Abstract
Hypersensitive C-reaction protein (hsCRP) may be a risk factor for cognitive impairment resulting from Alzheimer’s disease (AD), stroke, and vascular dementia. This study explored the correlation of peripheral blood hsCRP level with cognitive decline due to high altitude exposure. The study was conducted on 100 male military participants who had never been to high altitude. Cerebral oxygen saturation monitoring, event related potentials (P300, N200) detection, and neurocognitive assessment was performed and total hsCRP, interleukin-6 (IL-6), and homocysteine was estimated at 500m altitude, 3650m altitude, 3day, 1, and 3 month post arriving at the base camp (4400m), and 1 month after coming back to the 500m altitude. High altitude increased brain oxygen saturation, prolonged P300 and N200 latencies, injured cognitive functions, and raised plasma hsCRP levels. But they all recovered in varying degrees at 1 and 3 month post arriving at the base camp (4400m). P300 latencies and hsCRP levels were strongly correlated to cognitive performances. These results suggested that cognitive deterioration occurred during the acute period of exposure to high altitude and may recover probably owning to acclimatization after extended stay at high altitude. Plasma hsCRP is inversely correlated to neurological cognition and it may be a potential biomarker for the prediction of high altitude induced cognitive dysfunction.
Introduction
High altitude is a stressful condition which is always related to a decreased oxygen supply to tissues [1]. Past studies have demonstrated that human brain takes up only about 2% of the body weight, but it accounts for about 20% of the oxygen consumption [2]. Thus, Brain in particular is highly vulnerable to such hypoxia exposure at high altitude. A wide range of evidences have shown hypobaric hypoxia at high altitude results in a myriad of neurophysiological and neurocognitive dysfunction including vision, memory, language, mood, learning, and processing speed [3–5]. Further studies found that long term stay at high altitude caused psychological and cognitive impairment in adults [6, 7] while not in adolescents [8]. Moreover, neuropsychological deficits may be persistent even after coming back to sea level from high altitude regions [9]. With the continued growth of people travelling to high altitude due to various reasons, the cognitive impairments resulted from hypoxia are of major public health importance and have economic consequences.
Growing evidences suggest that inflammation is a risk factor for neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and vascular dementia [10, 11]. Another study clearly indicated increasing levels of C-reactive protein (CRP) were associated with brain white matter integrity loss in corticosubcortical pathways [12]. Besides, recent reports have demonstrated a positive relationship between peripheral blood levels of CRP [13, 14] and neurocognitive decline. Another inflammation biomarker interleukin-6 (IL-6) also correlated inversely with cognitive performance in middle and old aged people [15, 16]. In addition, previous data show that homocysteine, a product of methylation reactions, may contribute to the pathophysiology of stroke [17], Schizophrenia [18], and the cognitive changes associated with normal aging [19, 20]. However, the association of hypersensitive CRP (hsCRP), IL-6, and homocysteine in humans with cognitive function during longitudinal exposure to high altitude is still unknown and remains to be explored.
Event related potentials (ERP) including P300 and N200, indexes of the sensory speed and cognitive performance, can reflect cognitive functions like cognitive processing and performance monitoring objectively. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to investigate the longitudinal cognitive test performance of people after prolonged stay at high altitude accompanied by ERP detection and cerebral oxygen saturation monitoring and to explore its correlation with peripheral blood hsCRP, IL-6, and homocysteine. A longitudinal study was performed on a relatively large cohort of 100 Chinese lowlander population who travelled to an altitude of 4300–4500 m (Tibet regions) from 300 m altitude (Si-Chuang province, China P.R.) and stayed there for about 3 months.
Materials and Methods
The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki and the experimental design and procedures for conducting the experiment were approved by the ethics committee on human investigation of Southwest Hospital, Third Military Medical University. Informed written consent was obtained from all the participants or their caretakers (2 subjects aged 17) who volunteered for the study. Before giving written and verbal consent to participate, each volunteer was informed of the possible risk and discomforts involved in the study. They all agreed to join the study by signing the informed consent documents, which has been approved by the ethics committee.
Subjects
A total of 100 military participants comprising of male subjects with education of 9–16 yr and between age group of 17–34 yr enrolled voluntarily after being explained about the study purpose, protocol, and expected outcomes. They are lowlanders born and living at sea level below 500m and they are without any prior exposure to 4400 m altitude prior to their participation. They all have no documented neurological disorder or history of head injury with loss of consciousness and no previous history of drug abuse, stroke, epileptic seizures. The medical follow-up was performed during the expedition and physical examinations were used to determine health status.
Expedition to Qinghai-Tibet Plateau and experimental protocol
During days 1–3, the volunteers left Si-Chuang province (500m) for Lhasa, Tibet (3650 m) by train and stayed there for 7 days. Then the subjects spent 1 day reaching the base camp (4400m) and lived in there for about 3 months. They came back to Si-Chuang province approximately 3 months later and completed the expedition. Total 6 examinations including heart rate (HR), pulse oxygen saturation (SPO2), brain oxygen saturation, auditory evoked event-related potentials (ERP, including P300 and N200), peripheral blood hypersensitive C-reactive protein (hsCRP), homocysteine, and interleukin-6 (IL-6) accompanied by neurological cognitive tests such as digit symbol substitution test (DSST) was administered at 500m altitude (1 plain-first point), 3650m altitude (2 plateau- second point), during the follow-ups at 3day (3 plateau-third point), 1 (4 plateau-fourth point), and 3 (5 plateau-fifth point) month post arriving at the base camp (4400m), and about 1 month (6 plain-sixth point) after coming back to the 500m altitude (Fig 1).
Examinations of HR, SPO2, and brain oxygen saturation
One kind of oximeter (MD300C29, Chaosi corp. ltd, Peking, China) was used to non-invasively record HR and SPO2 at rest with probes attached to ring finger. Once the signal was steady, HR and SPO2 was produced by the apparatus.
Brain oxygen saturation monitor using non-invasive near-infrared spectroscopy technique (MNIR-P100, MingXi Corp. ltd, ChongQing, China) was applied to detect the brain frontal cortex oxygen saturation at rest with probes placed at the forehead. Left and right brain oxygen saturation was recorded once the signal was stable.
Digit symbol substitution test (DSST)
DSST is a paper-and-pencil test derived from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. This examination measures response, attention, and processing speeds [21]. The test consists of nine digit-symbol pairs followed by a list of digits. Under each digit, the subject must write down the corresponding symbol as quickly as possible. In our study, the number of correct responses in 180s was recorded as the score. All subjects were seated in a conference room and took the test at the same time.
Auditory evoked ERP (P300 and N200)
The test was performed using an auditory evoked potentials system (Keypoint 9033A07, Alpine Corp., Denmark) in an electrically shielded sound-proof room, with the volunteers lying comfortably, with eyes closed, in order to eliminate the artifacts caused by eye movement. Electrodes were positioned as follows: reference electrode situated at Cz, recording electrode linked to right and left earlobes. The impedance between electrodes was less than 5 KΩ. The click stimulus was presented through an earphone. The following parameters were used for the acquisition of P300 and N200: A random sequence of binaural tones (60 dB, 10 ms rise and fall, 50 ms plateau time) was presented, with a standard tone (1000 Hz) 70% of the time and a target tone (2000 Hz) 30% of the time. The latency of the largest positive potential occurring between 250 and 500 ms was designated the P300 component. The latency of the largest negative potential occurring between 175 and 250 ms was designated the N200 component.
Biochemical tests
The blood samples for homocysteine and hsCRP test were taken into tubes containing dry heparin from the volunteers in aseptic conditions by venous puncture after an overnight fast and were immediately centrifuged at +4°C. Plasma was then separated and collected in another tube for freezing at -20°C, and stored at this temperature until assay. Serum for IL-6 test was extracted from blood samples by centrifugation and stored at -20°C for assay.
The homocysteine levels in plasma were determined by an assay kit (JiuQiang Corp. ltd, Peking, China) using biochemistry analyzer (AU5400, Olympus, Japan). The plasma hsCRP levels were determined by a kit (Orion Diagnostica Oy, Finland) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Serum IL-6 was checked by an assay kit (Roche, Switzerland) with a fully automatic electrochemical luminescent immune analyzer (E170, Roche, Switzerland).
Statistical analysis
All data were reported as means ± SD. Statistical analysis of the data was performed by repeated measure ANOVA using software SPSS 18.0. The correlation among the DSST performance, P300, and hsCRP was analyzed using Pearson method with software SPSS 18.0. A probability value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Changes of HR, SPO2, and brain oxygen saturation
The HR, SPO2, and brain oxygen saturation was depicted in Fig 2. The HR declined gradually after a rise during the stay at high altitude. At the second point (2 plateau), HR was significantly higher than that of the first point. Then, HR showed a downward trend. Even at the sixth point, around 1 month after coming back to the 500m altitude, it continued to decrease and differed statistically from that of the first point.
SPO2, an indicator of tissue oxygen, displayed a downward state at high altitude, although it increased at 1 and 3 month post arriving at the base camp (4 plateau and 5 plateau). SPO2 of all the four high altitude time points was statistically different from that of the first point (Fig 2).
We further examined brain oxygen saturation directly using non-invasive near -infrared spectroscopy technique. The left and right brain oxygen saturation increased at the second, third, and fourth point, which was significantly higher than that at the first point. At the fifth (5 plateau) and the sixth (6 plain) point, the left brain oxygen nearly returned to normal, while the right brain oxygen was still at high level (Fig 2).
High altitude prolonged latencies of P300 and N200
Then P300 and N200 was quantitatively detected by auditory evoked ERP (Fig 3). The latencies of P300 prolonged at the second and third point, which were significantly longer than that of the first point respectively. Afterwards, the latencies shortened at the fourth, fifth, and sixth points, which showed no statistical significances compared to that of the first point.
N200 latency went up at the second point and increased much more at the third point than at the second. At the sixth point N200 latency returned to normal level. There was no statistical significance in N200 latency between the sixth and first point.
High altitude increased plasma hsCRP levels
Biochemical parameters such as hsCRP, homocysteine, and interleukin-6 (IL-6) were then investigated (Fig 4). hsCRP augmented evidently at the second and third point, which was much higher than that of the first point. Then, it showed a downward trend. At the sixth point, it returned to normal, which were statistically different from that at the second and third point. There was no statistical difference while compared with that of the first point.
The changes of homocysteine and IL-6 were also shown in Fig 4. They revealed no obvious alteration regularity.
High altitude injured neurological cognitive functions
DSST was used for the evaluation of neurological cognitive functions (Fig 5). DSST score reduced at the second point and decreased much more at the third point than at the second. Scores at the fourth, fifth, and sixth point increased, which have no statistical differences compared to that of the first point.
Association among biochemical parameters, ERP, and cognitive performance
Overall, P300 and hsCRP displayed the same changing tendency, which was opposite to the alteration of DSST score. Fig 6 illustrated Pearson’s correlation between DSST score and plasma hsCRP, a strong inverse correlation (correlation coefficient, CC = -0.912; p = 0.011) was observed between them. No significant correlation was found between DSST score and homocysteine or IL-6 (data not shown). P300 latency was also inversely correlated with DSST score (CC = -0.918; p = 0.010). In addition, P300 latency showed a positive association with hsCRP (CC = 0.963; p = 0.002).
Discussion
The study provided evidence that high altitude increased brain oxygen saturation, prolonged P300 and N200 latencies, injured cognitive functions, and increased plasma hsCRP levels. But they all recovered in varying degrees at 1 and 3 month post arriving at the base camp (4400m). P300 latencies and hsCRP levels were strongly correlated to cognitive performances. These data clearly indicate that cognitive impairments occur during the acute and not the prolonged duration of exposure to altitude. Plasma hsCRP may be a potential biomarker for the prediction of high altitude induced cognitive dysfunction.
In agreement with other studies [22], resting heart rate was significantly higher upon arrival at high altitude than at 500m altitude. Then it declined gradually even after rise of altitude (4300-4500m). About 1 month post coming back to the 500m altitude, it continued to decrease, which was statistically different from that of the 500m altitude. Interestingly, most military volunteers developed bradycardia 1 month after departing from altitude. In contrast to the changes of pulse oxygen saturation, the left and right brain oxygen saturation increased at 3 day upon arrival at high altitude and 3 day and 1 month post arriving at the base camp (4400m) compared with baseline values. Three month after ascent to the altitude, left brain oxygen saturation nearly returned to normal, while the right brain oxygen was still at high level. However, past studies demonstrated the resting regional brain oxygen saturation in the frontal cortex decreased progressively following ascent to high altitudes [23, 24], which was inconsistent with the present results. The method monitoring brain oxygen saturation applied in these studies are the same near-infrared spectroscopy, which is a non-invasive technique based on the differential absorptive properties of oxyhaemoglobin and deoxyhaemoglobin for near infrared light (700–1100 nm) and has been used extensively in research despite its poor clinical use. The underlying reason for the difference is not well known and deserves further research. It may be related to the good physical fitness of the subjects in this study. In addition, we found an interesting phenomenon that the right brain oxygen was still at high level even 3 months after ascent to high altitude. The potential mechanism remains unclear which may be associated with different function of the left and right brain and needs to be elucidated in future study.
Then the cognitive potentials including P300 and N200 were quantitatively detected by auditory evoked ERP. ERP components including P300 and N200, indexes of the sensory speed and cognitive performance, enables direct examination of mental operations ranging from cognitive processing to performance monitoring [25]. Previous studies have found that acute exposure to a hypoxic environment through fast altitude gain or hypobaric hypoxia induces increases of ERP latency, particularly for the P300 [26, 27]. However, the time course of ERP alteration at high altitude is still unclear. Our study showed that P300 latency prolonged after ascent to high altitude and recovered after extended exposure. It returned to normal at 1 and 3 month post staying at altitude. N200 latency displayed a similar changing trend to P300. This suggests people with chronic exposure to high altitude hypoxia could produce acclimatization or adaptation that occurs after a relatively short period of time, which is in line with the past report by Singh et al [26].
The relationship between inflammation and cognitive function has been extensively studied in recent years. Although there are some negative results [28], most obtained studies suggest the positive associations. Kliper and colleagues [29] reported that higher levels of C-reactive protein were associated with worse performance in cognitive tests after stroke. Further research demonstrated high hsCRP may be a marker of memory and visuo-spatial impairment in the elderly [13]. The mechanisms that hsCRP induces cognitive impairment may involve cytotoxicity and structural brain changes from inflammation [30]. An obvious association between smaller brain volumes as measured via magnetic resonance imaging and higher levels of CRP was observed by Jefferson et al and Satizabal et al [31, 32]. The present study found plasma hsCRP increased evidently following ascent to high altitude. Then, it decreased and returned to normal level after 3 months’ exposure to high altitude. Furthermore, hsCRP levels demonstrated an inverse strong correlation with cognitive scores and a positive significant association with P300 latencies, which is the index of cognitive performance. These findings indicate that hsCRP may be the possible marker of cognitive decline associated with acute exposure to high altitude.
IL6 is another inflammation cytokine that is thought to be related to cognitive performance. Previous report revealed that patients with vascular dementia had elevated IL-6 values [33]. Additionally, Jordanova and colleagues reported an association between cognitive activity and IL-6 in an African-Caribbean populations aged 55–75 years old [34]. However, this study did not replicate the above results in people during rapid ascent to high altitude, although there was a major change in IL-6 levels. We did not find the rules underlying the changes especially its alteration tendency similar to the cognitive changes. There was no statistical significance in the correlation between IL-6 and DSST performance.
Past reports have shown that elevation of homocysteine is associated with age related neurological disorders and chronic stress models [35]. Moreover, a recent study provided evidences that increases in circulation homocysteine were positively correlated to cognitive damage and duration of stay at high altitude [36]. Contrary to these findings, the present results revealed no elevation of plasma homocysteine at the initial ascent to altitude, which increased after prolonged (1 month and 3month) duration. However, Similar to IL-6, the changing tendency of homocysteine was not related to the changes of cognitive results without statistical significance.
The present study, to the best of our knowledge, is the first report on relationships among hsCRP, P300, and cognitive impairment of lowlander natives following extended stay for 3 months at high altitude. The results showed there were mutual correlations among them. These findings could be of considerable clinical relevance for occupational health at high altitude and have major public health importance with the sustained rise of people going to high altitude for different purposes.
In conclusion, this study reveals the novel findings that cognitive dysfunction occurs during the acute period of exposure to high altitude and may recover probably due to acclimatization after prolonged stay. Plasma hsCRP is strongly correlated to neurological cognition and it may be a potential biomarker for cognitive decline resulted from high altitude. A shortage of this study is that the cerebral oxygen saturation we examined is contrary to previous studies. The underlying reason is unknown and further research is needed to be explored in future work.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81471261), the major special program of PLA in China (No. AWS12J004), and Southwest Hospital Science Program (No. SWH2014JS01).
References
- 1.Savourey G, Launay JC, Besnard Y, Guinet A, Travers S. Normo- and hypobaric hypoxia: are there any physiological differences? Eur J Appl Physiol. 2003;89(2):122–6. Epub 2003/04/01. 10.1007/s00421-002-0789-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Raichle ME. Two views of brain function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(4):180–90. Epub 2010/03/09. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008 S1364-6613(10)00029-X [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Regard M, Oelz O, Brugger P, Landis T. Persistent cognitive impairment in climbers after repeated exposure to extreme altitude. Neurology. 1989;39(2 Pt 1):210–3. Epub 1989/02/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Shukitt-Hale B, Stillman MJ, Welch DI, Levy A, Devine JA, Lieberman HR. Hypobaric hypoxia impairs spatial memory in an elevation-dependent fashion. Behav Neural Biol. 1994;62(3):244–52. Epub 1994/11/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wilson MH, Newman S, Imray CH. The cerebral effects of ascent to high altitudes. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8(2):175–91. Epub 2009/01/24. 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70014-6 S1474-4422(09)70014-6 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Davis JE, Wagner DR, Garvin N, Moilanen D, Thorington J, Schall C. Cognitive and psychomotor responses to high-altitude exposure in sea level and high-altitude residents of Ecuador. J Physiol Anthropol. 2015;34:2 Epub 2015/02/05. 10.1186/s40101-014-0039-x s40101-014-0039-x [pii]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gao YX, Li P, Jiang CH, Liu C, Chen Y, Chen L, et al. Psychological and cognitive impairment of long-term migrators to high altitudes and the relationship to physiological and biochemical changes. Eur J Neurol. 2015;22(10):1363–9. Epub 2014/07/22. 10.1111/ene.12507 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Richardson C, Hogan AM, Bucks RS, Baya A, Virues-Ortega J, Holloway JW, et al. Neurophysiological evidence for cognitive and brain functional adaptation in adolescents living at high altitude. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122(9):1726–34. Epub 2011/03/08. 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.02.001 S1388-2457(11)00102-7 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bonnon M, Noel-Jorand MC, Therme P. Effects of different stay durations on attentional performance during two mountain expeditions. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2000;71(7):678–84. Epub 2000/07/21. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pratico D, Trojanowski JQ. Inflammatory hypotheses: novel mechanisms of Alzheimer's neurodegeneration and new therapeutic targets? Neurobiol Aging. 2000;21(3):441–5; discussion 51–3. Epub 2000/06/20. S0197-4580(00)00141-X [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Schmidt R, Schmidt H, Curb JD, Masaki K, White LR, Launer LJ. Early inflammation and dementia: a 25-year follow-up of the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Ann Neurol. 2002;52(2):168–74. Epub 2002/09/05. 10.1002/ana.10265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Miralbell J, Soriano JJ, Spulber G, Lopez-Cancio E, Arenillas JF, Bargallo N, et al. Structural brain changes and cognition in relation to markers of vascular dysfunction. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(5):1003 e9–17. Epub 2011/10/22. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.09.020 S0197-4580(11)00357-5 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Noble JM, Manly JJ, Schupf N, Tang MX, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Association of C-reactive protein with cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):87–92. Epub 2010/01/13. 10.1001/archneurol.2009.308 67/1/87 [pii]. . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bettcher BM, Wilheim R, Rigby T, Green R, Miller JW, Racine CA, et al. C-reactive protein is related to memory and medial temporal brain volume in older adults. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26(1):103–8. Epub 2011/08/17. 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.07.240 S0889-1591(11)00480-6 [pii]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marsland AL, Petersen KL, Sathanoori R, Muldoon MF, Neumann SA, Ryan C, et al. Interleukin-6 covaries inversely with cognitive performance among middle-aged community volunteers. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(6):895–903. Epub 2006/11/30. 68/6/895 [pii] 10.1097/01.psy.0000238451.22174.92 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mooijaart SP, Sattar N, Trompet S, Lucke J, Stott DJ, Ford I, et al. Circulating interleukin-6 concentration and cognitive decline in old age: the PROSPER study. J Intern Med. 2013;274(1):77–85. Epub 2013/02/19. 10.1111/joim.12052 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vermeer SE, van Dijk EJ, Koudstaal PJ, Oudkerk M, Hofman A, Clarke R, et al. Homocysteine, silent brain infarcts, and white matter lesions: The Rotterdam Scan Study. Ann Neurol. 2002;51(3):285–9. Epub 2002/03/14. 10.1002/ana.10111 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kinoshita M, Numata S, Tajima A, Shimodera S, Imoto I, Ohmori T. Plasma total homocysteine is associated with DNA methylation in patients with schizophrenia. Epigenetics. 2013;8(6):584–90. Epub 2013/06/19. doi: 10.4161/epi.24621 24621 [pii]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wright CB, Lee HS, Paik MC, Stabler SP, Allen RH, Sacco RL. Total homocysteine and cognition in a tri-ethnic cohort: the Northern Manhattan Study. Neurology. 2004;63(2):254–60. Epub 2004/07/28. 63/2/254 [pii]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vidal JS, Dufouil C, Ducros V, Tzourio C. Homocysteine, folate and cognition in a large community-based sample of elderly people—the 3C Dijon Study. Neuroepidemiology. 2008;30(4):207–14. Epub 2008/04/22. doi: 10.1159/000126914 000126914 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wang J, Ke T, Zhang X, Chen Y, Liu M, Chen J, et al. Effects of acetazolamide on cognitive performance during high-altitude exposure. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;35:28–33. Epub 2013/01/03. 10.1016/j.ntt.2012.12.003 S0892-0362(12)00179-1 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rostrup M. Catecholamines, hypoxia and high altitude. Acta Physiol Scand. 1998;162(3):389–99. Epub 1998/05/13. 10.1046/j.1365-201X.1998.00335.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Imray CH, Barnett NJ, Walsh S, Clarke T, Morgan J, Hale D, et al. Near-infrared spectroscopy in the assessment of cerebral oxygenation at high altitude. Wilderness Environ Med. 1998;9(4):198–203. Epub 2002/05/07. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hadolt I, Litscher G. Noninvasive assessment of cerebral oxygenation during high altitude trekking in the Nepal Himalayas (2850–5600 m). Neurol Res. 2003;25(2):183–8. Epub 2003/03/15. 10.1179/016164103101201175 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hayashi R. Correlation between coefficient of variation of choice reaction time and components of event-related potentials (P300): effect of benzodiazepine. J Neurol Sci. 2000;178(1):52–6. Epub 2000/10/06. S0022-510X(00)00362-2 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Singh SB, Thakur L, Anand JP, Panjwani U, Yadav D, Selvamurthy W. Effect of high altitude (HA) on event related brain potentials. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2003;47(1):52–8. Epub 2003/04/24. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hayashi R, Matsuzawa Y, Kubo K, Kobayashi T. Effects of simulated high altitude on event-related potential (P300) and auditory brain-stem responses. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116(6):1471–6. Epub 2005/06/28. S1388-2457(05)00075-1 [pii] 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.02.020 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gorelick PB. Role of inflammation in cognitive impairment: results of observational epidemiological studies and clinical trials. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1207:155–62. Epub 2010/10/20. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05726.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kliper E, Bashat DB, Bornstein NM, Shenhar-Tsarfaty S, Hallevi H, Auriel E, et al. Cognitive decline after stroke: relation to inflammatory biomarkers and hippocampal volume. Stroke. 2013;44(5):1433–5. Epub 2013/02/28. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.000536 STROKEAHA.111.000536 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Goldstein FC, Zhao L, Steenland K, Levey AI. Inflammation and cognitive functioning in African Americans and Caucasians. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014. Epub 2014/12/17. 10.1002/gps.4238 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jefferson AL, Massaro JM, Wolf PA, Seshadri S, Au R, Vasan RS, et al. Inflammatory biomarkers are associated with total brain volume: the Framingham Heart Study. Neurology. 2007;68(13):1032–8. Epub 2007/03/29. 68/13/1032 [pii] 10.1212/01.wnl.0000257815.20548.df [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Satizabal CL, Zhu YC, Mazoyer B, Dufouil C, Tzourio C. Circulating IL-6 and CRP are associated with MRI findings in the elderly: the 3C-Dijon Study. Neurology. 2012;78(10):720–7. Epub 2012/02/24. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318248e50f WNL.0b013e318248e50f [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zuliani G, Ranzini M, Guerra G, Rossi L, Munari MR, Zurlo A, et al. Plasma cytokines profile in older subjects with late onset Alzheimer's disease or vascular dementia. J Psychiatr Res. 2007;41(8):686–93. Epub 2006/04/08. S0022-3956(06)00052-5 [pii] 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.02.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jordanova V, Stewart R, Davies E, Sherwood R, Prince M. Markers of inflammation and cognitive decline in an African-Caribbean population. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;22(10):966–73. Epub 2007/03/09. 10.1002/gps.1772 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sudduth TL, Powell DK, Smith CD, Greenstein A, Wilcock DM. Induction of hyperhomocysteinemia models vascular dementia by induction of cerebral microhemorrhages and neuroinflammation. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2013;33(5):708–15. Epub 2013/01/31. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.1 jcbfm20131 [pii]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sharma VK, Das SK, Dhar P, Hota KB, Mahapatra BB, Vashishtha V, et al. Domain specific changes in cognition at high altitude and its correlation with hyperhomocysteinemia. PLoS One. 2014;9(7):e101448 Epub 2014/07/06. 10.1371/journal.pone.0101448 PONE-D-14-06996 [pii]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper.