Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
letter
. 2015 Nov 3;4:1207. [Version 1] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.7287.1

The dorsal posterior insula is not an island in pain but subserves a fundamental role - Response to: “Evidence against pain specificity in the dorsal posterior insula” by Davis et al.

Andrew R Segerdahl 1,2,a, Melvin Mezue 1,2,b, Thomas W Okell 1, John T Farrar 3, Irene Tracey 1,2
PMCID: PMC4706052  PMID: 26834997

Abstract

An interesting and valuable discussion has arisen from our recent article (Segerdahl, Mezue et al., 2015) and we are pleased here to have the opportunity to expand on the various points we made. Equally important, we wish to correct several important misunderstandings that were made by Davis and colleagues that possibly contributed to their concerns about power when assessing our paper (e.g. actual subject numbers used in control experiment and the reality of the signal-to-noise and sampling of the multi-TI technique we employed). Here, we clarify the methods and analysis plus discuss how we interpret the data in the Brief Communication noting that the extrapolation and inferences made by Davis and colleagues are not consistent with our report or necessarily, in our opinion, what the data supports. We trust this reassures the F1000Research readership regarding the robustness of our results and what we actually concluded in the paper regarding their possible meaning. We are pleased, though, that Davis and colleagues have used our article to raise an important discussion around pain perception, and here offer some further insights towards that broader discussion.

Keywords: Pain, insula, brain imaging, ASL


We actually agree with a main premise of Davis and colleagues’ comments; namely, that the hunt for a single pain region is redundant and relying on 'single pain region' inference-based logic is flawed. The main finding presented in our study is that the contralateral dorsal posterior insula (dpIns) was the only region that was observed to track pain intensity; one of the many fundamental variables that are integrated by a number of other key brain regions into a complete tonic pain experience. At no point in the paper do we say that by identifying this ‘fundamental role for the dpIns’ in tracking pain intensity does this mean we’re “promoting the concept of a single spot”, as Davis and colleagues have themselves interpreted from our data. Nor do we ever suggest that the results presented as a Brief Communication should be used to regress back to an expired ‘one region fits all’ pedagogy of where ‘pain is in the brain’. Indeed, that view would be completely contrary to the view and concept about ‘pain representation in the brain’ that we’ve long held and have written extensively about via original studies, reviews and editorials over the past 16 years. We have even explicitly discussed the issue surrounding the concept of an ‘N1’ or ‘P1’ for pain and stated that there is ‘no P1 for pain like V1 for vision’.

The concept we’ve long held (and still do) is that due to pain being multifactorial and highly variable (even in response to the same nociceptive input) - contingent upon the context, cognitive and emotional state of an individual, it must be reflected in a malleable, flexibly accessible set of brain regions that variably activate in concert (i.e. network connectivity is key to perceptions) - (for examples, see: Berna et al., 2010; Denk et al., 2014; Lee & Tracey, 2013; Leknes et al., 2013; Ploghaus et al., 1999; Ploner et al., 2010; Ploner et al., 2011; Wiech et al., 2008; Wiech et al., 2010; Tracey, 2005a; Tracey, 2005b; Tracey, 2008; Tracey, 2011; Tracey & Johns, 2010; Tracey & Mantyh, 2007; Wiech & Tracey, 2009). Pain is not a unitary thing. Most recently, we have drawn upon computational approaches to show even that some pain experiences can be influenced by priors in a Bayesian brain - over-riding the information from nociceptive inputs to ultimately decide the pain experience (e.g. Wiech et al., 2014a). Further, we have shown that even in sleeping babies an extensive network of activity subserves nociceptive processing ( Goksan et al., 2015). As we’ve explicitly written many times, pain – like all perceptions - requires a network of brain activity for it to emerge. However, to identify and dissect within that complex set of brain regions (using imaging or electrophysiological methods, neurophysiological or anatomical measures) what roles different regions or signals relate to in terms of the multifactorial pain experience, as well as to identify potentially nociceptive, pain intensity or other specific features of the pain experience is what many animal and human pain researchers have done for many years. The same aim held for this study, as set out, we thought clearly, in the introduction and references quoted. For the avoidance of doubt and in the interests of absolute clarity: we hold that if a region is found to have a specific role that appears quite fundamental and important to the pain experience – whether from animal or human studies – this does not mean it’s the sole region responsible for that complex pain experience, as Davis and colleagues have concluded and suggest our data implies. That is why we used throughout the paper and in the title the expression: “subserves a fundamental role in pain” – which doesn’t mean ‘it’s the only region involved’ in pain, which we agree would be naïve bordering on incredulous. Possibly the format of a brief communication didn’t help make this position clear.

Our study explored what brain regions over a long period of time continuously activate and track a specific feature of the pain experience (in this case intensity) in response to a controlled manipulation of the nociceptive input using a novel imaging technique that allows such exploration. This is a very different approach for examining the neural correlates of painful experiences – and not one whereby the normal framework for thinking about the data can be assumed by applying what we know from conventional imaging/electrophysiological approaches and paradigms used to date (even the single-TI arterial spin labeling (ASL) studies). Perhaps this is why the highly significant result we found was so intriguing and challenging for all of us researchers to interpret, including ourselves.

Obviously no brain region, especially the insula, works in isolation as an island. Indeed, we had just showed precisely that in a publication prior to our Brief Communication; using functional and structural imaging we identified how extensively different divisions of the insula (including posterior) connect and communicate to other brain regions – even at rest ( Wiech et al., 2014b). Yet, it is a fact that these other important brain regions were not continuously activated and involved in tracking a changing pain experience in the same fashion as we found was the case for the dpIns. We believe this was not for reasons of being underpowered (see below for correction to misunderstandings). In fact, even when lowering the statistical threshold dramatically, as we reported in the supplementary material of the paper but perhaps was missed, we did not find other areas significantly tracking the pain intensity ratings over the nearly two hour experiment. However, that result does not in itself nullify the relevance of these other brain regions for being necessary to bring about a complete painful experience, as we’ve shown many times (see refs above as examples). Indeed, we even showed that fact in the paper itself, as many regions were observed to have significant changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF) during the comparison of ‘peak pain’ to ‘rest’ [see: Supplementary Figure 2 in ( Segerdahl et al., 2015)]. Perhaps Davis and colleagues wanted to see these supplementary data interrogated further – to see how the various regions we observed to be active during peak pain relative to baseline relate to the dpIns result? That withstanding, we’re left with how to best interpret and semantically describe our highly significant and robust result regarding the dpIns; a discussion that this platform provides a forum for.

Let us now take each criticism in turn:

1) Experimental method

Unfortunately, Davis and colleagues provide an incorrect assessment of the imaging modality used, the analysis tools applied and the details that are actually reported in the manuscript. The following offers important corrections and clarifications to their review:

a. Multi-TI pCASL FMRI & absolute CBF quantification

Advances in ASL methods over recent years have been tremendous and are actually affording widespread penetration for clinical benefit (e.g. Alsop et al., 2014; Chappell et al., 2012; Kelly et al., 2013; Mezue et al., 2014; Okell et al., 2013). Davis and colleagues blur distinct, well described MRI methods and PET together into one way of 'measuring regional CBF'. Unfortunately, this ignores really important and different features of the various methods available, including the one used in our study, and the highly flexible paradigm designs that ASL based methods now afford neuroscience (compared to positron-emission tomography (PET)) to probe brain activity in very different ways and with varying degrees of improved signal-to-noise (SNR).

As we described in the paper, relative CBF (rCBF) volumes were actually collected approximately every 8s (not at 45s intervals as Davis and colleagues state). This is directly analogous to the ASL methods cited by Davis and colleagues. Further, the authors’ assertion that single-TI (or PET) is better than multi-TI ASL methods used is incorrect and unsupported when it comes to robustness of the measured blood flow and hence SNR. Rather, and as referenced in our paper, a key recognized benefit of the multi-TI approach is that it enables one to calculate absolute CBF (absCBF) at each voxel of the whole brain volume by incorporating each rCBF volume collected (i.e. at every 8s, at each TI) for the nearly two hour pain experience ( Chappell et al., 2009; Mezue et al., 2014; Okell et al., 2013). By collecting each rCBF volume at different TIs, we are additionally able to incorporate information about changes in the time at which arterial signal arrives (i.e. arrival time = AAT) at a given voxel: a feature that is known to vary across space (i.e. all regions of the brain are not perfused instantaneously) and time (i.e. the dynamics of how perfusion changes over time during slowly-fluctuating pain is not known). This is a problem in the single-TI ASL studies quoted and accounting for this variability has been shown to maximize the robustness and reliability of measuring CBF over time within- and between-subjects (e.g. Mezue et al., 2014). In short, the multi-TI ASL method we used does not suffer from reduced temporal SNR efficiency compared to single-TI ASL (or worse, PET). In fact quite the opposite; using a multi-TI approach allows the ASL signal to be sampled around its peak, improving the SNR efficiency compared to single-TI ASL methods where a long TI must be used to reduce sensitivity to arterial transit time at which point the ASL signal has decayed considerably ( Alsop et al., 2014).

To illustrate the robustness of our ASL data further, consider the temporal resolution of the data collection in our study versus the study by Owen et al. (2010) (cited by Davis and colleagues). While our behavioural data were collected at regular intervals comparable to what was used by Owen et al. (2010) [i.e. 21 behavioural data points in our study vs. 25 in Owens et al., 2010], in our study each ASL data point plotted in Figure 2 of the manuscript is far more rigorously sampled with more than double the number of rCBF time points being used to quantify CBF over the course of the full experimental paradigm. When Davis and colleagues assert that the study is significantly underpowered because it is only sparsely sampling brain activity once every 45s - this reflects a significant misunderstanding of the methods used in the study and the SNR benefits of our approach.

It is true that the temporal resolution of this method is longer than single-TI approaches. Any fast changes in CBF during this period will not be accurately represented in the output data. But that is not relevant here, as our experimental design was to observe brain activity linked to slowly changing sensory states that evolve over nearly two hours and were controlled by us – so the parameters of our measurement are appropriate for (indeed far faster than) the changing behavior being investigated (behavioural ratings were collected every 2.5 minutes, remember). Any variations that are occurring faster than the temporal resolution are attributed as noise in the fitting process, are represented in the variance maps that are utilized in subsequent levels of analyses and are therefore unlikely to bias the results in any significant way. We propagated all uncertainty in the CBF estimates through each stage of the analysis such that it was incorporated into the group level effects reported in the paper. In no way are we failing to observe potentially meaningful changes in CBF nor are we merely disregarding potential sources of variation that may be occurring on this time scale.

We did detail all these aspects in the relevant method sections and we were careful to highlight specific references to recent work that further details how the method is used and what the significant benefits are for such neuroscience applications. Please refer to the following references for further insight into these developments ( Chappell et al., 2009; Mezue et al., 2014; Okell et al., 2013).

b. FMRIB Software Library

All FMRIB Software Library (FSL) tools used in the paper are well-validated, publically available, ubiquitously employed across a range of experimental applications in over 1000 laboratories worldwide and are highly cited and cross-referenced. All brain activity is reported as significant using standardized and accepted criteria (e.g. voxels with supra-threshold statistics registered to a Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)-standardized brain using standard space coordinates in FSL). The challenge of localizing group mean statistical maps is common to all fMRI studies. In the case of imaging healthy adults this is easily mitigated by employing FSL tools like Boundary-Based Registration (BBR) and FMRIBs Nonlinear Image Registration Tool (FNIRT) and defining activation clusters using Mixed Effects (z>2.3, p<0.01 cluster corrected) as Segerdahl et al. (2015) did. It is difficult to address Davis and colleagues’ criticisms of these approaches and the validity of our result that used globally accepted criteria without discrediting decades of fMRI imaging development, analysis optimization, and of course, the various publications by many pain imaging authors who use these very same FSL tools and criteria in their own publications. We confirmed the anatomical location of our results using the Harvard-Oxford cortical atlas in FSL that reported the greatest probability of voxels within the identified cluster of activity as residing in the insula; an observation that Davis and colleagues counter by simply referencing a different probabilistic-based atlas (Juelich). We selected the Harvard-Oxford atlas because it more comprehensively and accurately maps the entire insular cortex compared to the Juelich Atlas and therefore provides consistency with mapping of activity in other regions of the insula (i.e. the mid and anterior subdivisions).

Our finding was further validated by cross-referencing with other reported studies exploring somatotopy to nociceptive inputs within this insula region and other studies in humans performing direct cortical stimulation in this region (Figure 3 in Segerdahl et al., 2015). This approach is valid and arguably, we think, goes beyond a comparison with studies that define activity within gross anatomical structures (e.g. ‘insula’, ‘posterior insula’ ‘cingulate’, ‘frontal cortex’, etc.), as is the case for the references (2,3,4,5,6,12) cited within the paper by Davis and colleagues. Excellent and relevant though these studies are, it is interesting to note that the reported coordinate of peak-activities from all these studies don’t overlap with our peak coordinate and is found to be proximal to the dpIns cluster when painful stimulation is used. We should also say though that even if they did overlap it could be argued that distinct specificity might still reside within that one cluster region considering the hundreds of thousands of neurons present – this vexing problem is common to many functional imaging studies and of course was one of the original motivations for employing multivariate pattern analyses, so that specificity to different tasks within ‘one blob’ of cluster activity could be better identified (e.g. Haynes, 2015). Also, it should probably be noted that earlier work of ours comparing the spatial specificity of neurally-derived functional activation as determined from BOLD data versus ASL highlighted ASL’s pre-eminence over BOLD – the latter being biased by local draining veins in some instances ( Tjandra et al., 2005).

However, Davis and colleagues do highlight a very meaningful topic of discussion about how best to map function across controversial boundary zones where two anatomically distinct regions align, as in the case of SII and dpIns. Although it was not possible to detail this important issue within the very strict word and reference limit of a Brief Communication, we did include references to discussions about the posterior insula medial operculum region (PIMO) in order to contextualize our results within the discussion regarding the possibility for nociceptive coding within a sub-region of the posterior insula (see: Craig, 2014; Evrard et al., 2014; Garcia-Larrea, 2012).

2) Control experiment

The control experiment was completed on 12 participants (not seven as stated by Davis and colleagues), and this misreading forms another basis for their comments about the statistical power of the study. For clarity, the pain paradigm was done on 17 subjects. None of the subjects reported the vibration as painful and the group mean saliency rating for the vibration was 3.12 (s.e.m = 0.265) out of 10 [see: Supplementary Figure 1 in ( Segerdahl et al., 2015)].). Power calculations suggest that a minimum of 12 subjects were necessary for group level statistical tests of absCBF data collected during a continuous sensory-motor task. This was clearly defined in both the Methods Checklist and the relevant methods sections of the paper.

However, Davis and colleagues list a few worthy points about the control experiment that we would like to discuss in more detail here. Unfortunately, there was not adequate space to fully report the extensive preliminary work we conducted in preparation for this study that explores these very issues. First, the control experiment should be a stimulus that is as relevant, captivating and as engaging as the pain but without ever being painful (or unpleasant/pleasurable) for approximately 2hrs. Obviously, the suggestion to use an innocuous warm stimulus is sensible. Unfortunately, in practice it fails to fulfill these criteria (i.e. subjects report that it is not salient and not readily perceptible after a short period of time [unpublished pilot data; Psychophysical study currently ongoing]). In this regard, vibration improves upon these limitations, as it has the salience Davis and colleagues recognize as important and is perceptible, so long as the duration of the constant stimulation is limited to the timeframe we scanned in the study. We note, even the work by Owen et al. (2010) suffers from having the 'control' infusion induce pain and therefore not being completely innocuous.

Therefore, it was not immediately evident how to compare pain and vibrotactile related effects directly given the nature of the experimental paradigms used.

Instead, the design was established to compare the anatomical locations of CBF changes triggered by innocuous versus noxious tonic input where saliency is closely matched. However, as Davis et al. correctly point out, a direct comparison of the two conditions is a standard statistical approach used in other imaging modalities and paradigms, so we agree it could be informative for this discussion. Therefore, we’ve performed a comparison of the group level effects of the correlation between CBF and intensity ratings during pain versus the correlation between CBF and intensity ratings during vibration - where all experimental variables are closely matched (i.e. 12 subjects in each group (randomly selected from the pain cohort), 14 minutes of absolute CBF data included, group mean intensity is 3 out of 10 for both modalities).

At the group level, the unpaired t-test across conditions shows that the correlation between pain and intensity ratings is localised to the contralateral dpIns (compared to the vibration-related effects); whilst a conjunction analysis confirms that there is no significant overlap between the group mean effects of either condition here (Randomise: voxelwise, p<0.05). The authors’ logic dictates that increasing the N or lengthening the vibration task to boost SNR would potentially ‘reveal’ activity related to vibration within the dpIns region. However, this logic might reversibly also predict that the subthreshold SII activity seen in the vibration results would be now shown in the pain intensity tracking result (to a suprathreshold level perhaps)- yet this does not occur even with 17 subjects and including all data points. Therefore, while we acknowledge that thermosensation might well be represented within the dpIns – ongoing work in the laboratory that we look forward to sharing (accepting the saliency issue is still a problem here) – we hope this has helped clarify the parameters used for the control task and issues surrounding its use within these more complex paradigm designs. As an aside, Davis and colleagues' reference to early PET studies of vibration (refs: 4,5) aren't precisely relevant to the current discussion as the aim of our study was to investigate unique correlates of perception not to report what the main effects of different stimulus conditions relative to baseline are.

3) Interpretation of the data

Davis and colleagues criticized the discussion of our findings in the light of other literature regarding insula activity, saying that we missed important references and broader discussions. Alas, we would have very much liked to include that extensive and informative literature. However, we remind Davis and colleagues (and the readership) that this was not possible within the very strict requirements for a Brief Communication where we’re only allowed 20 references and 1200 words. Further, as Davis and colleagues are aware, much of our own published work has (ironically) focused on characterizing and dissecting the roles that different insula divisions play in the multifactorial experience that is acute and chronic pain (e.g. Baumgärtner et al., 2010; Brooks et al., 2005; Brooks & Tracey, 2007; Duff et al., 2015; Fairhurst et al., 2012; Ploghaus et al., 1999; Ploner et al., 2010; Ploner et al., 2011; Schweinhardt et al., 2006; Wanigasekera et al., 2012; Wiech et al., 2010; Wiech et al., 2014a; Wiech et al., 2014b; Wise et al., 2002) alongside the somatotopic studies quoted in the original paper) – nearly all of these references we couldn’t discuss or quote either due to space constraints despite their absolute relevance. Therefore, with full awareness of the literature cited alongside our own substantial contributions in understanding the complex role that the insula plays in pain mechanisms, we believe that we carefully interpreted our results while drawing upon that corpus of knowledge in the limited words allowed. We don’t believe our data undermines Craig's novel and important theories about interoception and the 'sentient self' - mechanisms that are unlikely to be anchored solely to the posterior insula (as asserted) and instead necessitate dynamic interaction with other regions such as the anterior insula, as Craig has stated ( Craig, 2015).

We agree that a more insightful discussion centers on interesting recent work that attempts to rigorously interrogate the pain specificity of statistics maps by using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA), as we and others have been doing (see: Brodersen et al., 2012; Brown et al., 2011; Duff et al., 2015; Marquand et al., 2010; Wager, 2015; Wager et al., 2013). Recent work by Woo et al. (2014) employed this approach to derive unique patterns of activation in regions like the dpIns that are specific to physical but not emotional pain (previously and wrongly inferred to be the ‘same’ because of overlapping BOLD statistics maps, as discussed above). Interestingly, their data support the interpretation that a region like the dpIns (see Figure 3 of Woo et al., 2014) has an identifiable role in coding attributes about nociceptive driven painful experiences.

In keeping with basic principles of good experimental design, we formulated our hypothesis to test one theory about which brain regions track the intensity of a slowly varying experience of pain in response to a carefully controlled change in peripheral nociceptive input. Understanding which brain regions are coding this information is of interest, we believe. The affective coding of that stimulus is highly relevant too - processing that is likely to engage higher-level pain network processing secondary to the more initial intensity coding of the input, as nicely described in a recent review ( Garcia-Larrea & Peyron, 2013).

Conclusion

In closing, we would like to note that imaging tonic, slowly-fluctuating and spontaneous pain states in healthy controls (and in patients) is a small, emerging field within the pain neuroimaging community with very few laboratories to date using the method. In part, this is due to the difficulty in imaging the brain during pain experiences that are analogous to what patients are suffering from, alongside limitations in accessing state-of-the-art acquisition methods for measuring blood flow using ASL. It is difficult to induce a tonic pain state in volunteers that is robust, reliable, reproducible, easily controlled and is not invasive or risks permanent skin damage or infection. A number of important ASL/pain studies have been done by colleagues and us that have laid an excellent basis for the future (e.g. Howard et al., 2011; Howard et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2013; Owen et al., 2008; Owen et al., 2010; Owen et al., 2012; Maleki et al., 2013; Stagg et al., 2013; Segerdahl et al., 2012; Tracey & Johns, 2010; Tjandra et al., 2005 – alongside others quoted in the paper and embedded methods references). Neither should we ignore the foundational imaging physics work done in developing quantitative cerebral perfusion imaging (i.e. ASL) over the past several decades that have provided such opportunities for clinical and basic neuroscience (e.g. Detre & Aslop, 1999; Davies & Jezzard, 2003; Figueiredo et al., 2005; Guo & Wong, 2015; MacIntosh et al., 2008; Mutsaerts et al., 2015; Teeuwisse et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2003; Wong et al., 2006; Wong, 2007).

We hope our response informs the F1000Research readership about exciting developments in the field of novel ASL applications, underscores the robustness of our paradigm and reliability of the analyses used to quantify CBF dynamics; and lastly highlights the importance of evolving conceptually away from antiquated pain imaging dogmas (derived mostly from reverse inference-based statistical testing) about single “pain spots”. In that regard, we believe Davis and colleagues agree with us. We hold that if a brain region is continuously and metabolically active in response to a slowly varying peripheral nociceptive input and further that its degree of activity correlates with a concomitant changing subjective experience of pain intensity lasting over several hours then it must be important. We can argue semantics, but it seems to us that describing it as “subserving a fundamental role in pain” (does not mean only region) and potentially being an area that might be responsive only to nociceptive inputs that produce painful experiences – i.e. nociceptive/pain specific (alongside other areas to be yet possibly identified) best describes the data. We welcome alternative interpretations or descriptions of our results, having defended their reliability. We are of course eager to interrogate and explore the meaning of this result further within ongoing preclinical and clinical studies currently underway. We look forward to sharing these results with the wider community in due course.

Funding Statement

The author(s) declared no grants were involved in supporting this work.

[version 1; referees: 2 approved]

References

  1. Alsop DC, Detre JA, Golay X, et al. : Recommended implementation of arterial spin-labeled perfusion MRI for clinical applications: A consensus of the ISMRM perfusion study group and the European consortium for ASL in dementia. Magn Reson Med. 2014;73(1):102–116. 10.1002/mrm.25197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baumgärtner U, Iannetti GD, Zambreanu L, et al. : Multiple somatotopic representations of heat and mechanical pain in the operculo-insular cortex: a high-resolution fMRI study. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(5):2863–72. 10.1152/jn.00253.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berna C, Leknes S, Holmes EA, et al. : Induction of depressed mood disrupts emotion regulation neurocircuitry and enhances pain unpleasantness. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(11):1083–90. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brodersen KH, Wiech K, Lomakina EI, et al. : Decoding the perception of pain from fMRI using multivariate pattern analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;63(3):1162–70. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brooks JC, Tracey I: The insula: a multidimensional integration site for pain. Pain. 2007;128(1-2):1–2. 10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brooks JC, Zambreanu L, Godinez A, et al. : Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. Neuroimage. 2005;27(1):201–9. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown JE, Chatterjee N, Younger J, et al. : Towards a physiology-based measure of pain: patterns of human brain activity distinguish painful from non-painful thermal stimulation. PLoS One. 2011;6(9):e24124. 10.1371/journal.pone.0024124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chappell MA, Groves AR, Whitcher B, et al. : Variational Bayesian inference for a nonlinear forward model. Signal Processing, IEEE Transactions on. 2009;57(1):223–236. 10.1109/TSP.2008.2005752 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chappell MA, Okell TW, Payne SJ, et al. : A fast analysis method for non-invasive imaging of blood flow in individual cerebral arteries using vessel-encoded arterial spin labelling angiography. Med Image Anal. 2012;16(4):831–9. 10.1016/j.media.2011.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Craig AD: Topographically organized projection to posterior insular cortex from the posterior portion of the ventral medial nucleus in the long-tailed macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2014;522(1):36–63. 10.1002/cne.23425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Craig AD: How Do You Feel? An Interoceptive Moment With Your Neurobiological Self. UK: Princeton University Press,2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  12. Davies NP, Jezzard P: Selective arterial spin labeling (SASL): perfusion territory mapping of selected feeding arteries tagged using two-dimensional radiofrequency pulses. Magn Reson Med. 2003;49(6):1133–1142. 10.1002/mrm.10475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Denk F, McMahon SB, Tracey I: Pain vulnerability: a neurobiological perspective. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(2):192–200. 10.1038/nn.3628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Detre JA, Alsop DC: Perfusion magnetic resonance imaging with continuous arterial spin labeling: methods and clinical applications in the central nervous system. Eur J Radiol. 1999;30(2):115–24. 10.1016/S0720-048X(99)00050-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Duff E, Vennart W, Wise RG, et al. : Learning to identify CNS drug action and efficacy using multistudy fMRI data. Sci Transl Med. 2015;7(274): 274ra16. 10.1126/scitranslmed.3008438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Evrard HC, Logothetis NK, Craig AD: Modular architectonic organization of the insula in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2014;522(1):64–97. 10.1002/cne.23436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fairhurst M, Fairhurst K, Berna C, et al. : An fMRI study exploring the overlap and differences between neural representations of physical and recalled pain. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e48711. 10.1371/journal.pone.0048711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Figueiredo PM, Clare S, Jezzard P: Quantitative perfusion measurements using pulsed arterial spin labeling: effects of large region-of-interest analysis. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2005;21(6):676–82. 10.1002/jmri.20329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Garcia-Larrea L: The posterior insular-opercular region and the search of a primary cortex for pain. Neurophysiol Clin. 2012;42(5):299–313. 10.1016/j.neucli.2012.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Garcia-Larrea L, Peyron R: Pain matrices and neuropathic pain matrices: a review. Pain. 2013;154(Suppl 1):S29–43. 10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goksan S, Hartley C, Emery F, et al. : fMRI reveals neural activity overlap between adult and infant pain. Elife. 2015;4: e06356. 10.7554/eLife.06356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Guo J, Wong EC: Increased SNR efficiency in velocity selective arterial spin labeling using multiple velocity selective saturation modules (mm-VSASL). Magn Reson Med. 2015;74(3):694–705. 10.1002/mrm.25462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Haynes JD: A Primer on Pattern-Based Approaches to fMRI: Principles, Pitfalls, and Perspectives. Neuron. 2015;87(2):257–70. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Howard MA, Krause K, Khawaja N, et al. : Beyond patient reported pain: perfusion magnetic resonance imaging demonstrates reproducible cerebral representation of ongoing post-surgical pain. PLoS One. 2011;6(2): e17096. 10.1371/journal.pone.0017096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Howard MA, Sanders D, Krause K, et al. : Alterations in resting-state regional cerebral blood flow demonstrate ongoing pain in osteoarthritis: An arterial spin-labeled magnetic resonance imaging study. Arthritis Rheum. 2012;64(12):3936–46. 10.1002/art.37685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kelly ME, Rowland MJ, Okell TW, et al. : Pseudo-continuous arterial spin labelling MRI for non-invasive, whole-brain, serial quantification of cerebral blood flow following aneurysmal subarachnoid haemorrhage. Transl Stroke Res. 2013;4(6):710–8. 10.1007/s12975-013-0269-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lee MC, Tracey I: Imaging pain: a potent means for investigating pain mechanisms in patients. Br J Anaesth. 2013;111(1):64–72. 10.1093/bja/aet174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Leknes S, Berna C, Lee MC, et al. : The importance of context: when relative relief renders pain pleasant. Pain. 2013;154(3):402–10. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.11.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Liu J, Hao Y, Du M, et al. : Quantitative cerebral blood flow mapping and functional connectivity of postherpetic neuralgia pain: a perfusion fMRI study. Pain. 2013;154(1):110–8. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.09.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. MacIntosh BJ, Pattinson KT, Gallichan D, et al. : Measuring the effects of remifentanil on cerebral blood flow and arterial arrival time using 3D GRASE MRI with pulsed arterial spin labelling. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2008;28(8):1514–22. 10.1038/jcbfm.2008.46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Maleki N, Brawn J, Barmettler G, et al. : Pain response measured with arterial spin labeling. NMR Biomed. 2013;26(6):664–73. 10.1002/nbm.2911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Marquand A, Howard M, Brammer M, et al. : Quantitative prediction of subjective pain intensity from whole-brain fMRI data using Gaussian processes. Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):2178–89. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mezue M, Segerdahl AR, Okell TW, et al. : Optimization and reliability of multiple postlabeling delay pseudo-continuous arterial spin labelling during rest and stimulus-induced functional task activation. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2014;34(12):1919–27. 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mutsaerts HJ, van Osch MJ, Zelaya FO, et al. : Muli-vendor reliability of arterial spin labeling perfusion MRI using a near-identical sequence: implications for multi-center studies. Neuroimage. 2015;113(6):143–152. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Okell TW, Chappell MA, Kelly ME, et al. : Cerebral blood flow quantification using vessel-encoded arterial spin labeling. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2013;33(11):1716–24. 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Owen DG, Bureau Y, Thomas AW, et al. : Quantification of pain-induced changes in cerebral blood flow by perfusion MRI. Pain. 2008;136(1–2):85–96. 10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Owen DG, Clarke CF, Bureau Y, et al. : Measuring the neural response to continuous intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline by perfusion MRI. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2012;35(3):669–77. 10.1002/jmri.22814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Owen DG, Clarke CF, Ganapathy S, et al. : Using perfusion MRI to measure the dynamic changes in neural activation associated with tonic muscular pain. Pain. 2010;148(3):375–86. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, et al. : Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science. 1999;284(5422):1979–81. 10.1126/science.284.5422.1979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ploner M, Lee MC, Wiech K, et al. : Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(1):355–60. 10.1073/pnas.0906186106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ploner M, Lee MC, Wiech K, et al. : Flexible cerebral connectivity patterns subserve contextual modulations of pain. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(3):719–26. 10.1093/cercor/bhq146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schweinhardt P, Glynn C, Brooks J, et al. : An fMRI study of cerebral processing of brush-evoked allodynia in neuropathic pain patients. Neuroimage. 2006;32(1):256–65. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Segerdahl AR, Mezue M, Okell TW, et al. : The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(4):499–500. 10.1038/nn.3969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Segerdahl AR, Xie J, Paterson K, et al. : Imaging the neural correlates of neuropathic pain and pleasurable relief associated with inherited erythromelalgia in a single subject with quantitative arterial spin labelling. Pain. 2012;153(5):1122–7. 10.1016/j.pain.2011.12.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Stagg CJ, Lin RL, Mezue M, et al. : Widespread modulation of cerebral perfusion induced during and after transcranial direct current stimulation applied to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33(28):11425–31. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3887-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Teeuwisse WM, Schmid S, Ghariq E, et al. : Time-encoded pseudocontinuous arterial spin labeling: basic properties and timing strategies for human applications. Magn Reson Med. 2014;72(6):1712–1722. 10.1002/mrm.25083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tjandra T, Brooks JC, Figueiredo P, et al. : Quantitative assessment of the reproducibility of functional activation measured with BOLD and MR perfusion imaging: implications for clinical trial design. Neuroimage. 2005;27(2):393–401. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Tracey I: Nociceptive processing in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005a;15(4):478–87. 10.1016/j.conb.2005.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tracey I: Functional connectivity and pain: how effectively connected is your brain? Pain. 2005b;116(3):173–4. 10.1016/j.pain.2005.05.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Tracey I: Imaging pain. Br J Anaesth. 2008;101(1):32–9. 10.1093/bja/aen102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tracey I: Can neuroimaging studies identify pain endophenotypes in humans? Nat Rev Neurol. 2011;7(3):173–81. 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tracey I, Johns E: The pain matrix: reloaded or reborn as we image tonic pain using arterial spin labelling. Pain. 2010;148(3):359–360. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tracey I, Mantyh PW: The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron. 2007;55(3):377–91. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wager TD: Using Neuroimaging to Understand Pain: Pattern Recognition and the Path from Brain Mapping to Mechanisms. In Apkarian AV. Brain Adapting With Pain (Chptr 2) UK: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins.2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  55. Wager TD, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, et al. : An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N Engl J Med. 2013;368(15):1388–1397. 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wang J, Aguirre GK, Kimberg DY, et al. : Arterial spin labeling perfusion fMRI with very low task frequency. Magn Reson Med. 2003;49(5):796–802. 10.1002/mrm.10437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wanigasekera V, Lee MC, Rogers R, et al. : Baseline reward circuitry activity and trait reward responsiveness predict expression of opioid analgesia in healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(43):17705–10. 10.1073/pnas.1120201109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wiech K, Jbabdi S, Lin CS, et al. : Differential structural and resting state connectivity between insular subdivisions and other pain-related brain regions. Pain. 2014a;155(10):2047–55. 10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wiech K, Lin CS, Brodersen KH, et al. : Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain. J Neurosci. 2010;30(48):16324–31. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wiech K, Ploner M, Tracey I: Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(8):306–13. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wiech K, Tracey I: The influence of negative emotions on pain: behavioral effects and neural mechanisms. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):987–94. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wiech K, Vandekerckhove J, Zaman J, et al. : Influence of prior information on pain involves biased perceptual decision-making. Curr Biol. 2014b;24(15):R679–681. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wise RG, Rogers R, Painter D, et al. : Combining fMRI with a pharmacokinetic model to determine which brain areas activated by painful stimulation are specifically modulated by remifentanil. Neuroimage. 2002;16(4):999–1014. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wong EC, Cronin M, Wu WC, et al. : Velocity-selective arterial spin labeling. Magn Reson Med. 2006;55(6):1334–1341. 10.1002/mrm.20906 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wong EC: Vessel-encoded arterial spin-labeling using pseudocontinuous tagging. Magn Reson Med. 2007;58(6):1086–1091. 10.1002/mrm.21293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Woo CW, Koban L, Kross E, et al. : Separate neural representations for physical pain and social rejection. Nat Commun. 2014;5: 5380. 10.1038/ncomms6380 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2015 Dec 15. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.7854.r11550

Referee response for version 1

Luis Garcia-Larrea 1

In their ‘response paper’, Segerdahl and colleagues endeavour to correct what they consider “important misunderstandings” made by Davis et al. 1 in their target article. Most of the points raised by Davis and colleagues are of technical nature, and question the validity of the methods used by Segerdahl et al. to reach their conclusions. Here Segerdahl et al. thoroughly describe the methods employed, correct misunderstandings, and on this basis judge that the extrapolation and inferences made by Davis and colleagues are not consistent with their target report 2. Also, they take the opportunity to expand on a number of conceptual points that were made.

The response is broadly divided in two parts, respectively technical and interpretative. From the technical point of view, they rebut a number of points advanced in the Davis et al.’s paper, in particular their critique of the multi-delay ASL technique, the excessively long sampling intervals and the small number of control subjects, all of them contributing to a supposed underpower of the experiment. Segerdahl et al. vigorously correct several facts that seem to have been misunderstood in the Davis et al.’s comment, in particular that brain MRI volumes were acquired every 8 seconds –and not 45–, and that the control experiment was completed on 12 participants –not 7 as implied in Davis et al. These are important particulars that could have undermined the reliability of the MRI data under question; the clarification appears appropriate, and lends support to the reliability of Segerdahl et al.’s original results. I am unable to enter the highly technical discussion on the relative merits of uni- or multi-TI ASL methods. While the arguments advanced by Segerdahl to defend the superiority of multi-TI ASL appear solid, I must deplore that they are mostly based on their own productions, which limits generalisation. Reproducibility of functional imaging (or rather the lack of it) is a crucial problem 3 , 4 and quoting external sources on multi-TI procedures would have strengthened the authors’ argument.

I support and share one of the main notions in the response of Segerdahl et al., namely that they never claimed to have determined a sole ‘pain centre’, and that being fundamental for pain experience, as they wrote, does not equate being the sole region responsible for such experience. It seems clear to me that identifying a role for dorsal posterior insula (dpIns) in tracking pain intensity does not automatically promote the concept of a ‘single pain spot’. I would just add a nuance: while we have abundant experimental data of the dpIns coding properties under physiological conditions, this may not hold for neuropathic states where the dorsal insula is injured of highly deafferented, in which case the perceived intensity of pain may be coded by other areas –possibly including the insular cortex ipsilateral to the pain, which becomes disinhibited 5. Also, and probably because of extensive cortical plasticity, long-standing destruction of both posterior insulae has been shown to be compatible with the ability to continuously rate the moment-to-moment level of pain 6. Thus, the ‘fundamental’ role of the postero-dorsal insula tagged by Segerdahl et al. may be transferred to other networks in pathological conditions. 

The authors acknowledge the importance of one of the criticisms from Davis et al.’s comments, namely the lack of direct statistical comparison between pain and innocuous stimulation. Indeed, showing that one region exhibits significant activity changes in one condition (pain) and not in other (vibration) does not allow affirming that the activity of both regions would have differed significantly had they been compared directly. The error of comparing significance levels without directly testing their difference is said to be especially common in the neuroimaging literature 7 , 8. To respond to this criticism, now Segerdahl and colleagues performed a comparison of the correlation between CBF and intensity ratings during pain versus the correlation CBF / intensity ratings during vibration. This approach seems fair to me, although the formulation of the results remains ambiguous, and I remain unsure whether there was a significant difference between the two correlations, or a region X condition significant interaction –the straightforward result I should have expected. This could have been clarified further.

The notion of a gradient in the processing of noxious inputs from the lateral operculum (SII) to its medial portion (OP2) and posterior dorsal insula (dpIns) appears well backed by the literature. For instance, the sites where electrical stimulation induce non-noxious thermal sensations in humans predominate in the inner operculum (OP2) while those evoking clear pain tend to concentrate in the posterior insula 9. Also, while medial opercular sites responded to barely perceptive thermal increases, the posterior insular response only emerged clearly at noxious levels 10. Such a gradient is difficult to explain on a simple afferent connectivity basis, as the primate’s spinothalamic system projects in similar proportions to the granular insula and its adjacent operculum 11. Labelled lines entailing a significant predominance of nociceptive-specific afferents in the posterior insula might contribute an explanation 12, but this is difficult to reconcile with classical data showing the predominance of low threshold units in this same area 13 ' 14. Alternatively, functional differences between dpIns and operculum might reflect network properties, partially independent from those of isolated neurons. For instance, the posterior insula has a more extended connectivity pattern than the opercular region and S2 proper; the massive amount of afferent input to the insula may entail a greater background activity than in the operculum, hence obstructing the precise encoding of low-energy stimuli barely emerging from background noise. This might explain why posterior insula networks are biased toward encoding nociception despite the fact that approximately 70% of primate insular neurons can respond to non-noxious somatic inputs too 13, and that intracranial recordings demonstrate posterior insular responses to non-noxious stimuli in humans 15. Such a bias, however, reflects a progressive gradient, and not a clearcut functional cleavage, and failure to acknowledge this may lead us toward dangerous neo-phrenology. The suprasylvian operculum can also encode stimulus intensity in the painful ranges 10, and the dpIns can also respond to non-noxious stimuli 16.

In conclusion, I believe the authors make clear their case that any notion of a ‘single pain centre’ in the human brain is absent from their line of thinking –and absent from the conclusions in their target paper. They also point out various methodological points that seem to have been misunderstood in the Davis et al.’s comment, while accepting the importance of others, such as the lack of direct statistical comparison between conditions. The important role we can ascribe to the dpIns should not be confounded with an absolute specificity of this area –especially when it comes to neuropathic lesions. And importantly, Segerdahl et al. acknowledge that the comments from Davis and her colleagues have triggered a most interesting, valuable and enlightening discussion, which should help many readers to get insight into current controversies on pain perception. To me, this is exactly how science advances.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

References

  • 1. Davis KD, Bushnell MC, Iannetti GD, St Lawrence K, Coghill R: Evidence against pain specificity in the dorsal posterior insula. F1000Res.2015;4: 10.12688/f1000research.6833.1 362 10.12688/f1000research.6833.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Segerdahl AR, Mezue M, Okell TW, Farrar JT, Tracey I: The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat Neurosci.2015;18(4) : 10.1038/nn.3969 499-500 10.1038/nn.3969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Bennett CM, Miller MB: How reliable are the results from functional magnetic resonance imaging?. Ann N Y Acad Sci.2010;1191: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05446.x 133-55 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05446.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Reardon S: Neuroscience in court: The painful truth. Nature.2015;518(7540) : 10.1038/518474a 474-6 10.1038/518474a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Peyron R, Schneider F, Faillenot I, Convers P, Barral FG, Garcia-Larrea L, Laurent B: An fMRI study of cortical representation of mechanical allodynia in patients with neuropathic pain. Neurology.2004;63(10) :1838-46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Feinstein JS, Khalsa SS, Salomons TV, Prkachin KM, Frey-Law LA, Lee JE, Tranel D, Rudrauf D: Preserved emotional awareness of pain in a patient with extensive bilateral damage to the insula, anterior cingulate, and amygdala. Brain Struct Funct.2015; 10.1007/s00429-014-0986-3 10.1007/s00429-014-0986-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Poldrack RA, Fletcher PC, Henson RN, Worsley KJ, Brett M, Nichols TE: Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study. Neuroimage.2008;40(2) : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.048 409-14 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Nieuwenhuis S, Forstmann BU, Wagenmakers EJ: Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance. Nat Neurosci.2011;14(9) : 10.1038/nn.2886 1105-7 10.1038/nn.2886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Mazzola L, Isnard J, Mauguière F: Somatosensory and pain responses to stimulation of the second somatosensory area (SII) in humans. A comparison with SI and insular responses. Cereb Cortex.2006;16(7) : 10.1093/cercor/bhj038 960-8 10.1093/cercor/bhj038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Frot M, Magnin M, Mauguière F, Garcia-Larrea L: Human SII and posterior insula differently encode thermal laser stimuli. Cereb Cortex.2007;17(3) : 10.1093/cercor/bhk007 610-20 10.1093/cercor/bhk007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Dum RP, Levinthal DJ, Strick PL: The spinothalamic system targets motor and sensory areas in the cerebral cortex of monkeys. J Neurosci.2009;29(45) : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3398-09.2009 14223-35 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3398-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Craig AD: Topographically organized projection to posterior insular cortex from the posterior portion of the ventral medial nucleus in the long-tailed macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol.2014;522(1) : 10.1002/cne.23425 36-63 10.1002/cne.23425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Robinson CJ, Burton H: Somatic submodality distribution within the second somatosensory (SII), 7b, retroinsular, postauditory, and granular insular cortical areas of M. fascicularis. J Comp Neurol.1980;192(1) : 10.1002/cne.901920106 93-108 10.1002/cne.901920106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Schneider RJ, Friedman DP, Mishkin M: A modality-specific somatosensory area within the insula of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res.1993;621(1) :116-20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Perchet C, Frot M, Vartiainen N, Claude L, Garcia-Larrea L, Bastuji H: A comparison of the cerebral networks activated by painful and non-painful stimulations in humans: an intra-cerebral recording study. Paper to the 2015 European Congress of Clinical Neurophysiology.2015;ID 304:pp. 354 Reference source [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Björnsdotter M, Löken L, Olausson H, Vallbo A, Wessberg J: Somatotopic organization of gentle touch processing in the posterior insular cortex. J Neurosci.2009;29(29) : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0400-09.2009 9314-20 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0400-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2015 Nov 25. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.7854.r11308

Referee response for version 1

Arthur Craig 1

Segerdahl and colleagues have replied clearly and thoughtfully to the issues raised by Davis et al. regarding the short paper in Nature Neuroscience. Their response seems collegial in tone, granting trans-atlantic cultural differences (e.g., the imperative “remember” might be received less positively in America than in Britain). Further, they recognize the benefits of a constructive discussion that is open to all, rather than admonishing the commentators for overlooking key statements and details (one of which was buried near the end of the legend of Supplementary Figure 2). On the whole, though, the discussion between these two camps resembles recurring disagreements between “splitters” and “lumpers” in pain research (e.g., see Berkley, 1980) as well as, more generally, the persistent controversy about functional localization in brain research. Nevertheless, a major difference between these two sides is that one group incorporates certain knowledge from work in the monkey, which to my mind is fundamentally significant.

Davis and colleagues took issue mainly with the impression that Segerdahl et al. had proposed that the dorsal posterior insula (dpIns) is a “pain-specific” locus in human. That assertion is intimated in the initial portions of the paper, however the reply makes clear that the central issue is rather, why did the Multi-TI pCASL method reveal such “highly significant and robust” activation only in the dpIns after whole-brain correlation with subjective ratings of tonic “pain intensity” elicited by thermal modulation of post-capsaicin cutaneous hyperalgesia. This result is surprising because considerable prior evidence predicts that subjective feelings are associated with strong activation in the anterior insula and other sites (for review, see Ch. 7 in Craig 2015 2); apparently, that’s the reason that Segerdahl et al. submitted the short report instead of the full-length manuscript that they could (and perhaps should) have written.

Davis et al. suggested that such a singular result could have resulted from faulty methodology, such as inadequate temporal sampling or underpowered statistics, but Segerdahl et al. convincingly rule out those two possibilities. Davis et al. rightfully point out that the “control” vibration results are inadequate to support a claim of pain specificity, although the “robust vibrotactile [PET] activation” they cite actually occurred in the mid-insula (see Ch. 6 in Craig 2015 2). In response, Segerdahl et al. claim that an innocuous thermal control stimulus, though desirable, would not maintain salience; however, imaging evidence suggests that an oscillating thermal stimulus would be salient and motivating (Hua et al. 2005; see Ch. 2 in Craig 2015 2). Davis et al. suggested also that inadequate anatomical registration of the ensemble of brain images could have obscured the expected activation in the anterior insula that had been observed in similar experiments by Henderson et al. (2007) and Owen et al. (2010). But, Segerdahl et al. rectified the misunderstanding of their anatomical protocol and explained the co-localization of the activation they observed in dpIns with the comparable results from several prior studies that had used different techniques (see Fig. 3 of the original paper). Indeed, the very precise localization of activation in the most dorsal extent of the posterior insula matches exactly the location of the terminus of ascending lamina I nociceptive-specific activity, which was demonstrated in the fundus of the superior limiting sulcus in the macaque monkey by the paper they cite (Craig 2014 6; see Ch. 5 in Craig 2015 2). Further, the antero-posterior somatotopic gradient identified in the monkey fits with the topographic order reported in the studies that Segerdahl et al. collated in Fig.3 and also with the posterior location of the “very specific ‘spot’” that they found.

Lastly, Davis et al. challenged the interpretation of any activation locus as “pain-specific” and suggested that “the dpIns likely is involved in pain but overall is a non-specific perceptual way-station,” thereby ignoring the evidence in the monkey. Segerdahl et al. naturally replied that the evidence regarding multifactorial pain-related activation includes their “own substantial contributions,” meanwhile acknowledging the new conceptual contributions by the Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis (MVPA) study of Wager et al. (2013) and by the correlative functional connectivity patterns found in individual subjects by Cheng et al. (2015) (from Davis’ lab). Yet, on one hand they cite attributions of higher-level cognitive and affective coding to the anterior insula in reviews by Garcia-Larrea & Peyron (2013) and Craig (2015 2), and on the other they point out that the new MVPA findings “support the interpretation that a region like the dpIns (see Figure 3 of Woo et al. 2014) has an identifiable role in coding attributes about nociceptive driven painful experiences.” Hmm. Does that mean modality-specific? noci-specific? algosity (see Greenspan’s comments)? a “pain switch”? or perhaps, an embedded cluster of neurons? They cannot say. Instead, Segerdahl et al. reiterate and expand the carefully worded concluding statements of their original paper, saying: “A growing body of literature suggests that a subsection of the posterior insula is both anatomically and functionally well suited to serve a primary and fundamental role in pain processing.” And “Using … a newly developed procedure and analysis, we were able to identify the dpIns as subserving a fundamental role in pain and the likely human homolog of the nociceptive region identified from animal studies. Future work targeting dpIns activity might provide a window to explore fundamental mechanisms related to how pain emerges from nociception as well.” These ideas and words remind me of Chapters 5 and 6 of my book and I must certainly agree.

OK, so far, so good. Yet, the central issue remains: why did their rigorous quantitative analysis demonstrate robust activation only in the contralateral dpIns? Now, I will make a suggestion.

The published evidence in the monkey tells us that a somatotopically-organized region of posterolateral thalamus that contains only nociceptive neurons projects topographically to the dpIns (Craig, 2015 2; 2014 6). The thalamic region includes both nociceptive-specific (NS) and polymodal nociceptive (HPC for heat, pinch, and cold) neurons that receive monosynaptic input from lamina I spinothalamic neurons; an adjacent sub-region of thalamus similarly relays thermoreceptive-specific (COOL) activity to an adjacent sub-region of the dpIns (as depicted in Fig. 12 in Craig 2015 2). Both classes of nociceptive neurons in the monkey respond to topical capsaicin and become sensitized, with trajectories that parallel human pain reports (unpublished obs.). The HPC neurons constitute the only sensory channel that can quantitatively explain human reports of temporally summating second (burning) pain in response to repeated brief-contact heat stimulation (Craig, 2004) as well as pain reports in response to the thermal grill (Ch. 3 in Craig, 2015 2); we can posit that they are directly responsible for the pain reports in the study of Segerdahl et al. Thus, the terminus of the HPC sensory channel in the dpIns could alone explain the activation focus they observed. Particular components of the parallel NS (first, sharp pain) sensory channel most likely would also be involved.

However, the anomalous innocuous cold sensitivity of HPC neurons and the evidence for the involvement of some HPC neurons in signaling muscle work, vascular distension, and tissue immune and metabolic conditions all indicate that this sensory channel is not a binary, pain-specific signal but rather has a much broader role in interoception (Craig 2015 2). Along with sensory channels representing many other specific and non-specific bodily sensations, such as cool, warm, itch, sensual touch, taste, hunger, and thirst, the dpIns is thus proposed to contain interoceptive cortex. Both Davis et al. and Segerdahl et al. alluded to this perspective and also cited the evidence for thermoreceptive-specific activity in the dpIns, but Davis et al. seemingly regarded this knowledge as support for the assertion that the dpIns does not have a specific role in pain, which is a misrepresentation.

Yet, these considerations do not resolve the central issue. For instance, the HPC channel is also relayed to area 3a in the primary sensorimotor area (Vierck et al., 2013; Craig, 2015 2; 2014 6), but that region was not activated. More importantly, neither the mid-insula nor the anterior insula displayed activation, and both are normally associated with subjective reports of immediate feelings of pain.

To my mind, the key lies in the instructions to the individuals who participated in the experiment of Segerdahl et al., which can suggest what their brains were doing. Unfortunately, the exact instructions were not reported; we’re told only that they were asked to report the “pain intensity” at certain times and otherwise to remain focused on a fixation cross. We can surmise that, in order to perform as requested (and presumably earn a monetary reward), the participants (1) concentrated their attention on the intensity of sensation that originated from the stimulus site, but otherwise (2) ignored the ongoing feeling of unpleasantness as much as possible. Our brains are rather good at both tasks; first, we regard pain as a discriminative sensory capacity because we can focus our attention on the specific characteristics and the specific origin of the sensation, and second, as a homeostatic emotion that is crucial for survival, like hunger, pain has top motivational priority most of the time but can be inhibited when other emotional needs or goals are more urgent. Based on the available evidence for the neural correlates of focused attention and emotional feelings, that suggests (1) that focal activity at the somatotopically appropriate site in the HPC map within interoceptive cortex was enhanced by the center-surround (sombrero-like) modulation of endogenous attentional processes and (2) that activity that normally engenders the feeling of unpleasantness or pain was suppressed in interoceptive integration mechanisms in the mid-insula and anterior insula (and in related regions, such as area 3a and area 24c in the anterior mid-cingulate).

To my mind, the mechanisms I propose offer a potential explanation for the singular observations of Segerdahl et al. They are incomplete – for instance, focal activation was not reported in the posterolateral thalamus and activity supporting the behavior of reporting the discriminative readout is missing – nonetheless, they provide practical inroads for future experiments. Most importantly, they incorporate knowledge about the crucial and specific role of dpIns in nociception and pain, and they build on the significant advance that the work of Segerdahl et al. has provided.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

References

  • 1. Berkley KJ: Spatial relationships between the terminations of somatic sensory and motor pathways in the rostral brainstem of cats and monkeys. I. Ascending somatic sensory inputs to lateral diencephalon. J Comp Neurol.1980;193(1) : 10.1002/cne.901930119 283-317 10.1002/cne.901930119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Craig AD: How Do You Feel? An Interoceptive Moment with Your Neurobiological Self.2015;Princeton University Press(Princeton) : [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Hua le H, Strigo IA, Baxter LC, Johnson SC, Craig AD: Anteroposterior somatotopy of innocuous cooling activation focus in human dorsal posterior insular cortex. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol.2005;289(2) : 10.1152/ajpregu.00123.2005 R319-R325 10.1152/ajpregu.00123.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Henderson LA, Gandevia SC, Macefield VG: Somatotopic organization of the processing of muscle and cutaneous pain in the left and right insula cortex: a single-trial fMRI study. Pain.2007;128(1-2) : 10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.013 20-30 10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Owen DG, Clarke CF, Bureau Y, Ganapathy S, Prato FS, St Lawrence KS: Measuring the neural response to continuous intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline by perfusion MRI. J Magn Reson Imaging.2012;35(3) : 10.1002/jmri.22814 669-77 10.1002/jmri.22814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Craig AD: Topographically organized projection to posterior insular cortex from the posterior portion of the ventral medial nucleus in the long-tailed macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol.2014;522(1) : 10.1002/cne.23425 36-63 10.1002/cne.23425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Wager TD, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Roy M, Woo CW, Kross E: An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N Engl J Med.2013;368(15) : 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 1388-97 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Cheng JC, Erpelding N, Kucyi A, DeSouza DD, Davis KD: Individual Differences in Temporal Summation of Pain Reflect Pronociceptive and Antinociceptive Brain Structure and Function. J Neurosci.2015;35(26) : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5039-14.2015 9689-700 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5039-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Garcia-Larrea L, Peyron R: Pain matrices and neuropathic pain matrices: a review. Pain.2013;154 Suppl 1: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.001 S29-43 10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Craig AD: Lamina I, but not lamina V, spinothalamic neurons exhibit responses that correspond with burning pain. J Neurophysiol.2004;92(4) : 10.1152/jn.00385.2004 2604-9 10.1152/jn.00385.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Vierck CJ, Whitsel BL, Favorov OV, Brown AW, Tommerdahl M: Role of primary somatosensory cortex in the coding of pain. Pain.2013;154(3) : 10.1016/j.pain.2012.10.021 334-44 10.1016/j.pain.2012.10.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

RESOURCES