants matched for age, gender and premorbid intelligence, participated in the study. A standard cognitive battery, the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia Consensus Cognitive Battery, was used to investigate performance on seven cognitive domains with the use of 10 different tasks: speed of processing [Brief Assessment Of Cognition In Schizophrenia: Symbol Coding, Category Fluency: Animal Naming (Fluency) and Trail Making Test: Part A], attention/vigilance [Continuous Performance Test - Identical Pairs (CPT-IP)], working memory [Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS®-III): Spatial Span, and Letter-Number Span (LNS)], verbal learning [Hopkins Verbal Learning Test - Revised], visual learning [Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised], reasoning and problem solving [Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: Mazes], and social cognition [Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test: Managing Emotions]. Statistical analyses involved the use of multivariate and univariate analyses of variance.

RESULTS: Analyses conducted on the cognitive domain scores revealed no overall significant difference between groups nor any interaction between group and domain score [F(1,45) = 0.73, P = 0.649]. Analyses conducted on each of the specific tasks within the cognitive domains revealed significantly slower reaction times for false alarm responses on the CPT-IP task in AN [F(1,51) = 12.80, P < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.982] and a trend towards poorer performance in AN on the backward component of the WMS®-III Spatial Span task [F(1,51) = 5.88, P = 0.02, Cohen’s d = -0.665]. The finding of slower reaction times of false alarm responses is, however, limited due to the small number of false alarm responses for either group.

CONCLUSION: The findings are discussed in terms of poorer capacity to manipulate and process visuospatial material in AN.

Keywords: Cognition, Eating disorder, Body image, Spatial processing, Short-term memory


Core tip: The findings of this study suggest that individuals with anorexia nervosa (AN) have largely intact cognitive performance, which notably differs to the cognitive profile of other psychiatric illnesses, such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder, which are all associated with significant cognitive deficits. However, a trend for AN participants to perform poorer on the backward component of a spatial span task was revealed. This suggests a poorer capacity to process and manipulate visuospatial information in AN, which may be related to the distortions of body image experienced by these individuals.

INTRODUCTION

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a serious psychiatric condition with a mortality rate among the highest of any mental illness[1,2]. Yet, the factors involved in the genesis and maintenance of the illness remain unclear. A common feature of AN is perfectionism, which has been identified as a significant risk factor for the illness, and is a feature found to persist following long-term recovery[3,4]. Thus, cognitive assessments reported in the AN literature often focus on tasks related to perfectionism and rigid thinking patterns, such as cognitive set shifting tasks[5]. Individuals with AN have been found to perform significantly more poorly than healthy individuals during tasks of cognitive set shifting such as the Wisconsin Card Sort Test and certain target detection tasks, displaying stereotyped behaviours with rigid approaches to changing rules[6-8]. However, unlike other psychiatric illnesses, there is a paucity of research employing more comprehensive assessments of cognitive profile in AN.

Studies of cognition in AN have tended to use a range of cognitive assessments rather than utilising a standard cognitive battery; they have consequently reported conflicting findings[9-18]. Therefore, the use of a standard neurocognitive battery, such as the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB)[19], would be advantageous and would also allow for direct comparisons to be drawn between AN and other clinical populations. The MCCB was originally designed to assess cognitive domains most relevant to schizophrenia but has since been applied to assess cognitive impairments in other psychiatric illnesses, including bipolar disorder[20], posttraumatic stress disorder[21] and major depressive disorder[22]. With the use of 10 different tasks, the MCCB assesses the following 7 cognitive domains: speed of processing, attention/vigilance, working memory, verbal learning, visual learning, reasoning and problem solving, and social cognition.

Although cognitive batteries have been compiled in past research to assess cognition in AN[12,13], a standardised cognitive battery has rarely been used[23]. Furthermore, although each of the cognitive domains in the MCCB has been investigated in past research in AN, the findings are largely inconsistent. Under the speed of processing domain, intact fluency and symbol coding performance has typically been found[15,24]. Poorer performance on trail making tasks on the other hand have been reported by some researchers[25], but not by others[8]. In relation to the attention/vigilance domain, performance on a continuous performance task has not been found to differ from healthy individuals[26], though performance on a similar task requiring rapid visual information processing has been found to result in poorer performance in AN patients[27]. Performance on tasks assessing verbal and visuospatial working memory, on the other hand, have also been found to not differ from healthy individuals by some investigators[10,27], whilst others have reported poorer visuospatial working memory in AN[28]. In relation to visual learning, performance has typically been found to be intact in AN[9,29]. Furthermore, although performance on reasoning and problem solving tasks such as mazes has also been found to be largely intact in AN[23], other tasks assessing this domain such as object assembly and block design, have been found to result in poorer performance in AN[11,28]. Finally, studies assessing social cognition in AN also show inconsistent findings, with some reporting poorer performance[30] and others failing to find a significant difference from healthy individuals[31].

Therefore, the aim of this study was to utilise a comprehensive battery of tasks to investigate cognitive performance in AN. We hypothesised that individuals with AN would show poorer performance on tasks assessing each of the cognitive domains, except the tasks assessing speed of processing and visual learning, where the literature has reported intact performance to date. We further completed exploratory analyses of performance of each of the tasks in AN given that the MCCB was originally designed to examine cognitive profile in people with schizophrenia, who are reported to show substantial cognitive problems. Individuals with AN are not expected to show such extreme deficits, and thus may only show impairment at the individual task level rather than at the level of domains overall. As cognitive performance is significantly affected by malnutrition[32] and this is a likely factor in the inconsistent findings to date in AN, we sought to investigate AN participants who were at a uniform phase of their illness trajectory [medically stable but below the healthy body mass index (BMI) range].

MATERIALS AND METHODS

This study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Departments at the University of Melbourne, Swinburne University of Technology, the Melbourne Clinic, the Austin Hospital and St Vincent’s Hospital; all in Melbourne, Australia. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Participants

Participants were 26 right-handed females with AN and 27 healthy controls (HC). Groups were matched for age and premorbid intelligence quotient (IQ). HCs were recruited through public advertisements, whereas AN participants were recruited through public advertisements; the Body Image and Eating Disorders Treatment and Recovery Service at the Austin and St Vincent’s Hospitals; and The Melbourne Clinic; all in Melbourne, Australia. All patients were required to be medically stable (i.e., not requiring medical attention due to their physical state) prior to inclusion in the study.

All participants were English speaking and had no history of significant brain injury or neurological condition. Controls were required to have no history of an eating disorder or other mental illness; they were also required to not be taking any medications apart from hormonal contraceptives (11 HC participants were taking medications). AN participants were instructed to continue with their normal medications, which were: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (11), atypical antipsychotics (12), benzodiazepines (6), serotonin-noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) (3), hormonal contraceptives (3), melatonergic antidepressants (3), noradrenergic and specific serotonergic antidepressant (1) and cyclopyrrolones (1).

The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview, 5.0.0[33] was used to screen all participants for major Axis I psychiatric disorders according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). It was also used to confirm diagnoses of AN, with the exception of the amenorrhea criterion which is no longer included in the current DSM-5. AN was required to be the primary diagnosis of the AN group. AN participants with comorbid psychiatric conditions, other than psychotic conditions, were not excluded as this would not have represented a typical AN sample.

Premorbid intelligence was estimated using the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading[34]. Eating disorder symptomatology was investigated with the Eating Disorders Examination Questionnaire[35] (Table 1).

Table 1.

Clinical characteristics

AN HC F P Cohen’s d
M (SD) M (SD)
Age 22.81 (6.67) 22.46 (3.16) 0.06 0.81 0.07
WTAR 104.77 (8.11) 106.19 (7.11) 0.46 0.50 0.19
BMI 16.63 (1.19) 22.60 (3.53) 67.08 < 0.01 2.27
Illness duration 6.42 (7.43) - - - -
Age of illness onset 16.04 (3.40) - - - -
EDE-Q restraint 3.93 (1.42) 0.58 (0.63) 116.84 < 0.01 3.05
EDE-Q eating concern 3.78 (1.24) 0.25 (0.31) 188.56 < 0.01 3.91
EDE-Q shape concern 5.01 (0.90) 1.17 (0.84) 236.44 < 0.01 4.41
EDE-Q weight concern 4.50 (1.41) 0.66 (0.82) 136.11 < 0.01 3.33
EDE-Q global score 4.30 (1.12) 0.67 (0.54) 211.44 < 0.01 4.13

AN: Anorexia nervosa; HC: Healthy control; WTAR: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading; BMI: Body mass index; Age: Illness duration and age of illness onset reported in years; EDE-Q: Eating disorders examination questionnaire.

Cognitive battery

With the use of 10 different tasks, the MCCB assesses 7 cognitive domains. Scores on each task are entered into the MATRICS scoring program which produces cognitive domain scores.

Speed of processing: Speed of processing is assessed with three different tasks. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Symbol Coding task requires participants to use a key to correctly match and report as many digits that correspond to nonsense symbols as they can within 90 s; the number of correctly matched symbols constitutes the score. The Category Fluency: Animal Naming (Fluency) task requires participants to orally report as many animals as they can in 60 s, which constitutes the task score. The Trail Making Test: Part A task requires participants to draw a line connecting consecutive numbers from 1 to 25 irregularly placed on a sheet of paper as quickly as possible. The score is equivalent to the amount of time in seconds required to complete the task.

Attention/vigilance: Attention/vigilance is assessed with the Continuous Performance Test - Identical Pairs (CPT-IP). In this task, trials of 2-, 3- and 4-digit numbers are flashed briefly on a computer monitor and participants are required to click the mouse when the same number appears consecutively. The total number of possible hits is 90, the total number of possible false alarms is also 90, and total number of possible random responses is 270.

Working memory: Non-verbal working memory is assessed with the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS®-III): Spatial Span, a visual analogue of the digit span task. The Spatial Span task involves the administrator tapping a series of cubes in a specified sequence and participants are required to reproduce this sequence in the same order (“forward” component). Following the “forward” component, the “backward” component is administered in the same manner except participants are now required to reproduce the sequence in the reverse order. The number of correct sequences reproduced constitutes the task score.

The Letter-Number Span (LNS) task is a task of verbal working memory which requires the mental reordering of orally presented lists of intermixed letters and numbers. In this task, a list of letters and numbers is read out to respondents. The respondent is required to mentally reorder the sequence and verbally report the sequence beginning with the numbers from smallest to largest, followed by the letters in alphabetical order. The number of correct reordered sequences constitutes the score on the task.

Verbal learning: Verbal learning is tested with the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test - Revised. A list of 12 words from 3 semantic categories (four-legged animals, precious stones and human dwellings) is read out and respondents are required to report as many words as they can remember, in any order. The task comprises 3 trials, enabling a maximum score of 36. Following a period of 20-25 min, participants are asked to recall as many words as they can (trial 4). A retention score is also calculated by dividing the score for trial 4 by the higher score of trials 2 and 3, multiplied by 100. A longer list of 24 words is also read out to participants containing the original 12 words, as well as 6 semantically-related words and 6 semantically-unrelated words. A delayed recognition score is calculated by the total number of true positives minus the total number of false positives.

Visual learning: Visual learning is assessed with the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test - Revised which requires participants to reproduce 6 geometric figures following a 10 s presentation. The task comprises 3 trials, with one point each awarded for accuracy and correct placement of the figure, resulting in a maximum score of 36.

Reasoning and problem solving: Reasoning and problem solving is examined with the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: Mazes task. In this task, a set of 7 mazes of increasing difficult is administered to participants. The score received for each maze is determined by the speed in which it is completed. A maximum score of 26 is achievable.

Social cognition: Social cognition is assessed with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test: Managing Emotions. The tasks involve the respondent rating the effectiveness of alternative actions or responses in achieving a certain result in situations where an individual must regulate their emotions. Lower scores indicate poorer performance.

Statistical analysis

Following normality checking and the removal of outliers, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was first conducted on the seven cognitive domain scores across groups. Secondly, one way analysis of variance was used on each of the task variables to compare group performance, with alpha set at 0.01 to account for multiple comparisons. Statistical review of the study was performed by a biomedical statistician.

RESULTS

A MANOVA conducted on the cognitive domain scores revealed no overall significant difference between groups nor any interaction between group and domain score [F(1,45) = 0.73, P = 0.649] (Table 2). Analyses conducted on each of the specific tasks within the cognitive domains revealed significantly slower reaction times for false alarm responses on the CPT-IP task in AN [F(1,51) = 12.80, P < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.982] and a trend towards poorer performance in AN on the backward component of the WMS®-III Spatial Span task [F(1,51) = 5.88, P = 0.02, Cohen’s d = -0.665] (Table 3). The finding of slower reaction times of false alarm responses is, however, limited due to the small number of false alarm responses for either group.

Table 2.

Cognitive domain scores

AN HC F P Cohen’s d
M (SD) M (SD)
Speed of processing 57.42 (9.75) 59.85 (7.80) 1.01 0.32 0.28
Attention/vigilance 43.42 (8.08) 46.85 (8.47) 2.27 0.14 0.41
Working memory 56.12 (9.68) 58.56 (9.33) 0.87 0.36 0.26
Verbal learning 53.38 (10.33) 50.33 (8.19) 1.42 0.24 0.33
Visual learning 54.85 (6.25) 56.59 (9.77) 0.60 0.44 0.21
Reasoning and problem solving 55.15 (8.29) 58.11 (7.11) 1.95 0.17 0.38
Social cognition 49.42 (6.93) 48.07 (10.86) 0.29 0.59 0.15
Overall composite 54.31 (7.97) 56.19 (7.03) 0.83 0.37 0.25

AN: Anorexia nervosa; HC: Healthy control.

Table 3.

Specific task scores

AN HC F P Cohen’s d
M (SD) M (SD)
Speed of processing
BACS SC 63.23 (9.80) 68.11 (9.19) 3.50 0.07 0.51
Fluency 28.19 (6.43) 27.19 (5.54) 0.37 0.54 0.17
TMT-A 23.62 (57.50) 21.30 (4.83) 2.54 0.12 0.44
Attention/vigilance
CPT-IP
Hits proportion 0.82 (0.10) 0.86 (0.09) 1.65 0.21 0.42
Hits reaction time 550.25 (60.43) 533.05 (51.08) 1.26 0.27 0.31
False alarms proportion 0.11 (0.05) 0.09 (0.06) 1.04 0.31 0.36
False alarms reaction time 508.08 (147.39) 369.98 (133.55) 12.80 < 0.01 0.98
Random responses proportion 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 1.96 0.17 0.00
DPRIME score 2.55 (0.54) 2.78 (0.56) 2.37 0.13 0.42
Working memory
WMS®-III
Forward score 9.65 (2.30) 10.3 (1.88) 1.25 0.27 0.31
Backward score 9.12 (1.80) 10.3 (1.75) 5.88 0.02 0.67
Total score 18.77 (3.54) 20.59 (2.94) 4.18 0.05 0.56
LNS 17.38 (2.82) 16.74 (2.97) 0.66 0.42 0.22
Verbal learning
HVLT-R™
Total recall 29.38 (4.01) 28.07 (3.82) 1.48 0.23 0.34
Delayed recall 13.42 (17.74) 9.96 (1.63) 1.02 0.32 0.23
Delayed retention 92.80 (14.13) 91.98 (14.59) 0.04 0.84 0.06
Delayed recognition 11.04 (0.96) 10.93 (1.33) 0.12 0.73 0.10
Visual learning
BVMT-R™ 28.62 (3.98) 29.78 (6.23) 0.65 0.42 0.22
Reasoning and problem solving
NAB®: Mazes 22.46 (3.43) 23.70 (2.63) 2.20 0.14 0.41
Social cognition
MSCEIT™: Managing emotions 96.98 (5.92) 95.41 (9.57) 0.51 0.48 0.20

AN: Anorexia nervosa; HC: Healthy control; BACS SC: Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Symbol Coding; Fluency: Category Fluency: Animal Naming; TMT-A: Trail Making Task Part A reported in seconds; CPT-IP: Continuous Performance Test - Identical Pairs; WMS®-III: Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS®-III): Spatial Span; LNS: Letter Number Span; HVLT-R™: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test - Revised; NAB®: Mazes: Neuropsychological Assessment Battery Mazes task; MSCEIT™: Managing Emotions: Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test: Managing Emotions; reaction times on the CPT-IP reported in milliseconds.

DISCUSSION

Overall, the results from this study suggest intact cognitive performance in AN on the majority of the measures studied, despite significantly low BMIs and the potential long- and short-term effects of starvation. The only cognitive measure which showed a trend toward impairment in the AN group was visuospatial working memory. Poorer performance in AN was specifically found during the backward component of the task and not the forward component of the task, or the task overall. A study by Fowler et al[27] also reported intact overall spatial span performance in AN, but did not report whether AN and healthy individuals differed in the forward or backward components of the task. The forward component is thought to represent the capacity of the visuospatial sketchpad, whereas the backward component of this task is thought to represent a measure of executive function as it requires additional manipulation within temporary storage[36], suggesting that individuals with AN have specific working memory difficulties when the cognitive demand is high. This deficit appears to be specific to visuospatial working memory as LNS performance was intact in this cohort. Working memory deficits specific to visuospatial working memory have also been reported by Kemps et al[28], who found AN participants were poorer at recalling object locations, but did not differ in the recall of object names compared to healthy individuals. This finding is also in keeping with several studies reporting impairments in immediate recall on visuospatial memory tasks such as the Rey Complex Figure Test[37]. Poorer capacity to manipulate and process visuospatial material may also be related to the specific visuospatial processing deficits experienced in AN, in which patients overestimate the size of their own body[38]; though, this relationship would require specific investigation.

Contrary to expectations, individuals with AN did not differ from healthy individuals in performance on any other task. Though the AN group were found to make false alarm responses of longer reaction time than the control group, this finding is limited due to the small number of false alarm responses for either group, thereby not allowing accurate statistical analyses to be undertaken. The existing research utilising the same or similar tasks is particularly inconsistent with many studies reporting no cognitive deficits, while others report significantly poorer cognitive performance in AN. The inconsistency in findings may be largely related to differences in methodology, particularly the participants examined. Malnutrition certainly effects cognitive function as reported in studies of induced starvation[32]. Studies in AN patients often recruit individuals currently undergoing inpatient treatment as they are often easily accessible to researchers. The primary role of most inpatient treatment services is medical stabilisation. Therefore, patients admitted to such services are typically very physically unwell. The majority of patients in this study were outpatients at the time of testing. Furthermore, the few inpatients included were required to be medically stable. Despite all patients recruited for this study being medically stable, their BMIs were significantly below normal and their eating disorder symptomatology significantly high, suggesting that they were in an acute phase of the illness but were physically well enough to function. Therefore, the sample recruited is a significant strength of this study, especially as they were age, gender and IQ matched to the HC cohort, resulting in a homogenous sample which is often not achieved in research in AN.

The study is, however, not without its limitations. The MCCB is a standard cognitive battery originally compiled to assess the areas of cognition most relevant to schizophrenia and related disorders. Therefore, cognitions often associated with AN, such as cognitive set shifting[8,39,40], were not investigated. As the aim of the study was to investigate general cognition in AN, and the existing cognitive battery was already lengthy, an additional cognitive set shifting task was not considered feasible. Future research in AN would benefit from including set shifting tasks. Although the MCCB provides a comprehensive profile of basic cognitive tasks, a battery also including tasks related to the specific cognitive traits commonly associated with AN, and tasks allowing more detailed exploration of executive function would be beneficial in future research. Furthermore, the modest sample size may have contributed to the lack of significant differences between groups. Thus, further research utilising the same measures in a larger sample may reveal statistically significant group differences, rather than the trends reported in the current study. A further potential limitation is that the majority of patients were on medication at the time of testing, which may have influenced the findings.

The findings of this study suggest a cognitive profile in AN different to that of other psychiatric illnesses, such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder, which are all associated with significant cognitive deficits on the MCCB[20,22,41]. Similar findings to the current study have, however, been reported in obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). Intact performance on a range of cognitive tasks have been reported in OCD. Similarly to the current study, poorer performance on tasks of spatial working memory[42], but not specifically spatial span[43], have been reported in OCD patients. Similarly, poor visuospatial working memory has also been reported in body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), another psychiatric illness within the OCD-spectrum with prominent body image disturbance[44,45]. Though, unlike the current findings, poorer performance on tasks of verbal working memory and executive function have also been reported in BDD[45,46]. Unlike the current study, however, the spatial span task components in these studies were not separated to better investigate visuospatial working memory. The deficits in visuospatial working memory and otherwise intact cognitive performance in OCD illustrates the overlap in clinical presentation that is often reported in AN, and may provide support for the long-proposed hypothesis that AN and OCD share overlapping psychopathology[47].

Overall, visuospatial working memory was the only cognitive measure that groups were found to differ on in this study, and this may be related to AN patients’ difficulties in evaluating their own bodies. As cognitive functioning in general appears to remain largely unaltered in AN, it may suggest that the limited cognitive deficits observed may arise from quite restricted brain regions which may also be involved in the psychopathology of AN.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank Charlotte Keating for administering the clinical interview. The authors would also like to thank Chia Huang, Richard Newton, Lynley Gervasoni, Michelle Snell, Helen Shepherd, Philippa Harrison and Felicity Lawrence for their assistance in recruiting participants. Finally, the authors would like to thank everyone who took the time to participate in the study.

COMMENTS

Background

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a psychiatric condition associated with perfectionism and rigid thinking patterns. Investigations of cognitive performance in AN have often focused on related measures. However, comprehensive assessments of cognition in AN have rarely been undertaken and findings have been inconsistent. Therefore, this study utilised a comprehensive cognitive battery to gain a better understanding of cognitive performance in AN.

Research frontiers

Individuals with AN appear to have largely intact cognitive function, but demonstrate difficulties with complex visuospatial processing.

Innovations and breakthroughs

Findings in acute AN have rarely utilised a comprehensive set of cognitive tasks and have consequently tended to report conflicting findings. Employing a comprehensive set of cognitive assessments in a group of medically stable patients with acute AN suggests that AN may not be associated with significant cognitive deficits, but with subtle difficulties in manipulating visuospatial information.

Applications

As only limited cognitive deficits were observed, they may arise from relatively restricted brain regions which may also be involved in the psychopathology of AN.

Peer-review

The research is interesting and the manuscript is well written.

Footnotes

Supported by The Jack Brockhoff Foundation (3410); the Dick and Pip Smith Foundation; Australian Postgraduate Award; and the David Hay Memorial Fund Award.

Institutional review board statement: The study was granted independent ethics approval by the Human Research Ethics committees at St Vincent’s Hospital [Human Research Ethics Committee A (HREC-A)] (057/12), Austin Health [Non Drug Study Advisory Committee (NDSAC)] (H2012/04646) and The Melbourne Clinic [The Melbourne Clinic Research Ethics Committee (TMC REC)] (235). In addition, the study received expedited ethics approval from Swinburne’s Human Research Ethics Committee (SUHREC) (2012/277) and was registered with The University of Melbourne Health Sciences Human Ethics Sub-Committee (HESC) (1239068), on the basis of the prior St Vincent’s Hospital review.

Informed consent statement: All participants gave written informed consent prior to study inclusion.

Conflict-of-interest statement: Professor Castle reports grants and personal fees from Eli Lilly, grants and personal fees from Janssen-Cilag, Roche, Allergen, Bristol-Myer Squibb, Pfizer, Lundbeck, AstraZeneca and Hospira during the conduct of the study, outside the submitted work; and personal fees from Eli Lilly, Bristol-Myer Squibb, Lundbeck, Janssen-Cilag, Pfizer, Organon, Sanofi-Aventis, Wyeth, Hospira and Servier, outside the submitted work. Associate Professor Abel reports personal fees from Actelion Pharmaceuticals, Switzerland, outside the submitted work. Dr. Phillipou, Professor Rossell and Dr. Gurvich report no conflicts of interest.

Data sharing statement: Technical appendix, statistical code, and dataset available from the lead author at ap@unimelb.edu.au. No additional data are available.

Open-Access: This article is an open-access article which was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/

Peer-review started: June 29, 2015

First decision: August 26, 2015

Article in press: October 8, 2015

P- Reviewer: Contreras CM, Liu-Seifert H, Richter J S- Editor: Ji FF L- Editor: A E- Editor: Jiao XK

References

  • 1.Harris EC, Barraclough B. Excess mortality of mental disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1998;173:11–53. doi: 10.1192/bjp.173.1.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Arcelus J, Mitchell AJ, Wales J, Nielsen S. Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders. A meta-analysis of 36 studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:724–731. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bastiani AM, Rao R, Weltzin T, Kaye WH. Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 1995;17:147–152. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199503)17:2<147::aid-eat2260170207>3.0.co;2-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Doll HA, Welch SL. Risk factors for anorexia nervosa: three integrated case-control comparisons. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:468–476. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.5.468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Wu M, Brockmeyer T, Hartmann M, Skunde M, Herzog W, Friederich HC. Set-shifting ability across the spectrum of eating disorders and in overweight and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44:3365–3385. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zastrow A, Kaiser S, Stippich C, Walther S, Herzog W, Tchanturia K, Belger A, Weisbrod M, Treasure J, Friederich HC. Neural correlates of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:608–616. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McAnarney ER, Zarcone J, Singh P, Michels J, Welsh S, Litteer T, Wang H, Klein JD. Restrictive anorexia nervosa and set-shifting in adolescents: a biobehavioral interface. J Adolesc Health. 2011;49:99–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.11.259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Steinglass JE, Walsh BT, Stern Y. Set shifting deficit in anorexia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12:431–435. doi: 10.1017/s1355617706060528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Chui HT, Christensen BK, Zipursky RB, Richards BA, Hanratty MK, Kabani NJ, Mikulis DJ, Katzman DK. Cognitive function and brain structure in females with a history of adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa. Pediatrics. 2008;122:e426–e437. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gillberg IC, Billstedt E, Wentz E, Anckarsäter H, Råstam M, Gillberg C. Attention, executive functions, and mentalizing in anorexia nervosa eighteen years after onset of eating disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;32:358–365. doi: 10.1080/13803390903066857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gillberg IC, Råstam M, Wentz E, Gillberg C. Cognitive and executive functions in anorexia nervosa ten years after onset of eating disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:170–178. doi: 10.1080/13803390600584632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jones BP, Duncan CC, Brouwers P, Mirsky AF. Cognition in eating disorders. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1991;13:711–728. doi: 10.1080/01688639108401085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kingston K, Szmukler G, Andrewes D, Tress B, Desmond P. Neuropsychological and structural brain changes in anorexia nervosa before and after refeeding. Psychol Med. 1996;26:15–28. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700033687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lauer CJ, Gorzewski B, Gerlinghoff M, Backmund H, Zihl J. Neuropsychological assessments before and after treatment in patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33:129–138. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(98)00020-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mathias JL, Kent PS. Neuropsychological consequences of extreme weight loss and dietary restriction in patients with anorexia nervosa. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998;20:548–564. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.4.548.1476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ohrmann P, Kersting A, Suslow T, Lalee-Mentzel J, Donges US, Fiebich M, Arolt V, Heindel W, Pfleiderer B. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in anorexia nervosa: correlations with cognition. Neuroreport. 2004;15:549–553. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200403010-00033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Szmukler GI, Andrewes D, Kingston K, Chen L, Stargatt R, Stanley R. Neuropsychological impairment in anorexia nervosa: before and after refeeding. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14:347–352. doi: 10.1080/01688639208402834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Thompson SBN. Implications of neuropsychological test results of women in a new phase of anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 1993;1:152–165. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, Essock S, Fenton WS, Frese FJ, Gold JM, et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:203–213. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Van Rheenen TE, Rossell SL. An empirical evaluation of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2014;16:318–325. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kaye JL, Dunlop BW, Iosifescu DV, Mathew SJ, Kelley ME, Harvey PD. Cognition, functional capacity, and self-reported disability in women with posttraumatic stress disorder: examining the convergence of performance-based measures and self-reports. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;57:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Murrough JW, Wan L-B, Iacoviello B, Collins KA, Solon C, Glicksberg B, Perez AM, Mathew SJ, Charney DS, Iosifescu DV. Neurocognitive effects of ketamine in treatment-resistant major depression: association with antidepressant response. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013:Epub ahead of print. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3255-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hatch A, Madden S, Kohn MR, Clarke S, Touyz S, Gordon E, Williams LM. In first presentation adolescent anorexia nervosa, do cognitive markers of underweight status change with weight gain following a refeeding intervention? Int J Eat Disord. 2010;43:295–306. doi: 10.1002/eat.20695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Andrés-Perpiña S, Lozano-Serra E, Puig O, Lera-Miguel S, Lázaro L, Castro-Fornieles J. Clinical and biological correlates of adolescent anorexia nervosa with impaired cognitive profile. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20:541–549. doi: 10.1007/s00787-011-0216-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tchanturia K, Morris RG, Anderluh MB, Collier DA, Nikolaou V, Treasure J. Set shifting in anorexia nervosa: an examination before and after weight gain, in full recovery and relationship to childhood and adult OCPD traits. J Psychiatr Res. 2004;38:545–552. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bradley SJ, Taylor MJ, Rovet JF, Goldberg E, Hood J, Wachsmuth R, Azcue MP, Pencharz PB. Assessment of brain function in adolescent anorexia nervosa before and after weight gain. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19:20–33. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fowler L, Blackwell A, Jaffa A, Palmer R, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ, Dowson JH. Profile of neurocognitive impairments associated with female in-patients with anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 2006;36:517–527. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kemps E, Tiggemann M, Wade T, Ben-Tovim D, Breyer R. Selective working memory deficits in anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2006;14:97–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Key A, O’Brien A, Gordon I, Christie D, Lask B. Assessment of neurobiology in adults with anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2006;14:308–314. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Harrison A, Sullivan S, Tchanturia K, Treasure J. Emotion recognition and regulation in anorexia nervosa. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2009;16:348–356. doi: 10.1002/cpp.628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hambrook D, Brown G, Tchanturia K. Emotional intelligence in anorexia nervosa: is anxiety a missing piece of the puzzle? Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:12–19. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Keys A, Brozek J, Henschel A. The Biology of Human Starvation. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press; 1950. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 20:22–33; quiz 34-57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading: WTAR: Psychological Corporation, 2001 [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fairburn CG. Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disorders. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hester RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B. Effect of age on forward and backward span tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:475–481. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Stedal K, Rose M, Frampton I, Landrø NI, Lask B. The neuropsychological profile of children, adolescents, and young adults with anorexia nervosa. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;27:329–337. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Tovée MJ, Emery JL, Cohen-Tovée EM. The estimation of body mass index and physical attractiveness is dependent on the observer’s own body mass index. Proc Biol Sci. 2000;267:1987–1997. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Tchanturia K, Anderluh MB, Morris RG, Rabe-Hesketh S, Collier DA, Sanchez P, Treasure JL. Cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:513–520. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tchanturia K, Serpell L, Troop N, Treasure J. Perceptual illusions in eating disorders: rigid and fluctuating styles. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2001;32:107–115. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7916(01)00025-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tan EJ, Rossell SL. Building a neurocognitive profile of thought disorder in schizophrenia using a standardized test battery. Schizophr Res. 2014;152:242–245. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C. Cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder on tests of frontal-striatal function. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43:348–357. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00201-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Krishna R, Udupa S, George CM, Kumar KJ, Viswanath B, Kandavel T, Venkatasubramanian G, Reddy YC. Neuropsychological performance in OCD: a study in medication-naïve patients. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:1969–1976. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dunai J, Labuschagne I, Castle DJ, Kyrios M, Rossell SL. Executive function in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychol Med. 2010;40:1541–1548. doi: 10.1017/S003329170999198X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Deckersbach T, Savage CR, Phillips KA, Wilhelm S, Buhlmann U, Rauch SL, Baer L, Jenike MA. Characteristics of memory dysfunction in body dysmorphic disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6:673–681. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700666055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hanes KR. Neuropsychological performance in body dysmorphic disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4:167–171. doi: 10.1017/s1355617798001672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Holden NL. Is anorexia nervosa an obsessive-compulsive disorder? Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:1–5. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from World Journal of Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Baishideng Publishing Group Inc

RESOURCES