Abstract
Emerging evidence indicates that attention deficits, which are frequently observed as core symptoms of neuropsychiatric disorders, may be elicited by early life stress. However, the mechanisms mediating these stress effects remain unknown. The prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been implicated in the regulation of attention, including dysfunctions in GABAergic transmission, and it is highly sensitive to stress. Here, we investigated the involvement of neuroligin-2 (NLGN-2), a synaptic cell adhesion molecule involved in the stabilization and maturation of GABAergic synapses, in the PFC in the link between stress and attention deficits. First, we established that exposure of rats to stress during the peripubertal period impairs attention in the five-choice serial reaction time task and results in reductions in the GABA-synthesizing enzyme glutamic acid decarboxylase in different PFC subregions (ie, prelimbic (PL), infralimbic, and medial and ventral orbitofrontal (OFC) cortex) and in NLGN-2 in the PL cortex. In peripubertally stressed animals, NLGN-2 expression in the PL and OFC cortex correlated with attention measurements. Subsequently, we found that adeno-associated virus-induced rescue of NLGN-2 in the PFC reverses the stress-induced attention deficits regarding omitted trials. Therefore, our findings highlight peripuberty as a period that is highly vulnerable to stress, leading to the development of attention deficits and a dysfunction in the PFC GABAergic system and NLGN-2 expression. Furthermore, NLGN-2 is underscored as a promising target to treat stress-induced cognitive alterations, and in particular attentional deficits as manifested by augmented omissions in a continuous performance task.
Introduction
Attention deficits lie at the core of cognitive dysfunctions manifested in different neuropsychiatric disorders (Barnett et al, 2010; Millan et al, 2012). Early life stress is a known risk factor for the development of psychopathologies (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001), and emerging clinical (Chugani et al, 2001; Stevens et al, 2008) and preclinical (Carlyle et al, 2012; Comeau et al, 2014; Fuentes et al, 2014) evidence indicates that it can trigger attentional problems. However, the potential mechanisms mediating attentional deficits induced by exposure to early stress remain largely unknown.
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is one of the key brain regions implicated in the regulation of attentional processes (Robbins, 2002; Rossi et al, 2009) and is highly susceptible to stress (Arnsten, 2009; Liston et al, 2006). Converging evidence highlights alterations in the function of GABAergic transmission in the PFC as a relevant mechanism underlying attentional deficits: (i) the inhibition of GABAA receptors in the PFC leads to attentional deficits (Enomoto et al, 2011; Paine et al, 2011; Pehrson et al, 2013; Pezze et al, 2014) and (ii) a reduced GABAergic tone, including a reduction in the GABA-synthesizing enzyme glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) (Akbarian et al, 1995; Volk et al, 2000), was found in the PFC in schizophrenia, a disorder with core attentional deficits.
Early life stress has been reported to induce long-lasting GABAergic changes in several brain regions (Jacobson-Pick et al, 2008; Martisova et al, 2012; Tzanoulinou et al, 2014a, 2014b). Importantly, stress applied at adulthood was found to alter the expression levels of several cell adhesion molecules (Sandi, 2004; Sandi and Bisaz, 2007), including neuroligin-2 (NLGN-2) (van der Kooij et al, 2014), which is located at inhibitory GABAergic synapses and plays a key role in their regulation (Chih et al, 2005; Huang and Scheiffele, 2008; Sudhof, 2008). NLGN-2 belongs to the family of NLGN postsynaptic cell adhesion molecules, which regulate different aspects of synaptic function (Levinson and El-Husseini, 2005; Sudhof, 2008). However, the possibility that NLGN-2 expression is persistently altered by early life stress has not been addressed.
Here, we investigated whether alterations in the expression of NLGN-2 and GAD in the PFC are associated with attentional deficits programmed by early life stress. We used a rat model of peripubertal (comprising childhood and puberty) stress-induced psychopathology (Marquez et al, 2013; Toledo-Rodriguez and Sandi, 2011), as the functional maturation of local prefrontal GABAergic circuits occurs during this developmental period (Caballero et al, 2014; Thomases et al, 2013). Attention was evaluated in the five-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT), which is the rodent equivalent of the continuous performance task in humans (Dalley et al, 2008; Robbins, 2002). Furthermore, we determined the expression levels of NLGN-2 and GAD in different PFC regions and investigated the causal involvement of NLGN-2 in stress-induced attention deficits using a rescue experiment utilizing viral-induced NLGN-2 overexpression (OE).
Materials and Methods
Animals
Subjects were the offspring of Wistar Han rats (Charles River Laboratories, France), bred in our animal facility. They were kept on a 12 h light–dark cycle (lights on at 0700 hours). Food and water were available ad libitum, except for the food-deprivation period during the 5-CSRTT, when animals were kept to 85–90% of their free-feeding weight. All procedures were conducted in conformity with the Swiss National Institutional Guidelines on Animal Experimentation and approved by a license from the Swiss Cantonal Veterinary Office Committee for Animal Experimentation.
Experimental Design
At weaning (P21), male rats from different litters were distributed into different home cages in groups of two to three non-siblings, and each cage was randomly assigned to control (CTRL) or peripubertal stress (PPS) conditions. At P28, the stress protocol began (Marquez et al, 2013). At adulthood (P90), animals were handled for 3 consecutive days and subsequently either allocated to behavioral experiments (14 CTRL and 16 PPS animals that were subjected to the 5-CSRTT) or maintained under basal conditions and perfused for immunohistochemical analyses (9 CTRL and 6 PPS). Two days after the last behavioral testing, all rats were killed, and their brains were rapidly extracted, frozen in cold isopentane and stored at −80 °C until further processing. For the NLGN-2 OE study, an additional cohort of adult CTRL and PPS animals was subjected to surgery and virus infusion at adulthood, yielding the following four groups: CTRL-empty (N=9), CTRL-NLGN-2 OE (N=12), PPS-empty (N=10), and PPS-NLGN-2 OE (N=10).
Peripubertal Stress Protocol
PPS was performed as previously described (Marquez et al, 2013; Veenit et al, 2014) and involved exposure to two fear-inducing stressors (the synthetic fox odor trimethylthiazoline (Phero Tech, Delta, BC, Canada) administered in a plastic box (38 × 27.5 × 31 cm) and exposure to an elevated platform (12 × 12 cm)) presented in a variable schedule in the span of 7 days across the P28–P42 period. Each stressor lasted for 25 min. For more information, see Supplementary Materials and Methods.
Five-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task (5-CSRTT)
The 5-CSRTT requires the rats to allocate their limited attentional resources on randomly presented stimuli (spatially divided attention). The experiment was simultaneously performed in six animals using six testing boxes (Coulbourn Instruments, Bilaney Consultants, Germany). Each box consists of two aluminum walls, one of which is curved and equipped with five circular holes, whereas the opposite wall is equipped with a food well connected to a pellet dispenser. All of the circular holes and the food magazine are equipped with infrared beams crossing their entrance to detect the animals' responses. A previously described protocol (Bari et al, 2008) was used with minor modifications. Briefly, following habituation to the apparatus, rats were required to learn to nose poke into five randomly illuminated holes within a limited time period to obtain one sucrose pellet (45 mg, Bio-Serv, Flemington, NJ, USA). The rats received 5 daily 30-min sessions per week, with a 100 pellet cut-off (whichever condition was met first). The training began with easy task contingencies in the early stages to facilitate learning and became progressively difficult throughout a total of 12 training stages. When the predetermined criteria for each stage were met, the animals were advanced to the next stage. When the animals were well-trained, ie, their behavior was stable for 6–10 sessions at the most advanced stage, the major 5-CSRTT parameters from the last 4 sessions were averaged, thus representing their baseline performance.
A response (ie, a nose poke) into one of the holes during the inter-trial interval (ITI) was considered a ‘premature response', whereas failure to respond was scored as an ‘omission'. ‘Accuracy' was calculated as the number of correct responses divided by the sum of correct and incorrect responses and is expressed as a percentage. ‘Continuous hits into the magazine' and ‘latencies' to respond correctly and to collect the reward were also estimated. The latency to collect the reward and the total number of trials were examined and served as indices of motivation to perform the task, as previously proposed (Amitai and Markou, 2010). ‘Total errors' were calculated as the sum of omission and commission errors (ie, incorrect pokes) and were divided by the total number of performed trials. In addition to these standard parameters, we calculated (per animal and per session in a trial-by-trial manner) the ‘standard deviation (SD) of the reaction time' (RTSD) for the 4 days that constituted the well-trained performance phase or the baseline. Furthermore, to study compulsivity behaviors in more detail, different variables were calculated. Specifically, an index of continuous hits into the magazine was defined as the immediate repetition of head entries, whereas a second index was defined as the number of additional pokes into the magazine following food delivery. Finally, the total number of trials and premature responses were subtracted from the total magazine hits within a session to obtain an additional generic index of unnecessary repeated responses in the food dispenser.
In addition, to assess the long-term effects of PPS on the acquisition of the 5-CSRTT, the initial period of training was also examined. Specifically, basic parameters of the 5-CSRTT, such as accuracy, omissions, and total errors, as well as the cumulative number of days to reach the next stage were investigated during the first 15 sessions. For statistical analyses, acquisition data were also analyzed in blocks of five consecutive sessions. At the most advanced stage of testing, the light stimulus in the holes lasted for 0.5 s with the ITI being 5 s. For the NLGN-2 OE experiment (see below), the light stimulus lasted 1 s and the ITI was 5 s. For additional details on the 5-CSRTT procedure, see the Supplementary Materials and Methods.
Laser Capture Microdissection
Laser Capture Microdissection (LCM) was applied, as previously described (Timmer et al, 2011), on coronal sections of 20 μm aided by a Zeiss PALM laser system. The sections were put on membrane covered slides (Palm Microlaser Technologies, Carl Zeiss, Germany) and stored at −20 °C covered with RNAlater ICE (Ambion, TX, USA). Prior to LCM, the sections were stained with the HistoGene LCM Frozen Section Staining kit (Arcturus Bioscience, Switzerland). The orbitofrontal (OFC), prelimbic (PL), and infralibic (IL) cortex from both hemispheres were dissected, and the target tissue was placed in RNAase-free tubes and subsequently incubated in 100 μl of lysis buffer (RNAqueous Micro Kit, Ambion) for 30 min at 42 °C. After spinning down, the tissue was stored at −80 °C until further processing.
Gene Expression Analysis
Total RNA from the regions dissected with LCM was isolated using the RNAqueous Micro kit and cDNA was synthesized using the Superscript VILO kit (Life Technologies). For quantitative RT–PCR, PCR reactions were performed in triplicate using SYBR Green PCR Master Mix (Applied Biosystems) in an ABI Prism 7900 Sequence Detection system (Applied Biosystems). Two genes were used as internal controls: gamma-actin (actg1) and eukaryotic elongation factor-1 (eef1). The primers for NLGN-2 (RefSeq: NM_053992.1) were designed using the Assay Design Center software from the Roche Applied Science (FW sequence: 5′-CCAAAGTGGGCTGTGACC-3′, RV sequence: 5′-CCAAAGGCAATGTGGTAGC-3′). Gene expression was analyzed with qBase 1.3.5 software using the comparative cycle threshold method.
GAD-6 Immunohistochemistry and Quantification of Neuropil Immunoreactivity
To analyze whether PPS induced changes on the neuropil expression of GAD 65/67 (GAD-6), immunohistochemistry was performed as previously published (Tzanoulinou et al, 2014a). We analyzed PFC regions, namely the IL, PL (coordinates: bregma: 3.72 mm, interaural: 12.72 mm), cingulate 1 (Cg1), and Cg2 (bregma: 1.20 mm, interaural: 10.20 mm) regions of the medial PFC (mPFC), as well as the medial orbital (MO), ventral orbital (VO), lateral orbital (LO), and the dorsolateral orbital (DLO) part of the OFC cortex (bregma: 5.16 mm, interaural: 14.16 mm). For further information, see Supplementary Materials and Methods, and Supplementary Figure S4.
Viral OE of NLGN-2
Naive CTRL and PPS rats were subjected to surgeries in the PFC after P90. After a 4-week period required for viral-induced NLGN2 OE, the animals were handled for 3 days before training in the 5-CSRTT started. The conditions for viral NLGN-2 OE were selected following pilot experiments and our previous work. Briefly, an adeno-associated AAV1/2 vector (http://www.genedetect.com) containing a CAG-HA-tagged-NLGN-2-WPRE-BGH-polyA-expression cassette was used (Figure 4a; GeneDetect, New Zealand). Half of the CTRL and PPS animals were injected with an empty construct (AAV1/2-CAG-Null/Empty WPRE-BGH-polyA). The coordinates to target the mPFC were +3.1 mm anterior, ±0.5 mm lateral, and −4.2 mm ventral, and to target the OFC +4.9 mm anterior, ±1 mm lateral, and −3.7 mm ventral to bregma, according to the atlas by Paxinos and Watson (1998). For further information, see Supplementary Materials and Methods.
Statistics
Data were analyzed with Student t-tests, ANOVAs, and repeated measures ANOVAs, unless stated otherwise, using the statistical package SPSS 17.0 (Chicago, IL). Data is represented as the mean±SEM and significance set at p<0.05. Graphs were created using GraphPad Prism 5. For further details, see Supplementary Materials and Methods.
Results
Peripubertally Stressed Animals Exhibit Attention Deficits in the 5-CSRTT
During training, PPS rats exhibited decreased accuracy in the 5-CSRTT compared with CTRL rats (Figure 1a) (main effect of stress: F(1,28)=4.470, p=0.044; stress × time interaction: F(5,134)=2.331, p=0.048), which was particularly manifested during the first block of five sessions (F(1, 28)=7.079, p=0.013). PPS animals also exhibited a trend toward increased omissions (Figure 1b) (main effect of stress: F(1,28)=3.827, p=0.060), which was significant for the first block of five sessions (F(1,28)=4.699, p=0.039). Moreover, PPS rats performed more total errors (F(1,28)=4.359, p=0.046) and required significantly more days to advance through the training stages (χ2(1)=4.326, p=0.038) than CTRL rats (see Supplementary Results and Supplementary Figure S1).
When performance was assessed under well-trained, ie, baseline conditions, PPS rats did not exhibit accuracy deficits (Figure 1c) (t28=1.087, p=0.286). However, PPS rats exhibited a higher number of omissions than CTRL rats (Figure 1d) (t22=−2.145, p=0.043). PPS rats also exhibited trends toward increased latency to respond correctly (Figure 1e) (t28=−1.789, p=0.084). No differences were observed between CTRL and PPS rats in the amount of trials completed (Figure 1f) (t28=0.933, p=0.359) or in their latency to collect the reward (Figure 1g) (t28=0.968, p=0.342), indicating the rats' motivation to perform the task. No significant difference was observed between PPS and CTRL rats in the number of premature responses (Figure 1h) (t28=−0.243, p=0.810). A trend toward increased continuous responses in the food magazine was observed for PPS animals (Figure 1i) (t28=−1.776, p=0.087) but not for continuous responses in the food magazine following food delivery (Figure 1j) (t28=−0.577, p=0.569). However, PPS animals exhibited increased head entries into the food magazine when the pokes that ended in the completion of a trial or in a premature response were subtracted from the total magazine nose pokes (Figure 1k) (t28=−2.302, p=0.029), possibly indicating an increase in compulsive responses.
In addition, we assessed the animals' intra-individual reaction time variability (IIV), which is a parameter that is frequently perturbed in neuropsychiatric disorders that involve attention deficits (Di Martino et al, 2008; Geurts et al, 2008; Kaiser et al, 2008; Klein et al, 2006). Remarkably, PPS animals exhibited significantly higher variability in their reaction times for correct responses (Figure 1l) (t20=−2.384, p=0.027). Figure 1m shows representative panels of reaction times for correct responses for CTRL and PPS rats, illustrating that the observed variability in PPS animals is due to an augmented number of trials with high reaction times (8.31% trials with RT above 1 s), whereas significantly lower percentages were observed in CTRLs (2.41% trials with RT above 1 s; p=0.012, Mann–Whitney test). No significant differences were observed in reaction time for incorrect trials (data not shown).
A principal component analysis of the aforementioned 5-CSRTT parameters indicated that PPS animals performed poorly compared with CTRL rats in a composite index of general performance (see Supplementary Materials and Methods, Supplementary Results, and Supplementary Figure S2). Finally, PPS animals exhibited similar number of premature responses as CTRLs on prolongation of the ITI, which is a methodological manipulation used to probe the animal's impulsive behavior (McNamara et al, 2010) (see Supplementary Materials and Methods, Supplementary Results, and Supplementary Figure S3).
PPS Leads to Reduced GAD Immunoreactivity in the PFC
GAD-6 immunoreactivity was assessed in several PFC regions (Figure 2). PPS animals exhibited reduced GAD-6 expression across layers throughout the PL (Figure 2j) (F(1,12)=6.617, p=0.024) and IL (Figure 2k) (F(1,11)=5.512, p=0.039) cortices. No differences were observed between the groups throughout the Cg1 (Figure 2l) (F(1,13)=0.061, p=0.809) or Cg2 (Figure 2m) (F(1,13)=0.552, p=0.471). PPS animals had decreased levels of GAD-6 in the MO (Figure 2n) (F(1,13)=7.131, p=0.019) and in VO (Figure 2o) (F(1,13)=18.509, p=0.001). Moreover, PPS animals exhibited a trend toward a reduction in GAD-6 in the LO (Figure 2p) (F(1,13)=3.746, p=0.075) and in the DLO (Figure 2q) (F(1,13)=3.906, p=0.070). Collectively, these results indicate that PPS induces a long-term reduction in GAD in specific PFC subregions.
PPS Leads to Reduced NLGN-2 mRNA Expression in the PL Cortex
Compared with CTRL rats, PPS animals exhibited reduced NLGN-2 expression in the PL cortex (Figure 3a) (t27=2.200, p=0.037) without significant differences in the IL cortex (Figure 3b) (t25=0.630, p=0.534) or in the OFC (Figure 3c) (t24=0.499, p=0.623). Interestingly, we found a significant positive correlation between PL NLGN-2 expression and accuracy in the 5-CSRTT (Figure 3d) for analyses that included data from all animals (r27=0.434, p=0.019) or only PPS rats (r13=0.704, p=0.003) but not for analyses that only included data for CTRL rats. No correlations between accuracy and NLGN2 in either the IL (Figure 3e) or OFC (Figure 3f) were found. No correlations between omissions and PL NLGN-2 (Figure 3g) or IL NLGN-2 (Figure 3h) were found. Interestingly, omissions in the 5-CSRTT correlated negatively with NLGN-2 expression in the OFC (Figure 3i), which was apparent when considering data from all animals (r24=−0.416, p=0.034) or from the PPS group (r12=−0.569, p=0.034) but not from CTRL rats.
NLGN-2 OE in the PFC Alleviates Attention Deficits Induced by PPS
To directly study the contribution of PFC NLGN-2 expression to the attentional deficits induced by early life stress, we overexpressed NLGN-2 in the PFC at adulthood. AAV-induced NLGN-2 expression colocalizes with gephyrin, which is a post-synaptic molecule expressed at GABAergic neurons (van der Kooij et al, 2014). AAV-induced NLGN-2 OE was targeted to the mPFC and OFC subregions (Figure 4a–d). The localization and spread of the virus were confirmed using DAB immunostaining in PFC coronal sections following behavioral testing (Figure 4c–g). As expected, NLGN-2 OE was not observed in animals infused with the empty viral construct (Figure 4e and g). Expression levels measured with quantitative RT–PCR indicated a twofold expression of NLGN-2 in the targeted brain regions (Supplementary Figure S5).
Following a recovery period to allow for the spread of AAV-induced NLGN-2 OE within the targeted PFC areas, adult CTRL and PPS rats infused with either empty or NLGN-2 OE virus were subjected to the 5-CSRTT. As observed in the previous experiment, no differences in the accuracy were observed between the groups (Figure 4h). Importantly, a significant stress × virus interaction was observed for omissions (Figure 4i) (stress × virus interaction: F(1, 35)=12.333, p=0.001), with post hoc analyses revealing that PPS-empty animals perform significantly more omissions than CTRL-empty rats (p=0.008), as expected from the previous experiment, whereas PPS-NLGN-2 OE animals exhibit a reduction in omissions compared with PPS-empty animals (p=0.012). Strikingly, CTRL-NLGN-2 OE animals also performed more omissions than CTRL-empty rats (p=0.028). No significant differences were observed between the groups for total errors (Figure 4j), the number of trials completed (Figure 4k), or the latency to collect the reward (Figure 4l), indicating motivation to perform the task. These results support the view that NLGN-2 expression in the PFC plays a causal role in the attentional deficits observed, namely increased omissions, following PPS.
Discussion
In the present study, we identify the developmental period of peripuberty, comprising childhood and puberty, as a time window of vulnerability to stress, leading to the development of attentional deficits, and reveal novel mechanisms in the PFC in translating these stress effects. Specifically, we highlight NLGN-2 as a critical mediator of PPS detrimental effects on attention, as manifested in increased omissions.
We confirm a link between early life stress and persistent attentional deficits (Carlyle et al, 2012; Fuentes et al, 2014) and highlight the peripubertal period as a particularly vulnerable phase (Comeau et al, 2014). Importantly, deficits in PPS animals were observed both during task acquisition and following substantial training. Together with a delay in acquisition, in the early phases of training, PPS animals exhibited decreased accuracy, an increased number of omissions and a higher number of total errors, which are all indicative of attention deficits. Moreover, well-trained PPS animals continued to exhibit attention deficits as indicated by their higher number of omissions despite lack of changes in accuracy or motivation to perform the task (Amitai and Markou, 2010; Robbins, 2002). Furthermore, PPS animals exhibited an additional striking feature of cognitive dysfunction, ie, high intra-individual variability, which is frequently observed in patients of a wide variety of neuropsychiatric disorders (eg, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and autism) (Di Martino et al, 2008; Geurts et al, 2008; Kaiser et al, 2008; Klein et al, 2006). In addition, although indices of impulsivity were not affected in PPS animals, these animals exhibited certain signs of compulsivity. Collectively, the increased omissions, higher variability in the reaction times, increased aspects of compulsive responses, and difficulties in acquisition exhibited by PPS animals indicate a general cognitive deficit, a notion that was also supported by PCA analysis including all relevant 5-CSRTT parameters.
Consistent with the proposed role of inhibitory transmission in the PFC in attentional processes, we found that attentional deficits following PPS are accompanied by a reduction in GAD expression across different layers in the mPFC and in the medial and ventral OFC. These results are consistent with the notion that although contrasting effects are observed in the mPFC and OFC when stress is experienced at adulthood, alterations in these regions generally proceed in the same direction when stress occurs during early development (Liston et al, 2006). The functional relevance of these findings, in light of the attention deficits exhibited by PPS rats, is supported by pharmacological studies indicating that the inhibition of GABA synthesis (Asinof and Paine, 2013) or antagonism of GABAA receptors (Paine et al, 2011) in the mPFC results in increased omissions in the 5-CSRTT, whereas the inhibition of GAD systemically or within the anterior Cg (which did not exhibit alterations in GAD expression in PPS rats) does not affect the number of omissions (Pehrson et al, 2013).
Importantly, we also report that in PPS animals, NLGN-2 expression was downregulated in the PL cortex and positively correlated with accuracy, whereas its expression in the OFC was negatively correlated with omissions. Note that these correlations were found during baseline performance under well-trained conditions, when PPS animals show higher omissions while their accuracy does not differ from controls. Rescuing NLGN-2 expression through AAV genetic targeting in these regions reversed the increased omissions in well-trained PPS animals. Although the lack of effect of NLGN-2 OE on accuracy might seem counterintuitive given the correlation reported for PPS animals between this parameter and NLGN-2 expression in the PL cortex, it is important to note that this correlation was absent in controls in which values of this adhesion molecule were higher. This suggests that a correlational link between NLGN-2 expression in the PL cortex and accuracy might only be revealed under a deficient expression, not OE, of this adhesion molecule. Furthermore, the fact that whereas NLGN-2 expression in the OFC negatively correlated with omissions despite not being significantly different from controls points out at a combined effect of the NLGN-2 OE treatment throughout the targeted brain regions. However, the distributed nature of the NLGN-2 OE in this study precludes from exploring its potential effects when specifically targeting each of the PFC regions separately. It should be noted on the other hand, that NLGN-2 OE in controls led to increased omissions. This observation could suggest that optimal expression values of this adhesion molecule in the PFC can be important for accurate performance. This is in line with the view that altered balance in the expression of molecules necessary for synapse specification, such as NLGNs, can lead to deficits in circuit function and behavioral control (Dahlhaus et al, 2010). However, a note of caution should be drawn before future studies confirm this conclusion, as the quantitative validation of NLGN-2 expression in our study was performed in controls, not in peripubertally stressed animals.
Overall, these results are consistent with recent evidence suggesting that NLGN-2 may represent a relevant target of stress. Specifically, a reduction in NLGN-2 expression, concurrent with alterations in social behaviors, was reported in the hippocampus following exposure to chronic restraint stress at adulthood (van der Kooij et al, 2014). In contrast, genetic OE of NLGN-2 was shown to increase GAD levels (Kohl et al, 2013) and to reduce the excitation and inhibition ratio in the frontal cortex (Hines et al, 2008). Conversely, NLGN-2 genetic deletion reduced inhibitory synaptic function (Blundell et al, 2009).
To date, the majority of studies revealing a role for NLGNs in brain function and cognition have been of genetic origin. On the one hand, mutations in NLGN-encoding genes have been associated with cognitive diseases such as autism and schizophrenia. On the other hand, mice genetically modified for NLGN genes have confirmed the impact of these molecules in specific aspects of synaptic function and social behaviors (Hines et al, 2008; Kohl et al, 2013; Wohr et al, 2013; Kohl et al, 2015). Our findings strongly support a role for NLGN in cognition. Moreover, the apparently paradoxical observation of impaired attention in control animals overexpressing NLGN-2 suggests that the disruption of optimal NLGN-2 levels may result in negative consequences for cognitive performance, which is consistent with pharmacological studies demonstrating attention deficits following either activation or inhibition of PFC GABAergic function from normative levels (Pezze et al, 2014).
Given the link between NLGN gene mutations and certain developmental disorders (Sudhof, 2008), our finding that early stress can affect neurodevelopmental trajectories involving lasting alterations in NLGN-2 expression suggests that stress may affect specific cognitive functions by acting on mechanisms that are similar to those targeted by genetic disorders. Moreover, as patients with neurodevelopmental disorders (eg, autism, schizophrenia) generally exhibit enhanced emotional vulnerability (Mazefsky et al, 2013), stress may act as an active contributing factor to the development of the disorder. Accordingly, our results highlight NLGN-2 as a relevant target for future preclinical studies that combine animal models of neurodevelopmental disorders (eg, autism, schizophrenia) with early life stress. Our results also support the interest in developing research programs to specifically examine cognitive outcomes in NLGN-2 mutant mice subjected to early life stress.
An additional interesting implication derived from the observation that OE of NLGN-2 at adulthood alleviated the primary cognitive deficit elicited by PPS suggests that certain deficits induced by early stress are reversible even after the critical developmental window. Although the current success of therapies involving gene delivery into the brain have encountered technical and safety challenges, considerable ongoing progress in the field offers optimistic prospects for the use of CNS gene therapy in the near future (Aubourg, 2013). Alternatively, NLGN-2 function may be more easily targeted using pharmacological approaches based on recently developed peptide strategies (Berezin and Bock, 2010). Recently, we demonstrated that treatment with a synthetic peptide that comprises NLGN-2 sequences that interact with its presynaptic partner neurexin-1 efficiently and specifically modulated social behaviors (van der Kooij et al, 2014).
Therefore, the present study delineates GABAergic dysfunction in the PFC as a long-term consequence of early life stress and critically identifies NLGN-2 as a novel promising target to reverse stress-induced attention deficits, particularly omissions.
Funding and Disclosure
This work was supported by grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation (31003A-152614 and the NCCR Synapsy), the EU FP7 project MATRICS (no 603016), Oak Foundation, and intramural funding from the EPFL to CS and Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness BFU2012-32512, Generalitat Valenciana ACOMP/2012/229, and Prometeo Excellence Program PROMETEO2013/069 and the Fundación Alicia Koplowitz to JN.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Isabelle Guillot de Suduiraut, Vandana Veenit, Guillaume Poirier, Yasmina Curto, and Aurélie Papilloud for their valuable technical assistance. We would also like to thank Professor Peter Scheiffele for providing the mouse pCAG-HA-tagged-NLGN-2 plasmid.
Footnotes
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Neuropsychopharmacology website (http://www.nature.com/npp)
DISCLAIMER
This paper reflects only the authors' views and the European Union is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein.
Supplementary Material
References
- Akbarian S, Kim JJ, Potkin SG, Hagman JO, Tafazzoli A, Bunney WE Jr et al (1995). Gene expression for glutamic acid decarboxylase is reduced without loss of neurons in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52: 258–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amitai N, Markou A (2010). Disruption of performance in the five-choice serial reaction time task induced by administration of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists: relevance to cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 68: 5–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AF (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 410–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asinof SK, Paine TA (2013). Inhibition of GABA synthesis in the prefrontal cortex increases locomotor activity but does not affect attention in the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Neuropharmacology 65: 39–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aubourg P (2013). Gene therapy for disorders of the central nervous system. Handb Clin Neurol 113: 1859–1866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bari A, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2008). The application of the 5-choice serial reaction time task for the assessment of visual attentional processes and impulse control in rats. Nat Protoc 3: 759–767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett JH, Robbins TW, Leeson VC, Sahakian BJ, Joyce EM, Blackwell AD (2010). Assessing cognitive function in clinical trials of schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34: 1161–1177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berezin V, Bock E (2010). NCAM mimetic peptides: an update. Adv Exp Med Biol 663: 337–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blundell J, Tabuchi K, Bolliger MF, Blaiss CA, Brose N, Liu X et al (2009). Increased anxiety-like behavior in mice lacking the inhibitory synapse cell adhesion molecule neuroligin 2. Gen Brain Behav 8: 114–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caballero A, Flores-Barrera E, Cass DK, Tseng KY (2014). Differential regulation of parvalbumin and calretinin interneurons in the prefrontal cortex during adolescence. Brain Struct Funct 219: 395–406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlyle BC, Duque A, Kitchen RR, Bordner KA, Coman D, Doolittle E et al (2012). Maternal separation with early weaning: a rodent model providing novel insights into neglect associated developmental deficits. Dev Psychopathol 24: 1401–1416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chih B, Engelman H, Scheiffele P (2005). Control of excitatory and inhibitory synapse formation by neuroligins. Science 307: 1324–1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chugani HT, Behen ME, Muzik O, Juhasz C, Nagy F, Chugani DC (2001). Local brain functional activity following early deprivation: a study of postinstitutionalized Romanian orphans. Neuroimage 14: 1290–1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comeau WL, Winstanley CA, Weinberg J (2014). Prenatal alcohol exposure and adolescent stress—unmasking persistent attentional deficits in rats. Eur J Neurosci 40: 3078–3095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahlhaus R, Hines RM, Eadie BD, Kannangara TS, Hines DJ, Brown CE et al (2010). Overexpression of the cell adhesion protein neuroligin-1 induces learning deficits and impairs synaptic plasticity by altering the ratio of excitation to inhibition in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 20: 305–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley JW, Mar AC, Economidou D, Robbins TW (2008). Neurobehavioral mechanisms of impulsivity: fronto-striatal systems and functional neurochemistry. Pharm Biochem Behav 90: 250–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Martino A, Ghaffari M, Curchack J, Reiss P, Hyde C, Vannucci M et al (2008). Decomposing intra-subject variability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 64: 607–614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enomoto T, Tse MT, Floresco SB (2011). Reducing prefrontal gamma-aminobutyric acid activity induces cognitive, behavioral, and dopaminergic abnormalities that resemble schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 69: 432–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuentes S, Daviu N, Gagliano H, Garrido P, Zelena D, Monasterio N et al (2014). Sex-dependent effects of an early life treatment in rats that increases maternal care: vulnerability or resilience? Front Behav Neurosci 8: 56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geurts HM, Grasman RP, Verte S, Oosterlaan J, Roeyers H, van Kammen SM et al (2008). Intra-individual variability in ADHD, autism spectrum disorders and Tourette's syndrome. Neuropsychologia 46: 3030–3041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim C, Nemeroff CB (2001). The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol Psychiatry 49: 1023–1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hines RM, Wu L, Hines DJ, Steenland H, Mansour S, Dahlhaus R et al (2008). Synaptic imbalance, stereotypies, and impaired social interactions in mice with altered neuroligin 2 expression. J Neurosci 28: 6055–6067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang ZJ, Scheiffele P (2008). GABA and neuroligin signaling: linking synaptic activity and adhesion in inhibitory synapse development. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18: 77–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson-Pick S, Elkobi A, Vander S, Rosenblum K, Richter-Levin G (2008). Juvenile stress-induced alteration of maturation of the GABAA receptor alpha subunit in the rat. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 11: 891–903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser S, Roth A, Rentrop M, Friederich HC, Bender S, Weisbrod M (2008). Intra-individual reaction time variability in schizophrenia, depression and borderline personality disorder. Brain Cogn 66: 73–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein C, Wendling K, Huettner P, Ruder H, Peper M (2006). Intra-subject variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 60: 1088–1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohl C, Riccio O, Grosse J, Zanoletti O, Fournier C, Schmidt MV et al (2013). Hippocampal neuroligin-2 overexpression leads to reduced aggression and inhibited novelty reactivity in rats. PloS One 8: e56871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohl C, Wang XD, Grosse J, Fournier C, Harbich D, Westerholz S et al (2015). Hippocampal neuroligin-2 links early-life stress with impaired social recognition and increased aggression in adult mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 55: 128–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levinson JN, El-Husseini A (2005). Building excitatory and inhibitory synapses: balancing neuroligin partnerships. Neuron 48: 171–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liston C, Miller MM, Goldwater DS, Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Hof PR et al (2006). Stress-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology predict selective impairments in perceptual attentional set-shifting. J Neurosci 26: 7870–7874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquez C, Poirier GL, Cordero MI, Larsen MH, Groner A, Marquis J et al (2013). Peripuberty stress leads to abnormal aggression, altered amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity and increased prefrontal MAOA gene expression. Transl Psychiatry 3: e216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martisova E, Solas M, Horrillo I, Ortega JE, Meana JJ, Tordera RM et al (2012). Long lasting effects of early-life stress on glutamatergic/GABAergic circuitry in the rat hippocampus. Neuropharmacology 62: 1944–1953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazefsky CA, Herrington J, Siegel M, Scarpa A, Maddox BB, Scahill L et al (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 52: 679–688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNamara R, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ, Belin D (2010). Trait-like impulsivity does not predict escalation of heroin self-administration in the rat. Psychopharmacology 212: 453–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brune M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS et al (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov 11: 141–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paine TA, Slipp LE, Carlezon WA Jr (2011). Schizophrenia-like attentional deficits following blockade of prefrontal cortex GABAA receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 1703–1713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C (1998) The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press: Salt Lake City, UT. [Google Scholar]
- Pehrson AL, Bondi CO, Totah NK, Moghaddam B (2013). The influence of NMDA and GABA(A) receptors and glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) activity on attention. Psychopharmacology 225: 31–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezze M, McGarrity S, Mason R, Fone KC, Bast T (2014). Too little and too much: hypoactivation and disinhibition of medial prefrontal cortex cause attentional deficits. J Neurosci 34: 7931–7946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW (2002). The 5-choice serial reaction time task: behavioural pharmacology and functional neurochemistry. Psychopharmacology 163: 362–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi AF, Pessoa L, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (2009). The prefrontal cortex and the executive control of attention. Exp Brain Res 192: 489–497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandi C (2004). Stress, cognitive impairment and cell adhesion molecules. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 917–930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandi C, Bisaz R (2007). A model for the involvement of neural cell adhesion molecules in stress-related mood disorders. Neuroendocrinology 85: 158–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens SE, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Kreppner JM, Beckett C, Castle J, Colvert E et al (2008). Inattention/overactivity following early severe institutional deprivation: presentation and associations in early adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36: 385–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sudhof TC (2008). Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature 455: 903–911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomases DR, Cass DK, Tseng KY (2013). Periadolescent exposure to the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 impairs the functional maturation of local GABAergic circuits in the adult prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 33: 26–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timmer M, Cordero MI, Sevelinges Y, Sandi C (2011). Evidence for a role of oxytocin receptors in the long-term establishment of dominance hierarchies. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 2349–2356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toledo-Rodriguez M, Sandi C (2011). Stress during adolescence increases novelty seeking and risk-taking behavior in male and female rats. Front Behav Neurosci 5: 17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzanoulinou S, Garcia-Mompo C, Castillo-Gomez E, Veenit V, Nacher J, Sandi C (2014. a). Long-term behavioral programming induced by peripuberty stress in rats is accompanied by GABAergic-related alterations in the Amygdala. PloS One 9: e94666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzanoulinou S, Riccio O, de Boer MW, Sandi C (2014. b). Peripubertal stress-induced behavioral changes are associated with altered expression of genes involved in excitation and inhibition in the amygdala. Transl Psychiatry 4: e410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Kooij MA, Fantin M, Kraev I, Korshunova I, Grosse J, Zanoletti O et al (2014). Impaired hippocampal neuroligin-2 function by chronic stress or synthetic peptide treatment is linked to social deficits and increased aggression. Neuropsychopharmacology 39: 1148–1158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenit V, Riccio O, Sandi C (2014). CRHR1 links peripuberty stress with deficits in social and stress-coping behaviors. J Psychiatry Res 53: 1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volk DW, Austin MC, Pierri JN, Sampson AR, Lewis DA (2000). Decreased glutamic acid decarboxylase67 messenger RNA expression in a subset of prefrontal cortical gamma-aminobutyric acid neurons in subjects with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 237–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wohr M, Silverman JL, Scattoni ML, Turner SM, Harris MJ, Saxena R et al (2013). Developmental delays and reduced pup ultrasonic vocalizations but normal sociability in mice lacking the postsynaptic cell adhesion protein neuroligin2. Behav Brain Res 251: 50–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.