Why this topic?
Growing evidence from task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies consistently indicates network abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients (1,2). While a picture is emerging on how these changes affect cognition and behavior at various stages of the disease, there is less understanding of the changes in functional connectivity between spatially distant brain areas in the absence of a specific task, ie, in a “resting state.” Resting state fMRI focuses on spontaneous fluctuations in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal in distant yet temporally synchronous brain areas, which presumably reflect neural connectivity (3-5). It has been hypothesized that low-frequency (0.1-0.01 Hz) fluctuations in the BOLD signal may be attributed not only to spontaneous neuronal activity detectable in the absence of cognitive task, but also to specific functional domains. In other words, these temporally correlated fluctuations can indentify brain networks (6). The existing evidence suggests alterations in several resting-state networks in AD, such as the default mode network (7-9), fronto-parietal network, executive network, salience network (10), and language network (11).
Resting state functional connectivity (RS FC) changes in the AD brain involve both decreased and increased connectivity within specific networks. Interpretation of such alterations is far from trivial, and it remains largely unknown how they relate to continuing cognitive decline as the disease advances. Decreased RS FC presumably indicates lesion-driven disconnection processes, whereas increased connectivity indicates compensatory and adaptive connectivity mechanisms (11,12). While it remains unclear how such mechanisms allow relative preservation of cognitive function in the context of disease-related changes, the disease progression will eventually turn these mechanisms ineffective (9). In addition to changes in RS FC (13), early stages of AD are associated with pronounced hypometabolism in temporo-parietal regions in asymptomatic, ie, preclinical stage, as well as with atrophy originating in medial and lateral temporal areas and to some extent overlapping with hypometabolism in prodromal AD (14). Thus, as the disease develops, it affects more brain areas, and consequently cognitive changes become manifest in domains other than memory, including language. Anatomical changes in the AD brain and its structural connectivity have been reported in numerous post-mortem studies as well as in in vivo diffusion tensor imaging studies, while functional connectivity changes have been observed using other methods, such as rs- and task-based fMRI and positron emission tomography (PET).
Rs-fMRI is an excellent tool for assessing functional connectivity of distant brain areas in AD, because it does not require task performance in the scanner, which may be overwhelming to patients. In addition, the length of scanning in collecting rs-fMRI data are relatively short compared to task-based scanning sessions. The standard scanning duration in collecting rs-fMRI data are 5-7 min, although recent evidence suggests that scanning sessions that take 9-13 min result in improved reliability (15). Despite these obvious advantages, one reason for not utilizing this method to its full potential in clinical settings so far is related to some uncertainty regarding the relevance of intrinsic brain activity to behavior. However, the brain’s basal activity appears to be more relevant to the brain’s task-evoked activity than generally assumed (16). Recent studies suggest that FC of the RS brain networks reflects the connectivity of task-activated networks (17) and that its changes in disease may indicate residual abilities of the affected networks (18). Crucially, changes in RS FC of certain brain areas in AD significantly correlate with the patients’ poor performance on certain cognitive tests (19). For instance, abnormal RS FC between the left thalamus and left frontal areas (inferior frontal and middle frontal gyri) has been associated with AD patients’ immediate/delayed scores on Auditory Verbal Learning Test (19). In patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), who convert to AD at a rate of 10%-15% per year (6), RS FC values between the thalamus and temporo-parietal regions correlate with the patients’ scores on Mini Mental State Examination (13). Taken together, these studies indicate the relevance of RS thalamic connectivity with frontal and temporo-parietal brain areas to a range of cognitive functions that deteriorate across the stages of AD, such as memory and executive functions (speed of processing, working memory, attention). Furthermore, cognitively normal persons with increased levels of cortical β-amyloid also show abnormalities in RS FC in AD susceptible areas (6), including the thalamus (20), despite the absence of any cognitive symptoms.
It appears then that intrinsic brain activity assessed by rs-fMRI contains important information on the changes in AD brain that can be traced from preclinical and prodromal stages to dementia. Since aberrant patterns of RS FC in prodromal and dementia stages of AD have been associated with specific cognitive symptoms, as discussed above, these associations can be used in clinical settings to support diagnosis, monitoring of disease progression, and assessment of efficiency of disease-modifying treatments. The rest of the article discusses the relevance of thalamic RS FC with frontal areas for general verbal fluency in AD.
Verbal fluency deficit in Alzheimer’s disease
AD patients regularly perform considerably worse on verbal fluency tests than cognitively healthy elderly subjects (21). In these tests, the task is to produce as many words as possible in 1 minute, provided that the produced words belong to a specific category (category test) or begin with a specific letter (letter test). Impaired performance on verbal fluency tests in AD has been associated with rapid disease progression, probably reflecting advanced deterioration of the frontal networks (22).
Verbal fluency tests are often considered the tests of executive function, but as simple lexical tasks – cued word generation – they also implicate language. Even though some evidence suggests that category fluency better reflects verbal abilities whereas letter fluency better reflects executive function, not all findings support such a distinction (23). It has been long recognized that it may not be possible to disentangle the language system from the executive system (24), in particular in tasks that require a rapid turn of cognitive operations, as is the case in cued word retrieval. Tests of verbal fluency trigger a set of processes such as defining a search strategy, performing search either by a semantic category cue or letter cue, retrieving words from the semantic or phonological memory, assessing their semantic/ phonological representations, inhibiting words that do not match the search criteria, articulating the retrieved word, keeping track of the retrieved words while continuing the search, focusing on the task while keeping track of time given the time limit, and so on (25). Thus, although verbal fluency tests require both executive functioning and verbal abilities, they are not “pure” tests of either. Nevertheless, these tests remain a critical tool for diagnosis and monitoring of changes in cognitive status in a range of conditions.
Attempts to determine the neural basis of verbal fluency indicate a special role of Broca’s area in these tasks, while also implicating a number of other areas. An early task-based fMRI study of covert cued word generation in healthy subjects that used both letter and category fluency tests (26) reported involvement of the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG), superior temporal gyrus, posterior superior temporal-inferior parietal areas, insula, retrosplenial area, as well as pulvinar and dorsomedial thalamic regions. Interestingly, this study also reported a functional segregation of Broca’s area for fluency tests, with Brodmann area (BA) 45 supporting category fluency and BA 44/6 supporting letter fluency. The role of BA 45 in category fluency and BA 44 in letter fluency has been confirmed in other task-based fMRI studies among healthy participants (27) and aphasic patients (28). These findings have been interpreted as indicating that BA 45 may contribute to the word retrieval through their meaning, and BA 44/6 may contribute to word retrieval via an articulatory code (26).
Broca’s area-thalamus: functional connectivity at rest
A seminal study on RS FC of language networks that involved 970 healthy participants scanned at 22 institutions across the world suggests that the brain at rest reveals an extended language network, implicating the classical language areas (Broca’s, Wernicke’s) as well as a range of prefrontal, temporal, and parietal regions along with subcortical structures (29). Other studies confirmed the reliability and stability of the language network at rest (30). Thus, recent findings on low frequency fluctuations in FC underlying language networks (12,17,29,31) on the one hand, and findings from task-based fMRI studies indicating involvement of Broca’s region in verbal fluency on the other support an association between AD patients’ verbal fluency decline and RS FC abnormalities involving this region. An intriguing possibility would be that functional segregation of Broca’s area into a region responsible for category and a region responsible for letter fluency (26) would be reflected in the connectivity patterns of the AD brain at rest.
In addition, the thalamus appears to be implicated in a range of language-related tasks, eg, word generation, speech production, picture naming, reading, and syntactic and semantic processing (28,32-35). The thalamus is presumed to play a neuroregulatory role in language, by recruiting controlled processes when relying on automatic processes is not sufficient (32), or the role of a “central monitor” for cortical language-related activations (33). Given the thalamic role in control processes associated with lexical retrieval and thalamic changes in the AD brain, changes in functional interconnectivity between Broca’s area and thalamic regions at rest are likely to be related to verbal fluency decline in AD.
Early evidence for the role of the left pulvinar and some sections of the left ventrolateral nucleus in word retrieval comes from electrostimulation studies with patients undergoing awake brain surgery for the treatment of dyskynesias (36,37). An early model of the thalamic role in language postulated connections between the pulvinar and temporoparietal cortical areas, and between the left dorsomedial nucleus and frontal cortical language areas (38). A recent diffusion tensor imaging study involving 10 healthy young subjects demonstrated direct structural connectivity between the pulvinar and ventral anterior nucleus with Broca’s area (39). While evidence indicates that these thalamic regions are implicated in semantics (40), or in integration of semantic and phonological information (39), the dorsomedial nucleus may be contributing to phonological word retrieval. This nucleus has rich interconnectivity with prefrontal areas (41) and it plays a critical role in executive functions (42) and language (43,44). Shrinkage of the left dorsomedial thalamic nucleus correlates with the decline of executive function and general cognitive decline in AD patients (45). The role of this nucleus in working memory has been confirmed in studies using different research methods (42). Furthermore, damage to the left dorsomedial thalamic nucleus has been associated with word finding difficulty and verbal working memory deficits (phonological loop) in thalamic aphasia (46). Finally, RS FC of the dorsomedial thalamic nucleus with prefrontal areas has been established (47), and decreased FC at rest between the thalamus and left middle (although not inferior) frontal gyrus was found in MCI (13). Thus, it is likely that this nucleus, in addition to the pulvinar, contributes to the dysfunction of networks associated with verbal fluency decline in AD. Moreover, if letter fluency better reflects executive functions, as sometimes claimed (48), then AD patients’ letter fluency scores may correlate with FC values between this nucleus and BA 44. Future studies will test these hypotheses and investigate how RS functional connectivity associated with word deficits in patients on the AD spectrum (49) differs from the RS FC associated with word deficits in primary progressive aphasia with thalamic atrophy and in post-stroke thalamic aphasia due to focal lesions (eg, tuberothalamic infarction) (50).
Since considerable atrophic changes in frontal areas do not appear in early stages of AD (14), whereas regional thalamic atrophy is detectable in prodromal stage (13), abnormalities in RS FC between Broca’s area and thalamic regions are likely to appear due to thalamic dysfunction. Decreased RS FC of the thalamus was found in cognitively normal persons with increased cortical β-amyloid load (20), ie, in preclinical AD, where cognitive symptoms such as verbal fluency decline are not present. Thus, abnormal RS FC between Broca’s area and thalamic regions that correlates with verbal fluency decline in AD patients is potentially a useful indicator of transition toward advanced stages of the disease (prodromal to mild-to-moderate). In addition, RS FC between these areas and their association with verbal fluency may be useful in the evaluation of treatments that intend to slow down the disease progression.
In conclusion, it is important to better integrate cognitive neuropsychological and rs-fMRI data in clinical settings. This may provide a better insight into transition toward advanced stages of AD and help in the evaluation of treatments intended to slow down progression of the disease. Such integration of data are expected, given the increasing role of neuroimaging assessment in diagnosis and monitoring of AD, and the relative ease of incorporating rs-fMRI scanning into general scanning protocols.
Acknowledgments
I thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions, and Tania Midbo for careful proofreading of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Shannon BJ, LaRossa G, Sachs R, Fotenos AF, et al. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7709–17. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2177-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sperling RA, Dickerson BC, Pihlajamaki M, Vannini P, LaViolette PS, Vitolo OV, et al. Functional alterations in memory networks in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuromolecular Med. 2010;12:27–43. doi: 10.1007/s12017-009-8109-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34:537–41. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V, Dougherty RF. Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:72–8. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Damoiseaux JS, Grecius MD. Greater than the sum of its parts: a review of studies combining structural connectivity and resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct. 2009;213:525–33. doi: 10.1007/s00429-009-0208-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sheline YI, Raichle ME. Resting state functional connectivity in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:340–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:4637–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Seeley WW, Crawford RK, Zhou J, Miller BL, Greicius MD. Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron. 2009;62:42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Damoiseaux JS, Prater KE, Miller BL, Grecius MD. Functional connectivity tracks deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:828e19–e30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Agosta F, Pievani M, Geroldi C, Copetti M, Frisoni GB, Filippi M. Resting state fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease: beyond the default mode network. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:1564–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Dubovik S, Bouzerda-Wahlen A, Nahum L, Gold G, Schnider A, Giggisberg AG. Adaptive reorganization of cortical networks in Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wilke M, Hauser T-K, Kraegeloh-Mann I, Lidzba K. Specific impairment of functional connectivity between language regions in former early preterms. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:3372–84. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cai S, Huang L, Zou J, Jing L, Zhai B, Ji G, et al. Changes in thalamic connectivity in the early and late stages of amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: A resting-state functional Magnetic Resonance study from ADNI. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0115573. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:119–28. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Birn RM, Molloy EK, Patriat R, Parker T, Meier TB, Kirk GR, et al. The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. Neuroimage. 2013;83:550–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Raichle ME. A paradigm shift in functional brain imaging. J Neurosci. 2009;29:12729–34. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4366-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lohmann G, Hoehl S, Brauer J, Danielmeier C, Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I, Bahlmann J, et al. Setting the frame: the human brain activates a basic low-frequency network for language processing. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:1286–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zamboni G, Wilcock GK, Douaud G, Drazich E, McCulloch E, Filippini N, et al. Resting functional connectivity reveals residual functional activity in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:375–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zhou B, Liu Y, Zhang Z, An N, Yao H, Wang P, et al. Impaired functional connectivity of the thalamus in Alzheimer’s disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment: a resting state fMRI study. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2013;10:754–66. doi: 10.2174/15672050113109990146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Steininger SC, Liu X, Gieti A, Wyss M, Schreiner S, Gruber E, et al. Cortical amyloid beta in cognitively normal elderly adults is associated with decreased network efficiency within the cerebro-cerebellar system. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:52. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Henry JD, Crawford JR, Phillips LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1212–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cosentino S, Scarmeas N, Albert SM, Stern Y. Verbal fluency predicts mortality in Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2006;19:123–9. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000213912.87642.3d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology. Front Psychol. 2014;5:772. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Duffau H, Moritz-Gasser S, Mandonnet E. A re-examination of neural basis of language processing: Proposal of a dynamic hodotopic model from data provided by brain stimulation mapping during picture naming. Brain Lang. 2014;131:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Birn RM, Kenworthy L, Case L, Caravella R, Jones TB, Bandettini PA, et al. Neural systems supporting lexical search guided by letter and semantic category cues: A self-paced overt response fMRI study of verbal fluency. Neuroimage. 2010;49:1099–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Paulesu E, Goldacre B, Scifo P, Cappa SF, Gilardi MC, Castiglioni I, et al. Functional heterogeneity of left inferior frontal cortex as revealed by fMRI. Neuroreport. 1997;8:2011–7. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199705260-00042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Katzev M, Tuescher O, Hennig J, Weiller C, Kaller CP. Revisiting the functional specialization of left inferior frontal gyrus in phonological and semantic fluency: the crucial role of task demeands and individual ability. J Neurosci. 2013;33:7837–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3147-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Okada R, Okuda T, Nakano N, Nishimatsu K, Fukushima H, Onoda M, et al. Brain areas associated with sentence processing: A functional MRI study and a lesion study. J Neurolinguist. 2013;26:470–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2013.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Resting functional connectivity of language networks: characterization and reproducibility. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:841–54. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zhu L, Fan Y, Zou Q, Wang J, Gao J-H, Niu Z. Temporal reliability and lateralization of the resting-state language network. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e85880. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tie Y, Rigolo L, Norton HI, Huang RY, Wu W, Orringer D, et al. Defining language networks from resting-state fMRI for surgical planning: a feasibility study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:1018–30. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ketteler D, Kastrau F, Vohn R, Huber W. The subcortical role of language processing. High level linguistic feature such as ambiguity resolution and the human brain; an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2008;39:2002–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Klostermann F, Krugel LK, Ehlen F. Functional roles of the thalamus for language capacities. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013;7:32. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rosen HJ, Ojemman JG, Ollinger JM, Petersen SE. Comparison of brain activation during word retrieval done silently and aloud using fMRI. Brain Cogn. 2000;42:201–17. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wahl M, Marzinzik F, Friederici AD, Hahne A, Kupsch A, Schneider G-H, et al. The human thalamus processes syntactic and semantic language violations. Neuron. 2008;59:695–707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ojemann GA, Fedio P, Van Buren JM. Anomia from pulvinar and subcortical parietal stimulation. Brain. 1968;91:99–116. doi: 10.1093/brain/91.1.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ojemann GA, Ward AA. Speech representation in ventrolateral thalamus. Brain. 1971;94:669–80. doi: 10.1093/brain/94.4.669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Penfield W, Roberts L. Speech and brain-mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1959. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bohsali AA, Triplett W, Sudhyadhom A, Gullett JM, McGregor K, FitzGerald D, et al. Broca’s area – thalamic connectivity. Brain Lang. 2015;141:80–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Crosson B. Thalamic mechanisms in language: A reconsideration based on recent findings and concepts. Brain Lang. 2013;126:73–88. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Nadeau SE, Crosson B. Subcortical Aphasia. Brain Lang. 1997;58:355–402. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Watanabe Y, Funahashi S. Thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:134–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Nadeau SE. Subcortical language mechanisms. In: Stemmer B, Whitaker H. (Eds.) Handbook of the neuroscience of language. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2008. pp. 329-40. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Barbas H, Garcia-Cabezas MA, Zikopoulos B. Frontal-thalamic circuits associated with language. Brain Lang. 2013;126:49–61. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.De Jong LW, van der Hiele K, Veer IM, Houwing JJ, Westendorp RGJ, Bolen PW, et al. Strongly reduced volumes of putamen and thalamus in Alzheimer’s disease: an MRI study. Brain. 2008;131:3277–85. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Radanovic M, Azambuja M, Mansur LL, Porto CS, Scaff M. Thalamus and language. Interface with attention, memory, and executive function. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003;61:34–42. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X2003000100006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Yuan R, Di X, Taylor PA, Gohel S, Tsai Y-H, Biswal BB. Functional topography of the thalamocortical system in human. Brain Struct Funct. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1018-7. . Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Friesen DC, Luo L, Luk G, Bialystok E. Proficiency and control in verbal fluency performance across the lifespan for monolinguals and bilinguals. Lang Cogn Neurosci. 2015;30:238–50. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.918630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dubois B, Feldmann HH, Jacova C, Cummings JL, Dekosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, et al. Revising the definition of Alzheimer’s disease: a new lexicon. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:1118–27. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70223-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bogousslavsky J, Regli F, Uske A. Thalamic infarcts: clinical syndromes, etiology, and prognosis. Neurology. 1988;38:837–48. doi: 10.1212/WNL.38.6.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]