Abstract
Objectives
The source of research may influence one's interpretation of it in either negative or positive ways, however, there are no robust experiments to determine how source impacts on one's judgment of the research article. We determine the impact of source on respondents’ assessment of the quality and relevance of selected research abstracts.
Design
Web-based survey design using four healthcare research abstracts previously published and included in Cochrane Reviews.
Setting
All Council on the Education of Public Health-accredited Schools and Programmes of Public Health in the USA.
Participants
899 core faculty members (full, associate and assistant professors)
Intervention
Each of the four abstracts appeared with a high-income source half of the time, and low-income source half of the time. Participants each reviewed the same four abstracts, but were randomly allocated to receive two abstracts with high-income source, and two abstracts with low-income source, allowing for within-abstract comparison of quality and relevance
Primary outcome measures
Within-abstract comparison of participants’ rating scores on two measures—strength of the evidence, and likelihood of referral to a peer (1–10 rating scale). OR was calculated using a generalised ordered logit model adjusting for sociodemographic covariates.
Results
Participants who received high income country source abstracts were equal in all known characteristics to the participants who received the abstracts with low income country sources. For one of the four abstracts (a randomised, controlled trial of a pharmaceutical intervention), likelihood of referral to a peer was greater if the source was a high income country (OR 1.28, 1.02 to 1.62, p<0.05).
Conclusions
All things being equal, in one of the four abstracts, the respondents were influenced by a high-income source in their rating of research abstracts. More research may be needed to explore how the origin of a research article may lead to stereotype activation and application in research evaluation.
Keywords: Peer Review, Evidence based medicine, Bias, Diffusion of Innovation
Strengths and limitations of this study.
First study at the national level in the USA, to determine the impact of country-of-origin on the rating of healthcare research abstracts.
All core faculty members (full, associate and assistant professors) of every Council on the Education of Public Health-accredited Schools and Programmes of Public Health in the USA were invited to participate in the study.
Participants were blinded to the purpose of the study and randomised to receive high-income or low-income source abstracts.
Abstracts were rated on strength of the evidence and likelihood of referral to a peer.
Although 899 full, associate and assistant professors participated in the study, this corresponded to a 9.8% response rate.
Background
Ideally, research findings ought to be judged on the strength of the evidence and their relevance. However, there is subjectivity involved in interpreting research.1 Research certainly does not ‘speak for itself’—we give it a voice—and how we judge whether one piece of research constitutes evidence or not is complex and messy. Common standards for assessing the internal validity of research do not account for the potential cognitive biases in the consumption and interpretation of research postpublication, and each of us may reach a different conclusion as to whether the research presents strong evidence and whether we consider the research useful. In practice, we see many idiosyncracies. A rigorous randomised controlled trial (RCT) may convince a surgeon to change a certain practice, but may not have the same effect on a primary care physician.2 Government regulators consider the reliability (the degree to which an innovation is communicated as being consistent in its results) of an innovation more positively than do industrial scientists.3 Clinicians are more likely to adopt an innovation if they believe it has come from current users with similar professional, cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds.4 A legitimate source is important for innovation diffusion,5 6 but little is known about how legitimacy is defined or perceived. From the marketing literature, Bilkey and Nes5 showed that consumers tend to rate products from their own countries more favourably and that consumer preferences are positively correlated with the degree of economic development of the source country, probably evoked by the lower price cue of low income country products.7 Up to 30% of the variance of consumer product ratings can be attributed to the product's country-of-origin.8
In healthcare research, typically, the first pieces of information to be provided in a research article are the author's name, and the institution and country the research has been conducted in. Understanding anchoring to be a feature of heuristic thought,9–13 it follows that we should examine the extent to which the source affects our interpretation of that research. If one possesses a prior-held belief or attitude towards the source, how does this influence one's subsequent view of the research? All things being equal, would research conducted in Ethiopia be viewed in the same way as identical research conducted in the USA?14
The income and development level of the source country certainly seems to determine whether a manuscript is selected for publication.15 The number of publications from low income countries is significantly lower than the number from developed countries, in various research fields.15 16 In psychiatry, only 6% of the literature is published from regions that represent 90% of the global population.17 Similar under-representation exists in cardiology, HIV research and epidemiology.18 19 One argument for this is that research from low income countries (LICs) lacks the quality to meet publication criteria.20 Others argue that there are systematic selection biases. Editorial board members of international biomedical journals are more likely to come from high income countries (HICs).21–23 Reviewers from OECD (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development) countries view articles from their own country more favourably than they do articles from other countries.22 24 25 Studies recruiting participants from the USA are more likely to be published.21 23 In Peters and Ceci's26 controversial experiment, only one of the nine articles that were initially published in a highly regarded American journal was accepted on resubmission to the same journal after fabricating the name of the original institutions. Kliewer et al27 demonstrated that articles from outside of North America were less likely to be accepted for publication. It seems that source matters.
The major obstacle to this research question is that there are no controlled studies to ascertain the impact of the source of the research post-publication. To fill this research gap, we present here a randomised trial of Public Health research faculty in the USA. This national survey invites respondents, most of whom are experienced healthcare researchers and peer reviewers, to rate identical, typical healthcare research abstracts. To ascertain the impact of the source (institution and country) of the abstracts, we ensured that the abstracts that the respondents received were identical in every respect, however, we fictionalised the sources into either HIC or LIC and randomised the respondents to receive either type. We then compared their responses to two simple questions for each abstract—whether they think the evidence in the abstract is strong, and whether they would recommend the abstract to a peer. Under the null hypothesis, there should be no difference in the distribution of responses to the two types of abstract.
Methods
Survey design
We used a web-based survey using a Qualtrics survey platform. The survey was divided into two sections, the first to collect demographic and professional data, and the second for the respondent to read and respond to four research abstracts. Each abstract was followed by the same two questions—first, how strong is the evidence presented in this abstract? And second, how likely are you to recommend this abstract to a colleague? Responses were on a scale (1–10) with 1 as the least (ie, not at all strong, not at all likely) and 10 as the most (extremely strong, extremely likely). The time taken to read and respond to each abstract was measured by the survey platform. Each question was forced response to avoid the problem of missing data. Recipients were randomly allocated to one of two possible surveys. In the first, abstracts 1 and 4 were fictionalised to HIC sources (UK and Germany) and abstracts 2 and 3 were fictionalised to LIC sources (Malawi and Ethiopia). These sources were reversed in the second survey. Therefore, each survey (survey A and survey B) had two abstracts from LIC sources and two from HIC sources (figure 1).
In order to ensure that the abstracts were of sufficient quality and internal validity, we purposively selected abstracts of papers that had been included in Cochrane Reviews and that were also likely to be of at least some interest to most public health academics and health service researchers. Each abstract had therefore already been vetted for sources of bias prior to publication, using the Cochrane risk of bias tool, and we only selected abstracts that had a high internal validity for the type of study that it was describing. There is a trade off between choosing abstracts of interest to all potential respondents and the length of the survey. We decided to choose four abstracts—one randomised controlled pharmaceutical trial, one randomised controlled service intervention, one pharmaceutical intervention of cross-sectional design and one service intervention of cross-sectional design—to give a balance in terms of content and design. All four abstracts were of similar length and complexity. The abstracts were presented as found in their PubMed format, with all technical content preserved and in a format familiar to any healthcare researcher, however, for each abstract, the institution and country of origin was fictionalised to one of four different high-income or low-income sources. For one abstract, the trial acronym was removed to avoid the possibility that some respondents would recognise the research. High-income source countries were selected from the top 10 countries by gross domestic profit (GDP) per capita (>$36 000 per capita), and OECD membership. Low-income source countries were selected from the bottom 10 countries by GDP per capita (<$1046 per capita). The institutional affiliation was fictionalised to one of the top-five universities that also had a medical or healthcare faculty, in the respective countries. We used the 2014 Times Higher Education World rankings (http://www.timeshighereducation.co.uk/world-university-rankings/2014-15/world-ranking) for the HIC sources, and the http://www.4icu.org website for international rankings of institutions for the low-income sources.
We ensured that the source of the abstract was equally visible in each abstract and was mentioned in at least three locations throughout the abstract—the title, under the title and in the abstract itself. To avoid a possible order effect, the order in which the abstracts were presented in the survey was randomised for each participant. Neither the original nor the fictionalised journals were included in the source in order to avoid respondents reacting to the reputation of the publication type. Furthermore, in order to not influence the responses, the survey was described as a Speed Reading survey, designed to examine whether the time taken to read an abstract influences the interpretation of the information within it. The survey platform enabled us to measure the time taken to respond to the entire survey, and each abstract, and this information was provided to the respondent at the end of the survey to heighten the ‘psychological realism’ of the survey. The survey was pilot-tested with Masters in Public Health students at Imperial College London and some faculty members at New York University, to ensure face validity of the questions and also to ensure that the design and flow of the survey were straightforward.
Participants and survey management
We included all core faculty members of Schools and Programmes of Public Health—located in a US state—that had publically available contact information and that were accredited by the Council on the Education of Public Health (CEPH—http://ceph.org/accredited) (159 institutions; see online supplementary appendix 1 for full listing). We excluded administrators, managers, adjunct faculty members and visiting faculty members, and faculty members from our own institution. From this universe of potential respondents (n=9421 once duplicates were removed), we randomised them to receive either survey A or survey B and sent them an invite to take the survey. Block randomisation within respective institutions was used, with four, six and eight sequences, from a web-based randomisation service (http://www.sealedenvelope.com, seed 137526655595533).
The survey was designed so that only the email recipient could open the link to the survey and that it could be taken only one time. The survey could not be sent anonymously, and was inaccessible to search engines. The survey was active only within the specified time frame (20 January–4 February 2015, chosen so that faculty members were highly likely to be present at their institution), and two email reminders were sent on day 7 and day 14 following the first email invite (20 January 2015). Panel members did not receive prior invitation to participate in the survey, however, our email invite indicated clearly that all responses were to be de-identified and analysed in aggregate form only, and solely for the purposes of this research. It also indicated that there was no obligation to participate but by choosing to participate consent to use the response for research is implied. We offered participants entry into a lottery draw for a $500 Amazon voucher as an incentive to complete the survey. The study protocol, including the non-harmful deception around the ulterior motive of the study, was reviewed by the New York University Committee on Activities Involving Human Subjects and deemed exempt from full ethical review (#14-10332).
Statistical analysis and power calculation
Data were retrieved via Qualtrics in CSV format and analysed using Stata/SE V.13 (Statacorp, College Station, Texas, USA). We used demographic covariates (age, sex), professional experience covariates (research exposure, peer review experience, educational attainment) and institutional covariates (region, CEPH accreditation type and Ivy League status) to explain variation in the outcomes of interest. We grouped respondent age into categories based on a presumed mid-year birth and survey completion date of 31 January 2015. Educational attainment was categorised into two groups—Academic and Clinical Academic—based on the completed qualifications provided in the survey responses. We used a generalised ordered logit model for the multivariable analysis and two-tailed t tests to compare the differences in mean responses as well as for the descriptive characteristics of the survey samples. We also explored high and low cut points for the outcome variables in bivariate analysis and illustrate the distribution of scores as proportions of respondents at the high (≥8) and low (≤3) ends of the distribution, using a univariate logistic regression model containing the binary outcome (ie, above/below a certain threshold) and a binary indicator of the abstract’s country of origin. The corresponding test is a Wald test of the β coefficient for the abstract country of origin.
We calculated that sample sizes of 400 respondents for each survey would provide enough power (80%) to detect a statistically significant (95% confidence level) difference of 0.35 in mean scores between the two groups.28
Results
After randomisation, 4711 potential respondents received email-invites for survey A, and 4710 received email-invites for survey B. Fifty-one and 61 invitations bounced, respectively. A total of 567 started survey A and 594 started survey B. Of these, 433 completed survey A and 466 completed survey B. This corresponds to a response rate of 9.2% for survey A and 9.9% for survey B. Institutional characteristics (region and Ivy League representation) of responders and invitees were not significantly different, although there was a small over-representation of responders from CEPH-accredited Programmes in Public Health. The demographic characteristics of the respondents of both surveys were equal, suggesting that randomisation performed as was expected (table 1). Ninety per cent of respondents of both survey types serve as peer reviewers for academic journals.
Table 1.
All respondents (n=899) | Survey A (n=433) | Survey B (n=466) | |
---|---|---|---|
Males, % | 42.05 | 42.49 | 41.63 |
Age, mean | 50.26 | 50.35 | 50.17 |
Academic credentials only*, % | 84.58 | 84.69 | 84.48 |
Clinical credentials†, % | 15.42 | 15.31 | 15.52 |
US born‡, % | 81.65 | 82.68 | 80.69 |
Reads research daily§, % | 60.07 | 61.20 | 59.01 |
CEPH Programme of Public Health¶, % | 35.48 | 34.64 | 36.27 |
Ivy League university**, % | 12.46 | 12.93 | 12.02 |
Region Northeast % | 28.03 | 26.79 | 29.18 |
South % | 42.05 | 43.42 | 40.77 |
Midwest % | 18.24 | 17.32 | 19.1 |
West % | 11.68 | 12.47 | 10.94 |
*For example, BSc, BA, MSc, MPH, PhD.
†For example, MD, MBBS, MBChB.
‡Versus non-US born.
§Versus reads research less than daily.
¶Versus CEPH School of Public Health.
**Versus non-Ivy League institution.
CEPH, Council on the Education of Public Health.
On average, respondents spent between 72.5 and 109.9 s on each abstract with no significant differences between the groups. table 2 shows the mean (SD) ratings for strength and referral for the four abstracts by the type of source. Referral to a peer for abstract 3 (randomised controlled trial of a pharmaceutical intervention) was significantly more likely if the source was from a HIC. There were no other significant differences between the abstracts based on the source. The findings were unchanged when using a proportion rating higher than eight or lower than three. As might be expected, strength rating for abstracts that described a more robust research design, specifically RCTs (abstract 1 and 3), scored higher for strength than abstracts 2 and 4, which were of a cross-sectional design. Also, as might be expected, the disposal of these abstracts also correlated well with respondents’ view of the strength of the evidence contained within them. Correlation between the scores given for strength of evidence and subsequent referral was high (Spearman correlation coefficients varied between 0.71 and 0.85).
Table 2.
Abstract 1 |
Abstract 2 |
Abstract 3 |
Abstract 4 |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Source | High Income | Low Income | All | High Income | Low Income | All | High Income | Low Income | All | High Income | Low Income | All |
Strength | ||||||||||||
Mean (SD) | 5.77 (2.30) | 5.78 (2.11) | 5.77 (2.20) | 4.92 (1.95) | 4.90 (2.04) | 4.91 (1.99) | 6.92 (2.02) | 6.76 (2.03) | 6.84 (2.02) | 3.95 (2.14) | 4.05 (2.06) | 4.00 (2.10) |
≥8 (%) | 27.61 | 24.78 | 26.15 | 10.13 | 12.06 | 11.06 | 47.63 | 43.16 | 45.47 | 6.96 | 4.74 | 5.81 |
≤3 (%) | 22.04 | 18.10 | 20.00 | 27.59 | 30.63 | 29.05 | 8.19 | 9.05 | 8.60 | 48.49 | 45.91 | 47.15 |
Referral | ||||||||||||
Mean (SD) | 5.14 (2.54) | 5.38 (2.36) | 5.27 (2.45) | 4.50 (2.21) | 4.56 (2.26) | 4.53 (2.23) | 6.05* (2.40) | 5.68 (2.45) | 5.87 (2.43) | 3.79 (2.23) | 3.96 (2.21) | 3.88 (2.22) |
≥8 (%) | 21.58 | 23.71 | 22.68 | 10.34 | 11.60 | 10.95 | 32.97 | 27.61 | 30.39 | 7.66 | 7.33 | 7.49 |
≤3 (%) | 30.63 | 24.78 | 27.60 | 36.64 | 37.35 | 36.98 | 17.46 | 21.81 | 19.55 | 51.74 | 46.77 | 49.16 |
Mean time(s) (SD) | 87.4 (68.4) | 87.4 (118) | 87.4 (97.3) | 109.9 (169) | 103.0 (200) | 106.2 (186) | 109.8 (131) | 97.3 (304) | 103.8 (237) | 72.5 (56.4) | 79.4 (146) | 76.0 (112) |
*p<0.05.
Abstract 1, RCT/Service; Abstract 2, Cross-sectional/Service; Abstract 3, RCT/Pharmaceutical; Abstract 4, Cross-sectional/Pharmaceutical.
Tables 3 and 4 show the results of the multivariable analysis. Controlling for individual and institutional covariates, high-income source was a significant predictor of referral for abstract 3 only (OR 1.28, 1.02 to 1.62). For some abstracts, the time spent reviewing the abstract was negatively associated with the rating given to it for strength of evidence (abstract 1 OR 0.49, 0.34 to 0.71; abstract 3 OR 0.65, 0.46 to 0.92) or referral to a peer (abstract 1 OR 0.50, 0.35 to 0.72; abstract 2 OR 0.61, 0.44 to 0.84; abstract 3 OR 0.66, 0.44 to 0.84). However, rating for abstract 4 (both strength of evidence (OR 1.63, 1.06 to 2.51) and referral to a peer (OR 1.55, 1.01 to 2.38) improved when more time was spent on it. Individuals affiliated to CEPH Programmes of Public Health were significantly more likely to rate the strength of the evidence for this abstract higher (OR 1.38, 1.07 to 1.78) and to refer it to colleagues (OR 1.67, 1.30 to 2.15) than individuals affiliated to Schools of Public Health.
Table 3.
Abstract 1 OR 95% CI |
Abstract 2 OR 95% CI |
Abstract 3 OR 95% CI |
Abstract 4 OR 95% CI |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
High vs low country origin | 1.03 0.82 to 1.30 |
1 0.80 to 1.26 |
1.16 0.92 to 1.46 |
0.94 0.74 to 1.18 |
Male (vs female) | 0.93 0.73 to 1.18 |
0.87 0.68 to 1.10 |
0.97 0.76 to 1.23 |
0.87 0.68 to 1.10 |
41–50 years (vs 21–40) | 0.81 0.59 to 1.12 |
0.71* 0.52 to 0.97 |
1.01 0.73 to 1.39 |
0.87 0.64 to 1.20 |
51–60 years (vs 21–40) | 0.79 0.57 to 1.09 |
0.74 0.54 to 1.03 |
1.14 0.82 to 1.58 |
0.77 0.56 to 1.06 |
61+ years (vs 21–40) | 0.76 0.54 to 1.06 |
0.85 0.60 to 1.19 |
1.12 0.80 to 1.57 |
0.82 0.58 to 1.15 |
Clinical academic credentials (vs academic only) | 0.83 0.60 to 1.14 |
0.65** 0.47 to 0.89 |
0.95 0.68 to 1.32 |
0.78 0.57 to 1.08 |
US born (vs not) | 1.06 0.78 to 1.44 |
0.83 0.62 to 1.13 |
0.94 0.69 to 1.28 |
0.89 0.66 to 1.21 |
Reads research daily (vs <daily) | 1.03 0.81 to 1.31 |
0.94 0.74 to 1.20 |
0.85 0.67 to 1.08 |
1.14 0.89 to 1.45 |
CEPH programme (vs school) | 1.12 0.87 to 1.45 |
1.06 0.82 to 1.36 |
1.03 0.80 to 1.32 |
1.38* 1.07 to 1.78 |
Ivy League institution (vs others) | 0.78 0.50 to 1.21 |
0.67 0.43 to 1.06 |
1.14 0.73 to 1.78 |
1.08 0.69 to 1.68 |
South region (vs Northeast) | 0.71 0.50 to 1.00 |
1.08 0.77 to 1.52 |
0.84 0.59 to 1.18 |
1.05 0.74 to 1.47 |
Midwest region (vs Northeast) | 0.82 0.55 to 1.23 |
1.17 0.78 to 1.74 |
1.14 0.76 to 1.71 |
1.07 0.72 to 1.59 |
West region (vs Northeast) | 0.93 0.59 to 1.46 |
1.11 0.72 to 1.74 |
1.05 0.66 to 1.67 |
0.89 0.56 to 1.40 |
60–<120 s spent reading (vs <60 s) | 0.67** 0.51 to 0.87 |
0.87 0.66 to 1.15 |
0.98 0.74 to 1.28 |
1.33* 1.04 to 1.70 |
120+ s spent reading (vs <60 s) | 0.49*** 0.34 to 0.71 |
0.77 0.56 to 1.07 |
0.65* 0.46 to 0.92 |
1.63* 1.06 to 2.51 |
N‡ | 895 | 895 | 895 | 895 |
*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.
†Generalised ordered logit model controlling for all variables in each column.
‡Only survey responses with no missing data included in the multivariate analysis.
Abstract 1, RCT/Service; Abstract 2, Cross-sectional/Service; Abstract 3, RCT/Pharmaceutical; Abstract 4, Cross-sectional/Pharmaceutical.
RCT, randomised controlled trial.
Table 4.
Abstract 1 OR 95% CI |
Abstract 1 OR 95% CI |
Abstract 3 OR 95% CI |
Abstract 4 OR 95% CI |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
High vs low country origin | 0.85 0.67 to 1.07 |
0.94 0.75 to 1.19 |
1.28* 1.02 to 1.62 |
0.9 0.71 to 1.13 |
Male (vs female) | 0.95 0.75 to 1.20 |
0.78* 0.61 to 0.99 |
0.98 0.78 to 1.25 |
0.84 0.66 to 1.06 |
41–50 years (vs 21–40) | 0.98 0.72 to 1.34 |
0.85 0.62 to 1.16 |
1.06 0.77 to 1.46 |
0.83 0.61 to 1.15 |
51–60 years (vs 21–40) | 0.92 0.67 to 1.28 |
0.83 0.60 to 1.15 |
1.15 0.83 to 1.60 |
0.8 0.58 to 1.11 |
61+ years (vs 21–40) | 1.07 0.77 to 1.50 |
1.09 0.77 to 1.54 |
1.16 0.83 to 1.63 |
0.84 0.60 to 1.18 |
Clinical academic credentials (vs academic only) | 0.92 0.67 to 1.26 |
0.75 0.54 to 1.04 |
0.92 0.66 to 1.28 |
0.79 0.57 to 1.08 |
US born (vs not) | 0.91 0.67 to 1.23 |
0.8 0.59 to 1.09 |
0.84 0.61 to 1.14 |
1.01 0.74 to 1.38 |
Reads research daily (vs <daily) | 0.95 0.75 to 1.21 |
0.97 0.76 to 1.23 |
0.93 0.74 to 1.19 |
1.1 0.86 to 1.39 |
CEPH programme (vs school) | 1.26 0.98 to 1.62 |
1.12 0.87 to 1.43 |
1.11 0.86 to 1.43 |
1.67*** 1.30 to 2.15 |
Ivy League institution (vs others) | 0.8 0.52 to 1.24 |
0.71 0.46 to 1.11 |
0.92 0.59 to 1.43 |
0.96 0.62 to 1.49 |
South region (vs Northeast) | 0.91 0.65 to 1.29 |
1.14 0.80 to 1.61 |
0.93 0.66 to 1.30 |
1.01 0.72 to 1.43 |
Midwest region (vs Northeast) | 1.09 0.73 to 1.63 |
1.39 0.93 to 2.07 |
1.04 0.70 to 1.55 |
1.23 0.83 to 1.84 |
West region (vs Northeast) | 1.16 0.74 to 1.82 |
1.2 0.77 to 1.89 |
0.88 0.56 to 1.39 |
0.97 0.62 to 1.52 |
60–<120 s spent reading (vs <60 s) | 0.65** 0.50 to 0.84 |
0.73* 0.55 to 0.96 |
0.97 0.74 to 1.28 |
1.31* 1.02 to 1.67 |
120+ s spent reading (vs <60 s) | 0.5*** 0.35 to 0.72 |
0.61** 0.44 to 0.84 |
0.66* 0.47 to 0.93 |
1.55* 1.01 to 2.38 |
N‡ | 895 | 895 | 895 | 895 |
*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.
†Generalised ordered logit models controlling for all variables in each column.
‡Only survey responses with no missing data included in the multivariate analysis.
Abstract 1, RCT/Service; Abstract 2, Cross-sectional/Service; Abstract 3, RCT/Pharmaceutical; Abstract 4, Cross-sectional/Pharmaceutical.
Discussion
Two sinister issues may be occurring if the source of the research affects one's judgement of it. First, poor research may be given undue significance in part because of the perceived legitimacy of its source. The MMR scandal in the UK may have been a painful example of this. In this case, vaccination rates for the MMR immunisation plummeted when a study published by a high profile research group in a prestigious journal claimed a tenuous (and later discredited) connection between the immunisation and rates of autism.29
Second, good research from an unexpected source may be discounted early on, resulting in missed opportunities to learn from important innovations. LICs have developed novel innovations and there are multiple opportunities to learn from LICs, for example, around improved surgical procedures,30 improved long-term outcomes in mental illness,31–35 improved skill mix with scaled use of community health workers.36–38 However, there are strikingly few examples where these innovations have been adopted in HICs.39 Even in Health Links, where HICs and LICs collaborate explicitly and reciprocally, there are surprisingly few examples of attempts to adopt LIC innovations in high-income settings—HIC volunteers learn a lot personally and professionally, however, this does not translate into changes in their own healthcare systems, and the learning and exchange of expertise is predominantly directed from the HICs towards the LICs.40–43 The Reverse Innovation ‘movement’ sets out to unpack the barriers to adopting LIC innovations in HIC contexts. It is motivated in part by the rapidly changing global health landscape, and has gained interest in the USA and UK because the unsustainable growth in healthcare expenditure means that there is likely to be a genuine need to learn from LICs.44
We know already from the Diffusion of Innovation literature that healthcare professionals perform poorly when it comes to adopting innovations or evidence from ‘elsewhere.’2 7 The not-invented-here culture prevails. However, we also know that innovations are more likely to diffuse if actors perceive the source to be similar to their own. Health professionals are homophilus.4 We might ask, therefore, whether health professionals are even more discriminating when presented with research from very ‘unlikely’ sources? Do they discriminate against sources that they might perceive to be so different from their own, or perceive to be so unlikely to produce good research, that the evidence is discounted early on?
We were motivated to conduct this study due to a strong expectation that there would be a bias against LIC abstracts, or at least that source would make a difference to how the respondents viewed the strength of evidence in the abstract and whether they would choose to refer the abstract to a peer. Although we found no difference in three of the four abstracts, a high-income source did make a difference to participants’ view of the relevance of one of the abstracts. All things being equal, our sample population considered the RCT of the pharmaceutical intervention to be significantly more relevant to their peer group if its source was from the UK rather than from Malawi.
We did take several steps to ensure that if explicit biases existed then we would capture them. We randomised the survey abstracts to control for known and unknown confounders, and this was performed well, as evidenced by the balanced characteristics of the two survey groups. We framed the research as a Speed Reading survey to encourage respondents to spend the minimum time assessing the abstract and to allow anchoring to specific pieces of information in the abstract to occur, and we made no reference to the hypothesis that we were testing to not influence the responses. We achieved a large sample size to be able to detect small, but meaningful, differences in the distribution of the responses—the completed-survey response rate of nearly 10% is within the range expected for a time-consuming, internet-based survey with no preinvitation recruitment.45 The fact that the survey was presented as a Speed Reading test may also have reduced selection bias, in that its stated purpose would not necessarily appeal to one type of researcher, such as those with more global health experience.
However, the result was less dramatic than we expected, occurring in only one of the four abstracts, and it suggests that explicit biases are small and difficult to detect across a relatively small group of abstracts. The study provides an empirical baseline against which to compare future research into the effect of source on abstract evaluation. Indeed, it could be argued that the implications of this study are encouraging for the population that participated because the two groups of survey respondents treated three of the four abstracts almost identically, irrespective of the source. Public health faculty in the USA seem to be doing what is expected of them. Research is being assessed, by and large, according to its content rather than its origin. For those interested in exploring the barriers to Reverse Innovation, or types of publication bias, this finding may be encouraging.
In our study, we also found that respondents spent on average between 70 and 100 s per abstract. Rapid responders tended to rate abstracts higher, so it is possible that if less time is spent on the abstracts then anchoring to particular triggers might be having a greater effect. We did find that, in Abstract 4, if more time is taken to respond to the abstract then opinion of it improves (for both strength of evidence and referral), however, this is equal between high as well as low-income sources. We also found, as would be expected, that respondents tended to rate the randomised controlled trial abstracts higher for strength of evidence compared to the abstracts that were of a cross-sectional design. As the study was framed as a Speed Reading assessment, survey participants might have felt the need to speed-read the abstracts, which may not mirror normal practice.
We also note that the wide SDs in the outcomes indicate that, despite the large sample size, there is considerable variation in how readers view and consume research. The wide SDs might have reduced our ability to detect differences and further work should be conducted to validate measurement constructs in this context. GRADE46 and Jadad47 scores are widely used, but usually to assess entire research articles against judgement of research quality, risk of bias, inconsistency, indirectness, imprecision and publication bias.48–54 Our study, designed purposefully to be a rapid appraisal only of the research abstract, demonstrated extremely wide variation in the assessment of the limited information provided in the abstracts. This finding may have implications for systematic reviews, meta-analyses and for reviewers of abstracts submitted for conferences.
Considering the volume of abstracts read and consumed on a daily basis from all parts of the globe, if source impacts on one's perception, even by a tiny margin, this might, at scale, be an observable phenomenon. We cannot speculate as to the triggers individuals identify with when reading each individual abstract under relatively rapid, timed conditions, but it is encouraging that overall there were few differences between the two survey groups. As highly trained researchers in public health, we could expect an explicit bias to be extremely small, if present at all. It is possible that in other population groups this survey would present different findings. Policymakers, clinicians, journalists and health service managers are all important actors in innovation diffusion processes, and may also be involved in peer-review processes for academic publication. Our strategy to include academic public health professionals in this survey is based on a best-case assessment of likely bias. Future research ought to modify the approach we have chosen in accordance with the target population, using other abstracts or developing a research design that allows respondents to serve as their own controls. Although we found only one of the four abstracts eliciting a small (yet statistically significant) difference in rating, it is unclear whether this proportion would hold across the population level in practice. It certainly raises the question of whether abstracts and articles submitted for peer review should be masked to country-of-origin.55 The eighth International conference on peer review in biomedical research sets the stage for a more detailed examination of cognitive biases in healthcare evidence interpretation.56
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of New York University during this research. The authors would like to thank Professor Jane Noyes and Dr Martin Emmert, as well as the four external peer reviewers, for helpful comments on earlier drafts of the article.
Footnotes
Contributors: MH conceived and designed the research, collected and cleaned the data, helped to analyse the data, wrote the first draft and revised subsequent drafts for important intellectual content. JM analysed the data and helped to design the research, and revised the drafts for important intellectual content. MM conducted an initial pilot of the survey, helped to collect data, contributed to the first draft and revised subsequent drafts for important intellectual content. GJ helped to collect data, design the research and revised subsequent drafts for important intellectual content. CA helped to clean the data and to analyse it, and revised subsequent drafts for important intellectual content.
Funding: This study was conducted as part of a Harkness Fellowship awarded to MH from the US Commonwealth Fund (2014–2015). The authors are grateful for the support of the Commonwealth Fund. The article does not represent the views of the Commonwealth Fund.
Competing interests: None declared.
Ethics approval: New York University—University Committee on Activities Involving Human Subjects.
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Data sharing statement: No additional data are available.
References
- 1.Kaptchuk T. Effect of interpretive bias on research evidence. BMJ 2003;326:1453–5. 10.1136/bmj.326.7404.1453 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ferlie E, Fitzgerald L, Wood M et al. . The nonspread of innovations: the mediating role of professionals. Acad Manage J 2005;48:117–34. 10.5465/AMJ.2005.15993150 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dearing J, Meyer G, Kazmierczak J. Portraying the new: communication between university innovators and potential users. Sci Commun 1994;16:11–42. 10.1177/0164025994016001002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fitzgerald L, Ferlie E, Wood M et al. . Interlocking interactions, the diffusion of innovations in health care. Hum Relations 2002;55:1429–49. 10.1177/001872602128782213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bilkey W, Nes E. Country-of-origin effects on product evaluations. J Int Bus Stud 1982;13:89–100. 10.1057/palgrave.jibs.8490539 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Peterson RA, Jansson S. A meta-analysis of country-of-origin effects. J Int Bus Stud 1995;26:883–900. 10.1057/palgrave.jibs.8490824 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Greenhalgh T, Robert G, Macfarlane F et al. . Diffusion of innovations in service organizations: systematic review and recommendations. Milbank Q 2004;82:581–629. 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00325.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Damschroder L, Aron D, Keith R et al. . Fostering implementation of health services research findings into practice: a consolidated framework for advancing implementation science. Implement Sci 2009;4:50 10.1186/1748-5908-4-50 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 1981;211:453–8. 10.1126/science.7455683 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bargh J, Chen M, Burrows L. Automaticity of social behaviour: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;71:230–44. 10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lord CG, Ross L, Lepper M. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: the effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 1979;37:2098–109. 10.1037/0022-3514.37.11.2098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.MacCoun RJ. Biases in the interpretation and use of research results. Annu Rev Psychol 1998;49:259–87. 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 1974;185:1124–31. 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Harris M. Research papers should omit their authors’ affiliations. BMJ 2014;349:g6439 10.1136/bmj.g6439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Yousefi-Nooraie R, Shakiba B, Mortaz-Hejri S. Country development and manuscript selection bias: a review of published studies. BMC Med Res Methodol 2006;6:37 10.1186/1471-2288-6-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Horton R. North and South: bridging the information gap. Lancet 2000;355:2231–6. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02414-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Patel V, Sumathipala A. International representation in psychiatric literature: survey of six leading journals. Br J Psychiatry 2001;178:406–9. 10.1192/bjp.178.5.406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mendis S, Yach D, Bengoa R et al. . Research gap in cardiovascular disease in developing countries. Lancet 2003;361:2246–7. 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13753-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Yach D, Kenya P. Assessment of epidemiological and HIV/AIDS publications in Africa. Int J Epidemiol 1992;21:557–60. 10.1093/ije/21.3.557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Singh D. Publication bias- a reason for the decreased research output in developing countries. S Afr Psychiatry Rev 2006;9:153–5. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Keiser J, Utzinger J, Tanner M,et al. . Representation of authors and editors from countries with different human development indexes in the leading literature on tropical medicine: survey of current evidence. BMJ 2004;328:1229–32. 10.1136/bmj.38069.518137.F6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Opthofa T, Coronel R, Jansed M. The significance of the peer review process against the background of bias: priority ratings of reviewers and editors and the prediction of citation, the role of geographical bias. Cardiovasc Res 2002;56:339–46. 10.1016/S0008-6363(02)00712-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Link AM. US and non-US submissions. An analysis of reviewer bias. JAMA 1998;280:246–7. 10.1001/jama.280.3.246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tutarel O. Composition of the editorial boards of leading medical education journals. BMC Med Res Methodol 2004;4:3 10.1186/1471-2288-4-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Winnik S, Speer T, Raptis D et al. . The wealth of nations and the dissemination of cardiovascular research. Int J Cardiol 2013;169:190–5. 10.1016/j.ijcard.2013.08.101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Peters DP, Ceci SJ. Peer-review practices of psychological journals: the fate of published articles, submitted again. Behav Brain Sci 1982;5:187–255. 10.1017/S0140525X00011183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kliewer M, DeLong D, Freed K et al. . Peer review at the American Journal of Roentgenology: how reviewer and manuscript characteristics affected editorial decisions on 196 major papers. AJR Am J Roentgenol 2004;183:1545–50. 10.2214/ajr.183.6.01831545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kirkwood B, Sterne J. Essentials of medical statistics. Oxford: Blackwell Science, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wakefield AJ, Murch SH, Anthony A et al. . RETRACTED: ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 1998;351:637–41. 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)11096-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Abeygunasekera A. Learning from low income countries: what are the lessons? Effective surgery can be cheap and innovative. BMJ 2004;329:1185 10.1136/bmj.329.7475.1185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dogra N, Omigbodun O. Learning from low income countries: what are the lessons? Partnerships in mental health are possible without multidisciplinary teams. BMJ 2004;329:1184 10.1136/bmj.329.7475.1184-c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.McKenzie K, Patel V, Araya R. Learning from low income countries: mental health. BMJ 2004;329:1138 10.1136/bmj.329.7475.1138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Swartz HA, Rollman BL. Managing the global burden of depression: lessons from the developing world. World Psychiatry 2003;2:3. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Susser E, Collins P, Schanzer B et al. . Topics for our times: can we learn from the care of persons with mental illness in developing countries? Am J Public Health 1996;86:7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rosen A. Destigmatizing day-to-day practices: what developed countries can learn from developing countries. World Psychiatry 2006;5:1. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Haines A, Sanders D, Lehmann U et al. . Achieving child survival goals: potential contribution of community health workers. Lancet 2007;369:2121–31. 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60325-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Haider R, Ashworth A, Kabir I et al. . Effect of community-based peer counsellors on exclusive breastfeeding practices in Dhaka, Bangladesh: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2000;356:1643–7. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03159-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kumar V, Mohanty S, Kumar A et al. . Effect of community-based behaviour change management on neonatal mortality in Shivgarh, Uttar Pradesh, India: a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2008;372:1151–62. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61483-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Syed S, Dadwal V, Martin G. Reverse innovation in global health systems: towards global innovation flow. Global Health 2013;9:36 (30 August 2013). 10.1186/1744-8603-9-36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wright J, Walley J, Philip A et al. . Research into practice: 10 years of international public health partnership between the UK and Swaziland. J Public Health 2010;32:277–82. 10.1093/pubmed/fdp129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Baguley D, Killeen T, Wright J. International health links: an evaluation of partnerships between health-care organizations in the UK and developing countries. Trop Doct 2006;36:149 10.1258/004947506777978181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lam CLK. Knowledge can flow from developing to developed countries. BMJ 2000;321:830 10.1136/bmj.321.7264.830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Berwick D. Lessons from developing nations on improving health care. BMJ 2004;328:1124–9. 10.1136/bmj.328.7448.1124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Crisp N. Turning the world upside down. RSM books, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cook C, Heath F, Thompson R. A meta-analysis of response rates in web- or internet-based surveys. Educ Psychol Meas 2000;60:821–36. 10.1177/00131640021970934 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Malmivaara A. Methodological considerations of the GRADE method. Ann Med 2015;47:1–5. 10.3109/07853890.2014.969766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D et al. . Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Control Clin Trials 1996;17:1–12. 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Balshem H, Helfand M, Schünemann HJ et al. . GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:401–6. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist G et al. . GRADE guidelines: 4. Rating the quality of evidence—study limitations (risk of bias). J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:407–15. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R et al. . GRADE guidelines: 7. Rating the quality of evidence—inconsistency. J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:1294–302. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.03.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R et al. . GRADE guidelines: 8. Rating the quality of evidence—indirectness. J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:1303–10. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R et al. . GRADE guidelines 6. Rating the quality of evidence—imprecision. J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:1283–93. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Montori V et al. . GRADE guidelines: 5. Rating the quality of evidence—publication bias. J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:1277–82. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Sultan S et al. . GRADE guidelines: 9. Rating up the quality of evidence. J Clin Epidemiol 2011;64:1311–16. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Harris M, Weisberger E, Silver D,et al. . ‘They hear “Africa” and they think that there can't be any good services’—perceived context in cross-national learning: a qualitative study of the barriers to Reverse Innovation. Global Health 2015;11:45 10.1186/s12992-015-0130-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rennie D, Flanagin A, Godlee F et al. . Eighth international congress on peer review in biomedical publication. BMJ 2015;350:h2411 10.1136/bmj.h2411 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]