Abstract
Decisions in everyday life are prone to error. Standard models typically assume that errors during perceptual decisions are due to noise. However, it is unclear how noise in the sensory input affects the decision. Here we show that there are experimental tasks for which one can analyse the exact spatio-temporal details of a dynamic sensory noise and better understand variability in human perceptual decisions. Using a new experimental visual tracking task and a novel Bayesian decision making model, we found that the spatio-temporal noise fluctuations in the input of single trials explain a significant part of the observed responses. Our results show that modelling the precise internal representations of human participants helps predict when perceptual decisions go wrong. Furthermore, by modelling precisely the stimuli at the single-trial level, we were able to identify the underlying mechanism of perceptual decision making in more detail than standard models.
Perceptual decision making is a core aspect in everyday cognition. In difficult perceptual situations, for instance when driving a car through heavy rain and trying to read a traffic sign, perceptual decision making can be error prone. Standard models assume that these errors are either due to a high noise level in the sensory input (raindrops on the windshield), or to internal brain processes (neuronal noise) or to a mixture of both1,2,3.
In the laboratory, perceptual decision making is usually investigated by linking the perception of incoming sensory information to making a choice among several alternatives4,5,6,7,8, often under time pressure. For example, in the widely-used random dot motion task (RDM), participants are required to report the net direction of a cloud of moving dots within a certain time frame9. The difficulty of such a decision depends on the amount of noise in the stimuli, i.e. the percentage of coherently moving dots in the cloud of dots10,11. The two current standard approaches to modelling behavioural data of such perceptual decision making experiments are the drift diffusion model (DDM)12,13 and the nonlinear attractor model14,15. These models analyse average measures of the behavioural data (e.g. performance expressed as percentage of correct decisions and averages of the reaction times across trials). The implicit assumption of these analyses is that behaviour can be understood by the average effect of sensory noise on decision making performance12.
What standard models cannot explain however, is how each single decision depends precisely on the specific noise pattern in that particular input stimulus. By only looking at the average performance over many trials, one cannot tell what caused a particular incorrect decision: In the example above, the raindrops might be in one instance covering more informative parts of the traffic sign than in others. Following this intuitive notion, we hypothesized that errors in perceptual decisions can be explained better based on the precise pattern of sensory input noise, as compared to using the average noise level. Such a finding would mean that one can analyse perceptual decision making data in high detail, and thereby enable more precise inference about the underlying mechanisms of decision making.
To test this hypothesis, we developed a novel paradigm which enables tracking of the full details of what the observer has seen. This is necessary in order to test whether the participant utilizes the exact spatio-temporal dynamics of the input. We tested this by comparing a model which is informed about the actual trial-wise spatio-temporal noise details of the input stimuli; the exact input model (ExaM), and another model which does not include this information but relies, as standard models do, on average information about the stimuli plus noise as input. This model is mathematically equivalent to the pure drift-diffusion model (DDM)16, which is a standard model for perceptual decision making. We will therefore call this the DDM-equivalent model. Similarly constrained models as ExaM have been long developed in mathematical psychology17, and several studies in decision making used this technique to model traditional psychology paradigms, e.g.18,19,20. Here we applied this approach to computational models of perceptual decision making and demonstrate the implications based on systematic analyses.
We expected two key findings. First, the ExaM will be able to explain more precisely and on a single-trial basis, how human participants make their perceptual decisions, as compared to the DDM-equivalent model. Second, this increase in modelling precision can be used to answer more detailed questions about the underlying mechanism of perceptual decision making. To show this, we addressed the question whether participants use a specific mechanism to respond within a specific time window. One standing hypothesis is that participants dynamically lower their internal criterion of how much information is needed to commit to a decision by reducing their decision boundaries21,22. Neurophysiological experiments have found evidence that monkeys do employ this so-called collapsing bound model (or, similarly, the urgency signal model)21,22,23. However, in humans, there is less clear evidence presumably because the effect of time-pressure on behavioural data is small24,25. We hypothesized that a more precise model, as proposed here, should be able to detect whether human participants are indeed using a collapsing bound when making perceptual decisions.
Results
Task and Behavioural Data
We used a novel behavioural paradigm to investigate perceptual decision making. Human participants (n = 24) saw a single white moving dot, randomly jumping around two yellow target dots on a black computer screen. The white dot locations were sampled from a Gaussian distribution with its mean on one of the two targets with fixed standard deviation. The white dots jumped to a different location on the screen every 93.2 ms, for a maximum of 25 jumps, i.e., 2.33 s. Participants were told that the white dot represents a bee. The task was to decide whether the bee is coming from the left beehive or the right (the two yellow target dots) (Fig. 1a). The task had four difficulty levels, depending on the distance between targets (Fig. 1b). We recorded the decision (left/right, or timed-out) and reaction time (RT) for each trial. The participants were told to be as accurate and as fast as possible, and were given a monetary reward for overall high accuracy and a small number of timed-out trials. As expected, for higher difficulty levels, the accuracy decreased and RT increased (Fig. 1c). The mean accuracy over participants was between 94.4% (standard error (SE) = 0.33) at the easiest difficulty level (D4) and 61.0% (SE = 0.71) at the hardest difficulty level (D1). The corresponding mean RTs were 733 ms (SE = 4.6) and 947 ms (SE = 6.0) (Fig. 1c). The amount of timed-out trials was small (0.2% in total).
Models
DDM-equivalent model vs. Exact Input Model
We used a recently developed Bayesian model16 that consists of: (1) stimulus input to the model, (2) generative model(s) of the stimuli, (3) Bayesian inference as evidence accumulation and (4) a decision criterion. Depending on how we modelled the input, we obtained two different model instances, i.e., the DDM-equivalent model and the ExaM. Both models are exactly the same except for the input to each model: In the DDM-equivalent model, we obtained the equivalent to the pure DDM by modelling the input as the constant, correct target position (yellow dot) with random noise16 (Fig. 2a). In the ExaM, we used as input the exact white dot positions (the bee locations) plus some noise caused by sensory processing (Fig. 2b). This models the time-varying sensory input seen by the participant during each trial. The Bayesian modelling technique used here computes after each update of the white dot position what the participant should compute as evidence for a decision. This is the so-called posterior beliefs over the two decision alternatives (left/right) Fig. 2c,d. The decision is made when the posterior belief for one of the alternatives exceeds a bound (for details on the models, see Material & Methods).
To estimate the parameters of the two models for each participant, we used an advanced inference scheme called Expectation Propagation – Approximate Bayesian Computation (EP-ABC)26. We used seven free parameters that are typically used in modelling perceptual decision making12,16. (1) Noise standard deviation (SD): the amount of noise in the perceptual process in the brain, (2) the mean and (3) standard deviation of the Gaussian which is transformed to the log-normal distribution for the non-decision time (NDT), i.e., the portion of the RT which is irrelevant to the decision process (e.g., perceptual encoding, motor preparation) (see Material & Methods), (4) The bound: the amount of evidence required to commit to a decision, (5) Prior: the bias of a participant for a specific alternative, (6) Lapse probability: the fraction of trials with a random response, and (7) timed-out (TO) lapse probability: the fraction of timed-out trials within the lapse trials (for a complete list of parameters, see Material & Methods). We estimated the parameters from the behavioural data (reaction time and decision for each single trial) for each participant and each difficulty level (for details see Material & Methods).
We found two main differences between parameters fitted by the DDM-equivalent model and the ExaM (Table 1): As compared to the DDM-equivalent model, the ExaM has for all difficulty levels (i) significantly lower noise parameter estimates (~48 pixels lower over conditions) and (ii) significantly shorter non-decision times (~40 ms shorter over conditions) (NDT mode, see Material & Methods). These differences in parameters indicate that with the ExaM the variability in the participants’ behavioural data was explained by using less neuronal noise and a shorter unspecific decision delay, as compared to the DDM-equivalent model. This means that the ExaM is able to explain more variability of the data by the actual decision making mechanism, as opposed to unspecific noise and processing delays.
Table 1. Mean posterior parameter fit for the DDM-equivalent model and exact input model over the four difficulty levels for 24 participants.
Parameter | DDM-Equivalent Model |
Exact Input Model |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
D1 (hard) | D2 | D3 | D4 (easy) | D1 (hard) | D2 | D3 | D4 (easy) | |
Noise SD | 236.70 (22.16) | 76.90 (5.78) | 70.73 (3.94) | 51.29 (4.99) | 95.63** (7.31) | 58.27* (2.93) | 52.10** (2.82) | 38.22* (2.57) |
NDT mode | 563.26 (52.72) | 453.31 (43.54) | 431.24 (40.67) | 430.51 (36.10) | 520.73** (47.29) | 415.96** (37.83) | 382.16** (30.47) | 399.35* (33.16) |
NDT SD | 195.17 (20.38) | 121.61 (13.13) | 137.60 (16.62) | 164.97 (17.95) | 201.66 (16.42) | 181.27** (12.06) | 172.54* (12.17) | 167.54 (10.00) |
Bound | 0.71 (0.01) | 0.74 (0.01) | 0.83 (0.02) | 0.83 (0.02) | 0.65** (0.01) | 0.77 (0.02) | 0.87 (0.02) | 0.88 (0.02) |
Prior | 0.46 (0.01) | 0.46 (0.01) | 0.46 (0.01) | 0.43 (0.02) | 0.47* (0.01) | 0.47 (0.01) | 0.44 (0.01) | 0.42 (0.02) |
Lapse prob. | 0.03 (0.01) | 0.03 (0.00) | 0.02 (0.00) | 0.03 (0.00) | 0.02 (0.00) | 0.02 (0.00) | 0.02 (0.00) | 0.02* (0.00) |
Lapse (TO) prob. | 0.24 (0.02) | 0.16 (0.02) | 0.18 (0.02) | 0.14 (0.01) | 0.24 (0.02) | 0.17 (0.02) | 0.18 (0.02) | 0.15 (0.01) |
TO = Timeout, NDT = Non-decision time, SD = Standard deviation. NDT mode and NDT SDs are reported in milliseconds. Bound and prior are both probabilities (range: 0 to 1). For the prior, 0 means complete right bias and 1 complete left bias. Values in parentheses are SE over participants. The asterisks indicate differences between DDM-equivalent model and exact input model for specific conditions (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, paired t-test over 24 subjects). Most notably, both the noise SD and NDT mode were higher for the DDM-equivalent model, for each condition. As expected, with increasing difficulty level the bound for making decisions decreases. The prior (i.e. a bias for reporting one of the two alternatives) indicates that participants slightly favour the right hand target. This is in line with the observed data; participants made on average approximately 5% more right decisions (data not shown).
Input Stimuli and Evidence Accumulation
How is this noise reduction achieved by the ExaM? In Fig. 2a,b the sensory input for the DDM-equivalent model and ExaM is shown for an illustrative single trial. For the ExaM, the dot jumps around the right target but then (due to the random movement of the dot) jumps to the left side for several time points between 400 ms and 900 ms (red box in Fig. 2b).
Figure 2c,d show the evolution of the posterior beliefs for both models. The DDM-equivalent model mostly predicts a decision for the right, correct target (Fig. 2c). In contrast, the ExaM gives more specific predictions reflecting the dynamics in the actual sensory input over time (Fig. 2d). In this example trial, the participant’s (correct) response lies within the RT distribution predicted by the ExaM (Fig. 2e,f, asterisks below histograms). Interestingly, the ExaM also predicted that the participant may have responded earlier with the correct response, followed by a time period where the incorrect response would have been likely. This dynamic aspect of the ExaM predictions makes the model diverge from the predictions of the DDM-equivalent model, which (always) predicts a uni-modal RT distribution (Fig. 2e). This enables the ExaM to explain more variability in actual decision behaviour. While this is an exemplary single-trial result, this difference between models is the basis for the following results.
Model Comparison
We used Bayesian Model Selection27,28,29 and the posterior predictive log-likelihood (PPL) to formally compare the ExaM and DDM-equivalent model. Bayesian Model Selection is based on the so-called protected exceedance probability and the posterior model probability28,29. We found strong evidence for the ExaM compared to the DDM-equivalent model (Fig. 3a): the protected exceedance probability was approximately 1.00 for all difficulty levels, which means that the belief that the ExaM is more likely than the DDM-equivalent model is nearly 100%. The posterior model probability of the ExaM was on average above 90% (Fig. 3b). This means that the probability that the ExaM generated any randomly selected participant data is above 90%. These two results strongly in favour of the ExaM cannot be explained by different amounts of parameters in the model, because both models had exactly the same number of parameters; the only difference was how the input stimuli were modelled. The results indicate that using the exact input seen by participants results in a better model than relying on the average input assumptions made by the standard model, the DDM.
Next, to compare the two models at the single trial level, we calculated the PPL for each trial and each participant. The PPL quantifies how likely a participant’s response is under a given model after fitting the parameters. As we have shown the same 800 trials to each participant we can average the trial-specific PPL, over participants. Figure 4 shows that a larger number of trials, for each difficulty level, has a higher PPL for the ExaM than for the DDM-equivalent model. This is most obvious for the most difficult condition (D1), for which the stimuli display the highest levels of random movements. This further substantiates that behavioural responses can be explained better if one informs the model about the exact noise pattern of the stimulus.
To further show the difference between the two models, we calculated point-predictions for matches between model and participant responses (Fig. 5). We calculated the most probable decision for each trial (see Material & Methods) based on the PPLs, and compared the model choice to actual participant choice for both DDM-equivalent model and ExaM. Figure 5 shows that for each of the four difficulty levels, the ExaM predicted the participants’ decisions better than the DDM-equivalent model. Similarly, the ExaM provides for more accurate RT point estimates (Fig. S1). Again, as with the PPL results, the difference between the two models was more prominent in the most difficult conditions. Critically, we also found that the ExaM explained significantly more errors made by participants, as compared to the DDM-equivalent model (Table 2).
Table 2. Model performances on predicting choice.
DDM-Equivalent Model |
Exact Input Model |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
participantcorrect | participantIncorrect | participantcorrect | participantincorrect | |
model correct | 0.76 | 0.15 | 0.76 | 0.08** |
model incorrect | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.11** |
Proportion of correct and incorrect trials for DDM-equivalent model and exact input model (ExaM), pooled over all participants, and all conditions. The proportion of predicted incorrect trials was larger for the ExaM than for the DDM-equivalent model (**p < 0.01, paired t-test between DDM-Equivalent and ExaM over 24 subjects, and 4 conditions).
Decision Making Mechanisms
Next we tested our hypothesis that the ExaM outperforms the DDM-equivalent model in providing evidence for a collapsing bound decision-making mechanism. To do this, we modelled and compared, for both models, three different mechanisms that have been suggested for perceptual decision making: the standard evidence accumulation employed above, the so-called leaky accumulation30,31, and the collapsing bound model21,22,32. For the DDM-equivalent model, the Bayesian Model Selection result showed no evidence for the collapsing bound model; the standard evidence accumulation was the best model across all four difficulty levels and there was no evidence for leaky integration either (Fig. 6a,b). In contrast, with the ExaM we found a different picture: the collapsing bound model was frequent among the participants (Fig. 6d). This was the case for the intermediate difficulty levels. For the easiest and hardest difficulty levels there was strong evidence for the standard accumulation (Fig. 6c,d). These results are congruent with the PPL results, where we found for the ExaM that the collapsing bound model provides for each condition a closer fit to the behavioural data than the standard accumulation model (Fig. 7). Furthermore, for the ExaM there was no evidence of leaky integration (Fig. 6c,d).
In Fig. 8a–c, we show the three collapsing bound parameters (see Material & Methods), for both the DDM-equivalent model and the ExaM. These three parameters, the initial bound, the stretch (how far the bound falls), and shape (how early the bound falls) determine the overall effect of the collapsing bound. These parameters and their changes over conditions reveal how the collapsing bound may operate in our experiment: The initial bound (Fig. 8a) increases as the conditions get easier from D1 to D4 (Fig. 8b) for both DDM-equivalent model and ExaM. This effect is more prominent for the ExaM. For both the DDM-equivalent model and the ExaM, the stretch decreases as the conditions get easier. This means the overall effect of the collapsing bound (which also depends on the initial bound) is smaller for the easier trials. Again, this effect is more pronounced for the ExaM for all difficulty levels except for the easiest condition D1. For the shape parameter, we found that the bound drops earlier for the two most difficult conditions than for the two easiest conditions (See Material & Methods). See Fig. 9 for a visualisation of the fitted collapsing bounds.
Figure 8d shows the absolute amount of collapse for each difficulty level in terms of bound. Importantly, the amount of collapse closely resembles both the overall PPL pattern and the model frequency for the ExaM (see Fig. 6c,d and 7, number of trials under dotted vertical line). This indicates that both the PPL differences and model frequencies indeed capture the amount of collapse expected for different conditions.
Figure 10 shows an example of the evolution of the decision-making process under ExaM, with (Fig. 10b,d,f) and without (Fig. 10a,c,e) the collapsing bound from the same trial. Due to the first dot starting on the right side (Fig. 10a), early decisions tend to be the right alternative (Fig. 10c,e), with later decisions choosing the correct left decisions. However, with the collapsing bound, the initial bound is higher than the standard accumulation, thereby delaying the earlier decisions, which makes the erroneous right decision less frequent. This therefore increases the probability for the left decision, while reducing the probability for timed-out trials altogether (Fig. 10d,f). In other words, the collapsing bound shifts the probability mass from both early and late time points to more intermediate RTs.
Discussion
In the present study, we have shown that modelling the detailed sensory input observed by the participants enables predicting response time and choice at the single trial level for a simple visual perceptual decision making task. We found that with the detailed sensory input model, a collapsing bound and its shape can be identified, providing a deeper understanding of the underlying decision making mechanism in humans. The single trial analysis may be especially useful when applied to neural measurements, because the model predicts trial-specific variations of internal, decision-related variables. This can be used for investigating the representations of these variables in the brain33.
When we compared the exact input model (ExaM) to the DDM-equivalent model, there were three main findings: Firstly, we found that the behavioural data, both choices and reaction times were better explained by the ExaM, both at the individual, single-trial level and on average. Secondly, the fitted noise parameter and non-decision time were significantly smaller for the ExaM compared to the DDM-equivalent model. This indicates that the DDM-equivalent model explained more of the behavioural data by unspecific noise processes, as opposed to the ExaM, which attributed more variability of the behavioural data to the actual decision making mechanism. Thirdly, the ExaM explained significantly more errors made by the participants than the DDM-equivalent model.
Our findings point towards a re-conceptualization of what constitutes an error: In the ExaM approach, there is no such label as correct/incorrect; rather we quantify how well such an apparently erroneous decision can be explained given the concrete sensory input seen by the participant (Fig. 2). This probabilistic way of modelling binary choices34 in combination with RT histograms may be a better basis for understanding the underlying mechanisms of decision making, than analysing average performance rates using only two classes of correct/incorrect decisions in conjunction with RT histograms reduced to five quantiles as typically done in standard analyses12,13.
Many experimenters in the perceptual decision making field use the ’random-dot motion (RDM)’ task rather than a single-dot tracking task as used here. The RDM task gained popularity after it had been used in perceptual decision making experiments with monkeys10,35. The original motivation was to employ a visual stimulus which is optimal for activating extrastriate areas, especially the motion-sensitive area MT, for neuronal recording10. In the present study, this was not the primary concern, but the aim was to design a stimulus that is most certainly perceived by the participant over all time points. This is important because with many dots as in the RDM task it is uncertain which dot movements the participants perceived and have used in their internal decision making. This uncertainty makes predicting behaviour at a single trial level challenging. By using single-dot stimuli, we minimized this uncertainty to showcase a proof of concept that precise modelling of the sensory input is useful for addressing specific questions about the underlying decision making mechanism. In our stimulus, the single dot location captures all variability whereas in standard RDM stimuli variability is distributed across many dots in the display. Thus, it is possible that we only observe a strongly predictable effect of the stimulus (tested with the ExaM), because participants attend more to the particular variability present in the single dot stimulus. This would also suggest that our paradigm is effective in isolating the mechanisms of decision making by reducing the uncertainty effects of lower level sensory processing, i.e., stimulus feature extraction. Note, however, that there is evidence that low-level features of RDM stimuli affect choices. For example, it has been found that the transient motion energy content of a RDM stimulus influences choices even in zero coherence trials, especially early in long trials36, or when only the moment-to-moment fluctuations not associated with the motion strength nor direction is considered37. Further, Bair and Koch38 have shown that the firing of MT neurons is temporally highly consistent across repeated presentations of exactly the same RDM stimulus. Because these MT neurons are thought to provide the evidence for decision making in RDM tasks4, this finding suggests that the particular, dynamically changing features of the RDM stimuli affect perceptual decisions. Indeed, when performing an analysis of the responses recorded in an RDM experiment (online database used in11, http://www.neuralsignal.org database nsa2004.1), we found that responses to RDM stimuli with “frozen noise”, i.e., with exactly the same sequence of dot movements, were markedly less random than responses to RDM stimuli in which the particular dot movements varied across trials (see Fig. S2). Stimulus details, therefore, also matter in RDM tasks, but it remains to be shown how much, especially, because it is still unclear exactly which spatiotemporal features the brain uses to judge the direction of RDM stimuli. In our study, we aimed to show that these results showing the correlation between noise and behaviour can be modeled and predicted by accounting for the exact sensory input. Although we did not collect data with ‘frozen noise’, we used the exact same stimuli for all 24 participants. In Fig. S3–6, the predicted RTs pooled over all participants are shown for all difficulty levels. These figures identify those trials (by the trial-specific PPL difference) for which most of the participants chose the incorrect targets. This further supports the notion that details in the input stimuli were inducing similar behavior across participants.
A recent auditory decision making study has modelled the precise timing of a sequence of click sounds to better resolve specific decision making parameters, as compared to standard analysis procedures34. The authors found that the main source of variability was based in the sensory input, and the accumulation was without leak. In addition to the same single-trial approach in this study, we also analysed reaction times: By only modelling participants’ choices, one may be limited in fully resolving the underlying parameters of decision making. In particular, for our design, the reaction time of a single trial determines how many dots have been seen by the participant and is therefore clearly relevant for modelling the observed choices. Furthermore, one implicit assumption of the ExaM is that participants weight single pieces of evidence (here, a single dot shown for 93 ms) by their relevance for deciding between the two alternatives39,40,41. For example, the more a dot’s location is to the left, the higher the likelihood for the left target. We were able to show that this weighting of evidence actually holds by finding that the ExaM is a better model than the DDM-equivalent model.
Although most theoretical accounts agree on the accumulation of evidence as a general mechanism of perceptual decision making4,12,42,43,44, it is currently unclear whether humans use a collapsing bound to conform to time pressures of decision making. In behavioural studies, positive evidence is sparse22,45 and has been established using standard DDMs with only few free parameters (two or three). Milosavljevic et al.24 showed that there is no advantage of the collapsing bound DDM once the standard (i.e., more complex) DDM12 is considered. In a recent study25, the authors conclude from a literature review and additional experiments that evidence accumulation with a fixed bound (but not a collapsing bound) is the most favoured model in perceptual decision-making, both in humans and in primates. One reason for this finding may be that the measurable effect of the collapsing bound on reaction times is rather small. The standard DDM approach is based on a severe reduction of data which aggregates the RT data into typically just ten RT quantiles per condition12 and comparing these with the model predictions as a measure of fitness. This data-reducing method may not be able to catch the subtle effect of the collapsing bound. Here, we have provided evidence that this question can be investigated with an increased modelling resolution by using a single-trial analysis.
We found that the ExaM with a collapsing bound better predicted participant behaviour than the other two model variants (Fig. 7). In addition, we found trends towards a collapsing bound model using Bayesian model selection for the medium difficulty levels (Fig. 6c,d). We only found evidence for a collapsing bound with the ExaM, when the exact sensory input entered the analysis.
The collapsing bound has been suggested as a mechanism to cope with the time limit to execute a decision21,46,47. Therefore, intuitively, harder trials should benefit more from the collapsing bound. However, our results indicate that in the most difficult condition the drop in bound is rather low (Fig. 8d). This may be due to an interaction between collapsing bound and the speed-accuracy trade-off chosen by participants across difficulty levels. Figure 8a shows that the initial values for the bound decrease as the task becomes more difficult. We also observed this effect in the models without collapsing bound (Table 1) where the bound is thought to implement the speed-accuracy trade-off chosen by the participant, see also4,48. This finding suggests that participants adapt their bound, i.e., the speed-accuracy trade-off, to the difficulty level of a trial which is indicated before the stimulus comes on (in our task, yellow dots are shown before white dot, Fig. 1a). Especially, participants may choose low bounds in difficult trials to counter small momentary evidences and an otherwise slow accumulation. Since the bound is already low at the start of a difficult trial, the effect of a collapsing bound in the hardest condition D1 is limited. In the easiest condition D4, the amount of collapse is the lowest (Fig. 8d). This result is fairly intuitive - the participants can make fast decisions without having to fear the upcoming end of the trial. If we used a paradigm which does not indicate the difficulty level at the beginning, we expect to see a higher initial bound at the most difficult D1 condition, and therefore more measureable collapsing bound effect, under the ExaM.
The shape of the collapsing bound (Fig. 9) determines when the bound drops strongest throughout the trial. We have found that difficult conditions exhibit an early drop, whereas in easier conditions there is a more gradual decrease. Although this does not seem to comply with the intuition that bounds should collapse towards the end of the trial, it is consistent with previous findings21,22. Clearly, more research is needed to investigate the causes of these particular shapes of collapse. Overall, we suggest that the ExaM is in principle sensitive enough to find evidence for a collapsing bound based on experiments, but the actual implementation of a collapsing bound by participants may depend on several factors, for example on the precise experimental design, the level of training, and instructions to the participants25,49.
In conclusion, modelling random spatiotemporal dynamics in the sensory input provides better fits of participant behaviour and enhances the predictive power of perceptual decision making models. Further, the increased modelling precision gained through incorporating the spatiotemporal dynamics of input stimuli into the model has allowed us to detect subtle effects of different decision making strategies in the behaviour.
In our present study we employed a particularly simple stimulus to maximally control the spatiotemporal information available for decision making. The proposed Bayesian account of evidence integration and computation can be easily adapted to different, more standard or more natural stimuli as long as there is high certainty about what features or parts of the stimulus participants is used to make decisions. For example, for the RDM stimulus motion energy36 could be used as spatiotemporal stimulus feature in our model. The described approach thus opens up a wide range of new experimental-computational studies with a large variety of stimuli to enable improved insights into the underlying mechanism of perceptual decision making.
Material & Methods
Participants
25 right-handed healthy volunteers participated in the study (mean age 24.8 years; 12 females). All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and reported no history of neurological or psychiatric diseases. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant. The experimental procedure was approved and carried out in accordance with the guidelines by the ethics committee of the University of Leipzig. One participant’s data were excluded due to the participant not being able to follow the instructions.
Stimuli and Task
Visual stimuli were presented using the Presentation® software (Version 16.5, www.neurobs.com) on a 1024 × 768 pixels CRT monitor (refresh rate: 75 Hz). Participants were seated in a chair ~70–80 cm from the monitor.
Stimuli were composed of one white moving dot, and two yellow stationary target position dots on a black screen (Fig. 1a). Two yellow target position dots were located mirror-symmetrically from the vertical midline of the screen. The distance of the two dots from the centre determined the difficulty of the trial (Fig. 1b). There were four difficulty levels where the two targets’ horizontal positions had a distance of 55 (easy), 40, 25 and 10 pixels (hard) symmetrically from the centre of the screen (corresponding visual angles: 3°, 2.2°, 1.4°, 0.6°, respectively). Each target position pair (e.g., −10, 10) was presented simultaneously on the screen.
The white moving dot was presented for 93.2 ms (7 frames) at a specific location (Fig. 1a), and jumped to a new position every 93.2 ms. Each position of the white dot was drawn from a Gaussian distribution with a mean equal to the target positions (left and right) and a standard deviation of 70 pixels. Each difficulty level had 200 trials (100 left + 100 right), resulting in a total of 800 trials per participant.
Experimental Setup
The experiment consisted of three phases: 440 training trials with feedback followed by 40 rehearsal trials (without feedback) and finally the main phase with 800 trials (200 trials x 4 blocks, with breaks). In order to promote attentiveness and motivation, participants received a bonus for good performance (fast and accurate).
Before a trial started, a fixation cross was shown (duration: 300~500 ms). Next, two yellow target dot positions appeared for 700 ms, in addition to the fixation cross. Then the fixation cross disappeared (the yellow target position dots remained) and the moving white dot appeared (Fig. 1a). The task was to figure out around which target the white dot was moving. The stimulus was displayed until the participant reported the decision by pressing one of two buttons (left or right) on a 2-key button box, but maximally for 25 jumps of the white dot (≈2330 ms). If the participant failed to respond during this period, the trial timed-out and the next trial started.
Analysis of Behavioural Data
All analyses were performed in MATLAB® (version 7 or above, Mathworks, MA). The reaction time and response (left, right, timed-out) for each trial were recorded. Importantly, since the input stimuli were generated before the experiment, each participant saw the exact same 800 trials of white dot movements but in randomized trial order. Therefore, for each participant and trial, the precise visual input was available for subsequent analysis using the Bayesian model. The models were fit to the data five times and evidences showed little variation across EP-ABC repetitions. The models were fit to the actual response (left, right, timed-out) of the participants rather than the correct/incorrectness of the responses. Parameter estimates are shown in Table 1. The runs that gave the best fit to the data (largest model evidences) were used for further analyses.
We computed point estimates of choice and RT for each trial and each participant from the predicted response distributions. Specifically, in a given trial we chose the alternative (left, right) which was most frequent among 15000 simulations of that trial from the model with the fitted parameters. Further, we chose the corresponding RT as the most frequent time-point for the chosen alternative. Then, we did a simple match test between the real behavioural data and model predicted data (Fig. 5). For the RTs, we plotted the observed RTs against the predicted RTs as an indicator of a good match (Fig. S1).
Experimental Stimulus
Input stimuli were generated by drawing dot locations for target i
Where is the location of target , pixels is a standard deviation and is the two-dimensional identity matrix. We set where is a distance (in pixels) from the centre of the screen which is positive for target 2 (right), negative for target 1 (left) and the magnitude is one of 10, 25, 40 or 55 as determined by the condition (difficulty) of trial .
Bayesian Response Model
We recently presented a probabilistic Bayesian model which is equivalent to a pure drift diffusion model16. A major advantage of the Bayesian model is that it provides a direct link between stimulus and evidence accumulation whereas most other models of perceptual decisions abstract from the stimulus and only consider average effects of the stimulus on the evidence at any given point in time. We here exploit this property of the Bayesian model and simply deploy the mechanism we used to generate the stimulus for the experiment as generative model in the decision model. The Bayesian response model consists of: 1) input to the model (observations), (2) generative model(s) of the stimulus, (3) Bayesian inference as evidence accumulation and 4) decision criterion. We call 2–4 the decision model. Its free parameters are bias and confidence bound , as described below.
Given the generative models and observed dot positions the decision model accumulates evidence for a target using Bayesian inference. Starting from a prior (bias) , the decision model recursively computes the posterior beliefs that the observed dot positions were generated from (one of) the two targets:
where indicates all observed dot positions up to time point and for the previous posterior belief is replaced by the prior . We parameterise the prior as the a priori probability that the dots were drawn from target 1, i.e., . The a priori probability of target 2 then also derives from parameter as . represents the generative model within the decision model. The generative models define the likelihood of dot positions under the hypothesis that these were generated from the corresponding target . We assumed that the subjects had acquired a good representation of the possible stimuli after training and, consequently, used the same Gaussian distributions from which we created the stimuli as generative models. Specifically, we used and .
The computed posterior beliefs are the decision variables of the model. Consequently, the model makes a decision when either one of them reaches a set bound, i.e., when
where is a parameter of the model and can directly be interpreted as a level of confidence that a decision maker wants to reach before making a decision.
The target for which the posterior belief reached the bound is the choice of the model. The time at which the bound was reached first is the decision time which is a discrete variable counting the number of observed dot locations. As described in the main text, the total reaction time of the model is the decision time plus a variable non-decision time see, e.g.12,50, i.e.,
We modelled the non-decision time with a log-normal distribution to ensure positive non-decision times, i.e.,
where and are again free parameters of the model.
We then transform into a reaction time in millisecond, as recorded in the experiment, by multiplying with the time the single dot spent at the same location in the experiment, i.e., ms.
In the experiment, responses of subjects were timed out after a fixed deadline. We applied the same rule to the responses generated by the model, i.e., if was greater than 25, the trial was timed out. The non-decision time (NDT) and non-decision time standard deviation (NDT SD) were calculated as the mode and standard deviation of the log-normal distribution with the fitted parameters , respectively.
Additional to these evidence-based decisions we allowed for random lapses in the model. We followed previous approaches to model lapses22 and randomly selected trials as lapse trials with probability which was a free parameter of the model. When a trial was selected as lapse trial, it could either be a timed out trial, or a trial with random response. We set a lapse trial to timed out with probability (free model parameter). With probability we then generated a random response by drawing choice and reaction time from a uniform distribution ( where is the continuous uniform distribution within the range .
In summary, the free parameters of the decision model were the prior probability of choosing target 1 (), the bound on the posterior beliefs (), and of the non-decision time log-normal distribution, the lapse probability () and the probability that a lapse trial is timed out ().
Input to the Decision Model
The Bayesian response model makes decisions based on observations which provide evidence for or against an alternative. We considered two kinds of observations reflecting different amounts of information about the true stimulus constituting the DDM-equivalent model and the exact input model (ExaM).
The observations containing most information about the stimulus are the dot locations themselves. Since we assumed that participants use the full information available on the screen (horizontal and vertical), we provided the 2D-coordinates of the moving dot in the ExaM as observations to the model: . The DDM-equivalent model used minimal information about the time course of the presented stimulus, i.e., it used the fixed location of the true target as observations for the model: ( indicates the true target chosen in trial ). This is equivalent to how input is treated by previous perceptual decision making models, such as the drift diffusion model.
In both models we added noise to the mean (stimulus driven) observations such that the actual observations which were input to the decision model were
where is the standard deviation of sensory noise which was fit to data together with the parameters of the decision model and is the two-dimensional identity matrix.
Inference Over Models Given Participant Responses
We used a second level of Bayesian inference to infer model parameters from behavioural data of participants. Standard inference methods require knowledge of the likelihood of the model for given data and model parameters. For the Bayesian response model, however, we chose a likelihood-free inference method. This approach has the advantage that we can easily modify the model without having to derive the potentially very complex likelihood function for the modified model. Yet, inference works as for the original model.
Likelihood-free inference methods are also known under the name of “Approximate Bayesian Computation” ABC, see51 for a recent review. Computation of likelihood values is skipped in these methods by simulating data points from the model and comparing simulated data points to the recorded real data on the basis of a summary statistic of the complete data set. As data points are simulated using a particular sample of parameter values (a sampled parameter set), ABC then rejects parameter sets incompatible with the data and approximates posterior distributions of model parameters based on the accepted samples. The main drawback of ABC methods is that inference is computationally very demanding, especially, when the statistic summarising the data set is high-dimensional.
Recently, a variant of ABC has been proposed which circumvents the use of summary statistics of the data26. The method extends an efficient approximate inference method (expectation propagation: EP) to the likelihood-free setting and is called EP-ABC. Instead of comparing whole data sets, EP-ABC compares sampled against measured data points individually while speeding up likelihood-free inference by orders of magnitude.
EP-ABC has been applied to a diffusion model26. We here use it for the proposed Bayesian model. A data point (response of participant or model) consists of the combination of choice (left, right) and reaction time of a single trial. Data points simulated from the model lead to acceptance of the corresponding parameter sample, if the simulated choice equals that of the participant for the given trial and the difference between the simulated reaction time and that of the participant is smaller than a parameter . In all reported analyses we used ms (half as long as the dot stays in a location on the screen in the experiment).
EP-ABC cycles through all data points in a data set where in our application each data point corresponds to the recorded behaviour of a single trial (choice and reaction time). After reaching a minimum number of accepted parameter samples for the given data point (set to 300 in our analyses), it updates the posterior distribution over model parameters and moves to the next data point. The updating scheme implements expectation propagation (EP)52,53 in which the true, potentially complex posterior distribution is approximated with a product of feasible distributions. In our application of EP-ABC each factor of this product represents one data point (trial) and is a Gaussian distribution. Updates of the posterior adjust the mean and covariance of one Gaussian factor such that a hybrid distribution and the full product of Gaussian factors become maximally similar. The corresponding hybrid distribution for one data point is obtained by replacing the Gaussian factor under consideration with the distribution estimated from the accepted parameter samples. Consequently, the main operation in the update of the posterior simply consists of estimating the mean and covariance of the accepted parameter values. In our analysis we used the implementation of this updating scheme provided by the first author of26 at https://sites.google.com/site/simonbarthelme/software/abcep_release.zip.
With increasing number of data points the posterior approximated by EP-ABC converges. To ensure convergence we made four passes through all data points of a data set. Additional to the posterior distribution over model parameters, EP-ABC also provides an estimate of the model evidence for the used model. The model evidence is returned as the log-marginal likelihood of the model given the whole data set.
We performed EP-ABC separately for trials from single conditions of individual participants, i.e., we obtained model evidences and parameter posteriors for each experimental condition of each participant. Therefore, EP-ABC data sets consisted of 200 trials. We had four different data sets per participant and we computed 24 × 4 parameter posteriors and model evidences per considered model.
Because we performed Bayesian inference over model parameters, we also needed to define parameter priors. EP-ABC approximates posterior distributions with Gaussians and, by default, also requires Gaussian priors. For simplicity we chose standard normal priors internally within EP-ABC. Because EP-ABC is based on sampling parameter values, we could, however, easily include parameter transformations which allowed us to implement various constraints for different parameter values leading to non-Gaussian priors for the parameters. In particular, we used two types of transformation: 1) exponential-transform and 2) uniform-transform. The exponential-transform maps a variable defined on the real line to positive values only: . Thus, it transforms the Gaussian prior into a log-normal prior. The uniform-transform maps a normally-distributed variable through the cumulative density function of the standard normal distribution: . Thus, it transforms the Gaussian prior into a uniform prior on an interval defined by range and offset . We list the resulting priors in Table 3 and the corresponding prior densities in Fig. 11.
Table 3. Overview of free parameters in the Bayesian response model and its variants together with their prior distributions.
Model parameter | Prior | Prior parameters | Used by models |
---|---|---|---|
(noise std) | log-N | all | |
* | N | all | |
* | log-N | all | |
(prior) | uni | all | |
(bound) | uni | all | |
(lapse probability) | (cf. Fig. 11) | all | |
(time-out lapse probability) | uni | all | |
(discount rate) | uni | leaky | |
(stretch) | uni | collapsing | |
(shape) | log-N | collapsing |
log-N: log-normal prior with standard parameters and . uni: uniform prior in interval . We chose these priors to cover all plausible parameter values while adhering to basic constraints such that standard deviation parameters must be positive and that parameters expressed as probabilities range between 0 and 1. For completeness, we show in Fig. 11 the resulting prior densities for each parameter. *: parameters of the log-normal distribution for the non-decision time.
The posterior distribution over parameters inferred by EP-ABC for a given data set is a multivariate Gaussian distribution. Each dimension of that Gaussian corresponds to one parameter before its transformation. When reporting posterior parameter distributions, we report the distributions after transformation as estimated by sampling from the Gaussian EP-ABC posterior and transforming the samples according to the given transform functions. An example of a complete, sampled posterior distribution is shown in Fig. 12.
To compare models with different inputs and accumulation mechanisms, a random-effect Bayesian model selection (RFX-BMS) procedure as described by27,28,29 was used on model evidences produced by the ABC method. Protected exceedance probabilities were calculated with the VBM toolbox, available at: https://sites.google.com/site/jeandaunizeauswebsite/code/rfx-bms.
Posterior Predictive Likelihoods
To quantify how good a model fits to participants’ responses, we computed the posterior predictive likelihoods. These quantify how likely a participant’s response is under the model after the model has been fit to the participant’s behaviour. Formally, the posterior predictive likelihood for a trial is the probability of the response given the model and the responses (choice and RT) of all trials .
We approximated the posterior predictive likelihood for a trial i by sampling 15000 parameter sets from the EP-ABC parameter posterior, transforming the parameter samples to the original parameter space and then sampling a response from the model for each of the 15000 parameter sets. From the 15000 sampled responses we estimated the response distribution of the model for trial . The posterior predictive likelihood is estimated as the fraction of sampled responses which are close to the participant response in trial as determined by the EP-ABC distance criterion (matching choice and reaction time difference ms).
Since the posterior predictive likelihoods reuse the data to evaluate the model after adaptation to the data, they, in principle, may be too optimistic, when the model is too flexible and explains the noise in the data, i.e., when the model overfits. We have not found indication that the models we used overfit the data. Also, the models which only differ by the input (DDM-equivalent and exact input model) are equally complex. Comparisons based on the posterior predictive likelihoods, therefore, only reflect how beneficial knowledge of the corresponding input is for predicting the response of a participant.
Evidence Accumulation Variants
We investigated two additional decision models which implemented two additional mechanisms previously considered in perceptual decision making: leaky accumulation31,34,54 and collapsing bounds21,22,46,47,55. Leaky accumulation departs from the optimal accumulation of evidence as defined by the Bayesian decision model by continuously degrading the influence of past evidence on the current beliefs.
We implemented leaky accumulation by using the following instead of Eq. (2):
The exponent of the previous posterior beliefs is an additional free model parameter. Its effect is more clearly seen in the space of log-probabilities where we have
controls how much influence the previous posterior beliefs have on the current beliefs. We also call it the discount factor. It varies between 0 (no accumulation) and 1 (perfect accumulation).
Motivated by electrophysiological findings e.g.,21 it has been suggested that decision variables are also influenced by ‘urgency signals’ which increase towards the end of a trial and push participants towards a decision independent of sensory evidence. Theoretically, an urgency signal is equivalent to a collapsing bound22. A collapsing bound captures participant’s willingness to trade accuracy against the chance to make a choice at all in light of an upcoming deadline. The overall shape of urgency signals, or collapsing bounds is still unclear. Although intuition dictates that bounds should collapse towards the end of a trial, empirical estimates tend to show an early drop in bound21,22. We here opted for a functional form for the bound which can represent both cases depending on its parameters:
This function, describing the time-varying bound, has three parameters. is the initial (maximal) bound value. determines how far the bound collapses: for the bound collapses completely to the value of 0.5, for the bound does not collapse at all and stays at its initial value . determines the shape of the collapse: for (approximately) the bound collapses early, for the bound collapses late. We demonstrate these effects in Fig. 13.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Park, H. et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of random stimuli account for trial-to-trial variability in perceptual decision making. Sci. Rep. 6, 18832; doi: 10.1038/srep18832 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the Center of Information Services and High Performance Computing (ZIH) at Technische Universität Dresden for providing the computer resources. Also, we acknowledge support by the German Research Foundation and the Open Access Publication Funds of the TU Dresden.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author Contributions H.P., J.L., S.B. and S.J.K. designed the experiment, H.P. and J.L. acquired the data, H.P., J.L., S.B. and S.J.K. analysed the data, and H.P., J.L., K.V.K., S.B. and S.J.K. wrote the manuscript.
References
- Faisal A. A., Selen L. P. J. & Wolpert D. M. Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen M. N., Britten K. H., Newsome W. T. & Movshon J. A. A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion. J. Neurosci. 16, 1486–1510 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deneve S., Latham P. E. & Pouget A. Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 826–831 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold J. I. & Shadlen M. N. The neural basis of decision making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 535–574 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heekeren H. R., Marrett S. & Ungerleider L. G. The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 467–479 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen M. N. & Kiani R. Decision making as a window on cognition. Neuron 80, 791–806 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albantakis L. & Deco G. The encoding of alternatives in multiple-choice decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 10308–10313 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiech K. et al. Influence of prior information on pain involves biased perceptual decision-making. Curr. Biol. 24, R679–R681 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roitman J. D. & Shadlen M. N. Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task. J. Neurosci. 22, 9475–9489 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newsome W. T. & Paré E. B. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J. Neurosci. 8, 2201–2211 (1988). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britten K. H., Shadlen M. N., Newsome W. T. & Movshon A. J. The Analysis of Visual Motion: A Comparison Psychophysical Performance of Neuronal. J. Neurosci. 12, 4745–4765 (1992). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ratcliff R. & McKoon G. The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Comput. 20, 873–922 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ratcliff R. A theory of memory retrieval. Psychol. Rev. 85, 59–108 (1978). [Google Scholar]
- Wang X.-j. Probabilistic Decision Making by Slow Reverberation in Cortical Circuits. Neuron 36, 955–968 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong K. F., Huk A. C., Shadlen M. N. & Wang X. J. Neural circuit dynamics underlying accumulation of time-varying evidence during perceptual decision making. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 1, 10.3389/neuro.10.006.2007 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bitzer S., Park H., Blankenburg F. & Kiebel S. J. Perceptual decision making: drift-diffusion model is equivalent to a Bayesian model. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00102 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busemeyer J. R. & Rapoport A. Psychological models of deferred decision making. J. Math. Psychol. 32, 91–134 (1988). [Google Scholar]
- FitzGerald T. H. B., Schwartenbeck P., Moutoussis M., Dolan R. J. & Friston K. Active Inference, Evidence Accumulation, and the Urn Task. Neural Comput. 27, 306–328 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck B. B. Theory of Choice in Bandit, Information Sampling and Foraging Tasks. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11, 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004164 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee M. D. & Cummins T. D. R. Evidence accumulation in decision making: Unifying the “take the best” and the “rational” models. Psychon Bull Rev 11, 343–352 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchland A. K., Kiani R. & Shadlen M. N. Decision-making with multiple alternatives. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 851–851 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drugowitsch J., Moreno-Bote R., Churchland A. K., Shadlen M. N. & Pouget A. The cost of accumulating evidence in perceptual decision making. J. Neurosci. 32, 3612–3628 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanks T. D., Mazurek M. E., Kiani R., Hopp E. & Shadlen M. N. Elapsed Decision Time Affects the Weighting of Prior Probability in a Perceptual Decision Task. J. Neurosci. 31, 6339–6352 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milosavljevic M., Malmaud J., Huth A., Koch C. & Rangel A. The Drift Diffusion Model can account for the accuracy and reaction time of value-based choices under high and low time pressure. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 5, 437–449 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins G. E., Forstmann B. U., Wagenmakers E.-J., Ratcliff R. & Brown S. D. Revisiting the Evidence for Collapsing Boundaries and Urgency Signals in Perceptual Decision-Making. J. Neurosci. 35, 2476–2484 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barthelmé S. & Chopin N. Expectation Propagation for Likelihood-Free Inference. JASA 109, 315–333 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- Stephan K. E., Penny W. D., Daunizeau J., Moran R. J. & Friston K. J. Bayesian model selection for group studies. Neuroimage 46, 1004–1017 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigoux L., Stephan K. E., Friston K. J. & Daunizeau J. Bayesian model selection for group studies—Revisited. Neuroimage 84, 971–985 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daunizeau J., Adam V. & Rigoux L. VBA: A Probabilistic Treatment of Nonlinear Models for Neurobiological and Behavioural Data. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003441 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ossmy O. et al. The Timescale of Perceptual Evidence Integration Can Be Adapted to the Environment. Curr. Biol. 23, 981–986 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Usher M. & McClelland J. L. The time course of perceptual choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychol. Rev. 108, 550–592 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell B. A. et al. Neurally Constrained Modeling of Perceptual Decision Making. Psychol. Rev. 117, 1113–1143 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanks T. D. et al. Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation. Nature 520, 220–223 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunton B. W., Botvinick M. M. & Brody C. D. Rats and humans can optimally accumulate evidence for decision-making. Science 340, 95–98 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britten K. H., Newsome W. T., Shadlen M. N., Celebrini S. & Movshon J. A. A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT. Vis. Neurosci. 13, 87–100 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiani R., Hanks T. D. & Shadlen M. N. Bounded integration in parietal cortex underlies decisions even when viewing duration is dictated by the environment. J. Neurosci. 28, 3017–3029 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resulaj A., Kiani R., Wolpert D. M. & Shadlen M. N. Changes of mind in decision-making. Nature 461, 263–266 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bair W. & Koch C. Temporal precision of spike trains in extrastriate cortex of the behaving macaque monkey. Neural Comput. 8, 1185–1202 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kira S., Yang T. & Shadlen M. N. A Neural Implementation of Wald’s Sequential Probability Ratio Test. Neuron 85, 861–873 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Gardelle V. & Summerfield C. Robust averaging during perceptual judgment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 13341–13346 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Lange F. P., Van Gaal S., Lamme V. A. F. & Dehaene S. How Awareness Changes the Relative Weights of Evidence During Human Decision-Making. PLoS. Biol. 9, 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001203 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Lange F. P., Jensen O. & Dehaene S. Accumulation of Evidence during Sequential Decision Making: The Importance of Top-Down Factors. J. Neurosci. 30, 731–738 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyart V., De Gardelle V., Scholl J. & Summerfield C. Rhythmic fluctuations in evidence accumulation during decision making in the human brain. Neuron 76, 847–858 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Summerfield C. & De Lange F. P. Expectation in perceptual decision making: neural and computational mechanisms. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 745–756 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowman N. E., Kording K. P. & Gottfried J. A. Temporal Integration of Olfactory Perceptual Evidence in Human Orbitofrontal Cortex. Neuron 75, 916–927 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reddi B. A. J. & Carpenter R. H. S. The influence of urgency on decision time. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 827–830 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frazier P. & Yu A. Sequential Hypothesis Testing under Stochastic Deadlines. Adv. Neural Inf. Process Syst. 20, 465–472 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- Bogacz R., Brown E., Moehlis J., Holmes P. & Cohen J. D. The physics of optimal decision making: A formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol. Rev. 113, 700–765 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wenzlaff H., Bauer M., Maess B. & Heekeren H. R. Neural characterization of the speed-accuracy tradeoff in a perceptual decision-making task. J. Neurosci. 31, 1254–1266 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luce R. D. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization. (Oxford University Press, 1986). [Google Scholar]
- Beaumont M. A. Approximate Bayesian Computation in Evolution and Ecology. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 41, 379–406 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Bishop C. M. Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning (Information Science and Statistics). (Springer-Verlag New York, Inc., 2006). [Google Scholar]
- Minka T. P. Expectation Propagation for approximate Bayesian inference. In Proceedings of the 17th Conference in Uncertainty in Artificial Intelligence. (eds Koller D. & Breese J. S.) 362–369 (Morgan Kaufmann Publishers Inc., 2001). [Google Scholar]
- Smith P. L. Psychophysically principled models of visual simple reaction-time. Psychol. Rev. 102, 567–593 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- Rao R. P. N. Decision making under uncertainty: a neural model based on partially observable Markov decision processes. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4, 10.3389/fncom.2010.00146 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.