Abstract
Early adulthood is a developmentally important time period, with many novel life events needing to be traversed for the first time. Despite this important transition period, few studies examine the development of moral decision-making processes during this critical life stage. In the present study, college students completed moral decision-making measures during their freshman and senior years of college. Results indicate that, across four years, moral decision-making demonstrates considerable rank-order stability as well as change, such that people become more likely to help a friend relative to following societal rules. To help understand the mechanisms driving changes in moral decision-making processes, we examined their joint development with personality traits, a known correlate that changes during early adulthood in the direction of greater maturity. We found little evidence that personality and moral decision-making developmental processes are related. In sum, findings indicate that while moral decision-making processes are relatively stable across a four-year period, changes do occur which are likely independent of developmental processes driving personality trait change.
Introduction
“Friends and loved ones are special to us; we do not, and should not, assess their interests from the cold detached standpoint of the impartial observer, for to do so would be a repudiation of love. No ethical system worth its salt will attempt to require such impartiality from us in these contexts, on pain of making human fulfillment unattainable.” [1]
Throughout their lives, people are confronted with decisions in which they have to choose whether it is more important to help a friend as opposed to following society’s rules. These grey areas have no right or wrong answer, with both poles offering an arguably ethical choice depending on one’s rationale and reasoning. For example, not turning in a friend caught cheating on an exam, or leniently grading a friend’s exam to ensure he passes, present a dilemma with arguably two ethical choices—being a loyal friend or an ethical employee. Because these choices have implications for society at large it is important to understand the developmental course of such decisions and what processes serve to influence and shape moral decision-making across time.
Unfortunately, little is known about the development of moral decision-making, particularly after childhood and adolescence. Post-adolescence marks a particularly critical period in which moral decision-making likely develops, as young adults begin to take on new roles outside their parents’ home, embark on new relationships, and engage in profound self and identity examination [2]. Using a four-year longitudinal study of young adults, the present research investigates the development of moral decision-making—specifically, what young adults choose when faced with conflicting obligations towards their friends and societal rules. Further, we examine potential reasons why development occurs by examining the joint development with personality traits. Personality traits are a known correlate of morality and their development is conceptually linked with moral development [3,4]. However, to date these two constructs have not been examined simultaneously over time.
The Development of Moral Decision-Making
The development of moral decision-making has been described extensively through the work of Kohlberg and others [5–7] and tested in numerous studies [8,9]. However, few empirical studies extend past adolescence and use a longitudinal design. Those studies that have longitudinally examined post-adolescent samples suggest that people progress toward more sophisticated moral reasoning during college [10–13]. Some of this development may be unique to the college experience due to practices in collegiate settings that encourage more advanced moral development [14,15]. For instance, the deep learning approaches of some college classes that involve taking on divergent perspectives and integrating information from varied sources encourages growth in moral reasoning [16,17]. Overall, these studies indicate that young adulthood is particularly important with respect to moral development.
Unfortunately, these studies that have examined moral decision-making post-adolescence tend to lack in important ways. For example, previous examinations of college student moral development are often underpowered and occur over short periods of time, meaning researchers may be missing important aspects of development. Furthermore, these studies have not used sophisticated longitudinal models that would allow for assessing individual differences in development or shared development with other constructs. The current study sought to examine the development of moral decision-making by looking at three different ways to conceptualize change: rank-order stability, mean-level change, and individual differences in change [18]. To our knowledge this is the first study of moral development that has looked at these three types of change concurrently.
Relationship Between Moral Decision-Making and Personality Traits
It is not fully known why moral decision-making may develop past adolescence. One potential means to help understand the mechanisms driving stability and change is to look at the development of characteristics associated with moral decision-making. The dispositional traits of agreeableness and conscientiousness regularly emerge as important characteristics of the moral individual across a number of studies [19–21]. Individuals who are agreeable are motivated to maintain positive peer relations and are sympathetic and warm [22–24], whereas individuals who are conscientious are organized, dependable, and rule-abiding, and show great self-control [25–27]. Across multiple cultures, moral persons or individuals of good character are attributed terms associated with agreeableness and conscientiousness, like “compassionate” or “responsible” [20,28–30], and individuals recognized for their moral dedication are perceived by themselves and others as having high levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness [19,21,31,32].
Despite a consistent link between personality and moral personhood, research is less clear on how personality traits may relate to moral decision-making processes. The majority of past research has focused on whether personality traits predict a broad, higher-level moral reasoning stage [33]. Typically, studies of this kind find that self-reported agreeableness and conscientiousness have little or no relationship to moral reasoning skill [34,35], which appears discrepant with the work on moral exemplars and individuals’ perceptions of moral individuals. However, this discrepancy may result from the moral reasoning measures employed in this work, like the Defining Issues Test [33]. The scenarios assessed in this measure may be too detached from what individuals face in their daily lives. This is in contrast to examining the more everyday moral behaviors that individuals may realistically encounter. The focus on higher-level thinking may obscure whether personality traits influence more relatable moral judgments that manifest in everyday behaviors [36].
Studies that have examined more common moral decisions and behaviors are more likely than DIT studies to implicate agreeableness and conscientiousness. For example, individuals who report lower levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness report engaging in more unethical Internet behaviors including fraudulence, misuse, and plagiarism [37]. Similar findings have been found within academic settings, with respect to agreeableness and conscientiousness predicting lower levels of academic cheating [38]. In both cases, though, the actor is the primary individual to benefit from potentially deviant behavior, not a close other. As such, it remains unclear whether agreeableness and conscientiousness relate to the decision to help a friend, when doing so is at odds with societal rules.
Given the nature of the two traits, it may be the case that agreeableness and conscientiousness actually have conflicting influences on such decisions. Specifically, individuals who are higher in conscientiousness may be more likely to adhere to societal rules than help a friend because of their rule-abiding nature and motivation to adhere to moral obligations [26,27,39]. In contrast, individuals who are higher in agreeableness may be more likely to help a friend than follow societal codes because of their sympathetic nature and motivation to maintain positive peer relations [22,23].
The Development of Agreeableness and Conscientiousness
How individuals’ levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness develop over time may provide insight into how people respond to moral dilemmas. It is well-established that young adults, on average, tend to increase in agreeableness and conscientiousness [25,40–43]. These trends have been described as reflecting increases in maturity [42,43]. Longitudinal work also indicates that there are individual differences in these patterns [42]. That is, some people increase, others decrease, and still others stay consistent over time in their personality trait levels, including their levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness. Changes in personality traits are thought to coincide with changes in social roles [44,45] and an assortment of other social factors such as military experience [46].
No work to our knowledge has examined the role that personality trait development might play in the moral decision-making process and its development (or vice versa). Given that agreeableness, conscientiousness, and moral decision-making appear to change during the young adult years [11,47], one would expect these developments to coincide. Based on previous associations, as individuals increase in agreeableness, they likely will also increase their tendency to help their friend. Furthermore, as individuals increase in conscientiousness, they also should increase in the tendency to follow societal expectations.
Current Study
The present study examined the relationship between moral decision-making and the personality traits of agreeableness and conscientiousness. We examined two components of the moral decision-making process. First, we were interested in one’s sense of obligation to a friend. That is, does a friend have the right to expect favors, when those favors are at odds with societal rules or expectations? Second, we examined one’s decision to help a friend. When faced with the dilemma between helping a friend or following societal rules, what do individuals choose? While many moral dilemmas pit “getting along” motives (e.g., helping others, sometimes at the cost of personal gain) and “getting ahead” motives (e.g., personal gain, sometimes at the cost of hurting others) against each other—which research proposes are two of humans’ most driving motives [48–50]—our measure presents a conflict between two getting along motives. This presents an understudied perspective on how people navigate moral obligations and decisions.
Our first aim was to assess whether these components of the moral decision-making process are related to morally relevant personality traits. Our second aim was to examine the longitudinal development of these components alongside morally relevant personality traits. To test these questions, we used a four-year longitudinal study of college student students who, during their freshman and senior years, completed measures of moral decision-making and personality.
Method
Sample and participants
The present study uses archival data from the Harvard Student Study [51] that were collected at Harvard University in the 1960s before institutional review boards and formal consent. Data were anonymized and de-identified prior to use. The sample consists of undergraduate students (freshman year: N = 667, Mage = 18.10 years, SDage = .56) who were predominately White. At the time of data collection, Harvard only enrolled men. For the present study, 250 to 519 students completed relevant measures during their freshman year (Wave 1) and 197 to 376 students did so in their senior year (Wave 2). Sample size varied by analysis.
Measures
Moral decision-making
Participants completed a variety of questionnaires and interviews over four consecutive years in college (for more information on measures collected, see work by Harms and colleagues) [52]. Of particular interest to the current study, participants completed a moral decision-making questionnaire [53] during their freshman and senior years. The questionnaire consisted of 16 hypothetical scenarios that pitted responsibility toward society (i.e., following socially prescribed rules) against allegiance to one’s peers. Example scenarios included asking participants whether: as a member of a company’s board of directors, they would share private financial information with a friend; as a drama critic, they would write a positive review for a friend whose play is not actually good; as an exam proctor, they would not turn in their friend whom they catch cheating; and as a test grader, they would leniently grade a friend’s exam to ensure that he passes.
For each scenario, participants (a) reported to what extent they believed they were expected or obligated to help their friend (moral obligation) on a 3-point scale (where ‘1’ reflected no obligation to friend, ‘2’ reflected some obligation to friend, ‘3’ reflected definite obligation to friend) and (b) reported whether they would behave in accordance with socially prescribed rules or behave in a manner that would benefit their friend (moral decision) on a dichotomous scale (where ‘1’ reflected not helping friend and ‘2’ reflected helping friend). As such, higher scores indicated greater feelings of obligation to a friend, and a greater number of decisions to help a friend rather than following societal expectations. However, we do not suggest that lower (or higher) scores indicate a more advanced level of decision-making ability, or that one choice is the decidedly 'moral' one in any of the scenarios. We instead employ the measure as an indicator of whether individuals focus more on helping their friends or on adherence to societal codes as their basis for making these decisions. Reliabilities for these two 16-item measures were satisfactory at both time points (moral obligation: freshman year α = .86, senior year α = .86; moral decision: freshman year α = .70, senior year α = .72). Although these two moral decision-making scales were related (freshman year r = .37; senior year r = .30), their correlation was modest enough to suggest they are distinct components of moral decision-making, and should be examined separately.
Personality traits
In addition to a measure of moral decision-making, participants rated themselves on a variety of adjectives. Latent agreeableness and conscientiousness factors were created for each participant at the two time points, factors that were based largely on previous factor analytic work by Harms and colleagues [52] who examined descriptive properties of the Harvard Student Study [51]. Following Harms’ and colleagues’ work with the present data, agreeableness was measured using four items from the Brownfain Self-Rating Inventory (i.e., trustfulness, understanding of others, sincerity, and generosity) [54] completed on an 8-point scale (1 = low, 8 = high), and two combined scales from the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (i.e., the Feeling and the Thinking [reverse-scored] scales) [55,56] completed as a multiple choice test. The latent conscientiousness factor was created using four items from the Brownfain Self-Rating Inventory (i.e., initiative, dependability, neatness, and consistency) completed on an 8-point scale (1 = low, 8 = high), and two combined scales from the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (i.e., the Judging and the Perceiving [reverse-scored] scales) completed as a multiple choice test. Although now outdated measures, Harms’ and colleagues’ work [52] as well as others’ [24,57,58] has determined that items from the Brownfain Self-Rating Inventory and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator do an adequate job at capturing more modern measures of agreeableness and conscientiousness. We were not able to include items from the Omnibus Personality Inventory [59] as part of conscientiousness like Harms and colleagues did because it was not collected at Wave 1.
Analyses
All analyses were run using R statistical software. Using structural equation modeling, we first tested univariate latent change models for each of our moral decision-making (moral obligation and moral decision) and personality constructs (agreeableness and conscientious) using the “lavaan” package in R [60]. See Fig 1. Model construction was based on McArdle’s work [61] and has been successfully used in previous studies of personality trait development [62]. For the moral obligation and moral decision constructs, we fit models using four parcels containing four items each; agreeableness and conscientiousness items were not formed into parcels. For each model, item indicators at each wave loaded onto latent factors that represent Wave 1 and Wave 2. These latent factors were then used to construct latent intercept and change parameters. The intercept factors were scaled to represent initial levels of the construct at freshman year while the change factor represented the changes in each construct between freshman and senior years. We fixed all item factor loadings and item residual variances to be equivalent across time points, and same item residuals were allowed to correlate across time.
Next, we fit four bivariate latent change models to assess the relationship between initial values and changes between each moral decision-making component and each personality trait [62]. Intercept and change parameters for personality traits and moral decision-making were allowed to correlate. In these bivariate models, three particular parameters of interest were estimated: intercepts of each construct correlated with one another; change factors regressed onto the intercept of the other factor; and both change factors correlated with one another. To assess fit, we examined the chi-square fit statistic (χ2), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Data and full information on our analyses are available at https://osf.io/vxt6k/.
Results
Longitudinal Development of Moral Decision-Making
As shown in Table 1, moral obligations and decisions demonstrated high rank-order stability over the 4-year period (latent rs = .52 and .53, respectively). These analyses suggest that moral decision-making is a relatively stable construct across four years in young adulthood, though not at levels that would preclude changes in moral decision-making. In terms of cross-construct correlations, moral obligations during freshman year were modestly related to moral decisions during senior year (latent r = .14), and freshman moral decisions were modestly related to moral obligations during senior year (latent r = .21). These results provide support for these two components of moral decision-making to be considered separately in our analyses.
Table 1. Intercorrelations Among Latent Measures of Moral Decision-Making and Personality.
Construct | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Moral Obligation (Freshman Year) | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
2. Moral Decision (Freshman Year) | .41* | - | - | - | - | - | - |
3. Agreeableness (Freshman Year) | .07 | .04 | - | - | - | - | - |
4. Conscientiousness (Freshman Year) | -.09 | -.26* | .35* | - | - | - | - |
5. Moral Obligation (Senior Year) | .52* | .21* | .01 | -.04 | - | - | - |
6. Moral Decision (Senior Year) | .14* | .53* | -.14 | -.31* | .39* | - | - |
7. Agreeableness (Senior Year) | -.03 | .02 | .70* | .20 | -.12 | -.08 | - |
8. Conscientiousness (Senior Year) | -.09 | -.04 | .33* | .69* | -.14 | -.22* | .52* |
* = p < .05.
Next, we examined mean-level changes in obligations and decisions. Both of the longitudinal models fit the data well (Table 2; standardized item loadings ranged from .602 to .854 at Wave 1 and .651 to .844 at Wave 2 for obligations, and .410 to .758 at Wave 1 and .429 to .798 at Wave 2 for decisions). Our sample experienced mean-level changes in moral obligations, such that over the 4-year period, participants on average felt more obligated to help a friend rather than follow the rules of society (est. = .552, SE = .087, t = 6.317, p < .001). Similarly, our sample experienced increases in the tendency to decide to help a friend instead of following societal expectations (est. = .402, SE = .101, t = 3.974, p < .001).
Table 2. Means and Variances for Intercepts and Slopes of Moral Decision-Making and Personality Measures.
Intercept | Slope | Model Fit Statistics | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Construct | Est. (SE) [95% CIs] | Variance (SE) [95% CIs] | Est. (SE) [95% CIs] | Variance (SE) [95% CIs] | χ2 | df | RMSEA | CFI |
Moral Obligations | 1.550* (.016) [1.518, 1.582] | .096* (.009) [.078, .114] | .552* (.087) [.380, .723] | .047* (.007) [.032, .061] | 52.362 | 16 | .066 | .977 |
Moral Decisions | 1.482* (.011) [1.462, 1.503] | .027* (.004) [.019, .035] | .402* (.101) [.204, .601] | .009* (.002) [.004, .013] | 48.001 | 16 | .062 | .961 |
Agreeableness | 5.862* (.085) [5.695, 6.029] | .827* (.184) [.466, 1.187] | .278 (.448) [-.600, 1.156] | .158 (.130) [-.096, .413] | 28.067 | 28 | .002 | 1.000 |
Conscientiousness | 5.477* (.099) [5.283, 5.672] | 1.762* (.288) [1.197, 2.326] | 1.174* (.433) [.326, 2.022] | .354† (.182) [-.003, .710] | 41.461 | 28 | .031 | .972 |
* = p < .05
† = p < .10
Est. = unstandardized estimate, SE = standard error, 95% CIs = 95% confidence intervals.
Not everyone followed this pattern of mean-level changes though. As indicated by the variance estimates for the slope estimates, there were individual differences in change patterns for both moral obligations (est. = .047, SE = .007, t = 6.366, p < .001) and moral decisions (est. = .009, SE = .002, t = 3.80, p < .001). Thus, while the general trend was to increase one’s tendency to help out a friend in need, not everyone changed to the same degree.
Longitudinal Development of Personality Traits
Changes in agreeableness and conscientiousness across college were examined next. Across four years, both agreeableness and conscientiousness evidenced high rank-order stability (latent rs = .70 and .69, respectively; Table 1). Longitudinal models indicated a good fit to the data (Table 2; standardized item loadings ranged from .328 to .639 at Wave 1 and .101 to .658 at Wave 2 for agreeableness, and .306 to .632 at Wave 1 and .257 to .662 at Wave 2 for conscientiousness). In terms of mean-level change, participants did not change in their levels of agreeableness (est. = .278, SE = .448, t = .620, p = .535), but did increase in their levels of conscientiousness (est. = 1.174, SE = .433, t = 2.713, p = .007). There was some evidence for individual differences in change as evidenced by the variance components for conscientiousness (est. = .354, SE = .182, t = 1.943, p = .052) but there was less evidence for individual differences in agreeableness (est. = .158, SE = .130, t = 1.221, p = .222).
Joint Longitudinal Development of Moral Decision-Making and Personality Traits
First, we examined associations between the moral decision-making components and personality traits. All models fit the data well (χ2s ≤ 151.223, RMSEAs ≤ .031, CFIs ≥ .965). In examining the cross-sectional relationships among moral decision-making and personality (Table 1), we found that moral obligation was not strongly associated with agreeableness or conscientiousness (freshman year: latent rs = .07 and -.09, senior year: latent rs = -.12 and -.14, for agreeableness and conscientiousness, respectively). Similar findings were found for moral decisions (freshman year: latent rs = .04 and -.26; senior year: latent rs = -.08 and -.22, for agreeableness and conscientiousness, respectively).
Next we investigated the joint development of these constructs using bivariate latent change models (Table 3). First, we examined whether initial levels of personality were associated with the tendency to increase or decrease in the likelihood of helping a friend. No meaningful cross-lagged results emerged. We next tested whether initial levels of moral decision-making predicted changes in personality. We found that freshman levels of moral decisions were marginally associated with increases in conscientiousness (est. = .741, SE = .446, t = 1.662, p = .097). No other cross-lags evidenced meaningful relationships.
Table 3. Bivariate Latent Change Models.
Personality Intercept & Moral Intercept | Personality Intercept & Moral Slope | Moral Intercept & Personality Slope | Personality Slope & Moral Slope | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bivariate Model | Est. (SE) | [95% CIs] | Est. (SE) | [95% CIs] | Est. (SE) | [95% CIs] | Est. (SE) | [95% CIs] |
Moral Obligation & Agreeableness | .007 (.031) | [-.053, .067] | .047 (.031) | [-.014, .108] | -.143 (.168) | [-.472, .186] | -.007 (.006) | [-.019, .004] |
Moral Obligation & Conscientiousness | .036 (.035) | [-.033, .105] | .003 (.015) | [-.026, .032] | .001 (.221) | [-.433, .435] | -.015† (.008) | [-.032, .001] |
Moral Decision & Agreeableness | -.032 (.021) | [-.074, .009] | .010 (.018) | [-.025, .045] | -.065 (.381) | [-.813, .682] | -.001 (.003) | [-.006, .005] |
Moral Decision & Conscientiousness | .008 (.023) | [-.037, .053] | -.009 (.008) | [-.025, .007] | .741† (.446) | [-.133, 1.614] | -.006 (.003) | [-.012, .001] |
† = p < .10
Est. = unstandardized estimate, SE = standard error, 95% CIs = 95% confidence intervals. The first column of results displays the relationships between personality traits and the moral decision-making components’ intercepts. The second column of results displays the relationships between personality trait intercepts with changes in the moral decision-making components. The third column of results displays the relationships between the moral decision-making components’ intercepts with changes in personality traits. The fourth column of results displays the relationships between changes in personality traits and moral decision-making components from freshman year to senior year.
Lastly, we examined whether changes in obligations and decisions were associated with changes in agreeableness and conscientiousness (Table 3). Changes in moral obligation were slightly related to changes in conscientiousness (est. = -.015, SE = .008, t = -1.873, p = .061), such that individuals who increased in their obligations to societal code also increased in their conscientiousness. No other meaningful relationships were found.
Discussion
The present study examined moral decision-making in the context of dilemmas in which individuals had to decide between breaking the rules of society to help out a friend or following societal rules at the consequence of negatively affecting that friend. Specifically, we were interested in how responses to these dilemmas develop across young adulthood, and whether moral decision-making was associated with personality traits and personality trait development. Our findings advance and support previous research suggesting that moral decision-making continues to develop past childhood and adolescence and into the college and young adult years [11] while still showing considerable stability. In addition, these findings extend work examining the overlap between personality traits and moral decision-making [34,35] by examining their co-development. Our results suggest little joint development, indicating that moral decision-making and personality trait development processes are largely separable from one another [34]. As such, it is important to consider both types of variables separately when considering the complexities of moral personhood, and to extend work to other individual differences that may play a closer role in the development of moral decision-making.
Moral Decision-Making as a Relatively Stable Trait
Our study provides support for moral decision-making as a stable individual difference across a four-year period. Much work in morality focuses on the influence of the situation and neglects the predictive power of stable, individual characteristics for influencing moral judgments and behaviors [63–65]. The rank-order stability correlations for moral obligation and moral decisions found here are indeed quite similar to the stability estimates for personality traits at this age [66]. These findings fit well with recent work showing the moderate stability of morality in everyday life and across different situations [36,67], providing additional evidence for the existence of a relatively stable moral character. While situational conceptualizations of moral character are indeed important, these studies and the present findings highlight the need to incorporate relatively stable individual differences into theories of moral behavior [3].
Past longitudinal research on moral development has largely focused on children and adolescents, whereas our findings support previous research in suggesting that the young adult years—when people begin redefining themselves and creating a separate identity from their familial and hometown ties—also prove a crucial period for development [10,11]. Specifically, young adults in our study, on average, increased in their feelings of obligation to close others and were more likely to decide to help them rather than follow socially prescribed rules. Considered in the context of competing “getting along” goals [50], it is interesting that getting along with peers became stronger than the desire to get along with societal expectations throughout the college years, and raises the question of how competing “getting ahead” goals might change during these years. These findings also raise the question of what mechanisms drive changes in moral decision-making, as currently few studies exist that investigate the natural development of moral decision-making during young adulthood or that attempt to identify experiences that influence moral development. One mechanism of particular importance may be changes in what people value with respect to their social networks. Specifically, people tend to create a smaller social network as they age, and their social network increases in importance [68,69]. Assessing whether changes in moral decision-making are specific to the college age or are only the beginning of this normative trend to increasingly help out friends rather than follow societal codes will be helpful for understanding the mechanisms underlying these changes.
Moral Decision-Making Development and Personality Traits
Our results also suggest that changes in moral decision-making practices during adulthood are likely largely independent from the processes that drive personality development. Changes in conscientiousness and agreeableness during the early adulthood years are characterized as evidence for increases in maturity, as agreeableness is associated with being kind and warm, and conscientiousness reflects the ability to control one’s impulses [41]. As such, it is relatively surprising that these processes do not show greater overlap. This may be due in part to our measures used. For example, researchers might benefit from focusing on the various facets of personality. For instance, these moral dilemmas may actually cause two aspects of agreeableness—compliance and empathy—to be in conflict, thus obscuring the unique and contrasting influences both may play in moral decision-making [22]. It is also possible that agreeableness may play a bigger role in the development of non-deliberative moral action, for example, caring for others and other prosocial behaviors [22].
Other individual differences and developmental processes may prove more fruitful in understanding the processes behind changes in moral decision-making. For example, major life experiences that tend to increase one’s focus on the well-being of close others, such as marriage, child-rearing, and caring for older relatives [70], offer another important avenue for explaining stability and change in moral decision processes. Life goals may be another important individual difference to consider. Changes in life goals—for example, focusing on family over work—influence what life roles people find important and people then subtly changing their daily behaviors and values in order to achieve those goals [71]. If one’s goals shift toward a more communal orientation (e.g., friends and family members over personal advancements), someone is likely to change one’s moral decisions given the greater value placed on friends and family members. Future work that examines the goals and motives associated with different events and stages of development may help to explain developmental process leading to changes in moral decision-making.
Limitations, Future Directions, and Conclusions
It is necessary to note aspects of the current study that should serve as catalysts for future research. First, our longitudinal sample was predominately White and affluent, and attended an elite all-male school in the early 1960s. Future work thus should examine the generalizability of our findings to women, more traditional collegiate contexts, and non-collegiate samples. Second, our measure of moral dilemmas has received little empirical investigation [72–74] and its ability to predict moral behavior is unclear. Considering the evaluativeness of morality, this self-report of moral decision-making may be especially prone to biased self-report [75]. Ideally, future work will include more objective moral measures, rather than only self-reported decisions and behaviors [36]. In addition, although our personality measure has been validated in previous work [52], using more current personality measures would be useful, especially in order to examine how facets of agreeableness (e.g., sympathy vs. modesty) and conscientiousness (e.g., dutifulness vs. self-discipline) might relate differently to moral decision-making.
Taken together, the present study provides clear evidence that moral decision-making continues to develop past childhood and adolescence, and confirms and extends previous work suggesting that personality has little cross-sectional or longitudinal relationship to moral decision-making. Research would benefit from examining the longer-term implications of early adult experiences on later moral decisions and behaviors, as well as how moral decision-making may continue to change in later adult years. Furthermore, it is sometimes questionable what direction of changes in moral decisions and obligations should be considered a positive or mature development. These grey areas are a particularly intriguing aspect of morality research and are important to consider, especially when the goal of moral development research is often focused on understanding the “improvement” of moral character. Always siding with societal standards or always choosing friends’ needs may actually be the least mature option, as neither may be adaptive.
In closing, if it is true, as John Cottingham states in our opening quote, that close others cannot be evaluated from a “cold detached standpoint,” it is important to gain a better understanding of which individuals are especially prone to choosing friends over society and what factors contribute to their decision, especially when there may be large repercussions resulting from that decision.
Data Availability
Data and full information on our analyses are available at https://osf.io/vxt6k/.
Funding Statement
The authors have no support or funding to report.
References
- 1.Cottingham J. Ethics and impartiality. Philos Stud Int J Philos Anal Tradit. 1983. January 1;43:83–99. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am Psychol. 2000. May;55(5):469–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hill PL, Roberts BW. Propositions for the study of moral personality development. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2010. December;19(6):380–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lapsley DK, Hill PL. The development of the moral personality In: Narvaez D, Lapsley DK, Narvaez D (Ed), Lapsley DK (Ed), editors. Personality, identity, and character: Explorations in moral psychology. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press; 2009. p. 185–213. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kohlberg L, Hersh RH. Moral development: A review of the theory. Theory Pract. 1977. April 1;16(2):53–9. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kohlberg L. The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. 1st ed. San Francisco: Harper & Row; 1984. 729 p. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kohlberg L, Levine C, Hewer A. Moral stages: A current formulation and a response to critics. Contrib Hum Dev. 1983;10:174. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Jaffee S, Hyde JS. Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2000. September;126(5):703–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Stams GJ, Brugman D, Deković M, van Rosmalen L, van der Laan P, Gibbs JC. The moral judgment of juvenile delinquents: A meta-analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2006. October;34(5):697–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kitchener KS. A longitudinal study of moral and ego development in young adults. J Youth Adolesc. 1984. June;13(3):197–211. 10.1007/BF02089059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Myyry L, Juujärvi S, Pesso K. Change in values and moral reasoning during higher education. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2013. March;10(2):269–84. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shaver DG. A longitudinal study of moral development at a conservative, religious, liberal arts college. J Coll Stud Pers. 1985. September;26(5):400–4. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Shaver DG. Moral development of students attending a Christian, liberal arts college and a Bible college. J Coll Stud Pers. 1987. May;28(3):211–8. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Foster JD, LaForce B. A longitudinal study of moral, religious, and identity development in a Christian liberal arts environment. J Psychol Theol. 1999;27(1):52–68. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rest JR, Thoma SJ. Relation of moral judgment development to formal education. Dev Psychol. 1985. July;21(4):709–14. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mayhew MJ, Seifert TA, Pascarella ET. A multi-institutional assessment of moral reasoning development among first-year students. Rev High Educ J Assoc Study High Educ. 2010. Spr;33(3):357–90. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mayhew MJ, Seifert TA, Pascarella ET, Laird TFN, Blaich CF. Going deep into mechanisms for moral reasoning growth: How deep learning approaches affect moral reasoning development for first-year students. Res High Educ. 2012. February;53(1):26–46. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jackson JJ, Allemand M. Moving personality development research forward: Applications using structural equation models. Eur J Personal. 2014. May;28(3):300–10. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Matsuba MK, Walker LJ. Extraordinary moral commitment: Young adults involved in social organizations. J Pers. 2004. April;72(2):413–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Walker LJ. The perceived personality of moral exemplars. J Moral Educ. 1999. June;28(2):145–62. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Walker LJ, Frimer JA. Moral personality of brave and caring exemplars. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007. November;93(5):845–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Graziano WG, Tobin RM. Agreeableness In: Leary MR, Hoyle RH, Leary MR (Ed), Hoyle RH (Ed), editors. Handbook of individual differences in social behavior. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2009. p. 46–61. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jensen-Campbell LA, Graziano WG. Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict. J Pers. 2001. April;69(2):323–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.John OP, Srivastava S. The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed). New York, NY US: Guilford Press; 1999. p. 102–38. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jackson JJ, Bogg T, Walton KE, Wood D, Harms PD, Lodi-Smith J, et al. Not all conscientiousness scales change alike: A multimethod, multisample study of age differences in the facets of conscientiousness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009. February;96(2):446–59. 10.1037/a0014156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kalshoven K, Den Hartog DN, De Hoogh AHB. Ethical leader behavior and Big Five factors of personality. J Bus Ethics. 2011. May;100(2):349–66. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mike A, Harris K, Roberts BW, Jackson JJ. Conscientiousness In: Wright JD, editor. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (Second Edition) [Internet]. Oxford: Elsevier; 2015. [cited 2015 Nov 3]. p. 658–65. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780080970868250472 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lapsley DK, Lasky B. Protoypic moral character. Identity Int J Theory Res. 2001. October;1(4):345–63. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Smith KD, Smith ST, Christopher JC. What defines the good person?: Cross-cultural comparisons of experts’ models with lay prototypes. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2007. May;38(3):333–60. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Walker LJ, Pitts RC. Naturalistic conceptions of moral maturity. Dev Psychol. 1998. May;34(3):403–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Walker LJ, Frimer JA, Dunlop WL. Varieties of moral personality: Beyond the banality of heroism. J Pers. 2010. June;78(3):907–42. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00637.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Walker LJ, Hennig KH. Differing conceptions of moral exemplarity: Just, brave, and caring. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004. April;86(4):629–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rest JR, Narvaez D, Thoma SJ, Bebeau MJ. DIT2: Devising and testing a revised instrument of moral judgment. J Educ Psychol. 1999. December;91(4):644–59. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Derryberry WP, Wilson T, Snyder H, Norman T, Barger B. Moral judgment developmental differences between gifted youth and college students. J Second Gift Educ. 2005. Fal;17(1):6–19. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dollinger SJ, LaMartina AK. A note on moral reasoning and the five-factor model. J Soc Behav Personal. 1998. June;13(2):349–58. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bollich KL, Doris JM, Vazire S, Raison CL, Jackson JJ, Mehl MR. Eavesdropping on character: Assessing everyday moral behaviors. J Res Personal. 2015. 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Karim NSA, Zamzuri NHA, Nor YM. Exploring the relationship between Internet ethics in university students and the Big Five model of personality. Comput Educ. 2009. August;53(1):86–93. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Williams KM, Nathanson C, Paulhus DL. Identifying and profiling scholastic cheaters: Their personality, cognitive ability, and motivation. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2010. September;16(3):293–307. 10.1037/a0020773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Jackson JJ, Wood D, Bogg T, Walton KE, Harms PD, Roberts BW. What do conscientious people do? Development and validation of the Behavioral Indicators of Conscientiousness (BIC). J Res Personal. 2010. August;44(4):501–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bleidorn W. Hitting the road to adulthood: Short-term personality development during a major life transition. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2012. December;38(12):1594–608. 10.1177/0146167212456707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL. Personality development: Stability and change. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:453–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Roberts BW, Mroczek D. Personality trait change in adulthood. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008. February;17(1):31–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006. January;132(1):1–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lodi-Smith J, Roberts BW. Social investment and personality: A meta-analysis of the relationship of personality traits to investment in work, family, religion, and volunteerism. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2007. February;11(1):1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Roberts BW, Wood D, Smith JL. Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development. J Res Personal. 2005. February;39(1):166–84. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jackson JJ, Thoemmes F, Jonkmann K, Lüdtke O, Trautwein U. Military training and personality trait development: Does the military make the man, or does the man make the military? Psychol Sci. 2012. March;23(3):270–7. 10.1177/0956797611423545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Roberts BW, Wood D, Caspi A. The development of personality traits in adulthood In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed). New York, NY US: Guilford Press; 2008. p. 375–98. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hogan R, Roberts BW. A socioanalytic perspective on person–environment interaction In: Walsh WB, Craik KH, Price RH, Walsh WB (Ed), Craik KH (Ed), Price RH (Ed), editors. Person–environment psychology: New directions and perspectives (2nd ed). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2000. p. 1–23. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hogan R. A socioanalytic perspective on the Five-Factor Model In: Wiggins JS, Wiggins JS (Ed), editors. The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 1996. p. 163–79. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hogan R, Roberts BW. A socioanalytic model of maturity. J Career Assess. 2004. May;12(2):207–17. [Google Scholar]
- 51.King SH. Five lives at Harvard: Personality change during college. Oxford, England: Harvard U. Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Harms PD, Roberts BW, Winter D. Becoming the Harvard man: Person-environment fit, personality development, and academic success. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2006. July;32(7):851–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Stouffer SA, Toby J. Role conflict and personality. Am J Sociol. 1951;56:395–406. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Brownfain JJ. Stability of the self-concept as a dimension of personality. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1952. July;47(3):597–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Myers IB. The Myers-Briggs type indicator. Princeton, NJ US: Educational Testing Service; 1962. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Myers IB, McCaulley MH. Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA US: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 57.McCrae RR, Costa PT. Reinterpreting the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality. J Pers. 1989. March;57(1):17–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Furnham A. The Big Five versus the Big Four: The relationship between the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and NEO-PI Five Factor Model of personality. Personal Individ Differ. 1996. August;21(2):303–7. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Heist P, Williams P. Manual for the Omnibus Personality Inventory. Berkeley, CA, US: University of California, Berkeley, Center for Study of Higher Education; 1957. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 61.McArdle JJ. Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009. January;60:577–605. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mund M, Neyer FJ. The winding paths of the lonesome cowboy: Evidence for mutual influences between personality, subjective health, and loneliness. J Pers. 2015. July 15; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Doris JM. Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. Cambridge University Press; 2002. 299 p. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Graham J, Nosek BA, Haidt J, Iyer R, Koleva S, Ditto PH. Mapping the moral domain. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011. August;101(2):366–85. 10.1037/a0021847 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zhong C- B, Bohns VK, Gino F. Good lamps are the best police: Darkness increases dishonesty and self-interested behavior. Psychol Sci. 2010. March;21(3):311–4. 10.1177/0956797609360754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2000. January;126(1):3–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bleidorn W, Denissen JJA. Virtues in action—The new look of character traits. Br J Psychol. 2015. November;106(4):700–23. 10.1111/bjop.12117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Carstensen LL. The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science. 2006. June;312(5782):1913–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Wrzus C, Hänel M, Wagner J, Neyer FJ. Social network changes and life events across the life span: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2013. January;139(1):53–80. 10.1037/a0028601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC. Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011. October;101(4):862–82. 10.1037/a0024950 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Roberts BW, O’Donnell M, Robins RW. Goal and personality trait development in emerging adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004. October;87(4):541–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Atchley RC, McCabe MP. Socialization in correctional communities: A replication. Am Sociol Rev. 1968;33(5):774–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Wheeler S. Socialization in correctional communities. Am Sociol Rev. 1961;26:697–712. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Zurcher LA, Meadow A, Zurcher SL. Value orientation, role conflict, and alienation from work: A cross-cultural study. Am Sociol Rev. 1965;30(4):539–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Vazire S. Who knows what about a person? The self–other knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010. February;98(2):281–300. 10.1037/a0017908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data and full information on our analyses are available at https://osf.io/vxt6k/.