Abstract
Fat is an important source of both pleasure and calories in the diet. Dairy products are a major source of fat in the diet, and understanding preferences for fat in fluid milk can potentially inform efforts to change fat consumption patterns or optimize consumer products. Here, patterns of preference for fat in milk were determined in the laboratory among 100 free living adults using rejection thresholds. Participants also answered questions relating to their health concerns, the type of fluid milk typically consumed, and their declared preference for type of milk (in terms of fat level). When revealed preferences in blind tasting were stratified by these measures, we observed striking differences in the preferred level of fat in milk. These data indicate a non-trivial number of consumers who prefer low-fat milk to full fat milk, a pattern that would have been overshadowed by the use of a group mean. While it is widely assumed and claimed that increasing fat content in fluid milk universally increases palatability, present data demonstrate this is not true for a segment of the population. These results underscore the need to go look beyond group means to understand individual differences in food preference.
Keywords: milk fat, preference, consumer rejection threshold, health consciousness
1. Introduction
Fat is responsible for many sensory attributes in foods, affecting appearance, texture, flavor, and palatability (Mela, 1990; Richardson-Harman et al., 2000). Previously, many researchers have examined the sensory properties of milk with varying fat content, and it is clear that milk fat plays a role in appearance and texture attributes in fluid milk (Pangborn & Dunkley, 1964; Drewnowski & Greenwood, 1983; Pangborn & Giovanni, 1984; Mela, 1988; Pangborn, Bos, & Stern, 1985). Beyond appearance and texture, dairy fats are somewhat unique in that they also make a direct contribution to sensory aroma and flavor perception (Kinsella, Patton, & Dimick, 1967; Badings & Neeter, 1980; Tamsma, Kurtz, Bright, & Pallansch, 1969). A half century ago, Pangborn and Dunkley showed that panelists could detect less than 0.5% milk fat added to skim milk. As visual differences were masked in their study, one can presume that flavor, aromatic, and textural cues were primarily used to discern these differences (Pangborn & Dunkley, 1964; Drewnowski & Greenwood, 1983; Pangborn & Giovanni, 1984; Pangborn et al., 1985; Mela, 1988). Using descriptive analysis with 15 trained panelists, Tuorila investigated sensory differences between 0%, 1.9%, and 3.9% fat milk in Finland; the panelists identified significant differences in blue color, transparency, visual thickness, and greasy mouthfeel. Conversely, the panel did not note any significant differences in taste, flavor, or aroma attributes (Tuorila, 1987). More recently, trained panelists performing descriptive analysis were able to discern flavor differences, with increasing fat content significantly increasing creamy flavor and sweet taste and significantly decreasing boiled milk flavor (Frøst, Dijksterhuis, & Martens, 2001). Other research suggest naïve consumers are also able to accurately judge fat content differences in fluid dairy products (Mela, 1988). This discrimination is far from perfect, however. Chapman and Lawless estimated only 30% discriminators among the subjects completing a triangle test comparing skim and 2% fat milk (Chapman & Lawless, 2005). Even in a dual standards test, where subjects were presented with references for both skim milk and 2% milk and asked to match samples to those reference samples, discrimination was estimated at 76% (chance corrected) (Chapman & Lawless, 2005).
A handful of studies have investigated the effects of milk fat content on product liking and consumption. Tuorila measured milk liking of 236 subjects who regularly used nonfat (0.5% fat), low-fat (1.9% fat), or regular fat milk (3.9%) and found that subjects generally best liked the type of milk that they regularly consumed (Tuorila, 1987). In contrast, Chapman and Lawless examined preferences between 2% milk and non-fat milk among both non-fat and 2% fat milk consumers and found a preference for 2% milk in both groups (Chapman & Lawless, 2005). In a female Korean population, participants also showed generally higher liking for higher fat milks (4.0% fat) compared to lower fat milks (1.0% fat) (Chung, 2009). In another Korean study relating descriptive sensory attributes to consumer overall liking ratings, Lee and colleagues found cooked, creamy, and sweet attributes associated with increased ratings of overall liking (Lee, Lee, & Shin, 2003). Richardson-Harman and colleagues examined consumer creaminess perception, consumer overall liking, and trained panel descriptive profiles in New Zealand, using thickened and unthickened fresh and reconstituted fluid dairy beverages ranging from 0.1% fat to 40% fat. They found that consumers generally defined creaminess in a similar manner. Consumer ratings of creaminess were correlated with trained panel ratings of cream aroma and flavor, butter aroma and flavor, vanilla flavor, and oily/greasy, mouthcoating, slippery, and viscous textures. Consumer segments for overall liking were found with one segment preferring full fat beverages and another preferring low fat beverages (Richardson-Harman et al., 2000). Individual differences in liking of dairy products varying by fat content have been seen to vary due to PROP phenotype (Hayes & Duffy, 2008; Keller, Steinmann, Nurse, & Tepper, 2002), weight status (Pangborn et al., 1985), health status (Rapp et al., 2009), and level of fat consumption (Pangborn & Giovanni, 1984; Mattes, 1993), but the levels of fat investigated vary considerably between studies, often including levels of fat that would not typically found in beverages. Other research has shown that health attitudes can influence consumers’ perception and acceptance of milks of varying fat content (Shepherd, Sparks, Bellier, & Raats, 1991; Roininen, Lähteenmäki, & Tuorila, 1999).
The food environment and attitudes toward food and nutrition have both changed considerably since much of the research in this area was conducted. Over the past 25 years, milk consumption has decreased, and many more consumers have shifted from consuming whole milk to reduced-fat and skim milks (Briefel & Johnson, 2004). This is in part due to changes in dietary recommendations beginning in the 1980s, which were based on research linking fat consumption to cardiovascular disease and obesity. Adoption of a low-fat diet “became an overarching ideology, promoted by physicians, the federal government, the food industry, and the popular health media” (La Berge, 2008). More recent investigations into the role of dairy fat into the development of cardiovascular disease and obesity have called into question this association (German et al., 2009; Kratz, Baars, & Guyenet, 2013). Health concerns and consciousness (founded or not) can influence food choices (Hearty, McCarthy, Kearney, & Gibney, 2007; Sun, 2008), leading consumers to sometimes choose foods on the basis of health or weight consciousness rather than taste preferences per se (Visschers & Siegris, 2010). Given that food choices are influenced by such non-hedonic factors, grouping people based on their typical habits in addition to hedonics may give additional insights into the food choices and preferences for these individuals.
In 2005, Prescott and colleagues (Prescott, Norris, Kunst, & Kim, 2005) introduced the concept of a rejection threshold, which is defined as the concentration at which a substance becomes objectionable, rather than the concentration at which it is perceived (the detection threshold). Several other authors have used the method to study rejection of objectionable compounds in foods (Nikolantonaki & Darriet, 2011; Saliba, Bullock, & Hardie, 2009; Weekes, Walsh, Ferguson, & Ross, 2010; Yoo, Saliba, Prenzler, & Ryan, 2012). Harwood and colleagues showed the method could be extended by segmenting consumers by self-reported a priori preferences, demonstrating that compared to solid milk chocolate consumers, solid dark chocolate consumers tolerated a higher level of a bitterant in fluid chocolate milk (Harwood, Ziegler, & Hayes, 2012). However, use of this method is not limited to off flavors and taints. Blackman and coworkers examined sweetness tolerance in wine and found evidence for segmentation among novice wine consumers, experienced wine consumers, and wine experts. They also found that added sweetness was preferred at lower levels and became less tolerated at higher levels, applying this method to a situation where the added ingredient was not strictly objectionable (Blackman, Saliba, & Schmidtke, 2010).
Here, we use the rejection threshold method to further understand individual differences in preferences for fat in fluid milk within a convenience sample of adults in North American. We stratified our participants in terms of 1) self reported type of milk typically consumed, 2) stated preference for type of milk based on its taste, and 3) health consciousness. A priori, we hypothesized that patterns of milk preference determined in a blind taste test (i.e., revealed preferences) will better align with stated (declared) preferences than with the type of milk typically consumed. That is, those who report a preference for skim milk would prefer lower fat levels than whole or 2% milk preferers, but those who report drinking skim milk would not necessarily prefer lower fat levels. We also expected that health consciousness scores would be related to type of milk typically consumed milk type and not necessarily related to stated milk preference.
2. Methods
2.1 Ethics Statement
Testing was performed in a single session in the Sensory Evaluation Center in the Department of Food Science at The Pennsylvania State University. Procedures were exempted from Institutional Review Board review by The Pennsylvania State University Office of Research Protections under the wholesome foods/approved food additives exemption in 45 CFR 46.101(b)(6). Participants provided informed, implied consent and were paid for their time.
2.2 Participants
One hundred non-smoking adults (32 men) who regularly consumed dairy milk were recruited from the Pennsylvania State University campus and surrounding community (State College, PA). Forty-five participants reported typically drinking skim milk, 40 participants reported typically drinking 2% milk, and 15 participants reported typically drinking whole milk. Twenty-two participants reported preferring the taste of skim milk, 45 participants reported preferring the taste of 2% milk, and 31 participants reported preferring the taste of whole milk.
2.3 Stimuli
Samples were prepared one day prior to the experiment using freshly processed whole and skim milks from the Berkey Creamery (University Park, PA). Samples were standardized to 0.1, 0.5, 1, 2, and 3.5% milk fat using the Pearson square method (Olson, 1924). Fat content was verified with NMR analysis in a SMART Trac II system (CEM Corporation, Matthews, NC). These concentrations were selected to represent commercially available milk fat levels in the United States: 0.1% and 0.5% milk fat correspond to skim milk and 3.5% milk fat corresponds to whole milk, as legally defined by United States Food and Drug Administration (Food and Drug Administration, 1999)
2.4 Procedure
An ascending series of two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) preference tests were administered in accordance with the American Society for Testing and Materials method E-2263 (ASTM, 2004). Five pairs of samples were presented to each participant. Each pair contained an 0.1% fat sample (very low-fat control) and a sample where the fat level was varied systematically. Pairs were presented on one tray in ascending fat levels. Within pairs, sample order was counterbalanced. Participants were instructed to taste samples in order within each pair and indicate the sample they preferred before moving onto the next pair of samples. Samples were served cold and participants were allowed to retaste. Participants were also instructed to rinse their mouths with provided water between tastings with a twenty second forced time delay between pairs. Samples were presented under normal white lighting in individual booths with computers running Compusense Five v5.2 (Guelph, ON) for data collection and instruction. After tasting all pairs of samples, participants rated their level of concern regarding food and health issues (Table 2) on 9-point scales anchored at ‘I am not all concerned” on the left end, “Neutral” in the middle, and “I am extremely concerned” on the right end (Päivi, Tuorila, & Rita, 1996). Finally, participants indicated the type of milk they typically drank and the type of milk they preferred to drink based on its taste.
Table 2.
When thinking about your own health, how concerned are you about the following issues? |
Getting a lot of salt in my food |
Getting a lot of fat in my food |
Getting a lot of sugar in my food |
Getting many calories |
Getting sufficient energy from my food |
Food additives in my food |
Risk for high blood pressure |
Risk for coronary heart disease |
Getting a lot of cholesterol in my food |
Gaining weight |
2.5 Data Analysis
All analyses were conducted using SAS® version 9.2 (Cary, NC). A composite health consciousness score was calculated by summing all items (Table 2). Participants were then divided into three groups based on the summed scores: high health consciousness (≥ 71), medium health consciousness (<71 and >57) and low health consciousness (≤ 57). To test the hypothesis that milk type typically consumed, stated milk preference, and health consciousness would predict preferences for fat in milk, logistic regression was performed via PROC LOGISTIC statement on the following models:
Sample preferred (control or fat added) = % milk fat + milk type typically consumed + % milk fat * milk type typically consumed
Sample preferred (control or fat added) = % milk fat + stated milk preference + % milk fat *stated milk preference
Sample preferred (control or fat added) = % milk fat + health consciousness group + % milk fat *health consciousness group.
When appropriate (i.e. a chance adjusted proportion of 75% or 25% was achieved), rejection thresholds were calculated by using the PROC REG statement to regress the proportion of participants preferring the higher fat sample to the control sample (75%) or the proportion of participants preferring the control sample to the higher fat sample (25%) against fat concentration. Data for the 0.1% fat milk sample compared to itself was omitted from the model, as this should occur at chance. The resulting regression equation from the model was used to solve for the concentration at which 75% percent or 25% of the subjects preferred one sample over another (50% preference corrected for chance) defined as the ‘consumer rejection threshold’ (CRT). Because in some scenarios the test samples (added fat) were actually preferred to control samples, it became necessary to differentiate between the low fat rejection threshold (added milk fat preferred to low fat control) and the high fat rejection threshold (low fat control preferred to added milk fat). For individual paired comparison tests at a given concentration, the binomial distribution was used to test for significance among a given consumer segment.
To determine the effects of gender, milk type typically consumed, and stated milk preference on health consciousness ratings, the PROC ANOVA statement with the following model was used:
When appropriate post-hoc Tukey’s analyses were performed to identify inter-group differences. Because of the limited number of subjects within the group, participants who stated a preference for a type of milk other that skim, 2%, or whole milk were excluded from this analysis.
3. Results
3.1 Milk Fat Preference
While the combined data for all participants indicate a slight increase in preference with increasing fat content, preference varied as a function of the interaction between milk fat concentration and type of milk typically consumed (Wald Chi-Square = 12.9, p = 0.0016) and the interaction between milk fat concentration and stated milk preference (Wald Chi-square = 11.0, p =0.0041). Conversely, there was no evidence that health consciousness group influenced revealed preferences in the blind taste taste (Wald Chi-square < 1.1, p > 0.57). Table 3 shows the proportion of participants from each group selecting the added fat sample over the low fat control (0.1% fat).
Table 3.
Stratification | Number of subjects | Propotion preferring 0.1%* | Propotion preferring 0.5%* | Propotion preferring 1.0%* | Propotion preferring 2.0%* | Propotion preferring 3.5%* |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total Group | 104 | 0.50 | 0.51 | 0.55 | 0.55 | 0.63 |
Prefer Skim | 22 | 0.50 | 0.45 | 0.50 | 0.41 | 0.27 |
Prefer 2% | 45 | 0.49 | 0.49 | 0.56 | 0.56 | 0.67 |
Prefer Whole | 31 | 0.45 | 0.58 | 0.61 | 0.65 | 0.84 |
Drink Skim | 45 | 0.53 | 0.47 | 0.53 | 0.44 | 0.47 |
Drink 2% | 40 | 0.48 | 0.53 | 0.55 | 0.63 | 0.73 |
Drink Whole | 15 | 0.40 | 0.60 | 0.73 | 0.80 | 1.0 |
Low HC | 34 | 0.50 | 0.47 | 0.47 | 0.42 | 0.56 |
Medium HC | 34 | 0.47 | 0.53 | 0.56 | 0.56 | 0.65 |
High HC | 36 | 0.53 | 0.53 | 0.62 | 0.68 | 0.71 |
vs. a control sample containing 0.1% Fat
Bolded proportions are significant at p < 0.05 using the binomial distribution for paired preference tests.
As shown in Figure 1, individuals who typically consume skim milk showed no significant differences in liking between control and added fat milks, with a slight trend toward a slight decrease in preference as fat content increased. Conversely, individuals with a stated preference for the taste of skim milk showed a steep decline in preference with increasing fat content (see Figure 2), with a significant preference for the 0.1% fat sample over the 3.5% fat sample [16 of 22; binomial p=0.053]. Although this group did not quite meet reach a chance adjusted preference of 75% (our a priori definition for rejection threshold) in observed dagta, a high fat rejection threshold was estimated at 3.8% milk fat, just outside the range of samples tested.
As shown in Figures 1 and 2, participants who typically drink 2% milk [29 of 40] and the participants with a stated preference for 2% milk [30 of 45] significantly preferred 3.5% milk to 0.1% fat milk in blind testing (binomial p’s of <0.0064 and 0.036, respectively). Somewhere surprisingly, these two groups did not significantly prefer 2% milk to 0.1% milk [consumption: 25 of 40, binomial p = 0.15; stated preference: 25 of 45, binomial p = 0.55]. Nonethless, both groups still showed a trend toward increasing preference with increasing fat content, as can be seen in Figures 1 & 2. Two percent milk drinkers had an estimated low fat rejection threshold at 3.6% milk fat, just outside the range tested.
Participants who drank whole milk and participants who had a stated preference for whole milk showed increasing preference with increasing fat content. Those who typically drank whole milk significantly preferred 2% milk [12 of 15] and 3.5% milk [15 of 15] over 0.1% fat milk (binomial p’s < 0.035, p < 0.0001). Those individuals with a stated preference for whole milk significantly preferred 3.5% milk to 0.1% fat milk in blind testing [26 of 31; binomial p = 0.0002). These groups were also the only groups within our cohort that reached a chance adjusted preference of 75% in the range tested here. Low fat rejection thresholds were calculated to be 1.4% milk fat for whole milk drinkers and 2.6% milk fat for participants with a stated preference for whole milk.
As expected, a significant correlation (rho = 0.66, p < 0.001) was found between the type of milk consumers typically drank and their stated milk preference (see Table 4). As shown in Table 4, whole milk drinkers unanimously (15 of 15) reported a preference for whole milk based on taste, and the majority of two-percent drinkers reported preferring the taste of two-percent milk (29 out of 40). Conversely, fewer than half of skim milk drinkers (22 of 45) reported preferring the taste of skim milk.
Table 4.
Type of milk typically consumed | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Whole (n=15) | 2% (n=40) | Skim (n=45) | ||
Stated preference for milk type based on taste | Whole (n=31) | 15 | 10 | 6 |
2% (n=45) | 0 | 29 | 14 | |
Skim (n=22) | 0 | 0 | 22 | |
Other (n=6) | 0 | 1 | 3 |
3.2 Health Consciousness
Levels of concern regarding fat consumption, calorie consumption, sugar consumption, and weight gain differed significantly by type of milk typically consumed, with participants who drank skim milk being the most concerned with all measures (Table 5 and Figure 3). Levels of concern regarding sugar consumption differed significantly by stated milk preference with those with a stated preference for whole milk being less concerned with getting too much sugar than those stated a preference for skim milk (Table 5). Women had significantly higher composite health consciousness ratings and ratings for concern regarding fat consumption, calorie consumption, and weight gain (Table 5). Concerns over high salt, energy from food, food additives, coronary heart disease, cholesterol, and blood pressure were not related to gender, type of milk typically consumed or stated milk preference.
Table 5.
Typically Consumed | Stated Preference | Gender | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
Concern | F | p | F | p | F | p |
| ||||||
Composite | 1.2 | 0.3 | 1.4 | 0.3 | 5.2 | 0.03 |
Too Much Fat | 5.8 | 0.004 | 2.1 | 0.1 | 9.0 | <0.001 |
Too Much Sugar | 3.5 | 0.03 | 3.7 | 0.03 | 2.0 | 0.2 |
Too Many Calories | 3.8 | 0.03 | 2.5 | 0.09 | 11.4 | 0.001 |
Gaining Weight | 4.0 | 0.02 | 1.5 | 0.2 | 6.4 | 0.01 |
Bolded values are significant at p < 0.05
4. Discussion
In agreement with our hypothesis that stated preference would better predict revealed preferences in a blind taste test versus the type of milk typically consumed, those consumers who declared a preference for skim milk also showed a significant preference for lower fat milk compared to higher fat milk, whereas consumers who reported they typically drank skim milk showed no significant preferences between lower fat and higher fat milk samples. In contrast, those consumers who typically drink whole milk show stronger preferences for fat in milk, as compared to those who stated a preference for whole milk. This result makes sense when considering the nutritional implications of consuming whole milk. Per 8 ounce serving (237mL), whole milk provides 149 kilocalories compared to only 83 kilocalories for skim milk, so this suggests that only those consumers with a very strong preference for fat in milk continue to choose consume whole milk in spite of both the high energy density of whole milk, and the sustained public health messaging about reducing fat intake. This assertion is buttressed by the observation that every single participant who reported typically drinking whole milk also stated they preferred the taste of whole milk (see Table 3). In contrast, two percent drinkers and those who stated a preference for 2% milk did not actually prefer this level of fat over a low fat control in blind tasting (although they did significantly prefer 3.5% fat milk to a low fat control). This raises an interesting question – would those consumers who regularly drink 2% milk accept a change to skim milk without a loss in satisfaction? The taste test data would seem to suggest so, but many other factors affect food satisfaction and choice including expectation. Knowledge of fat content from the label is reinforced by visual cues of milk fat content, thereby creating different contexts and expectations than a blind taste test. Alternatively, this result could merely reflect Type II error, as it remains possible that testing even more two percent consumers beyond the 45 tested here may potentially reveal a significant preference for 2% milk in blind testing, especially given that these individuals did prefer 3.5% milk over the very low fat control. Also, we should note that our test only measured preferences for fluid milk within a beverage context. Many consumers use milk as an ingredient and complex interactions with other food components may render milk fat more important in those contexts (Raats & Shepherd, 1992; Raats & Shepherd, 1991). Consumers may choose to keep only one type of milk in the household, and therefore select the fat level that provides satisfactory results across a variety of contexts. Nevertheless, it may still be worth challenging consumers’ beliefs that they prefer higher fat milks. If consumers were presented with evidence that they might find lower fat milk acceptable, they may be more willing to switch to lower fat milk choices (e.g., I tried it and I like it). Fat is the most energy dense of the macronutrients, contributing more than twice as much energy per gram as protein or carbohydrates, so switching to lower fat milks may lead to positive health outcomes. A diet lower in energy density has been associated with positive health outcomes (Rolls, Drewnowski, & Ledikwe, 2005), but observational studies of fat consumption from dairy have not shown the same findings (Kratz et al., 2013). More research into the role of dairy fat in the diet is needed.
4.2 Limitations
The health consciousness questionnaire used in this study was relatively short and not all items were directly related to milk consumption. There were a noticeably limited number of whole milk drinkers in our study, as it was very difficult to recruit these individuals. The extent to which these results apply to dairy fat preferences in the general population is potentially limited: these data were collected in a university environment at a major land grant institution, leading to a participant pool that may not generalize to the general population. For example, the whole milk drinkers have have come disproportionally from farming or other agriculture production backgrounds, whereas other work suggests education level can influence health seeking behavior (Lantz et al., 1996). Nonetheless, the discordance between stated and revealed preferences were striking and deserve additional study. Finally, the only product used here was unsweetened fluid milk. Different results could potentially be found in flavored milk (Li, Hayes, & Ziegler, 2015) or other dairy products (Drewnowski, Shrager, Lipsky, Stellar, & Greenwood, 1989).
5. Conclusions
While fat is generally assumed to be a pleasant stimulus, there appears to be a segment of the population that truly prefers lower fat levels milks and is not simply choosing them for health reasons. Similar group differences have been observed in response to sweetness, a taste that is generally considered pleasant by most but disliked by a portion of the population (Looy, Callaghan, & Weingarten, 1992; Lundgren, Jonsson, Pangborn, & Sontag, 1978). These results add to extant literature by showing that some individuals may prefer lower fat products from a sensory standpoint in addition to a health standpoint. Future work on fat preferences could include further stratifications of test populations in a number of ways. Potentially meaningful data could be generated by dividing participants by their total fat intake (Pangborn & Giovanni, 1984), by their general fat preferences (Ledikwe et al., 2007), their ability to detect and discriminate between varying fat levels (Liang et al., 2012), or by their ability to detect free fatty acids (Running, Craig, & Mattes, 2015; Stewart, Feinle-Bisset, & Keast, 2011).
This study shows that stratifying participants by both their stated preferences and their typical food consumption patterns can lead to better understanding of food choices. As has been pointed out previously, using group means to summarize overall preferences may not be ideal as the mean may not actually represent the response of anyone in the group (Pangborn, 1981). Stratifying groups by stated preference and typical intake might lead to more accurate understanding of preference patterns. Recent research by Sharafi et al. (Sharafi et al., 2015) took a novel approach examining concepts of discordance (consuming foods that you do not like and/or disliking foods that you consume) and food hedonism (the relative liking of high-fat/sweet/salty food compared to other pleasurable activities). They found that children showing higher levels of discordance and food hedonism had higher BMIs. While there is a long history of encouraging consumers to change their dietary consumption habits, less effort has been focused on actually changing consumers’ dietary preferences. Their work seems to suggest that more research into changing preferences is warranted. Work by Chung and Vickers (Chung & Vickers, 2007a) demonstrates that it is possible to increase consumers’ preferences for a less liked, lower calorie beverage over time, although later work failed to show the increase in liking translated to more frequent consumption in a choice experiment (Chung & Vickers, 2007c). However, participants in that study were not provided with information about the nutrition content of their choices, which would also presumably alter real world choices. More research into the feasibility of changing consumer preferences and the resulting outcomes on actual consumption in informed conditions is warranted.
Table 1.
Product | Milk fat (%) |
---|---|
| |
Skim/nonfat milk | 0.0 – 0.5 |
Low fat milk | 0.5–3.25 |
Whole milk | >3.25 |
Half and half | 10.5 – 18 |
Light cream | 18–30 |
Light whipping cream | 30–36 |
Heavy cream | >36 |
Butter | 80 |
Highlights.
Segmenting consumers uncovered patterns that were obscured by group means
Stated preference, and milk type typically consumed associated with fat preferences
Regular whole milk drinkers show a strong revealed preference for fat in milk
Regular skim milk drinkers include a subpopulation who actively dislike higher fat
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Bonnie Ford and the staff of the Berkey Creamery for their assistance in the production and analysis of milk samples, Rachel Primrose MS, and the Sensory Evaluation Center staff for their assistance with data collection, and our study participants for their time and participation.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest disclosure
CVS has no conflicts of interests to declare. ABC was previously employed by a large dairy cooperative before she began working at Penn State. JEH has received speaking or consulting fees from corporate clients in the food industry. Additionally, the Sensory Evaluation Center at Penn State conducts routine consumer tests for industrial clients in the food, packaging and packaged goods industries to facilitate experiential learning for undergraduate and graduate students. None of these organizations have had any role in study conception, design or interpretation, or the decision to publish these data. This work was funded by the Pennsylvania State University as well as United States Department of Agriculture Hatch Project [PEN04332] funds; JEH also receives additional support from a National Institutes of Health grant from the National Institute of Deafness and Communication Disorders [DC010904].
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Badings HT, Neeter R. Recent advances in the study of aroma compounds of milk and dairy products. Netherlands Milk and Dairy Journal. 1980;34:9–30. [Google Scholar]
- Blackman J, Saliba A, Schmidtke L. Sweetness acceptance of novices, experienced consumers and winemakers in Hunter Valley Semillon wines. Food Quality and Preference. 2010;21(7):679–683. [Google Scholar]
- Briefel RR, Johnson CL. Secular trends in dietary intake in the United States. Annu Rev Nutr. 2004;24:401–431. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.23.011702.073349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman KW, Lawless HT. Sources of error and the no-preference option in dairy product testing. Journal of Sensory Studies. 2005;20:454–468. [Google Scholar]
- Chung SJ, Vickers Z. Influence of sweetness on the sensory-specific satiety and long-term acceptability of tea. Food Quality and Preference. 2007a;18(2):256–264. [Google Scholar]
- Chung SJ, Vickers Z. Long-term acceptability and choice of teas differing in sweetness. Food Quality and Preference. 2007c;18(7):963–974. [Google Scholar]
- Chung SJ. Effects of milk type and consumer factors on the acceptance of milk among Korean female consumers. J Food Sci. 2009;74(6):S286–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2009.01224.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drewnowski A, Greenwood MRC. Cream and sugar: Human preferences for high-fat foods. Physiology & Behavior. 1983;30:629–633. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(83)90232-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drewnowski A, Shrager EE, Lipsky C, Stellar E, Greenwood MRC. Sugar and fat: Sensory and hedonic evaluation of liquid and solid foods. Physiology & Behavior. 1989;45:177–183. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90182-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Food and Drugs 21 CFR 131.110 (1198).
- Frøst MB, Dijksterhuis G, Martens M. Sensory perception of fat in milk. Food Quality and Preference. 2001;12:327–336. [Google Scholar]
- German JB, Gibson RA, Krauss RM, Nestel P, Lamarche B, van Staveren WA, et al. A reappraisal of the impact of dairy foods and milk fat on cardiovascular disease risk. European Journal of Nutrition. 2009;48(4):191–203. doi: 10.1007/s00394-009-0002-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harwood ML, Ziegler GR, Hayes JE. Rejection thresholds in chocolate milk: Evidence for segmentation. Food Quality and Preference. 2012;26(1):128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes JE, Duffy VB. Oral sensory phenotype identifies level of sugar and fat required for maximal liking. Physiology & Behavior. 2008;95(1–2):77–87. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hearty AP, McCarthy SN, Kearney JM, Gibney MJ. Relationship between attitudes towards healthy eating and dietary behaviour, lifestyle and demographic factors in a representative sample of Irish adults. Appetite. 2007;48(1):1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.03.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller KL, Steinmann L, Nurse RJ, Tepper BJ. Genetic taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil influences food preference and reported intake in preschool children. Appetite. 2002;38(1):3–12. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinsella JE, Patton S, Dimick PS. The flavor potential of milk fat. A review of its chemical nature and biochemical origin. The Journal of The American Oil Chemists. 1967;44:449–454. [Google Scholar]
- Kratz M, Baars T, Guyenet S. The relationship between high-fat dairy consumption and obesity, cardiovascular, and metabolic disease. European Journal of Nutrition. 2013;52(1):1–24. doi: 10.1007/s00394-012-0418-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- La Berge AF. How the ideology of low fat conquered America. Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences. 2008;63(2):139–177. doi: 10.1093/jhmas/jrn001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lantz PM, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Williams DR, Mero RP, Chen J. Socioeconomic factors, health behaviors, and mortality results from a nationally representative prospective study of US adults. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1996;279(21):1703–1708. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.21.1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ledikwe JH, Ello-Martin J, Pelkman CL, Birch LL, Mannino ML, Rolls BJ. A reliable, valid questionnaire indicates that preference for dietary fat declines when following a reduced-fat diet. Appetite. 2007;49(1):74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee GH, Lee JS, Shin MG. Sensory attribute comparison of consumer milk using descriptive analysis. Food Science and Biotechnology. 2003;12(5):480–484. [Google Scholar]
- Li B, Hayes JE, Ziegler GR. Maximizing overall liking results in a superior product to minimizing deviations from ideal ratings: an optimization case study with coffee-flavored milk. Food Quality and Preference. 2015;42:27–36. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang LC, Sakimura J, May D, Breen C, Driggin E, Tepper BJ, et al. Fat discrimination: A phenotype with potential implications for studying fat intake behaviors and obesity. Physiology & Behavior. 2012;105(2):470–475. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Looy H, Callaghan S, Weingarten HP. Hedonic response of sucrose likers and dislikers to other gustatory stimuli. Physiology & Behavior. 1992;52:219–225. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90261-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundgren B, Jonsson B, Pangborn RM, Sontag AM. Taste discrimination vs hedonic response to sucrose in coffee beverage. An interlaboratory study. Chemical Senses and Flavor. 1978;3(3):249–265. [Google Scholar]
- Mattes RD. Fat preference and adherence to a reduced-fat diet. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1993;57(3):373–381. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/57.3.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mela DJ. Sensory assessment of fat content in fluid dairy products. Appetite. 1988;10(1):37–44. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(88)80031-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mela DJ. Sensory preferences for fats: What, who, why? Food Quality and Preference. 1990;2:95–101. [Google Scholar]
- Nikolantonaki M, Darriet P. Identification of ethyl 2-sulfanylacetate as an important off-odor compound in white wines. Journal of Agricultural and Food Chemistry. 2011;59(18):10191–10199. doi: 10.1021/jf201047u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson NE. A simplified method of standardizing the ice-cream mix. K. S. A. C. Agricultural Experiment Station; 1924. [Google Scholar]
- Päivi K, Tuorila H, Rita H. How information enhances acceptability of a low-fat spread. Food Quality and Preference. 1996;7(2):87–94. [Google Scholar]
- Pangborn R. Criteria of food acceptance. Zürich, Switzerland: Foster Publishing; 1981. Individuality in responses to sensory stimuli. [Google Scholar]
- Pangborn RM, Bos KEO, Stern JS. Dietary fat intake and taste responses to fat in milk by under-, normal, and overweight women. Appetite. 1985;6(1):25–40. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(85)80048-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pangborn RM, Dunkley WL. Sensory discrimination of fat and solids-not-fat in milk. Journal of Dairy Science. 1964;47(7):719–726. doi: 10.3168/jds.S0022-0302(66)87774-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pangborn RM, Giovanni ME. Dietary intake of sweet foods and of dairy fats and resultant gustatory responses to sugar in lemonade and to fat in milk. Appetite. 1984;5(4):317–327. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(84)80004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prescott J, Norris L, Kunst M, Kim S. Estimating a “consumer rejection threshold” for cork taint in white wine. Food Quality and Preference. 2005;16(4):345–349. [Google Scholar]
- Raats MM, Shepherd R. An evaluation of the use and perceived appropriateness of milk using the repretory grid method and the ‘item by use’ appropriateness method. Food Quality and Preference. 1991;3:89–100. [Google Scholar]
- Raats MM, Shepherd R. Free-choice profiling of milks and other products prepared with milks of different fat contents. Journal of Sensory Studies. 1992;7:179–203. [Google Scholar]
- Rapp E, Oström A, Osika W, Englund A, Annett J, Gustafsson I-B. Preference for full-fat over low-fat foods among individuals suffering from coronary heart disease and healthy controls. Physiology & Behavior. 2009;98(4):489–497. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson-Harman NJ, Stevens R, Walker S, Gamble J, Miller M, Wong M, et al. Mapping consumer perceptions of creaminess and liking for liquid dairy products. Food Quality and Preference. 2000;11:239–246. [Google Scholar]
- Roininen K, Lähteenmäki L, Tuorila H. Quantification of consumer attitudes to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Appetite. 1999;33:71–88. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls BJ, Drewnowski A, Ledikwe JH. Changing the energy density of the diet as a strategy for weight management. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2005;105(5 Suppl 1):S98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2005.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Running CA, Craig BA, Mattes RD. Oleogustus: The unique taste of fat. Chemical Senses. 2015;40(7):507–516. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjv036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saliba AJ, Bullock J, Hardie WJ. Consumer rejection threshold for 1,8-cineole (eucalyptol) in Australian red wine. Food Quality and Preference. 2009;20(7):500–504. [Google Scholar]
- Sharafi M, Peracchio H, Scarmo S, Huedo-Medina TB, Mayne ST, Cartmel B, et al. Preschool-Adapted Liking Survey (PALS): A brief and valid method to assess dietary quality of preschoolers. Childhood Obesity. 2015;11(5):530–540. doi: 10.1089/chi.2015.0037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd R, Sparks P, Bellier S, Raats MM. Attitudes and choice of flavoured milks: Extensions of Fishbein and Ajzen’s theory of reasoned action. Food Quality and Preference. 1991 Feb;3:157–164. [Google Scholar]
- Stewart JE, Feinle-Bisset C, Keast RS. Fatty acid detection during food consumption and digestion: Associations with ingestive behavior and obesity. Progress in Lipid Research. 2011;50(3):225–233. doi: 10.1016/j.plipres.2011.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun YH. Health concern, food choice motives, and attitudes toward healthy eating: the mediating role of food choice motives. Appetite. 2008;51(1):42–49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamsma A, Kurtz FE, Bright RS, Pallansch MJ. Contribution of milk fat to the flavor of milk. Journal of Dairy Science. 1969;52(12):1910–1913. [Google Scholar]
- Tuorila H. Selection of milks with varying fat contents and related overall liking, attitudes, norms and intentions. Appetite. 1987;8:1–14. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(87)80022-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visschers VHM, Siegris M. When reduced fat increases preference. How fat reduction in nutrition tables and numeracy skills affect food choices. Appetite. 2010;55(3):730–733. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weekes LN, Walsh D, Ferguson H, Ross CF. Impact of multicolored Asian lady beetles on the sensory properties of Concord and Niagara grape juice. Journal of Food Science. 2010;75(1):S68–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3841.2009.01423.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoo YJ, Saliba AJ, Prenzler PD, Ryan D. Total phenolic content, antioxidant activity, and cross-cultural consumer rejection threshold in white and red wines functionally enhanced with catechin-rich extracts. Journal of Agricultural and Food Chemistry. 2012;60(1):388–393. doi: 10.1021/jf203216z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]