Skip to main content
Taylor & Francis Open Select logoLink to Taylor & Francis Open Select
. 2015 Aug 27;21(1):67–75. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2015.1033115

The Role of Patient Activation in Preferences for Shared Decision Making: Results From a National Survey of U.S. Adults

Samuel G Smith a,b,*, Anjali Pandit a, Steven R Rush c, Michael S Wolf a, Carol J Simon d
PMCID: PMC4706032  PMID: 26313690

Abstract

Studies investigating preferences for shared decision making (SDM) have focused on associations with sociodemographic variables, with few investigations exploring patient factors. We aimed to investigate the relationship between patient activation and preferences for SDM in 6 common medical decisions among a nationally representative cross-sectional survey of American adults. Adults older than 18 were recruited online (n = 2,700) and by telephone (n = 700). Respondents completed sociodemographic assessments and the Patient Activation Measure. They were also asked whether they perceived benefit (yes/no) in SDM in 6 common medical decisions. Nearly half of the sample (45.9%) reached the highest level of activation (Level 4). Activation was associated with age (p < .001), higher income (p = .001), higher education (p = .010), better self-rated health (p < .001), and fewer chronic conditions (p = .050). The proportion of people who agreed that SDM was beneficial varied from 53.1% (deciding the necessity of a diagnostic test) to 71.8% (decisions associated with making lifestyle changes). After we controlled for participant characteristics, higher activation was associated with greater perceived benefit in SDM across 4 of the 6 decisions. Preferences for SDM varied among 6 common medical scenarios. Low patient activation is an important barrier to SDM that could be ameliorated through the development of behavioral interventions.


The health care community is increasingly embracing shared decision making (SDM; Kon, 2010), with several large organizations and patient groups recommending it as the preferred communicative approach in medical care (Davidson et al., 2007; Institute of Medicine, 2001; Mercurio, Maxwell, Mears, Ross, & Silber, 2008; Sheridan, Harris, & Woolf, 2004). These positions are supported by ethical principles and federal and state laws (Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, 2011; Kuehn, 2009). Although the definitions of SDM are contested, clarifying and including patient preferences within the medical decision-making process and discussing all available treatment options are generally accepted components (Makoul & Clayman, 2006). SDM can lead to improved decision making (Krones et al., 2008; McCaffery, Smith, & Wolf, 2010; Stacey et al., 2014; Zikmund-Fisher, Couper, Singer, Ubel, et al., 2010) and reduced health care costs (Arterburn et al., 2012; Veroff, Marr, & Wennberg, 2013; Wennberg, Marr, Lang, O'Malley, & Bennett, 2010). The relationship between SDM and health outcomes is mixed (Joosten et al., 2008; Stacey et al., 2014; Street, Makoul, Arora, & Epstein, 2009; Ward et al., 2003; Wilson et al., 2010).

The nationally representative DECISIONS survey found that the majority (80%) of Americans had made a medical decision in the previous 2 years (Zikmund-Fisher, Couper, Singer, Levin, et al., 2010). However, only 40% to 65% of the decisions were shared between the patient and the provider (Zikmund-Fisher, Couper, Singer, Ubel, et al., 2010). Preferences for involvement in medical decision making can vary according to the situation (Say, Murtagh, & Thomson, 2006). For example, members of the public express more interest in being involved in disease prevention decisions such as smoking cessation and lifestyle change (Mansell, Poses, Kazis, & Duefield, 2000) but prefer to be more passive in situations with more immediate consequences (e.g., cancer treatment; Chewning et al., 2012; Davis, Hoffman, & Hsu, 1999). Yet these generalizations are not universal, and research investigating variation across decision types is needed (Mansell et al., 2000; Waller et al., 2012).

Early studies showed that patient characteristics such as male gender, worse self-reported health, and lower socioeconomic status predicted less preference for SDM (Arora & McHorney, 2000; Kiesler & Auerbach, 2006; Say et al., 2006). However, these studies often used small samples from specific clinical populations, which can limit generalizations. A limited number of nationally representative studies are available (Levinson, Kao, Kuby, & Thisted, 2005; Zikmund-Fisher, Couper, Singer, Ubel, et al., 2010); however, additional data are needed that go beyond associations with sociodemographic factors, which have explained comparatively little variance in preferences for SDM (Say et al., 2006). SDM requires the patient to recognize his or her own values and incorporate them within the decision-making process. Thus, psychological factors such as motivation and engagement in a health and medical context may be important concepts to study. To a large extent, patient activation encapsulates these factors; it refers to the degree to which an individual possesses knowledge, motivation, skills, and confidence to make effective health-related decisions (Hibbard, Mahoney, Stockard, & Tusler, 2005). Activated patients have better physical and mental health (Smith, Curtis, Wardle, von Wagner, & Wolf, 2013), more optimal clinical indicators (Greene & Hibbard, 2012; Marshall et al., 2013), and lower health care costs (Greene & Hibbard, 2012). Disparities in activation exist across economic and ethnic groups (Greene & Hibbard, 2012; Hibbard & Cunningham, 2008), and simulated models indicate that meaningful reductions in health inequalities could occur if activation differences were eliminated (Hibbard et al., 2008).

The relationship between activation and SDM preferences is underinvestigated, but preliminary evidence suggests that activating patients can increase SDM (Wilkes et al., 2013). Activated patients seek more health-related information and are more knowledgeable about health care (Butler, Farley, Sleath, Murray, & Maciejewski, 2012; Fowles et al., 2009; Harvey, Fowles, Xi, & Terry, 2012). They may therefore be better prepared prior to consultations and show greater preferences for involvement in medical decision making. To progress the field and inform future intervention strategies, we used nationally representative data to investigate the role of patient activation and sociodemographic factors within preferences for SDM across six common medical decisions. Based on previous evidence, we hypothesized that (a) preferences for SDM would be higher in situations involving disease prevention (e.g., screening and lifestyle decisions) and lower when the consequences are more immediate/important (e.g., diagnostic testing and treatment); (b) higher patient activation would be associated with greater perceived benefit in SDM; (c) members of lower socioeconomic groups (as measured by income, education, and race), men, younger respondents, and those with worse health would have lower levels of activation; and (d) members of lower socioeconomic groups, younger respondents, and those with better health would perceive less benefit in SDM.

Methods

Design

Data were from a nationally representative survey of English-speaking U.S. adults age 18 years and older. The survey was conducted by Harris Interactive between August 14 and September 17, 2013. To ensure full representation of U.S. adults, a mixed-mode methodology was used that combined an online panel (n = 2,700) and random-digit dialing (RDD; n = 700) sampling and data collection. The telephone sample included landline and cell phone connections and was randomized prior to dialing. For the online sample, a consumer research panel associated with Harris Interactive was stratified by U.S. Census parameters (education, age by gender, race, region, and household income) and then invited by e-mail to take part in a self-administered survey (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012). The e-mail invitation included a secure and unique URL and password to ensure that the respondent could only complete one survey. Excluding disconnected lines, a total of 15,050 working phone numbers yielded 838 participating respondents, for an overall response rate of 5.6% in the RDD sample. Of the 838 respondents who were screened, 700 met the age qualification (U.S. resident, age 18 or older) specified in the study design. A response rate for the online sample is not reported as probability sampling was not used.

In accordance with polling industry standards, respondents' confidentiality was ensured such that they were not identified by name or any other personal identifiers. As a member of the Council of American Survey Research Organizations (CASRO), the research company conducted this survey in accordance with the CASRO Code of Standards and Ethics for Survey Research.

Measures

SDM

To assess preferences for patient involvement in SDM, a series of common medical decisions were presented to respondents with the following introductory statement:

There has been much discussion lately about patients participating with their healthcare providers in making decisions about their health. For which of these topic areas do you think it is beneficial for patients to share in decisions with their healthcare provider?

The following medical decisions were provided: lifestyle changes, such as diet and exercise; preventive screening tests; choosing between completely different approaches to treating a diagnosed condition (e.g., medication vs. surgery); choosing a specialist for referral; choosing between different medications that might be prescribed for a diagnosed condition; and deciding whether it is necessary to have a diagnostic test. The response items were yes and no. To prevent ordering effects, the decisions were presented in a random order for each participant.

Patient Activation Measure

Patient activation was assessed using the Patient Activation Measure Short Form (Hibbard et al., 2005). This is a 13-item measure designed to assess self-reported knowledge, skill, and confidence for self-management of one's health and chronic conditions. The measure has been shown to be reliable and valid across a range of clinical conditions and contexts (Hibbard, Stockard, Mahoney, & Tusler, 2004; Hung et al., 2013; Skolasky et al., 2011). Participants were asked to respond to statements that people sometimes make when they talk about their health. Example statements included “When all is said and done, I am the person who is responsible for managing my health condition(s)” and “I am confident that I can take actions that will help prevent or minimize some symptoms or problems associated with my health condition(s).” Response items included strongly disagree, slightly disagree, slightly agree, strongly agree, not applicable, don't know, and refused. A score of 0 (low activation) to 100 (high activation) was computed, and respondents were classified into four levels of activation using predefined cutoffs: Level 1—may not yet believe that the patient role is important (0–45.2), Level 2—lacks confidence and knowledge to take action (47.4–52.9), Level 3—beginning to take action (56.4–66.0), Level 4—has difficulty maintaining behaviors over time (68.5–100). The measure had good internal reliability in this sample (α = .90).

Participant Characteristics

Data on gender, age, race (White, Black, Asian or Pacific Islander, other), income (<$15,000, $15,000–$34,999, $35,000–$49,999, $50,000+), education (high school or less, some college, college graduate, graduate school), region (East, Midwest, South, West), self-rated health (poor, fair, good, very good, excellent), and self-reported chronic conditions (0, 1, or 2+) were collected.

Statistical Analysis

RDD data were weighted to U.S. Census targets for education, age by gender, race, region, and household income to bring them into line with the population of U.S. residents age 18 and older (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012). The weighting algorithm also took into account landline versus cell phone usage using information from the 2012 National Health Interview Survey (Blumberg & Luke, 2012) and probability of selection using number of telephone lines, number of adults in the household, and recent absence of a phone connection. The online data were also weighted to the same applicable parameters as the telephone RDD sample. However, the algorithm included a propensity score that allowed for adjustment for attitudinal and behavioral differences between online and offline populations, those who join online panels, and likelihood of response. The RDD and online sample data were combined into a proportioned total using Internet usage information (Pew Research Center, 2013).

Complex samples analyses using the weighting variable were performed using the following statistical tests. Chi-square analyses explored which participant characteristics were associated with patient activation. Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the associations between patient activation, participant characteristics, and perceived benefit in SDM in each decision were computed using multivariate logistic regression. Significance was set at p < .05, and all analyses were performed in SPSS Version 22.

Results

Sample Characteristics

As shown in Table 1, the weighted analyses showed that respondents were more likely to be female (52.8%), younger (18–30 = 21.9%, 31–40 = 16.6%), White (77.5%), and from the southern United States (33.4%). Half (50.0%) of the respondents had an income >$50,000, and more than a quarter (28.0%) had at least a university-level education. Self-reported health was high (good = 35.9%, very good = 33.4%, excellent = 14.1%), and the majority reported no chronic conditions (66.5%). In line with the recruitment strategy, most people completed the survey online (77.0%).

Table 1. Participant characteristics and percentage at highest level of activation.

Characteristic n % Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4 χ2 significance
Gender
 Male 1,382 47.2 12.4 12.1 30.3 45.2 .400
 Female 2,018 52.8 10.2 13.8 29.4 46.6  
Age
 18–30 585 21.9 16.4 12.3 26.2 45.0 .000
 31–40 537 16.6 7.6 11.3 34.2 47.0  
 41–50 608 18.5 14.4 11.5 29.3 44.9  
 51–60 649 17.3 8.9 12.2 33.5 45.4  
 61–70 656 16.1 7.4 12.7 26.2 53.7  
 71–80 281 7.3 12.1 19.6 30.9 37.4  
 80+ 84 2.4 7.1 28.9 32.1 32.0  
Ethnicity
 White 2,717 77.5 10.0 12.8 30.5 46.7 .149
 Black 346 11.4 12.9 10.5 27.2 49.5  
 Asian or Pacific Islander 101 2.2 18.1 18.4 31.7 31.7  
 Other 199 8.9 16.4 16.4 28.2 39.0  
Income
 <$15,000 396 9.7 15.1 16.5 30.0 38.4 .001
 $15,000–$34,999 773 18.3 13.1 15.5 30.2 41.2  
 $35,000–$49,999 449 12.9 14.0 14.7 31.7 39.6  
 >$50,000 1,500 50.0 8.4 10.6 28.8 52.1  
 Missing 282 9.2 15.1 14.7 31.8 38.4  
Education
 High school or less 884 43.2 13.9 13.9 29.8 42.3 .010
 Some college 1,202 28.9 9.8 12.6 32.7 45.0  
 College graduate 886 20.4 8.4 12.3 27.4 51.9  
 Graduate school 411 7.6 10.2 11.1 23.9 54.8  
Region
 East 764 21.5 10.3 12.5 31.6 45.5 .804
 Midwest 833 22.4 11.3 12.4 31.8 44.5  
 South 1,041 33.4 10.6 12.5 29.0 47.9  
 West 762 22.7 13.0 14.8 27.3 44.8  
Survey mode
 Online 2,700 77.0 10.8 12.5 29.7 47.1 .317
 Phone 700 23.0 12.9 14.7 30.3 42.1  
Self-rated health
 Poor 132 3.6 27.6 17.5 20.3 34.6 .000
 Fair 514 13.0 22.8 18.0 29.2 29.9  
 Good 1,324 35.9 13.0 16.5 34.7 35.8  
 Very good 1,042 33.4 5.4 9.4 30.4 54.7  
 Excellent 388 14.1 6.2 6.9 19.3 67.5  
Chronic conditions
 0 983 33.5 7.9 11.9 32.1 48.1 .050
 1 816 24.3 11.2 13.4 30.1 45.2  
 2+ 1,601 42.2 13.8 13.5 28.0 44.7  

Note. n may not round to 3,400 because of missing data; percentages are weighted.

Patient Activation

The mean level of patient activation was 66.6 (SD = 16.8). There were fewer people in the lower levels of patient activation (Level 1 = 11.3%, Level 2 = 13.0%, Level 3 = 29.8%), with nearly half (45.9%) of the sample reaching the highest level of activation (Level 4). There was a relationship between patient activation and age (p < .001). Respondents age 61–70 were most likely to be in the highest level of activation (53.7%), and the youngest and oldest age groups were less likely (see Table 1). Income (p = .001) and education (p = .010) were significantly associated with patient activation. The highest levels of activation were observed in those with the highest income ($50,000 + = 52.1%) and highest education (graduate school = 54.8%). Asians or Pacific Islanders (31.7%) or the “other” race category (39.0%) had notably lower levels of activation, but there was no overall effect of race (p = .149). Respondents with better self-rated health (p < .001) and fewer chronic conditions (p = .050) were more likely to have higher activation. Activation levels were similar between genders (p = .400), regions (p = .804), and survey modes (p = .317).

SDM

Respondents generally indicated that they saw benefit in SDM across all medical decisions (see Table 2). The proportion of endorsement ranged from 53.1% (deciding the necessity of a diagnostic test) to 71.8% (decisions regarding lifestyle changes). Table 2 describes the relationship between SDM and patient activation in univariate analyses for each of the six decisions. Differences were observed for lifestyle (p < .001), preventive screening (p < .001), choosing treatments (p = .012), choosing between medications (p = .018), and deciding the necessity of a diagnostic test (p = .021). There were no differences across activation groups for the decision related to choosing a specialist for referral (p = .804).

Table 2. Likelihood of perceived benefit in shared decision making by decision type and activation level.

Decision type Overall (%) Level 1 (%) Level 2 (%) Level 3 (%) Level 4 (%) χ2
Lifestyle change 71.8 62.6 70.5 71.5 77.8 .000
Preventive screening tests 59.2 47.6 59.7 59.1 64.6 .000
Choosing between different treatment approaches 60.1 56.1 58.7 57.7 65.7 .012
Choosing a specialist for referral 54.7 52.7 56.0 56.9 55.6 .804
Choosing between different medications 58.0 49.7 58.6 58.1 62.3 .018
Deciding the necessity of a diagnostic test 53.1 50.2 50.1 51.3 58.5 .021

In multivariate analyses, patient activation was associated with perceived benefit in SDM in four of the six decisions (see Table 3). Compared with the reference category (low activation, Level 1), respondents with the highest level of activation (Level 4) were more likely to see benefit in SDM for decisions related to lifestyle (OR = 2.13, 95% CI [1.43, 3.18]), preventive screening (OR = 2.02, 95% CI [1.41, 2.91]), choosing treatments (OR = 1.50, 95% CI [1.00, 2.13]), and choosing medications (OR = 1.82, 95% CI [1.25, 2.66]). Respondents in Level 3 activation were more likely to see benefit in SDM for decisions related to preventive screening (OR = 1.65, 95% CI [1.14, 2.38]) and choosing medications (OR = 1.53, 95% CI [1.04, 2.24]). Respondents in Level 2 activation were only more likely to see benefit for the preventive screening decision (OR = 1.62, 95% CI [1.08, 2.44]).

Table 3. Weighted multivariate logistic regression predicting a “yes” response to seeing perceived benefit in shared decision making.

Characteristic Lifestyle
Preventive screening
Choosing treatments
Choosing specialists
Choosing medications
Diagnostic testing
OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI]
Gender
 Male Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 Female 1.21 [0.97, 1.53] 1.10 [0.90, 1.36] 1.44 [1.17, 1.78] 1.46 [1.19, 1.79] 1.18 [0.96, 1.44] 1.14 [0.93, 1.39]
Age
 18–30 Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 31–40 1.08 [0.75, 1.55] 1.30 [0.92, 1.83] 0.95 [0.67, 1.35] 0.97 [0.68, 1.36] 1.02 [0.72, 1.44] 1.12 [0.79, 1.59]
 41–50 1.10 [0.77, 1.58] 1.25 [0.89, 1.75] 0.99 [0.71, 1.39] 0.92 [0.66, 1.28] 1.06 [0.76, 1.48] 1.21 [0.87, 1.70]
 51–60 2.03 [1.37, 3.00] 2.05 [1.45, 2.89] 1.63 [1.15, 2.30] 1.69 [1.21, 2.36] 1.38 [0.98, 1.93] 1.80 [1.29, 2.52]
 61–70 1.65 [1.13, 2.41] 2.06 [1.44, 2.94] 1.49 [1.04, 2.14] 1.81 [1.29, 2.55] 1.35 [0.95, 1.92] 2.09 [1.47, 3.00]
 71–80 1.86 [1.07, 3.22] 2.57 [1.57, 4.20] 1.46 [0.93, 2.31] 1.84 [1.16, 2.95] 1.12 [0.71, 1.77] 2.33 [1.49, 3.64]
 80+ 1.49 [0.70, 3.17] 1.29 [0.69, 2.45] 1.18 [0.61, 2.28] 2.05 [1.02, 4.11] 1.30 [0.67, 2.53] 2.83 [1.44, 5.58]
Ethnicity
 White Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 Black 0.87 [0.60, 1.26] 0.77 [0.55, 1.07] 0.81 [0.58, 1.13] 1.03 [0.74, 1.43] 0.67 [0.48, 0.93] 0.86 [0.61, 1.21]
 Asian or Pacific Islander 0.92 [0.42, 2.00] 0.53 [0.27, 1.05] 0.45 [0.26, 0.81] 0.59 [0.28, 1.23] 0.47 [0.25, 0.89] 0.83 [0.42, 1.63]
 Other 1.22 [0.79, 1.88] 1.07 [0.71, 1.62] 1.61 [1.04, 2.50] 1.49 [0.99, 2.23] 1.42 [0.94, 2.13] 1.80 [1.19, 2.72]
Income
 <$15,000 Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 $15,000–$34,999 0.87 [0.58, 1.30] 1.21 [0.83, 1.77] 1.22 [0.84, 1.76] 1.41 [0.98, 2.03] 0.91 [0.63, 1.33] 0.97 [0.67, 1.41]
 $35,000–$49,999 1.09 [0.69, 1.73] 1.09 [0.72, 1.66] 1.05 [0.69, 1.59] 1.17 [0.77, 1.78] 0.96 [0.63, 1.45] 0.77 [0.51, 1.18]
 $50,000+ 0.98 [0.67, 1.45] 1.12 [0.78, 1.61] 0.97 [0.68, 1.39] 1.42 [0.99, 2.04] 0.93 [0.65, 1.34] 0.95 [0.66, 1.37]
 Missing 1.43 [0.82, 2.49] 2.16 [1.29, 3.60] 1.14 [0.71, 1.85] 2.59 [1.58, 4.24] 1.36 [0.81, 2.26] 1.43 [0.87, 2.35]
Education
 High school or less Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 Some college 1.47 [1.12, 1.95] 1.24 [0.96, 1.59] 1.12 [0.87, 1.44] 1.15 [0.89, 1.47] 0.92 [0.71, 1.18] 1.00 [0.78, 1.28]
 College graduate 1.70 [1.23, 2.40] 1.60 [1.18, 2.15] 1.63 [1.21, 2.19] 1.24 [0.92, 1.65] 1.03 [0.78, 1.37] 1.02 [0.76, 1.37]
 Graduate school 1.99 [1.28, 3.11] 1.57 [1.06, 2.31] 2.54 [1.71, 3.79] 1.57 [1.08, 2.28] 1.17 [0.81, 1.68] 0.92 [0.63, 1.32]
Patient activation
 Level 1 Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 Level 2 1.33 [0.86, 2.08] 1.62 [1.08, 2.44] 1.06 [0.70, 1.60] 1.13 [0.74, 1.70] 1.47 [0.97, 2.22] 0.92 [0.61, 1.38]
 Level 3 1.42 [0.95, 2.12] 1.65 [1.14, 2.38] 1.05 [0.71, 1.54] 1.25 [0.86, 1.83] 1.53 [1.04, 2.24] 1.04 [0.71, 1.51]
 Level 4 2.13 [1.43, 3.18] 2.02 [1.41, 2.91] 1.50 [1.00, 2.13] 1.17 [0.81, 1.71] 1.82 [1.25, 2.66] 1.31 [0.97, 2.05]
Self-rated health
 Poor 1.35 [0.62, 2.93] 1.80 [0.88, 3.66] 1.39 [0.68, 2.82] 2.93 [1.51, 5.69] 1.51 [0.73, 3.13] 1.95 [0.95, 4.00]
 Fair 1.41 [0.88, 2.28] 1.13 [0.73, 1.74] 0.99 [0.64, 1.52] 1.22 [0.81, 1.85] 0.91 [0.59, 1.41] 0.82 [0.53, 1.25]
 Good 1.37 [0.93, 2.03] 0.80 [0.56, 1.12] 0.89 [0.62, 1.26] 0.87 [0.62, 1.23] 0.81 [0.57, 1.15] 0.67 [0.48, 0.95]
 Very good 1.40 [0.95, 2.05] 0.87 [0.61, 1.23] 1.03 [0.73, 1.45] 1.09 [0.78, 1.52] 0.85 [0.60, 1.19] 0.79 [0.56, 1.11]
 Excellent Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
Chronic conditions
 0 Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref Ref
 1 0.99 [0.74, 1.34] 1.17 [0.90, 1.55] 1.17 [0.88, 1.54] 0.95 [0.72, 1.25] 1.19 [0.98, 1.69] 0.98 [0.74, 1.29]
 2+ 1.34 [0.99, 1.82] 1.32 [1.00, 1.72] 1.36 [1.04, 1.79] 1.16 [0.90, 1.51] 1.62 [1.25, 2.11] 1.14 [0.88, 1.49]

Note. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval.

To various extents, participant characteristics were associated with SDM preferences. Women were more likely to see benefit in the decisions related to choosing treatment (OR = 1.44, 95% CI [1.17, 1.78]) and choosing a specialist (OR = 1.46, 95% CI [1.19, 1.79]). Older respondents were generally more likely to see benefit in SDM than the youngest participants. Black respondents were less likely to see benefit in decisions related to medication choice (OR = 0.81, 95% CI [0.58, 1.13]), whereas Asian or Pacific Islanders were less likely to see benefit in choosing treatments (OR = 0.45, 95% CI [0.26, 0.81]) and choosing medications (OR = 0.47, 95% CI [0.25, 0.89]). Members of the “other” race category were more likely to see benefit in choosing treatments (OR = 1.61, 95% CI [1.04, 2.50]) and diagnostic testing (OR = 1.80, 95% CI [1.19, 2.72]). Income was rarely a predictor of SDM preferences, with the exception of the “missing” category and decisions related to preventive screening (OR = 2.16, 95% CI [1.29, 3.60]) and choosing a specialist (OR = 2.59, 95% CI [1.58, 4.24]).

Higher education was associated with preferences for SDM. For example, compared with the least educated (high school or less), those who attended graduate school were more likely to perceive benefit in SDM for decisions related to lifestyle (OR = 1.99, 95% CI [1.28, 3.11]), preventive screening (OR = 1.57, 95% CI [1.06, 2.31]), choosing treatments (OR = 2.54, 95% CI [1.71, 3.79]), and choosing specialists (OR = 1.57, 95% CI [1.08, 2.28]). Respondents with college education were more likely to perceive benefit in the decisions related to lifestyle (OR = 1.70, 95% CI [1.23, 2.40]), preventive screening (OR = 1.60, 95% CI [1.18, 2.15]), and choosing treatment (OR = 1.63, 95% CI [1.21, 2.19]).

The impact of health status varied across decisions. Compared with those with excellent self-reported health, respondents with poor self-rated health were more likely to see benefit in SDM when choosing a specialist (OR = 2.93, 95% CI [1.51, 5.69]). Respondents with good health were less likely to see benefit in SDM in diagnostic testing (OR = 0.67, 95% CI [0.56, 1.11]), but there was no discernable effect for fair or very good health. Those with two or more chronic conditions were more likely to see benefit in SDM for decisions related to preventive screening (OR = 1.32, 95% CI [1.00, 1.72]), choosing treatment (OR = 1.36, 95% CI [1.04, 1.79]), and choosing medications (OR = 1.62, 95% CI [1.25, 2.11]).

Discussion

In this nationally representative survey the majority of U.S. adults perceived at least some benefit in SDM, but there was a sizeable minority who did not. The attitudes of the population should be considered by large organizations and policymakers who are increasingly advocating a position of SDM (Davidson et al., 2007; Institute of Medicine, 2001; Mercurio et al., 2008; Sheridan et al., 2004). Perceived benefit in SDM varied according to the decision. Patients preferred greater involvement in decisions involving disease prevention (e.g., screening) and lifestyle change. Less perceived benefit was reported for decisions involving immediate consequences, such as deciding the necessity of a diagnostic test. However, this assertion did not generalize to decisions involving treatment and medication, for which the consequences may also be expected to be important and immediate. Overall, estimates of interest in SDM broadly reflected actual occurrences of SDM reported in a nationally representative sample (Zikmund-Fisher, Couper, Singer, Ubel, et al., 2010).

Although our study design prohibits inferences of causality, our findings suggest that low levels of patient activation may be a barrier to SDM. Respondents reaching the highest activation level were consistently more likely to perceive benefit in SDM across all medical decisions, with the exception of choosing a specialist. The relationship between activation and perceived benefit in SDM among the lower levels of activation (Levels 2 and 3) was less consistent. For example, compared with the least activated respondents, those classified at Levels 2, 3, or 4 of activation were more likely to see benefit in SDM in the preventive screening decision and in choosing medications. However, for other decisions (e.g., lifestyle changes and choosing medical treatments), it was not until the highest levels of activation were reached that respondents perceived benefit.

Patient activation appeared to affect preferences most in decisions regarding preventive screening and choosing medications, in which even moderately activated respondents preferred greater involvement than those who were least activated. In contrast, decisions involving lifestyle changes and choosing treatments may need patients to increase to the highest activation level before benefits will be seen in terms of SDM preferences. Interventions such as preparing patients prior to consultations through preference elicitation and clarification have been somewhat effective at increasing activation (Deen, Lu, Rothstein, Santana, & Gold, 2011). However, they may not be sufficiently effective among the least activated for improvements to be seen in SDM preferences in some decisions. Decisions involving treatment choices may require alternative multifaceted approaches, such as decision aids plus patient preparation, to promote the necessary amount of activation (Deen et al., 2012; Hibbard, Greene, & Tusler, 2009).

Our data indicated that a little less than half (45.9%) of the population were classified as having Level 4 activation, which is reflective of previous national estimates (Hibbard & Cunningham, 2008). Our data also support evidence showing that patient activation is associated with markers of socioeconomic status, including income and education, and other demographic factors, such as age (Greene & Hibbard, 2012; Hibbard & Cunningham, 2008). Patient activation was higher among healthier individuals, as measured by self-reported health and the number of chronic conditions. To our knowledge, this has not been reported in nationally representative samples, although it has been suggested by research in specific clinical populations (Chubak et al., 2012). In contrast to previous national estimates, there was no overall effect of race on patient activation levels (Hibbard & Cunningham, 2008). However, the prevalence of high activation among Asian and Pacific Islander respondents was noticeably lower than among the other ethnicities. We found no evidence of gender differences, but only small effects were noted in the large sample reported by Hibbard and Cunningham (2008).

In line with previous research, low education was consistently associated with perceiving less benefit in SDM (Arora & McHorney, 2000). One possibility is that people with low levels of education lack the skills to adequately process health information and therefore prefer to rely on expert opinion to make medical decisions. The term health literacy generally describes these skills and has been defined by the Institute of Medicine as “the degree to which individuals have the capacity to obtain, process and understand basic health information and services needed to make appropriate health decisions” (Ratzan & Parker, 2000, p. 19). In previous analyses we showed that health literacy and patient activation influenced health outcomes through different pathways, and this may also be the case with SDM (Smith et al., 2013). Future research should consider investigating the importance of these two variables in the formation of SDM preferences.

No effects were consistently observed for income on SDM preferences, perhaps because of confounding with education. Effects by race were observed, with respondents classified as “other” more likely to see benefit in choosing treatments, specialists, and diagnostic testing. Differences across race/ethnicity were also observed for the treatment and medications decisions. In support of previous findings we observed higher preferences for SDM among those who were older (Levinson et al., 2005), but gender effects did not often reach statistical significance. These inconsistencies across conditions further emphasize the importance of specifying the type of decision when asking about SDM preferences. They also highlight areas in which disparities are most likely to occur, providing guidance for prospective interventions.

This study has limitations. Items were phrased to ask whether respondents could “see benefit” in SDM rather than specifically using the word preference. The introductory statement to the SDM item had a readability score of 46 out of 100, which is equivalent to a reading age of 16 years. The text may therefore have been difficult for people with lower levels of literacy to comprehend. The study is further limited by the simplicity of the items used. Alternative approaches (e.g., qualitative approaches) may yield a richer data set but would be limited by their low external validity (Edwards & Elwyn, 2006; Elwyn, Edwards, Kinnersley, & Grol, 2000). The decisions presented were hypothetical, and therefore reported preferences may not reflect attitudes held by patients actually making those judgments. Similarly, we were unable to collect data on previous experience with decision making, and it is unclear whether respondents had experience in each scenario. Response and selection biases were limited through weighting procedures, but it is possible that the online panel sample may have been more activated than an offline population. We may therefore have overestimated the level of patient activation in the general U.S. sample, and associations with demographic variables may be conservative estimates. The response rates in our RDD survey were also low in comparison with similar studies (Cantor et al., 2009; Cantor, Covell, Davis, Park, & Rizzo, 2007). Finally, it should be acknowledged that our data were cross-sectional, which limits our ability to assert causality.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this nationally representative study indicates that there is a discord between policy and public attitudes toward SDM. Although clinicians are encouraged to promote SDM, a large proportion of the public in this study reported not perceiving any benefit in the practice. Patient activation was a consistent predictor of SDM preferences, indicating that intervention strategies targeting the construct may have beneficial effects on public attitudes toward communication and decision making in clinical encounters. Further prospective and experimental research is warranted on the basis of these findings.

Acknowledgment

This work was presented at the April 2014 conference of the Society of Behavioral Medicine in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.

References

  1. Arora N. K., McHorney C. A. Patient preferences for medical decision making: Who really wants to participate? Medical Care. 2000;38(3):335–341. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200003000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arterburn D., Wellman R., Westbrook E., Rutter C., Ross T., McCulloch D., … Jung C. Introducing decision aids at group health was linked to sharply lower hip and knee surgery rates and costs. Health Affairs. 2012;31(9):2094–2104. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blumberg S. J., Luke J. V. Wireless substitution: Early release of estimates from the National Health Interview Survey, January–June 2012. 2012. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhis/earlyrelease/wireless201212.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  4. Butler M. G., Farley J. F., Sleath B. L., Murray M. D., Maciejewski M. L. Medicare part D information seeking: The role of recognition of need and patient activation. Research in Social & Administrative Pharmacy. 2012;8(5):433–442. doi: 10.1016/j.sapharm.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cantor D., Coa K., Crystal-Mansour S., Davis T., Dipko S., Sigman R. Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS) 2007 final report. 2009. Retrieved from http://hints.cancer.gov/docs/HINTS2007FinalReport.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cantor D., Covell J., Davis T., Park I., Rizzo L. Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS) 2005 final report. 2007. Retrieved from http://hints.cancer.gov/docs/HINTS_2005_Final_Report.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  7. Medicare program; Medicare shared savings: accountable care organizations: proposed rule. Federal Register. 2011;76(57):19527–19654. Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chewning B., Bylund C. L., Shah B., Arora N. K., Gueguen J. A., Makoul G. Patient preferences for shared decisions: A systematic review. Patient Education and Counseling. 2012;86(1):9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chubak J., Anderson M. L., Saunders K. W., Hubbard R. A., Tuzzio L., Liss D. T., … Reid R. J. Predictors of 1-year change in patient activation in older adults with diabetes mellitus and heart disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2012;60(7):1316–1321. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04008.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cvengros J. A., Christensen A. J., Hillis S. L., Rosenthal G. E. Patient and physician attitudes in the health care context: Attitudinal symmetry predicts patient satisfaction and adherence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2007;33(3):262–268. doi: 10.1007/bf02879908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Davidson J. E., Powers K., Hedayat K. M., Tieszen M., Kon A. A., Shepard E., … Armstrong D. Clinical practice guidelines for support of the family in the patient-centered intensive care unit: American College of Critical Care Medicine Task Force 2004–2005. Critical Care Medicine. 2007;35(2):605–622. doi: 10.1097/01.CCM.0000254067.14607.EB. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Davis M. A., Hoffman J. R., Hsu J. Impact of patient acuity on preference for information and autonomy in decision making. Academic Emergency Medicine. 1999;6(8):781–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.1999.tb01206.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Deen D., Lu W.-H., Rothstein D., Santana L., Gold M. R. Asking questions: The effect of a brief intervention in community health centers on patient activation. Patient Education and Counseling. 2011;84(2):257–260. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Deen D., Lu W. H., Weintraub M. R., Maranda M. J., Elshafey S., Gold M. R. The impact of different modalities for activating patients in a community health center setting. Patient Education and Counseling. 2012;89(1):178–183. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2012.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Edwards A., Elwyn G. Inside the black box of shared decision making: Distinguishing between the process of involvement and who makes the decision. Health Expectations. 2006;9(4):307–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-7625.2006.00401.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Elwyn G., Edwards A., Kinnersley P., Grol R. Shared decision making and the concept of equipoise: The competences of involving patients in healthcare choices. British Journal of General Practice. 2000;50(460):892–899. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ford S., Schofield T., Hope T. Are patients' decision-making preferences being met? Health Expectations. 2003;6(1):72–80. doi: 10.1046/j.1369-6513.2003.00211.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fowles J. B., Terry P., Xi M., Hibbard J., Bloom C. T., Harvey L. Measuring self-management of patients' and employees' health: Further validation of the Patient Activation Measure (PAM) based on its relation to employee characteristics. Patient Education and Counseling. 2009;77(1):116–122. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2009.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Greene J., Hibbard J. H. Why does patient activation matter? An examination of the relationships between patient activation and health-related outcomes. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2012;27(5):520–526. doi: 10.1007/s11606-011-1931-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harvey L., Fowles J. B., Xi M., Terry P. When activation changes, what else changes? The relationship between change in Patient Activation Measure (PAM) and employees' health status and health behaviors. Patient Education and Counseling. 2012;88(2):338–343. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2012.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hibbard J. H., Cunningham P. J. How engaged are consumers in their health and health care, and why does it matter. Research Briefs. 2008;8:1–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hibbard J. H., Greene J., Becker E. R., Roblin D., Painter M. W., Perez D. J., … Tusler M. Racial/ethnic disparities and consumer activation in health. Health Affairs. 2008;27(5):1442–1453. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.27.5.1442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hibbard J. H., Greene J., Tusler M. Improving the outcomes of disease management by tailoring care to the patient's level of activation. American Journal of Managed Care. 2009;15(6):353–360. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hibbard J. H., Mahoney E. R., Stockard J., Tusler M. Development and testing of a short form of the Patient Activation Measure. Health Services Research. 2005;40(6p1):1918–1930. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2005.00438.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hibbard J. H., Stockard J., Mahoney E. R., Tusler M. Development of the Patient Activation Measure (PAM): Conceptualizing and measuring activation in patients and consumers. Health Services Research. 2004;39(4p1):1005–1026. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2004.00269.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hung M., Carter M., Hayden C., Dzierzon R., Morales J., Snow L., … Samore M. Psychometric assessment of the Patient Activation Measure Short Form (PAM-13) in rural settings. Quality of Life Research. 2013;22(3):521–529. doi: 10.1007/s11136-012-0168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Improving the 21st century healthcare system. Crossing the quality chasm: A new healthcare system for the 21st century. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2001. Institute of Medicine. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Joosten E. A. G., DeFuentes-Merillas L., de Weert G. H., Sensky T., van der Staak C. P. F., de Jong C. A. J. Systematic review of the effects of shared decision-making on patient satisfaction, treatment adherence and health status. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2008;77(4):219–226. doi: 10.1159/000126073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kiesler D. J., Auerbach S. M. Optimal matches of patient preferences for information, decision-making and interpersonal behavior: Evidence, models and interventions. Patient Education and Counseling. 2006;61(3):319–341. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kon A. A. The shared decision-making continuum. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2010;304(8):903–904. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Krones T., Keller H., Sönnichsen A., Sadowski E.-M., Baum E., Wegscheider K., … Donner-Banzhoff N. Absolute cardiovascular disease risk and shared decision making in primary care: A randomized controlled trial. Annals of Family Medicine. 2008;6(3):218–227. doi: 10.1370/afm.854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kuehn B. M. States explore shared decision making. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301(24):2539–2541. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Levinson W., Kao A., Kuby A., Thisted R. A. Not all patients want to participate in decision making. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2005;20(6):531–535. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.04101.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Makoul G., Clayman M. L. An integrative model of shared decision making in medical encounters. Patient Education and Counseling. 2006;60(3):301–312. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mansell D., Poses R. M., Kazis L., Duefield C. A. Clinical factors that influence patients' desire for participation in decisions about illness. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2000;160(19):2991–2996. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.19.2991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Marshall R., Beach M. C., Saha S., Mori T., Loveless M. O., Hibbard J. H., … Korthuis P. T. Patient activation and improved outcomes in HIV-infected patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2013;28:668–674. doi: 10.1007/s11606-012-2307-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McCaffery K. J., Smith S. K., Wolf M. The challenge of shared decision making among patients with lower literacy: A framework for research and development. Medical Decision Making. 2010;30(1):35–44. doi: 10.1177/0272989x09342279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mercurio M. R., Maxwell M. A., Mears B. J., Ross L. F., Silber T. J. American Academy of Pediatrics policy statements on bioethics: Summaries and commentaries: Part 2. Pediatrics in Review. 2008;29(3):e15–e22. doi: 10.1542/pir.29-3-e15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Health fact sheet, May 2013. 2013. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/Commentary/2011/November/Pew-Internet-Health.aspx. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ratzan S. C., Parker R. M. Introduction. In: Selden C. R., Zorn M., Ratzan S. C., editors; Parker R. M., editor. National Library of Medicine current bibliographies in medicine: Health literacy. NLM Pub. No. CBM 2000–1. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health, U.S Department of Health and Human Services; 2000. pp. 19–30. [Google Scholar]
  41. Say R., Murtagh M., Thomson R. Patients' preference for involvement in medical decision making: A narrative review. Patient Education and Counseling. 2006;60(2):102–114. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Sheridan S. L., Harris R. P., Woolf S. H. Shared decision making about screening and chemoprevention: A suggested approach from the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2004;26(1):56–66. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2003.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Skolasky R. L., Green A. F., Scharfstein D., Boult C., Reider L., Wegener S. T. Psychometric properties of the Patient Activation Measure among multimorbid older adults. Health Services Research. 2011;46(2):457–478. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2010.01210.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Smith S. G., Curtis L. M., Wardle J., von Wagner C., Wolf M. S. Skill set or mind set? Associations between health literacy, patient activation and health. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(9):e74373. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Stacey D., Légaré F., Col N. F., Bennett C. L., Barry M. J., Eden K. B., … Wu J. H. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2014;1:CD001431. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001431.pub4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Street R. L., Jr., Makoul G., Arora N. K., Epstein R. M. How does communication heal? Pathways linking clinician–patient communication to health outcomes. Patient Education and Counseling. 2009;74(3):295–301. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Current population survey, March supplement. 2012. U.S. Census Bureau. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/cps/data/ [Google Scholar]
  48. Veroff D., Marr A., Wennberg D. E. Enhanced support for shared decision making reduced costs of care for patients with preference-sensitive conditions. Health Affairs. 2013;32(2):285–293. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Waller J., Macedo A., von Wagner C., Simon A. E., Jones C., Hammersley V., … Campbell C. Communication about colorectal cancer screening in Britain: Public preferences for an expert recommendation. British Journal of Cancer. 2012;107(12):1938–1943. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2012.512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ward M. M., Sundaramurthy S., Lotstein D., Bush T. M., Neuwelt C. M., Street R. L., Jr. Participatory patient-physician communication and morbidity in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus. Arthritis & Rheumatism. 2003;49(6):810–818. doi: 10.1002/art.11467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wennberg D. E., Marr A., Lang L., O'Malley S., Bennett G. A randomized trial of a telephone care-management strategy. New England Journal of Medicine. 2010;363(13):1245–1255. doi: 10.1056/nejmsa0902321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilkes M. S., Day F. C., Srinivasan M., Griffin E., Tancredi D. J., Rainwater J. A., … Hoffman J. R. Pairing physician education with patient activation to improve shared decisions in prostate cancer screening: A cluster randomized controlled trial. Annals of Family Medicine. 2013;11(4):324–334. doi: 10.1370/afm.1550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wilson S. R., Strub P., Buist A. S., Knowles S. B., Lavori P. W., Lapidus J., Vollmer W. M. Shared treatment decision making improves adherence and outcomes in poorly controlled asthma. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine. 2010;181(6):566–577. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200906-0907oc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Zikmund-Fisher B. J., Couper M. P., Singer E., Levin C. A., Fowler F. J., Ziniel S., … Fagerlin A. The DECISIONS Study: A nationwide survey of United States adults regarding 9 common medical decisions. Medical Decision Making. 2010;30(5 Suppl):20S–34S. doi: 10.1177/0272989x09353792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Zikmund-Fisher B. J., Couper M. P., Singer E., Ubel P. A., Ziniel S., Fowler F. J., … Fagerlin A. Deficits and variations in patients' experience with making 9 common medical decisions: The DECISIONS survey. Medical Decision Making. 2010;30(5 Suppl):85S–95S. doi: 10.1177/0272989x10380466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Health Communication are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES