Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2015 Nov 12;106:143–162. doi: 10.1007/s11192-015-1775-3

Gender differences in research performance and its impact on careers: a longitudinal case study

Peter van den Besselaar 1,, Ulf Sandström 2,3
PMCID: PMC4709377  PMID: 26798162

Abstract

We take up the issue of performance differences between male and female researchers, and investigate the change of performance differences during the early career. In a previous paper it was shown that among starting researchers gendered performance differences seem small to non-existent (Van Arensbergen et al. 2012). If the differences do not occur in the early career anymore, they may emerge in a later period, or may remain absent. In this paper we use the same sample of male and female researchers, but now compare performance levels about 10 years later. We use various performance indicators: full/fractional counted productivity, citation impact, and relative citation impact in terms of the share of papers in the top 10 % highly cited papers. After the 10 years period, productivity of male researchers has grown faster than of female researcher, but the field normalized (relative) citation impact indicators of male and female researchers remain about equal. Furthermore, performance data do explain to a certain extent why male careers in our sample develop much faster than female researchers’ careers; but controlling for performance differences, we find that gender is an important determinant too. Consequently, the process of hiring academic staff still remains biased.

Keywords: Gender bias, Academic careers, Performance differences, Longitudinal study

Introduction

The research literature has shown the performance gaps between male and female researchers since long been indicated, with men on average publishing more papers, and receiving more citations than female researchers (Cole and Zuckerman, 1984; Long, 1992; Xie and Shauman, 1998; Nakhaie, 2002; Prpic, 2002; Penas and Willett, 2006; Symonds, et al., 2006; Taylor, et al., 2006; Ledin, et al., 2007; Abramo, et al., 2009). However, with regard to citations per publication some studies claim that no gender differences exist (Penas and Willett, 2006; Ledin, et al., 2007; Tower, et al., 2007). A few studies even found a higher citation score for women than for men (Long, 1992; Powell, et al., 2009; Sandström, 2009a, b). Nevertheless, on average, total impact of female researchers used to remain lower than of male researchers, due to the lower productivity. This productivity difference emerges already in the early career and then does not disappear (Symonds, et al., 2006).

Why is this productivity difference important? As science claims to be a meritocracy, gender related variation in performance could (and should!) explain gender related differences in grant decisions, and gender related differences in academic careers. But if differences in career success and in grant success do not relate to differences in performance, we would have typical examples of gender bias in contrast to the claimed meritocracy.

Gender bias in science

That bias may play a role was very clearly put on the agenda when Wennerås and Wold (1997) published their results of a study on grant decision-making. They showed that not meritocracy was the standard, but cronyism: having friends in the relevant committees proved to help considerably in getting a grant. On top of that, they also showed the role of gender bias: women needed a substantially (160 per cent) higher performance than male researchers to be successful in biomedical grant applications. Replicating that study some 10 years later, Sandström and Hällsten (2008) found again nepotism, but no sexism anymore: female researchers even had a slightly better chance than male researchers. Obviously, the council studied in both papers changed its gender policy in the meantime.1 After the Wennerås and Wold study, quite a few other studies found that that gender matters, but some evidence suggests differently in different disciplines. For example, a Dutch study showed that in science fields female researchers received positive evaluations and high success rates, even higher than could be expected from past performance. In contrast, the life sciences were characterized by negative gender bias, as no substantial differences between track records of unsuccessful women and successful men were found (Brouns 2000).

Apart from the issue of gender bias in grant reviewing, there is also an issue about gender bias in academic recruitment and selection, leading to lower success rates of female applicants (Van den Brink et al. 2006; Van den Brink 2009). Female researchers have a slower career, and on average end at lower positions; women are still underrepresented in the higher academic positions and men outnumber women in positions of formal power, authority and high income (Xie and Shauman 1998; Timmers et al. 2010). Here again the question is at stake as whether these career differences relate to performance differences, or are based on biased decision-making.

In a recent review, Ceci and Williams (2011) discuss the evidence about gender bias in science, in journal reviewing, grant funding, and in hiring. They claim not to find evidence supporting the existence of discrimination against women in science. The authors conclude that the unequal position of women in science would be based on quality differences between male and female researchers that may partly be based on own career related choices, and partly on discriminatory arrangements not in science but in society at large — e.g., inequalities related to division of domestic work and child care. If this analysis is correct, we are back from gender bias to performance differences.

The main problem with most studies arguing that no gender bias exists is that they do not take performance into account at all. This is also the case with recent meta-reviews (Mutz et al. 2014; Marsh et al. 2009), claiming that gender bias no longer exists in peer review. These studies do not refer to performance, but look only at the success rates.

This is remarkable, as bias can only be measured against performance differences. More directly, if the slower career and the glass ceiling would be determined by performance differences alone, one might be willing to accept this—and focus policy on improving female performance. But with this omission, the studies mentioned actually are not very instructive and fail to show what they claim. They have no information about whether the applicants are statistically representative of their respective groups (male–female), and they also lack information about any self-selection processes that correlate with performance. For example, it might be that self-selection of female applicants applying for a job is stronger than of male researchers in the sense that female applicants have a higher average performance than male applicants. If that is the case we would expect better average bibliometric scores for the female group and, on average, less good results for the male group. These performance data then could be correlated with the committee decisions. We assume that bibliometric data are more or less unbiased for each applicant group and that full bibliometric data with relative citations scores will produce relevant correlates to the grading and ranking procedures of standing and ad hoc committees.

Productivity differences

Several explanations of the productivity puzzle (Cole and Zuckerman 1984) have been proposed: scientific ability, self-selection, social selection, and accumulated disadvantage (Zuckerman 2001).2 According to the scientific ability explanation, male and female academics differ in biological and psychological characteristics, influencing research output. However, more recent research did not find a direct gender ability effect as was established in earlier research (e.g. Xie and Shauman 1998). Above that, one can observe at all levels of education that girls tend to outperform boys, which also contradicts this assumption (Buchmann et al. 2008; Pekkarinen 2008).

The second explanation is more widespread. In the early career, researchers get children and start a family. In fact, this may affect women more than men, as women still to the larger part of domestic work and child-care. This may translate in less time for research and therefore a lower scientific performance in the early career (Long 1992; Symonds et al. 2006). Only later in their career women more or less catch up with male researchers (Long 1992; Symonds et al. 2006), but this lower early productivity has a negative effect on careers (Prozesky 2008; Fuchs et al. 2001; Hunter and Leahey 2010; Karamessini 2004).3 However, other studies indicate that the academic career of female scientists does not suffer from parenthood (Fox & Faver 1985; Cole & Zuckerman 1991; Astin & Bayer 1979; Dryler 2011).

One may argue that getting kids is choice, but the fact that the bulk of domestic labor and of child-care is done by women, is no ‘self-selection’ but a social selection process (c.f. Fox 2005, Fox et al. 2011). This brings us to a variety of differences between male and female researchers, which are partly choice (self-selection) and partly based on social selection. Female researchers have a lower degree of specialization (Leahey 2006), tend to work in other disciplines, tend to focus more on teaching, tend to work at universities and departments with lower reputation, and have a less developed international collaboration and co-authoring network (Allison and Long 1990; McNamee et al. 1990; Dundar and Lewis 1998; Prpic 2002; Lee and Bozeman 2005; Bland et al. 2006; Carayol and Matt 2006; Leahey 2006; Taylor et al., 2006; Puuska 2010), which all affects performance and career in a negative way. However, Badar et al., (2013) report the opposite, but in the context of a developing country. This suggests that a specific study on gender and performance focusing on the latter context may be useful. Furthermore, women receive less academic support and mentoring than men (Landino and Owen 1988; Fuchs et al. 2001). This may be a disadvantage for women too, as academic careers depend on support by academic mentors (Van Balen et al. 2012). A slower career progress recursively may lead to less research and more teaching (Taylor et al. 2006; Snell et al. 2009), and consequently to less scholarly productivity. The accumulation of all these self and social selections over time leads to cumulative disadvantage (Zuckerman 2001).

However, over time gender roles and responsibilities in family life are changing (Xie and Shauman 1998; Taylor et al., 2006; Prozesky 2008), which may also influence work and career orientation of women. One may expect that the gradually changing gender roles in the last decades in many countries may also result into changed behavior. Indeed we have witnessed increased performance of girls in the educational system, and at all levels, girls are outperforming boys (Buchmann et al. 2008; Pekkarinen 2008). Recent data suggest that the performance gap has been closed for PhD students (Miller & Wai 2015; Ceci et al. 2014). If this is indeed a slow and generational process, one would wonder whether this gender performance turn has also reached the research system, first of all the generation of early career researchers.4

An earlier study (Van Arensbergen et al. 2012) indeed confirmed (i) the traditional gendered performance pattern for the older generations of researchers, but (ii) when isolating the group of early career researchers, gender differences have become smaller (in e.g., economics) or are disappearing (in psychology). The question remains as whether women now at a later moment face the performance drop.

Research questions

So our first research question is: Do gender differences in research performance emerge at a later age? The general question behind this is whether we are observing a change in the academic life cycle of female researchers (the productivity dip emerges now later than in the past), or whether is it a generational change meaning that the productivity gap has disappeared in the younger generations.

Around 2004, the young male and female researchers in our sample not only had about the same average performance, but also had about the same position: they were all <3 years after their PhD and generally at the postdoc level. This makes the sample useful for studying career development, which in a meritocracy is expected to go hand in hand with performance development. Therefore our second research question is: Have gender career differences occurred, and, if so, can they be explained by performance differences? With this study we hope to improve our understanding of gender differences in science, by taking performance differences into account—something that is lacking from many former studies.

Data and method

Our sample consists of some 400 researchers that have submitted proposals to an early career grant program of a social science council in the Netherlands between 2003 and 2005. As applying for (such) grants is considered an essential activity during the early research career, it can be assumed that the data cover the community of young social science researchers in the Netherlands during that period rather well.

From the total set of young researchers, we selected here the young researchers in psychology, in behavioral & educational research, and in economics. This selection was made because for these fields the Web of Science is covering academic output relatively well, enabling us to use a bibliometric approach to performance measurement. Relatively well does not mean that all output is covered; here it means that the WoS indexed journals are considered as the most important publication venues, within relevant communities such as the faculties involved and the research council (Van den Besselaar & Leydesdorff 2009).5

This selection resulted in a set of 262 researchers, of which 19 are deleted because of missing data—we could not trace them anymore. This leaves us 104 early career economics researchers (73 % male), 48 behavior and education researchers (38 % male), and 91 psychology researchers (44 % male). On average 45 %, of the researchers in the sample are female. The researchers are in our sample as they applied for an early career grant in 2003 (24 %), 2004 (38 %), or 2005 (38 %).

Homepage, CV and Web of Science provide us with the following information about academic performance and career:

  • An overview of their publications—which was used to validate the performance data as downloaded from the WoS.

  • The academic position in 2003–2005,6 which (in most cases) was postdoctoral researcher;

  • The current position (early 2014) is measured on an ordinal scale with values from 10 (teacher) to 16 (full professor); this are the codes used for the positions in universities’ job structure. About 20.6 % of the cases was early 2014 full professor (score = 16), 28.3 % was associate professor (score = 14), 27.5 % assistant professor (score = 12), 2.1 % senior researcher (score = 11), 14.2 % researcher (score = 11), and 3 % of the applicants was in teaching positions (score = 10). The remaining 4.3 % has a career outside academia. The small number of applicants that went to positions outside academia is not included in the analysis, as it is difficult to integrate their positions into the academic rank system. Furthermore, those that left academia stopped publishing, so we do not have the scores on the independent variables.

  • In between affiliations;

  • The level of mobility: 47 % of the sample showed no mobility, 32 % showed national mobility, and 21 % showed international mobility.

In order to make an adequate bibliometric dataset, we retrieved publication data from the Web of Science (SCI-expanded, Social Science Citation Index; Arts and Humanities Citation Index) using the following query:

  • AU = last_name first–initial* AND (CU = Netherlands OR CU = country name) AND DT = (article OR letter OR note OR proceedings paper OR review) and PY = 2001–2012

‘Country name’ in the query refers to countries where the researcher has worked according to his or her CV. The data were manually cleaned, by comparing the found WoS records with the publication lists found on the Web. In this way we could delete papers that were authored by others with the same name. In cases where we missed paper from the publication list, we searched for the missing titles in WoS, and added it to the set. Generally, missing papers was due to the fact that authors used different initials. As well known, disambiguation and entity resolution are time consuming. But it creates a reliable data set, which is needed given sample size involved.7 With the BMX tool (Sandström & Sandström 2009) the following field normalized citation indicators were calculated:

  • P: Number of publications, full counting

  • Frac P: Number of publications, fractional counting based on author shares

  • NCSf: Field normalized citation score, until 2014

  • NCSf2y: Field normalized citation score, 2 years window

  • TOP x %: Share of publications in the set of (1, 5, 10, 25 and 50 %) highest cited publications, field normalized

The sample consists of three groups applying for a career grant in 2003, 2004, and 2005 respectively. Publications were included from 3 years before the grant (including the application year) until 2012. Citations were measured in two ways: (i) With a time window of 3 years (including the publication year). So for a paper published in 2005, citations are counted until 2007. (ii) Without a time window, which of course means that the early publications (in each period) have higher chance to become cited, than more recent papers. We include citations until the end of 2014. Of course, citations as such say something about overall impact, but not about the real important scientific contributions. For identifying those, we use the share of publications of a researcher in the set of highest cited publications. We use several classes of ‘top papers’: the top 1 % highest cited papers; the top 5 % highest cited papers; the top 10 % highest cited papers; the top 25 % highest cited papers; the top 50 % highest cited papers. All citation-based indicators are size-independent and field normalized. Obviously these different measures are influenced by the number of years, a researcher is active. Therefore we include the application year (reflecting the academic age) as confounding variable.

As the data are not normally distributed, but rather skewed, we use of non-parametric statistics. We compare means, medians, and the distributions of male and female performance. Comparison is done using the SPSS22 procedures non-parametric tests and Anova. Apart from testing bivariate gender differences, we also test a multivariate model predicting career level, using the following independent variables: performance (publications; field normalized citation score), academic age (see note 7), level of mobility, discipline, and gender. As the dependent variable is ordinal, we use of Generalized Linear Models the ordinal response (multinomial) model with a cumulative logit link function.

Findings

Early career phase

In (Van Arensbergen et al. 2012), it was shown that the gendered performance differences seem to be disappearing in the youngest generation of researchers. The analysis was based on publications and citations, and differentiating between social science disciplines. However, no field–normalized indicators were used. We now first test whether the revealed pattern remains when using the field-normalized indicators. As Table 1 shows, the differences are small. In economics, the scores of the male applicants are generally somewhat higher than of females, in behavioral and education studies and in psychology it is the other way round.

Table 1.

Past performance of male and female early career researchers

P Frac P NCSb NCS2yb TOP1%b TOP5%b TOP10%b TOP25%b TOP50%b
All
M Mean 2.63 0.97 0.91 0.84 0.01 0.06 0.11 0.22 0.42
N = 134 Median 2.00 0.79 0.48 0.42 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.40
F Mean 2.41 0.82 1.07 0.88 0.01 0.06 0.11 0.29 0.50
N = 111 Median 2.00 0.60 0.83 0.54 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.50
Economics
M Mean 1.59 0.72 0.81 0.71 0.00 0.07 0.11 0.29 0.35
N = 76 Median 1.00 0.50 0.31 0.11 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.06
F Mean 1.24 0.70 0.65 0.40 0.01 0.03 0.05 0.15 0.29
N = 29 Median 1.00 0.50 0.07 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
Behavioral and educational science
M Mean 5.44 1.70 1.10 1.01 0.01 0.09 0.11 0.23 0.39
N = 18 Median 4.00 1.22 0.47 0.74 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.06 0.26
F Mean 3.03 0.87 1.10 1.03 0.02 0.05 0.10 0.33 0.53
N = 31 Median 2.00 0.67 0.86 0.58 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.17 0.57
Psychology
M Mean 3.35 1.13 1.02 0.99 0.01 0.05 0.10 0.25 0.56
N = 40 Median 3.00 0.79 0.83 0.77 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.19 0.61
F Mean 2.76 0.88 1.30 1.08 0.01 0.09 0.16 0.34 0.61
N = 50 Median 2.00 0.67 1.16 0.86 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.33 0.78

Cohort early career grant applicants 2003–2005—psychology, economics, behavioral and education studies

aCitations based indicators are field-normalized

In Table 2, we test whether the small differences in medians and distributions are statistically significant. For none of the performance variables this is the case if we use all applicants. Also when distinguishing between the three fields, none of the differences between medians is statistically significant at 0.05, and only one at 0.10. Only a few mean rank distributions differ significantly: In economics male researchers have significantly more papers in the top 1 % cited class, but the other differences are not significant. In behavioral and education research, men publish significantly more than women—in this case, the difference between the medians is also significant at 0.10. In psychology, none of the differences are statistically significant. In other words, the here deployed field normalized indicators confirm the earlier findings (in Van Arensbergen et al. 2012) about disappearing gendered performance differences in the youngest generation researchers. The main difference in relation to the earlier contribution by Van Arensbergen et al. (2012) is that they found that women tend to outperform men in the top of the distribution. The new data and analysis suggest equal performance.

Table 2.

Gendered performance differences—early-career researchers

P Frac P NCS NCS2y TOP1% TOP5% TOP10% TOP25% TOP50%
All
Mann–Whitney U 7251 7043.5 6851 7262 7187.5 7080.5 6964 6760.5 6668
Wilcoxon W 13,467 13,259.5 15,896 16,307 16,232.5 16,125.5 16,009 15,805.5 15,713
Z −0.343 −0.721 −1.073 −0.326 −1.259 −0.965 −1.085 −1.35 −1.445
Sign. distribution 0.732 0.471 0.283 0.744 0.208 0.335 0.278 0.177 0.149
Sign. median 0.885 0.220 0.278 0.753 0.347 0.391 0.290 0.356 0.278
Economics
Mann–Whitney U 962.5 1031.5 960 877 1026 1046.5 1065.5 996.5 1016
Wilcoxon W 1397.5 1466.5 1395 1312 3952 1481.5 1500.5 1431.5 1451
Z −1.038 −0.520 −1.049 −1.768 −2.300 −0.748 −0.418 −0.961 −0.669
Sign. distribution 0.299 0.603 0.294 0.077 0.021 0.454 0.676 0.337 0.504
Sign. median 0.773 0.833 0.416 0.127 0.130 0.701 0.961 0.461 0.567
Behavioral and educational science
Mann–Whitney U 185.5 181.5 272 267 276 256 272 237.5 222.5
Wilcoxon W 681.5 677.5 443 763 772 752 768 408.5 393.5
Z −1.960 −2.031 −0.146 −0.253 −0.118 −0.598 −0.168 −0.903 −1.200
Sign. distribution 0.050 0.042 0.884 0.801 0.906 0.550 0.867 0.367 0.230
Sign. median 0.095 0.112 0.435 0.685 0.742 0.815 0.945 0.851 0.851
Psychology
Mann–Whitney U 922.5 915 883 917 992 880 908.5 866 909
Wilcoxon W 2197.5 2190 1703 1737 2267 1700 1728.5 1686 1729
Z −0.636 −0.693 −0.953 −0.677 −0.182 −1.366 −0.853 −1.132 −0.760
Sign. distribution 0.525 0.488 0.340 0.498 0.856 0.172 0.394 0.257 0.448
Sign. median 0.832 0.525 0.138 0.832 0.775 0.312 0.789 0.289 0.138

See notes Table 1

Mid career phase

How did these researchers develop in the about 10 years after the previous measurement? In that period, various gendered mechanisms may have worked, as suggested in the literature. For example, male and female researchers may have faced different family responsibilities, or different career paths. As the average age of the group is about 43, this should be visible in the collected performance data. Therefore we test whether performance differences between men and women have increased between 2004 and 2014, in a for men favorable direction. In Table 3, we show mean and median scores of male and female researchers.

Table 3.

Post performance of male and female researchers, 9 years later

P Frac P NCS NCS2y TOP1% TOP5% TOP10% TOP25% TOP50%
All
M Mean 16.0 5.6 1.31 1.23 1.49 7.03 14.4 31.0 58.4
N = 128 Median 10.5 4.5 1.11 1.06 0.00 0.8 9.93 29.0 59.8
F Mean 13.66 3.8 1.29 1.18 1.54 5.83 13.5 35.0 62.4
N = 105 Median 8.00 2. 7 1.11 1.17 0.00 0 5.86 35.0 63.3
Economics
M Mean 8.82 3.91 1.28 1.20 1.37 6.31 14.2 29.0 56.4
N = 71 Median 8.00 3.50 0.98 0.91 0.00 0.00 6.33 26.0 56.3
F Mean 5.81 2.59 1.28 1.11 1.72 5.94 14.0 34.0 59.5
N = 27 Median 5.00 2.33 1.09 1.22 0.00 0.00 0.00 35.0 56.4
Behavioral and educational science
M Mean 23.2 7.00 1.25 1.20 1.45 6.58 12.4 32.0 59.6
N = 17 Median 21.0 7.76 1.18 1.11 0.00 5.05 11.1 32.0 58.7
F Mean 16.9 3.88 1.30 1.24 0.94 6.26 12.7 35.0 64.5
N = 28 Median 9.0 3.26 1.37 1.32 0.00 0.10 7.67 35.0 69. 5
Psychology
M Mean 25.6 7.88 1.40 1.31 1.71 8.49 15. 8 35.0 61.6
N = 40 Median 21.0 7.73 1.23 1.24 0.00 4.28 11.8 36.0 67.0
F Mean 16.1 4.34 1.29 1.19 1.77 5.54 13.7 35.0 62. 8
N = 50 Median 10.5 3.00 1.08 1.09 0.00 1.09 8.66 34.0 63.0

See notes Table 1

In publications, we see a difference in favor of men in all the fields, so productivity of male researchers has developed stronger; in citations the differences are rather small, sometimes in favor of male researchers, sometimes in favor of female. Do we have to reject the null-hypothesis that men and women perform equally well? In order to test this, we use a Mann–Whitney test. With this we can test whether the performance distributions for male and female researchers are different, and whether the medians are different. The results are in Table 4.

Table 4.

Gendered performance differences—mid-career researchers

P Frac P NCS NCS3y TOP1% TOP5% TOP10% TOP25% TOP50%
All
Mean rank (male) 126.7 133.1 115.8 115.0 119.8 120.9 120.7 112.4 112.3
Mean rank (female) 105.2 97.4 119.4 119.8 113.6 112.3 112.3 121.4 122.8
Mann–Whitney U 5476 4666 6566 6469 6363 6226 6229 6148.5 6113
Wilcoxon W 11,041 10,231 14,822 14,725 11,928 11,791 11,794 14,277 14,369
Z −2.432 −4.012 −.301 −.490 −.950 −1.043 −.977 −1.020 −1.186
Sign. distributionsa 0.015 0.00 0.76 0.62 0.34 0.30 0.33 0.31 0.24
Sign. mediana 0.24 0.05 0.95 0.59 0.45 0.51 0.32 0.29 0.27
Economics
Mean rank (male) 54.60 54.04 49.28 49.16 50.49 50.98 51.42 48.12 48.89
Mean rank (female) 36.09 37.56 50.07 50.39 46.89 45.61 44.44 51.28 51.11
Mann–Whitney U 596.5 636 943 934.5 888 853.5 822 883.5 915
Wilcoxon W 974.5 1014 3499 3490.5 1266 1231.5 1200 3368.5 3471
Z −2.887 −2.565 −.123 −.191 −.951 −1.012 −1.131 −.495 −.346
Sign. distributionsa 0.004 0.01 0.90 0.85 0.34 0.31 0.26 0.62 0.73
Sign. mediana 0.06 0.024 0.65 0.37 0.47 0.32 0.18 0.61 1.0
Behavioral and educational science
Mean rank (male) 28.97 31.24 21.53 21.71 25.35 25.06 23.82 22.24 20.59
Mean rank (female) 19.38 18.00 23.89 23.79 21.57 21.75 22.50 23.46 24.46
Mann–Whitney U 136.5 98 213 216 198 203 224 225 197
Wilcoxon W 542.5 504 366 369 604 609 630 378 350
Z −2.378 −3.277 −0.585 −0.515 −1.141 −0.847 −0.332 −0.305 −0.960
Sign. distributionsa 0.017 0.001 0.56 0.61 0.25 0.40 0.74 0.76 0.34
Sign. mediana 0.01 0.01 0.62 0.62 0.42 0.47 0.91 0.91 0.27
Psychology
Mean rank (male) 54.08 56.61 49.23 49.00 48.43 50.13 48.94 46.13 45.61
Mean rank (female) 38.64 36.61 42.52 42.70 43.16 41.80 42.75 45.00 45.41
Mann–Whitney U 657 555.5 851 860 883 815 862.5 975 995.5
Wilcoxon W 1932 1830.5 2126 2135 2158 2090 2137.5 2250 2270.5
Z −2.787 −3.609 −1.210 −1.137 −1.187 −1.557 −1.125 −0.203 −0.037
Sign. distributionsa 0.005 0.00 0.23 0.26 0.24 0.12 0.26 0.84 0.97
Sign. mediana 0.006 0.001 0.53 0.29 0.36 0.53 0.83 0.83 0.53

Gender = grouping variable; N = 134 (male), N = 109 (female)

See notes Table 1

aTwo tailed

First for the whole sample: As far as (fractionalized) publications are concerned, the median performance and the distribution of performance of women are lower than the median and distribution of the male researchers. However, in all (field normalized) citations-based impact indicators, the differences are small, non-significant, and sometimes women score higher, sometimes men—as Table 4 (part: All) shows. For the three fields separately, the pattern is about the same—although in the case of psychology, male researchers have also in the citation-based indicators a somewhat (but non-significant) higher score.

In other words, the null hypothesis that men and women perform equal impact-wise cannot be rejected for most of the indicators. But, we have to reject the hypotheses that productivity is equal among the sexes. Indeed, the productivity gap has increased over the 10 years as becomes visible when comparing Table 1 and Table 3. The Male/Female productivity ratio was 1.09 (mean) and 1 (median), around 2004, and 9 years later the figures were 1.17 (mean) and 1.31 (median). Male researchers now publish on average about 17 % more publications than female researchers, and the former have a 31 % higher median production. For fractional counted publication the increase of the difference is even substantially higher, as is the case for the individual disciplines.8

Performance and careers

After analyzing the development of male and female research performance, the second question can be addressed: does performance determine academic careers? We collected data about the current position of the researchers in our dataset. Figure 1 shows that gender differences in career are still obvious.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Position by gender

Does performance explain the differences in careers, or gender? In order to test this, we use the SPSS routine nonparametric tests to compare the careers of male and female researchers, as the distributions obviously are skewed. The following Table 5 shows the results: The null hypothesis that the career levels of male and female researchers are equal has to be rejected—and the differences are statistically significant (at p = 0.10). In this and the following analyses, we have not included those that left higher education. And because of the small size of the group of senior researchers, we took those together with the class of researchers.

Table 5.

Average function level by gender

Average N
All fields* Male 13.7 Almost associate professor 125
Female 12.6 Slightly above assistant professor 98
Economics** Male 14.0 Associate professor 69
Female 13.2 Halfway assistant and associate professor 26
Behavioral and educational science** Male 13.2 Halfway assistant and associate professor 17
Female 12.2 Slightly above assistant professor 25
Psychology* Male 13.6 Almost associate professor 39
Female 12.4 Slightly above assistant professor 47

* Sign < 0.01; ** Sign < 0.10

In terms of careers, male researchers score in average 13.7—almost associate professor. Female researchers score in average 12.6—slightly above assistant professor. Associate professor is also the median position for male applicants, whereas assistant professor is the median for women; so the gender difference is substantial. For the disciplines separately, we find about the same differences, with economics slightly higher and behavioral & education research slightly lower than psychology. Figure 2 shows that the unequal pattern is also visible at the level of the various disciplines separately.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Position by gender by discipline (%)

Taking only one variable into account (gender) gives an incomplete picture. In a meritocratic system one would expect that research performance has a strong influence on careers. As the gender differences are in the same (small) order of magnitude for all citation-based indicators, we selected only one impact variable (field normalized citations, with a citation window) for the model, plus productivity. We use fractional counted productivity as we expect panel and committee members to account in some imprecise way for the number of coauthors.

Apart from the performance variables, a few other variables should be taken into account. As we have three academic age groups and relative young researchers, the year of PhD may be a factor: the younger, the less chance to promote to higher positions. The average differences are small—only 1 year—between the groups (2003, 2004, 2005), but as we only consider a 10-years period, a difference of 1 or 2 years is substantial. Another factor may be mobility. If mobility influences the career, and men are more mobile than women, this together may lead to gender differences in careers. We distinguish three levels of mobility: (i) no mobility, (ii) national mobility, and (iii) international mobility. Finally, labor academic markets may de different for the different fields under study. Therefore we also use discipline of the researcher as variable.

We test a model with the researchers’ early 2014 achieved career level as dependent variable, and as independent variables performance (productivity and citations), gender, mobility, academic age, and discipline. Table 6 gives the result of the analysis. For the nominal variables, the effect is tested against one of the conditions: male against female; 2003 and 2004 against 2005; economics and education & behavior against psychology; no mobility and national mobility against international mobility. The fit of the model is good, as the deviance is 555.552 with 879 degrees of freedom.

Table 6.

Career level by gender, age, mobility and performance

Parameter B Std. Error 95 % Wald Confidence Interval Hypothesis Test Exp(B)
Lower Upper Wald Chi2 df Sig.
Threshold
Position = 10.00 −1.143 0.5587 −2.238 −0.048 4.185 1 0.041 0.319
Position = 11.00 1.265 0.4748 0.335 2.196 7.099 1 0.008 3.544
Position = 12.00 3.044 0.5075 2.050 4.039 35.983 1 0.000 20.995
Position = 14.00 4.826 0.5600 3.728 5.923 74.264 1 0.000 124.67
Economics versus psychology 1.523 0.3249 0.886 2.160 21.982 1 0.000 4.587
Behavior & edu versus psychology −0.411 0.3612 −1.119 0.297 1.296 1 0.255 0.663
Year = 2003 versus 2005 0.578 0.3327 −0.075 1.230 3.014 1 0.083 1.782
Year = 2004 versus 2005 0.489 0.2917 −0.082 1.061 2.814 1 0.093 1.631
Mobility: no versus international −0.073 0.3496 −0.758 0.612 0.044 1 0.835 0.930
Mobility: national vs international 0.080 0.3742 −0.654 0.813 0.046 1 0.831 1.083
FracP 0.305 0.0473 0.212 0.398 41.586 1 0.000 1.357
NCS2y 0.356 0.1682 0.026 0.686 4.471 1 0.034 1.427
Male versus female 0.549 0.2804 0.000 1.099 3.835 1 0.050 1.732
Scale 1a

Ordinal response (multinomial) model with a cumulative logit link function

Dependent Variable: achieved rank early 2014

Model: (Threshold), FracP, NCSf2y, year, gender, discipline, mobility

Model fit: deviance = 555.552, df = 879

aFixed at the displayed value

First of all, the labor market seems better and careers seem faster in economics than in psychology, and in psychology better than in education & behavior.

Secondly, the year group variable has an effect on the achieved career level: if one belongs to the older cohorts 2003 and 2004, the average achieved career level is higher than for those in the cohort 2005. This is of course expected: the longer the career, the more time one has had for moving up.

Thirdly, mobility has only a small (and non-significant) effect. Note that the small effect does follow what would be expected: international mobility is slightly better than national mobility, which is slightly better than no mobility.

Fourthly, scholarly performance has a statistically significant positive effect on the career.

Finally, the analysis shows that gender has an effect on achieved career level as male researchers have achieved a higher average career level than female researchers, when controlling for all other variables.

Conclusions and discussion

Quite some research literature focuses on gender bias in career and grant decisions. Unfortunately most of these studies do not control for possible performance differences, which makes conclusions about gender bias impossible. In this paper we provide an alternative to this practice as we combine data on performance with data on careers and mobility. Our sample consists of researchers who around 10 years ago were all about 3 years after their PhD, and as Table 1 and 2 show, female and male researchers had on average about the same performance. Using better data and more sophisticated indicators than the earlier paper (Van Arensbergen et al. 2012) we come to the same conclusion. The analysis confirms that gender performance differences in the early career may be disappearing, a result that sharply contrasts to what has been concluded from earlier studies.

The dataset enabled us to investigate whether gender differences in performance and career have developed over time. The average impact of papers of male and female researchers remained about equal. But in a period of some 10 years, the productivity of the male researchers has become higher than of female researchers. This divergence suggests that we observe a change in the academic life cycle (differences emerge later) than a generational effect (no differences any more in the current generation).

In a second step, we analyzed the development of careers, and we find that male researchers had a much better career in the first about 13 years after their PhD than female researchers. In our sample, about 61 % of the male researchers became full or associate professor within this period. For female researchers this was half of that: 32 %. When also taking into account academic age, performance, mobility and discipline, the picture remains the same: gender has considerable effect on the achieved career level. Obviously, gender bias seems to prevail in academic hiring, as the differences in career development cannot be explained in terms of performance indicators only—next to objective differences such as cohort and discipline.

The most burning question concerns the mechanisms underlying performance and career differences. Why these differences in productivity and career have emerged remains an issue for further research. However, several possibilities can be discerned.

  1. Mobility had no significant effect on careers. This may change if one takes the quality of the mobility into account, by distinguishing the affiliations in terms of the ranking of the institution (e.g., in the Leiden Ranking), and distinguishing between upward and downward mobility.

  2. We also showed that differences in career partly are an effect of gender bias. This may be the effect of organizational processes and procedures, which were not under study in this paper. But they remain an important research topic.

  3. The observed productivity differences may be spurious and an effect of different topic choice within the disciplines. By using field-adjusted productivity indicators (Sandström and Wold 2015) one may be able to test this, at disaggregated level. Together with other improvements such as the deployment of size-dependent indicators9 (Van den Besselaar & Sandström 2015), we will further investigate this in a coming article.

  4. Another possible explanation for gender differences in research productivity relates to marriage and family building. However, the research literature is quite consistent: Several studies show that the academic career of female scientists is not necessarily affected negatively by parenthood (Fox & Faver 1985; Cole & Zuckerman 1991; Astin & Bayer 1979; Dryler 2011), and therefore we wouldn’t search in this direction for explanations. Some analyses indicate that for specific academic positions, e.g. assistant professor, there is a slight advantage for women to have children of school age (Dryler 2011). One should be aware that these findings come from studies mainly covering the US and the Nordic countries. Despite the fact that all other circumstances are about the same, i.e. modern, highly developed nations with strong science systems, the situation in the country under study (the Netherlands) may be different and could be further investigated.

  5. Alternatively, there are good arguments to search for explanations in the area of self-selection. Men and women tend to leave the academic career for other options due to different reasons, among them structural discrimination on the labor market. Gender differences can roughly be described in terms of men searching for a new career and higher pay, while women move away from academia because they are dissatisfied with their working conditions and more seldom find an alternative career (Dryler 2011). As a consequence, lower performing men may leave academia for another career more often than lower performing female researchers who may more often continue their academic career. This kind of patterns may explain our findings.

  6. A still not very much explored set of models relates to team collaboration and co-author relations, which could moderate the effect between gender and performance (Badar et al., 2014; Verbree et al., 2015).

  7. We showed that differences in career partly are an effect of gender bias. Apart from that, performance differences play a role. However, the growing gender difference in productivity may itself be an effect of bias. It could be well the case that in the beginning equally performing men and women enter—through gender stereotyping and biased decisions—into diverging early career trajectories (e.g., lower and teaching oriented positions versus higher and more research oriented positions) which in turn leads to increasing productivity differences. And these increased performance differences together with existing gender bias may reinforce the career differences between male and female researchers. These possible explanations are as many hypotheses for further research.

Acknowledgments

This paper is a follow-up study of Van Arensbergen et al., (2012). The authors are grateful to Charlie Mom for his effort in the manual collection of the data, to Pleun van Arensbergen for her contribution in collecting and reanalyzing the new data (Van Arensbergen 2014), and to two reviewers for useful comments on a previous version. The authors acknowledge support from the ERC through the GendERC grant (610706), and the Riksbankens Jubileumsfond (P12-1302:1).

Footnotes

1

This may be the strongest policy effect of studies on gender bias: such studies focus attention on the problem, and force councils to act and to try to solve it. Despite these efforts, the causal mechanisms under gender bias are not taken away. Consequently, without permanent attention gender bias can be expected to creep into the system again.

2

This section is based on Van Arensbergen, et al. (2012).

3

Although there is evidence suggesting that the effect of marital status is less univocal (Fox 2005).

4

The processes described in this paragraph may not take place everywhere and also not in the same way or speed.

5

For most applicants we have a publication list obtained from their own website or the universities’ website. However, researchers differ in what they include in that list. So these lists can only be used to correct the retrieved publications, and not as a reliable source as such.

6

One of these years depending on the researcher: the data come from career grant applications over a three years period. As the applications have to be done within three years after obtaining the PhD, the year reflects the academic age. One or two years longer may influence the overall performance until 2014, and also the career steps made.

7

In (Van Arensbergen et al. 2012) the data were based on automatic coupling of application data with Social Sciences Citation Index publication data with a Dutch address, using family name and first initial. This procedure accepts some error based on homonyms and synonyms. Additionally, some applicants use different first initials in applications compared to publications, and in some cases the academic titles were taken for first names (for the last point: Van den Besselaar et al. 2014). We recollected all data manually in order to avoid these problems. In contrast to the previous version, we also extended the search, as we now also included non-Dutch addresses and Science Citation Index-expanded publications. Finally, we now use an equal citation window (up to 31-12-2006; and a 2-years citation window). The results of the analyses and the conclusions remain highly similar, suggesting that the differences even out. The main difference is that in (Van Arensbergen et al. 2012) we found that women seem to outperform men in the top of the distribution. The new and better data and analysis suggest equal performance (see the findings section below).

8

Table 1 and Table 3 also report the figures for the individual disciplines. For economics, the men were on average 28 % more productive in the initial measurement, and ten years later this has increased to 50 %. For psychology, the corresponding figures are 21 % and 59 %.

9

Size-dependent indicators measure e.g. the absolute number of top papers, and not the share of top papers within the oeuvre of a researcher.

References

  1. Abramo G, D’Angelo CA, Caprasecca A. Gender differences in research productivity: A bibliometric analysis of the Italian academic system. Scientometrics. 2009;79(3):517–539. doi: 10.1007/s11192-007-2046-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allison PD, Long JS. Departmental effects on scientific productivity. American Sociological Review. 1990;55(4):469–478. doi: 10.2307/2095801. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Astin HS, Bayer A. Pervasive sex differences in the academic reward system: scholarship, marriage, and what else? In: Lewis DR, Becker WE, editors. Academic Rewards in Higher Education. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger; 1979. pp. 211–229. [Google Scholar]
  4. Badar K, Hite JM, Badir YF. Examining the relationship of co-authorship network centrality and gender on academic research performance: the case of chemistry researchers in Pakistan. Scientometrics. 2013;94(2):755–775. doi: 10.1007/s11192-012-0764-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bland CJ, Center BA, Finstad DA, Risbey KR, Staples J. The impact of appointment type on the productivity and commitment of full-time faculty in research and doctoral institutions. The Journal of Higher Education. 2006;77(1):89–123. doi: 10.1353/jhe.2006.0002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brouns M. The gendered nature of assessment procedures in scientific research funding: The dutch case. Higher Education in Europe. 2000;25:193–201. doi: 10.1080/713669261. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buchmann C, DiPrete TA, McDaniel A. Gender inequalities in education. Annual Review of Sociology. 2008;34:319–337. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.34.040507.134719. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carayol N, Matt M. Individual and collective determinants of academic scientists on productivity. Information Economics and Policy. 2006;18:55–72. doi: 10.1016/j.infoecopol.2005.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ceci SJ, Ginther DK, Kahn S, Williams WM. Women in Academic Science: A Changing Landscape. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2014;15(3):75–141. doi: 10.1177/1529100614541236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cole JR and Zuckerman H (1984). The productivity puzzle: persistence and change in patterns of publication of men and women scientists. In: P Maehr and MW Steinkamp. Advances in motivation and achievement 2. Greenwich, JAI Press. 217-258.
  11. Cole JR and Zuckerman H (1991). Marriage, Motherhood, and Research Performance in Science. In: H Zuckerman, JR Cole and JT Bauer. The outer circle: Women in the scientific community. New York/London: Norton. [First published in Scientific American, 1987]. [DOI] [PubMed]
  12. Dryler H (2011). Forskarkarriär för både kvinnor och män?—statistisk uppföljning och kunskapsöversikt (In Swedish). [Research Career for both women and men?] Högskoleverket—Swedish National Agency for Higher Education Report 2011:6 R [www.hsv.se].
  13. Dundar H, Lewis DR. Determinants of research productivity in higher education. Research in Higher Education. 1998;39(6):607–630. doi: 10.1023/A:1018705823763. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Egghe L. The dependence of the height of a Lorenz curve of a Zipf function on the size of the system. Mathematical and computer modeling. 2006;43:870–879. doi: 10.1016/j.mcm.2005.09.033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fox MF. Gender, family characteristics, and publication productivity among scientists. Social Studies of Science. 2005;35(1):131–150. doi: 10.1177/0306312705046630. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fox MF, Faver CA. Men, women, and publication productivity: patterns among social work academics. Sociological Quarterly. 1985;26(4):537–549. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.1985.tb00243.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fox MF, Fonseca C, Bao JH. Work and family conflict in academic science: Patterns and predictors among women and men in research universities. Social Studies of Science. 2011;41(5):715–735. doi: 10.1177/0306312711417730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fuchs S, von Stebut J, Allmendinger J. Gender, science, and scientific organizations in Germany. Minerva. 2001;39(2):175–201. doi: 10.1023/A:1010380510013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hunter LA, Leahey E. Parenting and research productivity: New evidence and methods. Social Studies of Science. 2010;40(3):433–451. doi: 10.1177/0306312709358472. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Karamessini M. Women’s representation and progression in science careers in Greece. Athens: KETHI Research Centre for Gender Equality; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  21. Kyvik S, Teigen M. Child care, research collaboration and gender differences in scientific productivity. Science, Technology and Human Values. 1996;21(1):54–71. doi: 10.1177/016224399602100103. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Landino RA, Owen SV. Self-efficacy in university faculty. Journal of Vocational Behavior. 1988;33:1–14. doi: 10.1016/0001-8791(88)90030-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Leahey E. Gender differences in productivity—research specialization as a missing link. Gender and Society. 2006;20(6):754–780. doi: 10.1177/0891243206293030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ledin A, Bornmann L, Gannon F, Wallon G. A persistent problem - traditional gender roles hold back female scientists. EMBO Reports. 2007;8(11):982–987. doi: 10.1038/sj.embor.7401109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lee S, Bozeman B. The impact of research collaboration on scientific productivity. Social Studies of Science. 2005;35(5):673–702. doi: 10.1177/0306312705052359. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Long JS. Measures of sex differences in scientific productivity. Social Forces. 1992;71(1):159–178. doi: 10.1093/sf/71.1.159. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Marsh HW, Bornmann L, Mutz R, Daniel HD, O’Mara A. Gender effects in the peer reviews of grant proposals: A comprehensive meta-analysis comparing traditional and multilevel approaches. Review of Educational Research. 2009;79:1290–1326. doi: 10.3102/0034654309334143. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. McNamee SJ, Willis CL, Rotchford AM. Gender differences in patterns of publication in leading sociology journals, 1960–1985. The American Sociologist. 1990;21(2):99–115. doi: 10.1007/BF02692855. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Miller DI & Wai J (2015). The bachelor’s to Ph.D. STEM pipeline no longer leaks more women than men: a 30-year analysis. Frontiers in Psychology 6, Article 37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  30. Mutz R, Bornmann L, Daniel H-D. Does Gender Matter in Grant Peer Review? An Empirical Investigation Using the Example of the Austrian Science Fund. Zeitschrift für Psychologie. 2012;220(2):121–129. doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Nakhaie MR. Gender differences in publication among university professors in Canada. Canadian Review of Sociology and Anthropology-Revue Canadienne de Sociologie et d’Anthropologie. 2002;39(2):151–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-618X.2002.tb00615.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pekkarinen T. Gender differences in educational attainment: Evidence on the role of tracking from a Finnish quasi-experiment. Scandinavian Journal of Economics. 2008;110(4):807–825. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9442.2008.00562.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Penas CS, Willett P. Gender differences in publication and citation counts in librarianship and information science research. Journal of Information Science. 2006;32(5):480–485. doi: 10.1177/0165551506066058. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Powell A, Hassan TM, Dainty ARJ, Carter C. Exploring gender differences in construction research: A European perspective. Construction Management and Economics. 2009;27(9):803–807. doi: 10.1080/01446190903179736. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Prozesky H. A career-history analysis of gender differences in publication productivity among South African academics. Science Studies. 2008;21(2):47–67. [Google Scholar]
  36. Prpic K. Gender and productivity differentials in science. Scientometrics. 2002;55(1):27–58. doi: 10.1023/A:1016046819457. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Puuska HM. Effects of scholar’s gender and professional position on publishing productivity in different publication types. Analysis of a Finnish university. Scientometrics. 2010;82(2):419–437. [Google Scholar]
  38. Sandström, U. (2009a). Bibliometric evaluation of research programs: a study of scientific quality. Stockholm: Swedish Environmental Protection Agency, Report 6321.
  39. Sandström U. Combining curriculum vitae and bibliometric analysis: Mobility, gender and research performance. Research Evaluation. 2009;18(2):135–142. doi: 10.3152/095820209X441790. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Sandström, U. (2014). Bibliometric evaluation of SEPA-funded large research programs 20032013. Stockholm: Swedish Environmental Protection Agency. Report,6636, doi:10.13140/2.1.5191.248710.13140/2.1.5191.2487.
  41. Sandström, U., &Wold, A. (2015). “Centres of excellence: Reward for gender or top-level research”, Thinking ahead: Research, funding and the future. In RJ yearbook 2015/16. Stockholm: Riksbankens Jubileumsfond and Makadam.
  42. Sandström U, Hällsten M. Persistent nepotism in peer-review. Scientometrics. 2008;74(2):175–189. doi: 10.1007/s11192-008-0211-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sandström U, Sandström E. The field factor: towards a metric for academic institutions. Research Evaluation. 2009;18(3):243–250. doi: 10.3152/095820209X466892. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Snell C, Sorensen J, Rodriguez JJ, Kuanliang A. Gender differences in research productivity among criminal justice and criminology scholars. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2009;37(3):288–295. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2009.04.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Symonds MRE, Gemmell NJ, Braisher TL, Gorringe KL, Elga MA. Gender differences in publication output: Towards an unbiased metric of research performance. PLoS ONE. 2006;1(1):e127. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Taylor SW, Fender BF, et al. Unraveling the academic productivity of economists: The opportunity costs of teaching and service. Southern Economic Journal. 2006;72(4):846–859. doi: 10.2307/20111856. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Timmers TM, Willemsen TM, Tijdens KG. Gender diversity policies in universities: A multi-perspective framework of policy measures. Higher Education. 2010;59(6):719–735. doi: 10.1007/s10734-009-9276-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Tower G, Plummer J, Ridgewall B. A multidisciplinary study of gender based research productivity in the worlds’ best journals. Journal of Diversity Management. 2007;2(4):23–32. [Google Scholar]
  49. Van Arensbergen P, Van der Weijden I, Van den Besselaar P. Gender differences in scientific productivity, a persisting phenomenon? Scientometrics. 2012;93:857–868. doi: 10.1007/s11192-012-0712-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Van Balen B, Van Arensbergen P, Van der Weijden I, Van den Besselaar P. Determinants of academic careers. Higher Education Policy. 2012;25:313–334. doi: 10.1057/hep.2012.14. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Van den Besselaar P, Leydesdorff L. Past performance, peer review, and project selection: A case study in the social and behavioral sciences. Research Evaluation. 2009;18:273–288. doi: 10.3152/095820209X475360. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Van den Besselaar P, Sandström U. Early career grants, performance, and careers: A study on predictive validity of grant decisions. Journal of Informetrics. 2015;9(4):826–838. doi: 10.1016/j.joi.2015.07.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Van den Besselaar P, Bornmann L, Leydesdorff L. Letter to the editor: Correction. Journal of Informetrics. 2014;8(4):801. doi: 10.1016/j.joi.2014.07.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Van den Brink M (2009). Behind the scenes of science: Gender in the recruitment and selection of professors in the Netherlands. PhD thesis. Nijmegen: Radboud Universiteit.
  55. Van den Brink M, Brouns M, Waslander S. Does excellence have a gender? A national research study on recruitment and gender. Employee Relations. 2006;28:523–539. doi: 10.1108/01425450610704470. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Verbree M, Horlings E, Groenewegen P, Van der Weijden I, Van den Besselaar P. Organizational factors influencing scholarly performance: a multivariate study of biomedical research groups. Scientometrics. 2015;102:25–49. doi: 10.1007/s11192-014-1437-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wennerås C, Wold A. Nepotism and sexism in peer-review. Nature. 1997;387:341–343. doi: 10.1038/387341a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Xie Y, Shauman KA. Sex differences in research productivity: New evidence about an old puzzle. American Sociological Review. 1998;63(6):847–870. doi: 10.2307/2657505. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Zuckerman H. The careers of men and women scientists: Gender differences in career attainment. In: Wyer M, editor. Women, science and technology: A reader in feminist science studies. New York: Routledge; 2001. pp. 69–78. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scientometrics are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES