Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2015 Nov 3;40(1):71–76. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.213

Adolescent self-control behavior predicts body weight through the life course: a prospective birth cohort study

S Koike 1,2,*, R Hardy 1, M Richards 1
PMCID: PMC4704138  EMSID: EMS64800  PMID: 26449420

Abstract

Background:

Weight gain has become one of the biggest issues for healthy aging in middle- and high-income countries. Self-control of emotional reward cues is an important behavioral factor for regulation of weight gain through voluntary diet control and physical activity.

Methods:

We tested the associations between teacher-rated self-control at ages 13 and 15 years, and measured body mass index (BMI) between ages 15 and 60–64 years, controlling for confounding factors such as affective symptoms and cognition, using 3873 study members in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development, also known as the British 1946 birth cohort.

Results:

Multivariable regression analysis after adjustment for all covariates showed that lower self-control was associated with higher BMI in all measure points (P<0.05). Multilevel modeling using a cubic model showed that there was an association between self-control and BMI at 15 years in females (male: BMI=−0.00 kg m−2 per 1 s.d. on the self-control score (95% confidence interval (CI): −0.12 to 0.11), P =0.94; female: BMI=−0.27 (−0.42 to −0.11), P<0.001). The association became stronger with age in both sexes (BMI=−0.065 (−0.082 to −0.048), P<0.001; BMI=−0.036 (−0.057 to −0.015), P<0.001). By age 60–64 years, the association between self-control and BMI in men had increased to −0.70 (−0.96 to −0.44) and −0.67 (−1.04 to −0.30) in women.

Conclusions:

Lower adolescent self-control was associated with higher BMI through the life course, and this becomes stronger with age. Investigations to test whether intervention to self-control improves obesity are recommended.

Introduction

Weight gain has become one of the biggest issues for healthy aging in middle- and high-income countries, as rapid lifestyle changes in these countries have led to an increased tendency toward energy-dense diet and decreased physical activity.1 Self-control of emotional reward cues2 is likely to be important for regulation of weight gain through voluntary diet control and physical activity, as these often require resolving conflict between immediate reward from tempting energy-dense food and from physical relaxation, and reducing these for the longer-term rewarding consequences of weight control.3 A well-known developmental model of this is the marshmallow test for children, in which the instant gratification of a small reward (one marshmallow) is pitted against the prospect of a larger reward (more marshmallows) for delayed gratification through waiting.4

This self-control emerges in early childhood and matures in adolescence, which is consistent with the developmental timing of hierarchical top–down regulation in the prefrontal cortex.5, 6 It then develops into a higher order skill over adulthood, capable of achieving more abstracted goals over a longer period. Previous prospective studies have found that self-control in childhood is associated with a range of outcomes in later life including social, for example, academic and occupational achievement, financial management, family stability and citizenship;2, 7, 8, 9 and health related, for example, body mass index (BMI),10, 11 metabolic abnormality,2 midlife verbal memory,8 addictive behavior2 and psychotic symptoms.12

The present study focused on the outcome of BMI over the life course as a marker of weight gain. Studies testing associations between adolescent self-control and adiposity may be vulnerable to confounding factors such as affective symptoms and cognition, which are both associated with adult BMI,13, 14, 15, 16 and which were not consistently controlled in the above previous studies. Furthermore, one previous study used self-reported height and weight, which is subject to systematic bias;10 and to the best of our knowledge little is known about adolescent self-control behavior in regard to body weight change over the life course.

The Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD), also known as the British 1946 birth cohort, offers an excellent opportunity to investigate the relationship between childhood self-control and life course BMI: teachers provided behavioral ratings of study members at ages 13 and 15 years, from which dimensions of self-control, emotionality and conduct problems were derived, and repeated measures of BMI at most sweeps were calculated from measured heights and weights. We hypothesized that lower adolescent self-control is associated with higher and faster increase in BMI over the life course, independently of a range of potential confounders including childhood cognition.

Materials and methods

Participants

The NSHD is one of the longest-running prospective large cohort studies, consisting of a social class-weighted sample of 5362 children drawn from all single births within marriage during 1 week in March 1946 in England, Scotland and Wales. The most recent data collection was conducted between 2006 and 2011, when study members were aged 60–64 years. Study members still alive and with a known current address in mainland Britain (n=2856) were invited for assessment at one of six clinical research facilities; those unable or unwilling to travel were offered a home visit by a research nurse. A total 2229 participants out of the 2856 invited (78.0% age range=60.3–65.0 years, mean=63.4, s.d.=1.1) underwent assessment: 1690 attended the Clinical Research Facility and the remaining 539 were seen in their homes.17 Invitations were not sent to those who had died (n=778), who were living abroad (n=570), had previously withdrawn from the study (n=594) or had been lost to follow-up (n=564). All study members participating had their height and weight measured.

A total of 3873 study members had teacher ratings of behavior at both ages 13 and 15 years as well as at least one measure of BMI between ages 15 and 60–64 years (mean number=5.0, s.d.=1.9). Those excluded from analyses had lower birth weight (P<0.001), higher weight at age 2 years (P=0.009), lower childhood social class (P<0.001), lower adult social class (P=0.014), and higher BMI at age 15 and lower BMI at age 60–64 years (P=0.032 and 0.008, respectively).

Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Greater Manchester and Scottish committees. All study members gave written informed consent.

Measures

Body mass index

Heights and weights were measured by school doctors at age 15 years and by trained research nurses using standardized protocols at ages 36, 43, 53 and 60–64 years, and by self-reports at ages 20 and 26 years. BMI was calculated at each age using the standard weight/height2 formula.

Adolescent behavioral problems

Behavior problems in adolescents were rated at age 13 and 15 years by teachers using a forerunner of the Rutter A scale.18, 19 These ratings have been classified into three behavioral dimensions reflecting conduct (externalizing) problem (for example, a quarrelsome and aggressive child), emotional (internalizing) problem (for example, extremely fearful) and self-control (for example, a poor worker or lazy).8 Each dimension score was calculated from the standardized sum of the factor scores at both ages.8

Confounding variables

The following childhood variables were treated as potential confounders: birth weight, weight at age 4 years, childhood cognitive ability, family social class at age 11 years (or, if this was unknown, at age 4 or 15 years) and emotional and conduct problems at age 13 and 15 years. Birth weight to the nearest quarter of a pound was extracted from medical records within a few weeks of delivery and converted into kilograms. Weight at 4 years was measured. Childhood cognition was represented as the sum of four tests of verbal and nonverbal ability taken at age 8 (reading comprehension, word reading, vocabulary and picture intelligence) devised by the National Foundation for Education Research.20 Father's occupational social class was classified according to the Registrar General and re-grouped into three categories (I or II, III and IV or V).

The following were treated as potential confounders in adulthood: educational attainment at age 26 years and register general head of household occupation social class at age 53 years (three categories, if this was unknown, from age 26 to 43 years). Educational attainment was classified into five categories: none; vocational only; ordinary (O) level, that is, examinations taken at minimum school leaving age; advanced (A) level, that is examinations taken in the final year of secondary education; and higher (degree or equivalent, or beyond).

Statistical analysis

Multivariable regression analysis was used to test associations between adolescent self-control and BMI at each age, adjusting for potential confounders. Multilevel modeling was then used to model the longitudinal BMI trajectory. This method allows for incomplete outcome data on the assumption of missing at random.21 BMI from age 15 to 60–64 years was modeled with linear (age), quadratic (age2) and cubic terms (age3). Self-control score was added to the model along with emotional problem score as it had a significant association with BMI in our previous study.15 Interactions between each personality variable and age were also examined to assess the association with rate of change of BMI.

Statistical significance was set at P<0.05. The regression analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics 22 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA), and multilevel modeling was performed using ‘nlme' package version 3.1-117 in R version 3.1.1 (The R foundation, Vienna, Austria).21, 22

Results

Mean BMI in both male and female participants increased by age (Table 1). Males had lower BMI at age 15 years and then higher BMI at ages between 20 and 43 years (P<0.001), but the difference became non-significant at ages 53 and 60–64 years (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic characteristics in this study.

  Male
Female
P-value
  n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.)  
BMI, kg m−2
 15 years 1881 19.6 (2.4) 1700 20.6 (3.0) <0.001
 20 years 1829 22.6 (2.5) 1735 21.8 (2.9) <0.001
 26 years 1822 23.4 (2.8) 1782 22.4 (3.2) <0.001
 36 years 1632 24.8 (3.2) 1648 23.6 (4.1) <0.001
 43 years 1617 25.7 (3.5) 1608 25.2 (4.8) <0.001
 53 years 1452 27.4 (4.0) 1496 27.4 (5.5) 0.81
 60–64 years 1061 27.9 (4.1) 1158 27.9 (5.5) 0.92
 
Adolescents behavioral problemsa
 Self-control 2044 −0.13 (1.01) 1883 0.14 (0.97) <0.001
 Conduct problem 2044 0.11 (0.98) 1883 −0.12 (1.01) <0.001
 Emotional problem 2044 −0.09 (0.96) 1883 0.10 (1.03) <0.001
Birth weight, g 2798 3444 (558) 2529 3302 (513) <0.001
Weight at age 2 years, kg 2530 13.2 (1.4) 2303 12.6 (1.4) <0.001
Cognition at age 8 yearsa 2282 −0.03 (0.88) 2102 0.02 (0.85) 0.070
Childhood social class         0.80
 I or II 638   559    
 III 1193   1090    
 IV or V 621   572    
Educational attainment         <0.001
 None 930   835    
 Vocational only 141   212    
 O level 343   520    
 A level 590   450    
 Higher 304   107    
Occupational social class at age 53 years         <0.001
 I or II 1015   674    
 III 857   937    
 IV or V 272   445    

Abbreviation: BMI, body mass index.

a

Z-scores.

Association of adolescent characteristics with adult BMI

The results of multivariable regression analysis to test associations between adolescent self-control and BMI at each age, and results stratified by sex are presented in Table 2. After adjustment for all covariates, which resulted in slight attenuation of the association, lower self-control was associated with higher BMI (P<0.05, Figure 1 and Table 2). There was also significant sex × self-control interaction at ages 15, 26 and 36 years (P<0.05). When stratified by sex, lower self-control was associated with higher BMI at ages 26, 36, 53 and 60–64 years in males; and at ages 15–36 and 60–64 years in females (P<0.05, Figure 1 and Table 2).

Table 2. The association of self-control with BMI at each age.

Age Total crude Adjusted model Male crude Adjusted model Female crude Adjusted model
15 Years 0.03 (−0.06 to 0.13) 0.20 (0.35 to −0.04)* 0.10 (−0.02 to 0.21) −0.01 (−0.20 to 0.18) −0.19 (−0.35 to −0.04)* −0.40 (−0.64 to −0.16)
20 Years −0.30 (−0.39 to −0.20) −0.28 (−0.44 to −0.13) −0.16 (−0.28 to −0.04) −0.16 (−0.35 to 0.04) −0.35 (−0.50 to −0.20) −0.41 (−0.65 to −0.17)
26 Years −0.51 (−0.62 to −0.40) −0.40 (−0.57 to −0.22) −0.35 (−0.49 to −0.21) −0.27 (−0.49 to −0.04)* −0.55 (−0.72 to −0.38) −0.53 (−0.78 to −0.27)
36 Years −0.57 (−0.71 to −0.42) −0.37 (−0.58 to −0.15) −0.40 (−0.57 to −0.23) −0.28 (−0.55 to −0.01)* −0.58 (−0.80 to −0.36) −0.44 (−0.78 to −0.10)*
43 Years −0.57 (−0.73 to −0.41) −0.29 (−0.55 to −0.04)* −0.42 (−0.60 to −0.23) −0.17 (−0.46 to 0.13) −0.67 (−0.94 to −0.40) −0.39 (−0.81 to 0.02)
53 Years −0.63 (−0.82 to −0.43) −0.50 (−0.81 to −0.19)* −0.58 (−0.80 to −0.35) −0.52 (−0.88 to −0.15) −0.71 (−1.03 to −0.38) −0.49 (−0.99 to 0.01)
60–64 Years −0.72 (−0.95 to −0.49) −0.59 (−0.96 to −0.23) −0.75 (−1.02 to −0.48) −0.54 (−0.98 to −0.10)* −0.74 (−1.12 to −0.37) −0.68 (−1.26 to −0.10)*

BMI (95% confidence interval). Bold showed significant coefficient (*P<0.05, P<0.01, P<0.001).

Adjusted model is adjusted for sex, birth weight, weight at age 4 years, overall cognition at age 8 years, childhood social class, conduct and emotional problem at ages 13 and 15 years, educational attainment, and head of household occupational social class at age 53 years.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Trajectories in regression coefficient of self-control with BMI. Regression coefficients of self-control considered for potential confounding variables were plotted in total, male and female participants. Bars showed 95% confidence intervals.

Association with longitudinal BMI trajectory in later life

Multilevel modeling showed that BMI increased significantly with age (P<0.001, Table 3 and Figure 2). There was a significant sex × age interaction (P<0.001), indicating that BMI in males was lower at age 15 years, and increased faster with age, than in females.

Table 3. Multilevel modeling for adolescent self-control and BMI through the life course.

Independent variables Total (n=3873), BMI (95% CI) P-value Male (n=2015), BMI (95% CI) P-value Female (n=1858), BMI (95% CI) P-value
(Intercept) 18.9 (18.6 to 19.2) <0.001 19.9 (19.8 to 20.0) <0.001 20.9 (20.7 to 21.0) <0.001
Sex 0.98 (0.80 to 1.16) <0.001        
Age 0.75 (0.71 to 0.79) <0.001 0.43 (0.41 to 0.45) <0.001 0.11 (0.09 to 0.13) <0.001
Age2 −0.025 (−0.027 to −0.023) <0.001 −0.011 (−0.012 to −0.010) <0.001 0.003 (0.002 to 0.004) <0.001
Age3 (× 10−4) 2.8 (2.5 to 3.0) <0.001 1.2 (1.1 to 1.4) <0.001 −0.3 (−0.4 to −0.2) <0.001
Sex × age −0.32 (−0.35 to −0.29) <0.001        
Self-control 0.13 (−0.14 to 0.40) 0.28 −0.00 (−0.12 to 0.11) 0.94 −0.27 (−0.42 to −0.11) <0.001
Sex × self-control −0.19 (−0.36 to −0.01] 0.035        
Age × self-control −0.059 (−0.077 to −0.040) <0.001 −0.065 (−0.082 to −0.048) <0.001 −0.036 (−0.057 to −0.015) <0.001
Sex × age × self-control 0.005 (−0.004 to 0.013) 0.27        

Abbreviation: BMI, body mass index.

Significant coefficients were shown in bold (P<0.05). BMI at age 15 years was set as reference. Male was coded as 1 and female as 2. These models also include other independent variables such as emotional problem, and sex × age2, sex × age3, sex × emotional problem, age × emotional problem, age2 × self-control, age3 × self-control, and sex × age × emotional problem interactions.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

BMI trajectories by self-control. BMI trajectories from age 15 to 60–64 years were shown in total, male and female participants. The trajectories of those with high (mean+s.d.), mean and low (mean−s.d.) self-control for each group were illustrated with pale to dark color gradation.

The main effect of self-control was not significant (P=0.28, Table 3), but there was a significant sex × self-control interaction (P=0.035), indicating a stronger association between self-control and BMI at 15 years in females compared with males (male: BMI=−0.00 kg m−2 per 1 s.d. on the self-control score (−0.12 to 0.11), P=0.94; female: BMI=−0.27 (−0.42 to −0.11), P<0.001; Table 3). The association became stronger with age in both sexes (P<0.001), and there was no gender difference in this association (P=0.27). By age 60–64 years, the association between self-control and BMI in men had increased to −0.70 (−0.96 to −0.44) kg m−2 per unit self-control score and −0.67 (−1.04 to −0.30) in women.

Discussion

A population representative prospective cohort study revealed that lower self-control was associated with higher BMI, independently of adolescent conduct and emotional problems, early growth and social position, with this association becoming stronger with age. In addition, the association between self-control and BMI emerged earlier in females.

The strengths of this study include the use of a national population-based sample with objective measures of body height and weight over the life course, as well as independent- and prospective-rated behavioral characteristics in adolescence, which are thus not subject to recall bias. The regression analyses were also controlled for several important potential confounding factors in childhood and adulthood. Several limitations should also be considered in this study. Although demographic characteristics in NSHD are still broadly representative of the general source population in the age 60–64 survey,17 there was a disproportionate loss to follow-up of those less socially advantaged, although we have no reason to believe that this would have affected the pattern of results. Second, although self-control was assessed from teachers, which is thought to be more objective than ratings from the parents, these ratings could have influenced study member's school performance, to the extent that they reflect teacher–pupil interaction. A mixture of subjective and objective measurements of self-control ability such as delay discounting tasks may help to clarify the relationship between self-control and outcomes.

These results imply that self-control is related to regulatory behavior toward controlling weight, such as dietary control and healthy activity, and this may develop over the life course. There are several possible explanations for the observation that the association strengthens with age. First is the increasing dominance of personal choice with age. Second is the increased availability of energy-dense foods along with increased environmental limitations on routine physical activity experienced by this cohort in adulthood. Thus we would expect self-control to have yet more of an influence on BMI in children who were born after 1946.23 Of relevance to this is the fact that food rationing initiated during World War II persisted well into the childhood of NSHD study members. Third, as both higher BMI and lower self-control are associated with lower midlife cognitive function and cognitive decline,8, 10, 24, 25 the stronger association with age in this study may be partially mediated by the trajectory of cognitive function in adulthood.

The present study also showed an earlier association between self-control and BMI in females compared with males. There are several potential biological and psychosocial explanations for this. First, as males had lower self-control in adolescence than females, self-control in females may have particular significance to regulation of voluntary diet control and physical activity. Second, associations between self-control and weight change may be stronger among women during perinatal period, which influences postpartum weight retention and in turn may result in increased regulation of dietary and physical activity patterns in those with higher self-control.26 Third, although the results were significant after controlling for adult occupational social class, male participants tend to expend more physical activity at work,27 which may to some extent limit weight gain regardless of self-control. Fourth, there are also sex differences in neural correlates of reward systems and inhibitory control.28, 29, 30 Maturation of self-control occurs in accordance with cognitive regulation of impulsive and reward responses, which is greater in males through adolescence.29 The emotional response greatly decreases at the end of adolescence, following which adult males may attain more self-control ability in the face of emotional cues, which might also affect weight control.

In conclusion, a population representative prospective cohort study showed that lower self-control was associated with higher BMI through the life course, with this association becoming stronger with age. Attempts using physical activity and computer training programs in childhood for helping the development of self-control in adolescence may be effective for the performance of inhibition and executive function, as well as real world outcomes such as school performance.31 More investigations of effective intervention to efficient maturation of self-control will be needed for better physical and psychosocial outcomes.

Acknowledgments

SK was supported by the grants from JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 25870143, 26118703, and Strategic Young Researcher Overseas Visits Program for Accelerating Brain Circulation. RH and MR are funded by the UK Medical Research Council (Unit Programme numbers MC_UU_12019/3 and 4, respectively). The authors are grateful to NSHD study members for their continuing support. We thank members of the NSHD scientific and data collection team at the following centers for the age 60–64 data collection: MRC Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL; Wellcome Trust (WT) Clinical Research Facility (CRF) Manchester; WTCRF at the Western General Hospital in Edinburgh; WTCRF at University Hospital Birmingham; WTCRF at University College London Hospital; CRF at the University Hospital of Wales; CRF and Twin Research Unit at St Thomas' Hospital London.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1Kuh D, Cooper R, Hardy R, Richards M, Ben-Shlomo Y. A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  2. 2Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108: 2693–2698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. 3Von Hippel W, Henry JD. Aging and self-regulation In: Vohs KD, Baumeister RF (eds) Handbook of Self-Regulation. 2nd edn The Guilford Press: New York, USA, 2011; 321–335. [Google Scholar]
  4. 4Mischel W, Ebbesen EB, Zeiss AR. Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol 1972; 21: 204–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. 5Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Brown TT, Kuperman JM, Chung Y, Hagler DJ Jr et al. Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109: 19620–19625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. 6Casey BJ, Somerville LH, Gotlib IH, Ayduk O, Franklin NT, Askren MK et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108: 14998–15003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. 7Piquero NL, Moffitt TE. Can childhood factors predict workplace deviance? Justice Q 2014; 31: 664–692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. 8Xu MK, Jones PB, Barnett JH, Gaysina D, Kuh D, Croudace TJ et al. Adolescent self-organization predicts midlife memory in a prospective birth cohort study. Psychol Aging 2013; 28: 958–968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. 9McClelland MM, Acock AC, Piccinin A, Rhea SA, Stallings MC. Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Child Res Q 2013; 28: 314–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. 10Schlam TR, Wilson NL, Shoda Y, Mischel W, Ayduk O. Preschoolers' delay of gratification predicts their body mass 30 years later. J Pediatr 2013; 162: 90–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. 11Johnson F, Pratt M, Wardle J. Dietary restraint and self-regulation in eating behavior. Int J Obesity 2012; 36: 665–674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. 12Nishida A, Xu KM, Croudace T, Jones PB, Barnett J, Richards M. Adolescent self-control predicts midlife hallucinatory experiences: 40-year follow-up of a national birth Cohort. Schizophr Bull 2014; 40: 1543–1551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. 13Liem ET, Sauer PJ, Oldehinkel AJ, Stolk RP. Association between depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence and overweight in later life: review of the recent literature. Archi Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008; 162: 981–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. 14Richards M, Black S, Mishra G, Gale CR, Deary IJ, Batty DG. IQ in childhood and the metabolic syndrome in middle age: extended follow-up of the 1946 British Birth Cohort Study. Intelligence 2009; 37: 567–572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. 15Gaysina D, Hotopf M, Richards M, Colman I, Kuh D, Hardy R. Symptoms of depression and anxiety, and change in body mass index from adolescence to adulthood: results from a British birth cohort. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 175–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. 16Hasler G, Pine DS, Kleinbaum DG, Gamma A, Luckenbaugh D, Ajdacic V et al. Depressive symptoms during childhood and adult obesity: the Zurich Cohort Study. Mol Psychiatry 2005; 10: 842–850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. 17Stafford M, Black S, Shah I, Hardy R, Pierce M, Richards M et al. Using a birth cohort to study ageing: representativeness and response rates in the National Survey of Health and Development. Eur J Ageing 2013; 10: 145–157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. 18Elander J, Rutter M. Use and development of the Rutter parents' and teachers' scale. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1996; 6: 63–78. [Google Scholar]
  19. 19Rutter M, Tizard J, Whitmore K. Education, Health and Behaviour. Longman Publishing Group: London, UK, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  20. 20Pigeon DA. Tests Used in the 1954 and 1957 Surveys., Macgibbon & Kee, 1964.
  21. 21Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS. Springer Verlag: New York, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  22. 22R Core TeamR: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  23. 23Chriqui JF, Pickel M, Story M. Influence of school competitive food and beverage policies on obesity, consumption, and availability: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatr 2014; 168: 279–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. 24McIntyre RS, Cha DS, Jerrell JM, Soczynska JK, Woldeyohannes HO, Taylor V et al. Obesity and mental illness: implications for cognitive functioning. Adv Ther 2013; 30: 577–588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. 25Whitmer RA, Gunderson EP, Barrett-Connor E, Quesenberry Jr CP, Yaffe K. Obesity in middle age and future risk of dementia: a 27 year longitudinal population based study. BMJ 2005; 330: 1360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. 26Ohlendorf JM. Stages of change in the trajectory of postpartum weight self-management. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 2012; 41: 57–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. 27Wareham NJ, Jakes RW, Rennie KL, Mitchell J, Hennings S, Day NE. Validity and repeatability of the EPIC-Norfolk Physical Activity Questionnaire. Int J Epidemiol 2002; 31: 168–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. 28Riccardi P, Park S, Anderson S, Doop M, Ansari MS, Schmidt D et al. Sex differences in the relationship of regional dopamine release to affect and cognitive function in striatal and extrastriatal regions using positron emission tomography and [(1)(8)F]fallypride. Synapse 2011; 65: 99–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. 29Casey BJ, Duhoux S, Malter Cohen M. Adolescence: what do transmission, transition, and translation have to do with it? Neuron 2010; 67: 749–760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. 30Whittle S, Yucel M, Yap MB, Allen NB. Sex differences in the neural correlates of emotion: evidence from neuroimaging. Biol Psychol 2011; 87: 319–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. 31Goldin AP, Hermida MJ, Shalom DE, Elias CostaM, Lopez-Rosenfeld M, Segretin MS et al. Far transfer to language and math of a short software-based gaming intervention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014; 111: 6443–6448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Obesity (2005) are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES