Abstract
Food intake patterns begin to be shaped at the earliest points in life. Early exposures and experiences are critical for the acceptance of some foods, particularly healthful foods such as vegetables, which often have a bitter component in their flavor profiles. In addition to repeated exposure to these foods, the quality and emotional tone of parent-child interactions are important in facilitating children’s acceptance of vegetables. During early childhood, parents are challenged by children’s developmental characteristics related to eating, such as the emergence of child neophobia, and by individual characteristics of the child that are more biologically based, including genetic predispositions to bitter taste and sensory sensitivities. Experimental studies consistently show that repeated exposure to novel and rejected familiar foods is the most powerful method to improve acceptance. However, the manner and persistence with which these exposures are performed are critical. Research investigating influences on children’s vegetable acceptance and ingestion has focused on associations among availability, parent intakes, child neophobia, and the parental feeding response to children’s reluctance to try and consume vegetables. Because young children’s dietary intakes are low and below dietary recommendations, investigations have focused more on factors that impede children’s vegetable acceptance, such as controlling feeding practices, than on positive influences. Research that addresses the multifaceted nature of these interactions among different levels of social-ecological environment, individual traits, parental feeding styles and practices, and socioeconomic influences and that uses longitudinal designs and complex statistical approaches is called for to ascertain more effective methods to improve children’s vegetable acceptance.
Keywords: child, neophobia, food acceptance, feeding behavior, vegetables, parenting, obesity, growth and development
Introduction
Definition of the issue
In a world of readily available, highly palatable foods, caregivers report that it is particularly challenging to teach children to like and eat vegetables. Studies of children’s food preferences indicate that foods that are most highly preferred are energy-dense items (particularly sweet and salty foods) and that vegetables are least liked (1). These reported food preferences are consonant with preschool-aged children’s food intake patterns (2–4). Indicators from available data suggest that vegetable acceptance, as well as variety of vegetable intake, is at its peak during early infancy, declines after 1 y of age, and continues to fall through the preschool years (5).
In the United States, young children’s vegetable intakes fall far short of dietary recommendations. Approximately 25–30% of young children do not ingest any vegetables on a given day and the variety of vegetables typically consumed by US preschool-aged children does not often include dark-green vegetables, which are rich sources of micronutrients (3). Data from large cohort studies indicate that preschool-aged childrens’ average intakes of vegetables do not meet the standards in many countries (6–8).
Why this is important
The statistics of obesity and its related chronic diseases throughout the life course are clearly linked to worsening dietary intake patterns (9). The necessity to shift toward more healthful intakes to prevent these illnesses and to attain optimal health requires an understanding and integration of the research related to children’s development of food preferences and intakes and the factors that influence children’s eating. Early experience with foods, e.g., vegetables, has the potential for long-lasting effects on an individuals’ diets because this period is a sensitive, if not critical, period for sensory, motor, and experiential learning (10). Development and learning occur across multiple and varied contexts and understanding the influences of environments and caregivers will help to identify modifiable factors for effective interventions to improve children’s vegetable and overall dietary intakes.
This review summarizes the evidence for developmental and environmental influences on children’s vegetable preferences and intakes. It integrates findings from epidemiologic, laboratory, and observational studies and concludes with suggestions for future research to support the development of effective interventions and public health messages. Of note, this review seeks to focus on vegetable acceptance and ingestion (instead of fruit or fruit and vegetables combined) because the trajectory of children’s acceptance and intake of vegetables appears to differ from that of fruit. Where possible, data for vegetables, separated from fruit, are presented and discussed.
Current Status of Knowledge
Developmental influences, early experiences, and children’s vegetable preference and consumption
Biological and developmental influences.
Children’s taste preferences change with age and development (11, 12). Preferences for sweet and salty tastes are highest during early childhood and decline somewhat with age (13). Children are known to prefer higher intensities of salt and sugar than adults. These preferences are innate as is the inborn distaste for bitter. However, this is not to suggest that individuals cannot learn to acquire preferences for foods (such as vegetables) that have bitter components in their taste profiles. Children’s food preferences are predicted by early intake patterns but can change with learning and exposure (5, 10).
Ultimately, vegetable intake is influenced by early exposures that increase the likelihood that children will learn to like and consume healthful foods. Indeed, the few longitudinal studies that assess children’s food preferences suggest that food preferences acquired during early childhood carry on into adolescence and predict the quality of the diet in adult years (14). The case for focusing on improvements in early childhood eating behaviors, with the aim of achieving optimal health and mitigating later chronic disease, is strongly made in the Scientific Report of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines for Americans (15).
The role of early experience.
Early experiences in utero and during breastfeeding provide opportunities for exposure to and learning about flavors and are posited to increase familiarity with a variety of flavors. These early experiences are positively associated with infants’ and young children’s general food acceptance and specifically with vegetable acceptance and intakes (16). Breastfeeding and longer duration of breastfeeding have been associated with greater fruit and vegetable consumption in infancy (2, 17). The timing of introduction of fruit and vegetables has been reported to be important for later consumption of fruit and vegetables and seems to be related to a sensitive period for the introduction and acceptance of foods of varied texture (18, 19). The reader is referred to the article by Mennella et al. (20) in this supplement for more detailed information related to early learning experiences and their impact of food preference development.
Coulthard et al. (21) examined the relation between timing and frequency of introduction of fruit and vegetables and fruit and vegetable consumption at age 7 in UK children (n = 7821). Children who had not been introduced to home-prepared vegetables by 6 mo of age consumed fewer vegetables at age 7 y (21). However, the frequency of consumption moderated this effect such that children who were introduced to vegetables at a later age, but at a higher frequency, consumed amounts of vegetables at age 7 comparable to those who had experienced earlier introduction. In the study by Coulthard et al. most infants had been introduced to vegetables before 3 mo of age. A report in a small sample in the United States (n = 129) reported that early introduction of complementary foods (before age 4–6 mo) increased the odds of children’s picky eating and was associated with reduced dietary variety (22). Therefore, more research is required to ascertain the optimal timing and method of introduction of vegetables (i.e., textures) and other complementary foods during the weaning period.
Not only is timing of introduction of complementary foods important but the variety of vegetables introduced during the weaning period sets the stage for future vegetable acceptance (23). The few studies that focused on introduction of variety suggest that exposure to a variety (across and within meals) during infancy results in greater generalized acceptance of vegetables (24). Throughout the weaning period, infants’ diets become more varied as the development of eating skills progresses and as they gain more experience with food and eating. Food preferences from infancy track into later childhood such that infants who accept greater variety in the first year of life tend to do so in the second year (25). However, by the end of the second year of life, neophobia and food refusal become common and difficulty in children’s feeding and eating is reported by the majority of mothers (26). During the toddler and preschool years, when neophobia is at its height, the variety of vegetable intake diminishes (25, 27).
The role of sensory learning.
Children overcome neophobia and learn about novel foods in a number of ways including experiential learning. When presented with novel foods, preschool-aged children display a number of sensory-based exploratory behaviors [smelling, licking, chewing, spitting, and swallowing (28)]. Engaging in these behaviors is thought to provide experience with foods that facilitates acceptance and consumption. These behaviors are often discouraged by caregivers because they are perceived to fall in the realm of poor table manners (26, 29, 30). A focus on etiquette may impede acquisition of novel preferences, particularly if social interactions during these eating occasions are negative.
Preschool-aged children’s characteristics that influence vegetable preference and consumption
Individual characteristics.
Individual characteristics such as genetically based responses to bitter taste have also been associated with children’s food acceptance and vegetable intake (31–33). Early literature reported that children who displayed sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) rated vegetables as more bitter; however, the relation to vegetable intake has not been consistent across studies (34). The literature suggests that PROP sensitivity interacts with other individual characteristics, such as being “food adventurous,” and that individuals who are more adventurous eaters consume a wider variety of strong-tasting foods, including vegetables, irrespective of PROP taster status (35).
Children’s temperament has been related to food responsiveness, or how children react to or engage with food. Infants who were rated higher on approach (novelty seeking) showed fewer distasteful faces, consumed more food, and were perceived by their mothers to enjoy eating more (36). However, in this study, the length of time that children ate mediated the relation between infant temperament and maternal ratings of child enjoyment (36), suggesting that some of the information that mothers rely on to determine child enjoyment is more related to how long children engage in eating instead of the child’s facial expression. Higher surgency, or outgoingness, has been correlated with higher fruit and vegetable consumption (37). Vollrath et al. (37) suggested that when children respond and adapt well to new foods, it serves as reinforcement to parents to continue to offer these foods. In contrast, children with externalizing temperaments (hyperactive and aggressive) who react more negatively may entrain parents to avoid repeatedly offering new foods so that negative interactions can be circumvented. In general, emotional reactivity and shyness have been associated with food fussiness (38), neophobia (39), and food avoidance (40).
Food neophobia, or the fear and rejection of novel foods, is thought to be part of typical child development, and first emerges during the toddler period and, for most children, dissipates after the preschool years (39). Children who are perceived by caregivers to display high levels of neophobia are 1) reported to consume less food variety in general (41–47) and 2) display lower preferences for and intakes of vegetables (1, 46). Parents, when faced with neophobic behavior, often use pressuring tactics in attempts to improve children’s food intake (47–49). Pressuring feeding strategies also have been correlated in studies that used cross-sectional designs to lower intakes of vegetables in preschool-aged children (45–50). The relation between child neophobia and parenting practices is likely bidirectional in nature; however, evidence to support the interaction between feeding practices and child characteristics has not been studied adequately in longitudinal studies (51).
Overcoming neophobia.
Sufficient repeated exposure to novel foods is one mechanism through which children learn about new foods and come to accept them (52–59). Laboratory studies using repeated exposure protocols indicate that the optimal number of required exposures is at least 5–6 exposures to a new food (60), and perhaps as many as 8–12 exposures (28, 53, 61), with fewer exposures seeming to be required in infancy, before children come to accept and like a novel food. Studies of parent perseverance in offering new or rejected foods reveal that parents display far less persistence in re-offering initially rejected foods (26, 62). Although children may be exposed occasionally to novel foods, it is unlikely that they encounter these foods often enough, and without pressure to eat them, it is unlikely they will learn to overcome their initial reluctance.
Parenting influences related to children’s vegetable consumption
Home environment effects.
Young children are heavily influenced by the home environment and caregivers who make decisions regarding foods and eating experiences offered to children. Caregivers purchase and prepare food, serve as models for appropriate and normative eating behavior, determine the structure of meals and snacks, and use feeding styles and practices that influence children’s eating and food acceptance patterns (63). Larson and Story (63) referred to caregivers as the “nutritional gateway” for children’s eating and pointed out that not only are purchasing decisions important but so too are storage capacity and shelf life of the foods purchased, because more processed, energy-dense foods and snacks (compared with the shelf life of fresh vegetables) can be bought in bulk and safely stored for long periods of time. Consistent associations have been reported across the childhood period, in numerous countries and across socioeconomic strata, for the influence of availability of vegetables in the home and children’s vegetable intake (64–67). In addition to availability, accessibility (particularly for older children) is a significant influencer in children’s food choices: that which can be accessed and easily eaten has a higher probability of being chosen and consumed (68).
Parental beliefs and feeding self-efficacy.
Parents hold strong beliefs regarding children’s eating behaviors. These ideas may relate to parental beliefs about all children or to beliefs about their particular child’s preferences for taste, color, texture, and need for sameness. In a unique qualitative research study aimed at investigating parental beliefs regarding the mutability of children’s eating behaviors and food acceptance patterns, Australian parents conveyed the belief that children are influenced by individual child attributes such as neophobia and temperament, but that children’s preferences change with time and development and can be influenced by socialization experiences with parents and peers and in child care (43). One study revealed that parent child-feeding self-efficacy mediated the extent to which children were exposed to new foods and to children’s vegetable intake and variety (69). That is, mothers who were confident that children can be encouraged to accept new foods and vegetables were more likely to offer these foods to their children and their child’s intake of vegetables was higher. Therefore, parental beliefs are important in that they influence the kinds of foods that parents purchase and make available to their child and possibly their expectations for their children’s consumption (70, 71).
Vegetable availability and parental vegetable intakes.
Parental purchasing and dietary intake patterns also have been related to child vegetable intakes. In lower socioeconomic status families enrolled in WIC (Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children), greater purchasing behavior of vegetables was associated with preschool-aged children’s willingness to try vegetables (72). In general, greater home availability of vegetables has been associated with higher maternal and preschool-aged children’s intakes (64, 65, 73). Numerous studies have reported a positive relation between maternal vegetable intake and children’s vegetable consumption, and this effect has been attributed to both the availability of vegetables in the home and to parental modeling of vegetable consumption (50, 63, 65, 74–79). This view of parental modeling, i.e., routine parental consumption of foods serving as social facilitation of children’s food acceptance patterns, aligns well with the mere exposure theory as put forth by Zajonc (80). Zajonc suggested that mere exposure effects are “subliminal in nature,” with the requirement that the stimulus (vegetables being ingested) is accessible to the individual’s sensory receptors (in this case, the child being able to observe this behavior), but that cognition is not required. Thus, repeated observations of parents consuming vegetables may be a plausible mechanism through which parents influence the development of children’s eating norms and their willingness to engage in the behavior. One study by Goldman et al. (64) directly queried parents about the extent to which they modeled fruit and vegetable consumption and reported that, in combination with availability and accessibility, parent modeling of vegetable consumption was associated with higher child vegetable intakes. Direct observations of caregiver modeling influences on children’s vegetable consumption are limited but support the theory that young children are more likely to place a food in their mouths when viewing adult role models (81).
Parental feeding styles and practices.
The feeding styles and strategies that parents use to convince young children to try and eat vegetables are varied in tactic, emotional tone, and effectiveness and are critical influencers of children’s vegetable preferences and consumption. Both the feeding style, the overall approach, and emotional overtone of the feeding environment that is molded by parent experience and behaviors and specific feeding practices have been reported to influence children’s vegetable liking and consumption (82, 83). In general, parental feeding styles that are responsive to the child, use negotiation (but not coercion or bribery), make appropriate demands for mastery, and that are influenced by health-related goals for children’s eating are associated with reduced child neophobia, greater willingness to try vegetables, and greater intake of vegetables (38). This feeding style aligns with the general authoritative parenting style described by Baumrind (84) and others (58, 67), which is characterized by limit setting and negotiation with an underlying tone of responsivity and warmth.
Positive parental feeding practices that have been associated with greater acceptance and ingestion of vegetables by young children are child-centered in nature and are consistent with what has been termed “responsive feeding” (85, 86). Among these practices are the use of encouragement and praise (75, 76, 87); parent modeling of vegetable consumption (48, 64, 65, 75, 76); monitoring of low-nutrient, highly palatable foods (75); offering home-prepared vegetables (64, 65, 75); and the use of structure and rules for mealtimes and feeding (64, 65, 87–89). The use of appropriate nonfood rewards (e.g., stickers) has been reported to improve children’s willingness to try vegetables (53). In contrast, feeding styles that are uninvolved, overly rigid, or permissive are all associated with poor vegetable acceptance and ingestion by young children, with permissive feeding styles (e.g., children are indulged or decide which foods are offered) being associated with the lowest vegetable intake (90). Negative feeding practices that have been associated with lower vegetable intake include the use of contingencies [rewards of desired foods for eating less desired vegetables (83, 88, 91)], pressure to eat (48, 50, 83, 91), and catering to children’s demands for foods (83, 91).
As previously noted, parental feeding practices are related to the child’s eating characteristics: parents who perceive their child to be more neophobic report that they use more pressure to try to get their child to eat (46, 50). Parents who report higher neophobia scores for their child offer new foods less persistently and have children who like fewer foods and fewer vegetables (46). It should also be noted that parental feeding practices, especially modeling and social facilitation of eating behavior, are also associated with parent neophobia: parents who self-report as being more neophobic consume fewer vegetables and have less vegetable variety and have children who are more neophobic and consume fewer vegetables (47). Thus, it seems that there is an intergenerational component to vegetable acceptance and intake that is related to individual characteristics (e.g., neophobia) and parental eating and feeding practices. Very few longitudinal studies, to our knowledge, have been conducted that have simultaneously collected information on parental feeding practices, child characteristics, parents’ and children’s food preferences, and dietary intakes. Therefore, the relations among young children’s food acceptance, parental feeding practices, and children’s food intake should be interpreted as associations and developmental trajectories of preschool-aged children’s food preferences, and intake cannot be inferred from these data.
The conditions that seem to promote child vegetable acceptance are consistency (in availability of vegetables with fewer unhealthful competing foods, structured mealtimes during which parents model vegetable consumption, and high expectations for consumption), flexibility (willingness to negotiate and provide choice), and responsiveness (warmth and encouragement, engagement, positive affect in the eating environment, and avoidance of pressuring or coercive strategies). These parenting attributes require high cognitive and attentional input as well as confidence and trust that children will accept foods with time and positive experience. This combination of parental feeding style and practices is difficult to achieve under everyday circumstances and when resources are sufficient; but for the parent with lower educational attainment, stressors related to poverty and violence and the demands of work, multiple children, and other social pressures, the pathway to achieving healthy dietary intake patterns for their children is quite challenging for parents and children alike.
Sociodemographic Influences Related to Children’s Vegetable Consumption
Children’s consumption of vegetables is also influenced by maternal and familial socioeconomic factors. Maternal education and nutrition knowledge are consistent predictors of maternal vegetable intake as well as her preschool-aged child’s intake (2, 92, 93). In lower socioeconomic status families, availability and accessibility of vegetables in the home are consistently associated with cost, both in terms of monetary expense and preparation time and with maternal self-efficacy to offer fruit and vegetables to her preschool-aged child (68). Financial support to low-income women and children, via WIC, appears to improve infant vegetable intake and variety, underscoring the effectiveness of simultaneously providing education and financial support to families to provide vegetables to their young children (94).
Class disparities in vegetable consumption have increased in the United Kingdom and the United States, with children from lower socioeconomic status families reporting lower intakes and less variety in vegetable consumption (95). Food insecurity has been associated with lower preschool-aged children’s intakes of fruit and vegetables (92) and, in particular, of dark-green vegetables of high nutrient density (96). The drivers of social class differences in vegetable intakes are reported to be the higher cost (per calorie) of vegetables (97, 98), poorer access to healthful foods [in neighborhoods and because lower income families are less likely to own a vehicle to go elsewhere to purchase foods (97, 98)], lack of cooking skills and time for preparation (98), and apathy of low-income families toward nutrition messages (98). Class differences result in disparities in total vegetable consumption and in negative impacts on overall nutrient intakes [β-carotene, folate, vitamin C and fiber (95)].
These impediments exist in stark contrast to marketing incentives for low-nutrient/high-energy foods (95). The effects of television viewing and branding on children’s vegetable consumption have not been extensively studied. However, investigations of marketing content reported that the preponderance of food cues during programming (on public television shows aimed at preschool-aged children) more often gave positive endorsement to unhealthy (over healthy) foods (99). Targeted advertising for high-energy, low-nutrient-dense foods has been observed to be higher in areas with greater proportions of black children and lower-income households (100). Findings from large cohort studies reported that television viewing is negatively associated with diet quality of preschool-aged children and, specifically, with lower intakes of fruit and vegetables (101–103).
Kraak et al. (104) underscored that companies spend far less in marketing for nutrient-dense foods (e.g., vegetables) than for less healthful and highly palatable foods that compete with vegetable consumption in young children. They further noted that interventions that aim to increase the availability and consumption of healthy foods target motivation and behavior change without addressing cost for low-income, high-risk families and children. The effectiveness of such interventions, without incentives to support purchases, has been questioned (95, 104).
Findings from Experimental Studies Aiming to Improve Children’s Vegetable Acceptance and Consumption
The classic conundrum of young children’s food acceptance patterns is that children “like what they know and eat what they like” (105). That is, young children, particularly those whom have entered the period when neophobia is highly expressed, appear to need adequate knowledge or familiarity to agree to engage in sensory learning and to taste a novel food (Figure 1). It is with increased familiarity that children’s taste preferences change: the food becomes known, tasted, potentially liked, and eventually ingested (106). However, neophobia and novelty conspire to increase the barrier to children choosing to taste the food. In addition, for some children, sensory characteristics such as genetically determined responsiveness to bitter taste and sensory sensitivities may result in negative affective experiences related to trying new foods and thus increase the difficulty of increasing children’s consumption of novel foods (21, 107). These conditions seem to be especially potent in the case of children’s vegetable acceptance and ingestion. The challenge for researchers and caregivers alike is in identifying the circumstances in which children can be persuaded to try foods and that result in positive learning experiences such that children acquire food preferences and, ultimately, eat sufficient quantities to affect diet quality positively.
Laboratory studies undertaken with the aim of improving children’s liking and consumption of vegetables have focused on several areas of inquiry: the effects of repeated exposure (taste and visual) (80, 108), flavor-flavor and flavor-nutrient learning (108), manipulation of portion size offered (109), and the use of rewards to increase willingness to try, preference, and consumption (108, 109). The most consistent positive effects are found for repeated exposure on children’s vegetable acceptance (54–56, 60) and, to some extent, on children’s consumption (56, 59, 110–113). Irrespective of child age and neophobia, persistent offering of a vegetable results in improvements in children’s preference and (in the few studies that have measured it) ingestion of a vegetable—even those vegetables that are initially ranked as disliked (114). However, individual characteristics such as child age, parental reports of neophobia, and sensory sensitivity do have an impact on the trajectory of acceptance and number of required exposures.
The dogma has been that young children require 8–10 exposures of a novel food to learn to accept and acquire positive food preferences for foods, although it should be noted that this was established based on studies that enrolled preschool-aged children (61). Newer research suggests that toddlers younger than 24 mo may require as few as 5 exposures (60) and that these learned preferences result in increases in children’s vegetable consumption (24, 60). Furthermore, it appears that individual differences in trajectories of food acceptance come into play, with some children displaying easier adoption of novel foods (“plate clearers”) than children for whom repeated exposure does not seem to be sufficient to overcome food rejection [“noneaters” (57)]. Whether noneaters are more neophobic or have greater sensory sensitivity (i.e., greater sensitivity to smell, taste, or texture of foods) has only just begun to be explored.
Aldridge et al. (106) suggested that increasing familiarity, through exposure, is a key component of reducing neophobia and improving the likelihood that children will become willing to try foods (102). Familiarity is thought to be attained through a number of paths: visual cues (recognition and awareness), taste (sensory knowledge, experience, predictability, physiologic feedback from ingestion), context (food preparation and presentation), and category (the category in which the food belongs). Each of the modes of familiarity could result in decreases in the neophobic child’s fear and anxiety. These conditions are not unique to the construct of food acceptance. Zajonc (80, 115), the acknowledged thought leader in the realm of mere exposure theory, posited that the development of any preference (be it music, food, or ideological values) occurs when an individual is exposed repeatedly to a particular stimulus. Furthermore, any unconditioned stimulus paired with exposure (e.g., emotion or experience) is likely to have an impact on the individual’s preference development and this unconditioned stimulus can be positive or negative. For example, in the food acceptance domain, exposures paired with negative experiences (e.g., being pressured to eat a food or an aversive taste) reinforce the development of negative taste preferences. Therefore, not only do foods such as vegetables need to be persistently offered, the manner in which they are offered is important and can have long-lasting effects.
Studies investigating the impact of exposure via visual cues (e.g., books or pictures) have not reported increases in children’s vegetable acceptance and consumption, although some effects have been noted for fruit acceptance (58). Heath et al. (108) argued that visual exposures may serve to increase knowledge, recognition, and sensory learning (as shown by increases in toddlers’ looking times) about foods and thereby potentially affect willingness to approach and try a new food. Studies that focused on food preparation to alter visual cues (making foods attractive or “cute”) reported no impact on children’s consumption and preference (116, 117).
Some evidence of effectiveness of flavor-flavor pairing (e.g., providing a familiar dip to eat with vegetables or sweetening vegetables) on children’s willingness to try novel foods has been reported; however, findings are mixed with respect to the effectiveness of these strategies on children’s vegetable intake (55, 56, 112, 118, 119). As with repeated exposure, toddlers appear to be more positively influenced by flavor-flavor learning. However, the results of flavor-flavor learning, as distinct from repeated exposure, are difficult to deduce because flavor-flavor learning protocols must also include repeated exposure. The additive effects of flavor-flavor learning, over and above repetition, may be ascribed to 1) reducing children’s reluctance to try new foods by increasing familiarity, particularly in the case of pairing familiar palatable dips with vegetables, or 2) reducing negative sensory input (i.e., masking bitter taste). As depicted in the model of children’s acquisition of vegetable preferences and ingestion (Figure 1), willingness to try is a necessary and antecedent step to improving food preference and ultimately consumption.
Flavor-nutrient learning (e.g., the addition of energy to increase the energy density of a food) has been shown in animal (120) and human (120, 121) studies to increase food preference. Studies have manipulated energy density by varying both carbohydrate and fat content and subjects’ preferences have aligned with the version of the food that is higher in energy density. In general, these methods have not been shown to have positive effects on children’s liking or consumption of vegetables (green or root vegetables) when fat is added to vegetable formulations (56, 60). This is particularly interesting because it is inconsistent with data supporting children’s high consumption of fried vegetables (4, 122, 123). Of note, the literature that focuses on flavor-nutrient learning most often uses target foods that are sweet [e.g., yogurt (121)] and it may be that sweet-fat combinations are more easily (or differently) conditioned than vegetables that are paired with fat [e.g., artichoke purée with added fat (60)].
The portion size and the timing of vegetable portions offered are among experimental manipulations that have shown positive impacts on the amounts of vegetables that young children eat. One study that offered large portions of vegetables as a first or appetizer course (such that vegetables were consumed while children were still hungry) resulted in increased consumption of vegetables at the meal (124). Another study that offered vegetables as a snack with a dip (such that vegetables did not compete with other, more palatable foods) resulted in increased consumption of vegetables (118). Similarly, increasing the portion offered by concealing vegetables in other dishes also increased consumption, albeit not vegetable preference or overt acceptance (125). Increasing the portion of vegetables offered when fruit is simultaneously available led to increases in consumption of fruit but not of vegetables (126).
In-home observations of children’s consumption at dinner meals revealed that the amounts children ingested were predicted by the amounts that they were served, both with respect to total energy intake and the portion of vegetables offered and consumed (127). In this study, parents selected the vegetables served to their children and it is possible that they offered those that their preschool-aged children already knew and liked. A qualitative study focusing on mothers’ decisions regarding what and how much food to serve their child at meals at home reported that mothers typically serve those vegetables that they know their children are likely to consume to avoid waste of time, money, and effort (128). Continued offering of rejected vegetables was not considered to be an effective strategy, particularly for mothers of children who reported that their children were “picky eaters.”
An additional rationale for trying to increase vegetable portion size beyond the aim of improving vegetable intake is to decrease energy density, and therefore energy intake, at the meal. Although the use of “hidden” vegetables at a meal to decrease energy density (the “stealth” effect) may be effective in passively reducing energy consumption at a meal, altering portions served as vegetables at a meal, to either increase liking or intake or to reduce overall energy consumption at the main meal, has proven to be ineffective (126).
Perhaps the most debated approach to influencing children’s food acceptance patterns has been the use of reward. The effectiveness of reward to modulate children’s responses to foods appears to be determined by 3 different aspects of the type of reward used: 1) affect associated with the use of reward, 2) the extent to which the reward affects child agency, and 3) the size or level of the reward being offered. Birch et al. (129, 130) showed that the use of contingencies or bribery to influence children’s food preferences, although effective in the short run to achieve ingestion, was not sufficient to improve liking of target foods. This may have been due to the nature of the foods that were used in the studies: highly palatable foods for rewards and vegetables as target foods that were selected to mimic parental feeding practices. Subsequently, Wardle and colleagues (53, 131, 132) undertook a series of studies to determine whether rewards such as stickers could be offered to a child to improve children’s willingness to try foods and their consumption of foods. These studies have consistently shown that use of an immediate reward is effective in improving child compliance to try foods and, with repeated exposure, in improving liking and consumption (immediately and up to 6 mo postintervention). Important characteristics of the rewards used in these studies were that the stickers were offered with neutral or positive affective valence (e.g., “good job”) and that the size of the reward was in proportion to the task that the child was being asked to perform. The use of this category of rewards is time-honored in the behavior change/motivation literature for children (133). As noted by Cooke et al. (132), this type of reward is less likely to have negative impacts on children’s intrinsic motivation to try and like foods and may therefore be a practical method to improve children’s willingness and motivation to try foods. Appropriate rewards may serve to bridge the barrier between neophobia and food acceptance, which is a significant hurdle for caregivers. A limitation to this area of inquiry is the inability to separate exposure from reward effects (i.e., repeated exposure is part of reward-based paradigms). Furthermore, to our knowledge, no studies have been conducted that include extinction intervals as part of the experimental design, which would allow for the determination of lasting effects of appropriate rewards on children’s food acceptance behaviors.
Conclusions
Summary of the evidence
Children’s caregivers are the gatekeepers of exposure to a variety of foods. What, when, and how caregivers offer foods to children arguably has as much influence as biology on children’s food preferences and acceptance. Caregivers’ decisions about the foods they choose to consume (and model); which foods they offer to their children; the frequency, consistency, and amounts that they offer; and their expectations for children’s consumption are critically important in understanding children’s dietary intakes. The available evidence supports that early exposure to vegetable flavors in utero and through breastfeeding, as well as the timing, variety, and consistency of introduction of vegetables during the weaning period and beyond, sets the stage for optimal vegetable acceptance and consumption. However, the latter part of this interval, from toddlerhood through the preschool years, is also the time of the greatest surge in children’s autonomy seeking (134) and neophobia (45). The result is that children’s vegetable intakes are significantly limited and parents often avoid addressing the issue, or do so ineffectively and become frustrated and anxious (128). Given the links between early dietary behaviors and later dietary and chronic disease risks, it is worrisome that vegetable intakes are so low at the time when children should be learning how to consume them so that future health is positively affected (135).
Integration of the research into an overarching model (Figure 1) proposes that the path to vegetable acceptance and consumption has at least 2 major stages: 1) willingness to try vegetables and 2) vegetable consumption. These stages have different potential inputs, with the majority of research that has yielded positive outcomes having been focused on children’s willingness to try vegetables. Influencers that are positively associated with improving children’s willingness to try vegetables include the following: 1) experiential learning (early flavor and food experiences, repeated exposure, sensory-based learning, timing, and variety of introduction); 2) environmental effects (vegetable availability and accessibility); 3) parental knowledge, attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors (intake and modeling); and 4) parental feeding styles and practices (responsive feeding styles, use of negotiation, reasoning and knowledge transfer, appropriate incentives). Factors positively associated with children’s vegetable consumption relate to the following: 1) environmental effects (availability, accessibility), 2) norms and routines (family meals, structure, limit setting, and expectations for consumption), 3) parental vegetable intakes, 4) food preparation and presentation (home-prepared vegetables, portion, and timing of offering in the day), and 5) socioeconomic factors (cost, income, and education). Conceptualizing the path to vegetable consumption as a 2-stage process of repeated trying that can lead to significant consumption, instead of a singular goal of getting children to consume their vegetables, may not be a conscious part of parental beliefs and practices. Lowering expectations for consumption and focusing on exposure may also relieve some cost concerns of families with limited resources.
Experimental studies confirm a strong role for exposure and persistence—a difficult task for many parents who have more immediate concerns of getting their child to eat the meal and foods at hand (128). Wardle et al. (53) provided evidence for positive effects of appropriate reward. Experimental designs that investigate the lasting effects of proper types of reinforcement (as opposed to bribes and food rewards) should be developed.
With respect to parenting styles and practices, the evidence suggests that negative, controlling, parent-centered styles and practices impede the development of children’s vegetable preferences and consumption. The preponderance of evidence has focused on negative parental behaviors and styles. Building the evidence base for positive parent child-feeding interactions is difficult because capturing these processes (e.g., modeling, use of encouragement and praise, positive emotional tone and warmth) is methodologically challenging to operationalize and comes with significant time and funding costs.
Research gaps and opportunities
A number of gaps exist in the current literature regarding the acquisition of children’s vegetable preferences and the influences that affect their consumption. Longitudinal studies that shed light on the development of vegetable preferences and the interactions between child, parent, socioeconomic status, and organizational influences would require cohort studies of significant magnitude that are designed to capture mediating and moderating effects. Furthermore, the inclusion of extended families (grandparents homes with multiple caregivers, and children who spend time in more than one family home) and of larger environmental strategies (“nudges” and behavioral economic strategies implemented at the community level) are also important to investigate because these caregivers and environments play an important role in children’s dietary intake (136). Studies are called for that more fully examine social class impacts, as well as the role of economic policies on food availability, purchasing practices, and cultural values. Such investigations should consider positive-deviance designs that identify families who are succeeding in feeding their children well. In particular, understanding what motivates some mothers to continue to offer their children initially rejected foods such that children exhibit greater food acceptance will be critical for developing interventions that improve children’s vegetable and overall dietary intakes.
Further study of the role of texture, timing, and variety of vegetables introduced during the ages of 6–24 mo will be important for informing the development of guidelines for vegetable feeding practices. In addition, the exploration of the influence of additional forms of sensory learning (e.g., visual cues and olfactory experiences) and how these interact with sensory sensitivity on children’s willingness to try novel foods and vegetables could provide valuable information to help address the barrier of children’s neophobia.
The extent to which specific parenting practices influence children’s vegetable acceptance, compared with the style of feeding and emotional tone during eating occasions, is an important area of discovery. From a pragmatic standpoint, it may be easier and more achievable to focus on specific feeding practices rather than endeavoring to alter parental warmth and responsivity. Determining the contribution of practices and styles would provide valuable information for intervention development—particularly for those families who have increased stressors, limited resources, and who are most at risk of chronic diseases related to diet. Investigating the impact of providing economic assistance, in addition to tailored education about effective feeding practices, is important because education alone does not change fears about wasting foods nor the capacity to purchase more and healthier foods. Last, given the consistent association between parent and child intakes, the focus of research and intervention strategies should be on the entire family rather than simply on child-feeding practices. The Healthy People 2020 goals call for increasing vegetable intakes for all individuals aged ≥2 y and for increasing vegetable variety (137). Improving the entire family’s intake of vegetables may be more effective than focusing only on children’s vegetable acceptance and intake.
Acknowledgments
The sole author had responsibility for all parts of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Cooke L, Wardle J, Gibson EL. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2–6-year-old children. Appetite 2003;41:205–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Deming DM, Briefel RR, Reidy KC. Infant feeding practices and food consumption patterns of children participating in WIC. J Nutr Educ Behav 2014;46(3, Suppl):S29–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel RR, Reidy KC, Deming DM. Food consumption patterns of young preschoolers: are they starting off on the right path? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110(12, Suppl):S52–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Siega-Riz AM, Deming DM, Reidy KC, Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel RR. Food consumption patterns of infants and toddlers: where are we now? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110(12, Suppl):S38–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Grimm KA, Kim SA, Yaroch AL, Scanlon KS. Fruit and vegetable intake during infancy and early childhood. Pediatrics 2014;134(Suppl 1):S63–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Huybrechts I, Matthys C, Vereecken C, Maes L, Temme EH, Van Oyen H, De Backer G, De Henauw S. Food intakes by preschool children in Flanders compared with dietary guidelines. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2008;5:243–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Manios Y, Kourlaba G, Kondaki K, Grammatikaki E, Birbilis M, Oikonomou E, Roma-Giannikou E. Diet quality of preschoolers in Greece based on the Healthy Eating Index: the GENESIS study. J Am Diet Assoc 2009;109:616–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ramírez-Silva I, Rivera JA, Ponce X, Hernandez-Avila M. Fruit and vegetable intake in the Mexican population: results from the Mexican National Health and Nutrition Survey 2006. Salud Publica Mex 2009;51(Suppl 4):S574–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ezzati M, Riboli E. Behavioral and dietary risk factors for noncommunicable diseases. N Engl J Med 2013;369:954–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ventura AK, Worobey J. Early influences on the development of food preferences. Curr Biol 2013;23:R401–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mennella JA, Finkbeiner S, Lipchock SV, Hwang LD, Reed DR. Preferences for salty and sweet tastes are elevated and related to each other during childhood. PLoS ONE 2014;9:e92201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mennella JA, Reed DR, Roberts KM, Mathew PS, Mansfield CJ. Age-related differences in bitter taste and efficacy of bitter blockers. PLoS ONE 2014;9:e103107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mennella JA. Ontogeny of taste preferences: basic biology and implications for health. Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99(Suppl):704S–11S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Northstone K, Emmett PM. Are dietary patterns stable throughout early and mid-childhood? A birth cohort study. Br J Nutr 2008;100:1069–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Millen BE. Scientific report of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee. Washington (DC): Department of Health and Human Services; 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Beauchamp GK, Mennella JA. Early flavor learning and its impact on later feeding behavior. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 2009;48(Suppl 1):S25–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.de Lauzon-Guillain B, Jones L, Oliveira A, Moschonis G, Betoko A, Lopes C, Moreira P, Manios Y, Papadopoulos NG, Emmett P, et al. . The influence of early feeding practices on fruit and vegetable intake among preschool children in 4 European birth cohorts. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98:804–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Harris G. Development of taste and food preferences in children. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 2008;11:315–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Blossfeld I, Collins A, Kiely M, Delahunty C. Texture preferences of 12-month-old infants and the role of early experiences. Food Qual Prefer 2007;18:396–404. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mennella JA, Reiter AR, Daniels LM.. Vegetable and fruit acceptance during infancy: impact of ontogeny, genetics, and early experiences. Adv Nutr 2016;7(Suppl):S211–9S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P. Long-term consequences of early fruit and vegetable feeding practices in the United Kingdom. Public Health Nutr 2010;13:2044–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shim JE, Kim J, Mathai RA. Associations of infant feeding practices and picky eating behaviors of preschool children. J Am Diet Assoc 2011;111:1363–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nicklaus S. Development of food variety in children. Appetite 2009;52:253–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM. Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav 2008;94:29–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Houck KS, Bounds W, Morris M, Cox DR, Moran J III, Coletta F. Longitudinal study of nutrient and food intakes of white preschool children aged 24 to 60 months. J Am Diet Assoc 1999;99:1514–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Carruth BR, Skinner JD. Revisiting the picky eater phenomenon: neophobic behaviors of young children. J Am Coll Nutr 2000;19:771–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite 2005;44:289–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Johnson SL, Bellows L, Beckstrom L, Anderson J. Evaluation of a social marketing campaign targeting preschool children. Am J Health Behav 2007;31:44–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Orrell-Valente JK, Hill LG, Brechwald WA, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. "Just three more bites": an observational analysis of parents’ socialization of children's eating at mealtime. Appetite 2007;48:37–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Campbell KJ, Crawford DA, Hesketh KD. Australian parents’ views on their 5–6-year-old children's food choices. Health Promot Int 2007;22:11–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Baranowski T, Baranowski JC, Watson KB, Jago R, Islam N, Beltran A, Martin SJ, Nguyen N, Tepper BJ. 6-n-Propylthiouracil taster status not related to reported cruciferous vegetable intake among ethnically diverse children. Nutr Res 2011;31:594–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bell KI, Tepper BJ. Short-term vegetable intake by young children classified by 6-n-propylthoiuracil bitter-taste phenotype. Am J Clin Nutr 2006;84:245–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lumeng JC, Cardinal TM, Sitto JR, Kannan S. Ability to taste 6-n-propylthiouracil and BMI in low-income preschool-aged children. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16:1522–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tepper BJ. Nutritional implications of genetic taste variation: the role of PROP sensitivity and other taste phenotypes. Annu Rev Nutr 2008;28:367–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ullrich NV, Touger-Decker R, O’Sullivan-Maillet J, Tepper BJ. PROP taster status and self-perceived food adventurousness influence food preferences. J Am Diet Assoc 2004;104:543–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Forestell CA, Mennella JA. More than just a pretty face: the relationship between infant's temperament, food acceptance, and mothers’ perceptions of their enjoyment of food. Appetite 2012;58:1136–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Vollrath ME, Stene-Larsen K, Tonstad S, Rothbart MK, Hampson SE. Associations between temperament at age 1.5 years and obesogenic diet at ages 3 and 7 years. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2012;33:721–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bergmeier H, Skouteris H, Horwood S, Hooley M, Richardson B. Associations between child temperament, maternal feeding practices and child body mass index during the preschool years: a systematic review of the literature. Obes Rev 2014;15:9–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Pliner P, Loewen ER. Temperament and food neophobia in children and their mothers. Appetite 1997;28:239–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Powell FC, Farrow CV, Meyer C. Food avoidance in children: the influence of maternal feeding practices and behaviours. Appetite 2011;57:683–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tsuji M, Nakamura K, Tamai Y, Wada K, Sahashi Y, Watanabe K, Ohtsuchi S, Ando K, Nagata C. Relationship of intake of plant-based foods with 6-n-propylthiouracil sensitivity and food neophobia in Japanese preschool children. Eur J Clin Nutr 2012;66:47–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav 2002;34:310–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Russell CG, Worsley A. Why don't they like that? And can I do anything about it? The nature and correlates of parents’ attributions and self-efficacy beliefs about preschool children's food preferences. Appetite 2013;66:34–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Russell CG, Worsley A. A population-based study of preschoolers’ food neophobia and its associations with food preferences. J Nutr Educ Behav 2008;40:11–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JC. Food neophobia and 'picky/fussy’ eating in children: a review. Appetite 2008;50:181–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Galloway AT, Lee Y, Birch LL. Predictors and consequences of food neophobia and pickiness in young girls. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:692–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kaar J, Buti A, Johnson S. Food neophobia and food preference concordance among parent-child dyads and parents’ offering of new foods. FASEB J 2014;28(1 Suppl):252.1. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gregory JE, Paxton SJ, Brozovic AM. Maternal feeding practices predict fruit and vegetable consumption in young children: results of a 12-month longitudinal study. Appetite 2011;57:167–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Fisher JO, Mitchell DC, Smiciklas-Wright H, Birch LL. Parental influences on young girls’ fruit and vegetable, micronutrient, and fat intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 2002;102:58–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Galloway AT, Fiorito L, Lee Y, Birch LL. Parental pressure, dietary patterns, and weight status among girls who are "picky eaters". J Am Diet Assoc 2005;105:541–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Skouteris H, McCabe M, Ricciardelli LA, Milgrom J, Baur LA, Aksan N, Dell'Aquila D. Parent-child interactions and obesity prevention: a systematic review of the literature. Early Child Dev Care 2012;182:153–74. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Birch LL, Marlin DW. I don't like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences. Appetite 1982;3:353–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Wardle J, Herrera ML, Cooke L, Gibson EL. Modifying children's food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr 2003;57:341–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lakkakula A, Geaghan J, Zanovec M, Pierce S, Tuuri G. Repeated taste exposure increases liking for vegetables by low-income elementary school children. Appetite 2010;55:226–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Anzman-Frasca S, Savage JS, Marini ME, Fisher JO, Birch LL. Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children's liking of vegetables. Appetite 2012;58:543–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hausner H, Olsen A, Moller P. Mere exposure and flavour-flavour learning increase 2–3 year-old children's acceptance of a novel vegetable. Appetite 2012;58:1152–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Caton SJ, Blundell P, Ahern SM, Nekitsing C, Olsen A, Moller P, Hausner H, Remy E, Nicklaus S, Chabanet C, et al. . Learning to eat vegetables in early life: the role of timing, age and individual eating traits. PLoS ONE 2014;9:e97609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Osborne CL, Forestell CA. Increasing children's consumption of fruit and vegetables: does the type of exposure matter? Physiol Behav 2012;106:362–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bouhlal S, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S. 'Just a pinch of salt’: an experimental comparison of the effect of repeated exposure and flavor-flavor learning with salt or spice on vegetable acceptance in toddlers. Appetite 2014;83:209–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Caton SJ, Ahern SM, Remy E, Nicklaus S, Blundell P, Hetherington MM. Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavor-flavour and flavor-nutrient learning. Br J Nutr 2013;109:2089–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Sullivan SA, Birch LL. Pass the sugar, pass the salt—experience dictates preference. Dev Psychol 1990;26:546–51. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. Effects of repeated exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in on acceptance of initially 7-month old infants. Food Qual Prefer 2007;18:1023–32. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Larson N, Story M. A review of environmental influences on food choices. Ann Behav Med 2009;38: Suppl 1:S56–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Goldman RL, Radnitz CL, McGrath RE. The role of family variables in fruit and vegetable consumption in pre-school children. J Public Health Res 2012;1:143–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wyse R, Campbell E, Nathan N, Wolfenden L. Associations between characteristics of the home food environment and fruit and vegetable intake in preschool children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 2011;11:938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.van Ansem WJC, Schrijvers CTM, Rodenburg G, van de Mheen D. Is there an association between the home food environment, the local food shopping environment and children's fruit and vegetable intake? Results from the Dutch INPACT study. Public Health Nutr 2013;16:1206–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Patrick H, Nicklas TA. A review of family and social determinants of children's eating patterns and diet quality. J Am Coll Nutr 2005;24:83–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Owens E, Marsh T, Rittenberry L, de Moor C. Availability, accessibility, and preferences for fruit, 100% fruit juice, and vegetables influence children's dietary behavior. Health Educ Behav 2003;30:615–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Koh GA, Scott JA, Woodman RJ, Kim SW, Daniels LA, Magarey AM. Maternal feeding self-efficacy and fruit and vegetable intakes in infants: results from the SAIDI study. Appetite 2014;81:44–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hildebrand DA, Betts NM. Assessment of stage of change, decisional balance, self-efficacy, and use of processes of change of low-income parents for increasing servings of fruits and vegetables to preschool-aged children. J Nutr Educ Behav 2009;41:110–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Shriver LH, Hildebrand D, Austin H. Determinants of fruit and vegetable availability in Hispanic head start families with preschool-aged children living in an urban midwestern area. J Nutr Educ Behav 2010;42:299–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Busick DB, Brooks J, Pernecky S, Dawson R, Petzoldt J. Parent food purchases as a measure of exposure and preschool-aged children's willingness to identify and taste fruit and vegetables. Appetite 2008;51:468–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Wyse RJ, Wolfenden L, Campbell E, Brennan L, Campbell KJ, Fletcher A, Bowman J, Heard TR, Wiggers J. A cluster randomised trial of a telephone-based intervention for parents to increase fruit and vegetable consumption in their 3- to 5-year-old children: study protocol. BMC Public Health 2010;10:216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Spence A, Campbell K, Hesketh K. Parental correlates of young children's dietary intakes: a review. Australasian Epidemiologist 2010;17:17–20. [Google Scholar]
- 75.McGowan L, Croker H, Wardle J, Cooke LJ. Environmental and individual determinants of core and non-core food and drink intake in preschool-aged children in the United Kingdom. Eur J Clin Nutr 2012;66:322–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Vereecken C, Rovner A, Maes L. Associations of parenting styles, parental feeding practices and child characteristics with young children's fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite 2010;55:589–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Papas MA, Hurley KM, Quigg AM, Oberlander SE, Black MM. Low-income, African American adolescent mothers and their toddlers exhibit similar dietary variety patterns. J Nutr Educ Behav 2009;41:87–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Sweetman C, McGowan L, Croker H, Cooke L. Characteristics of family mealtimes affecting children's vegetable consumption and liking. J Am Diet Assoc 2011;111:269–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Skala K, Chuang RJ, Evans A, Hedberg AM, Dave J, Sharma S. Ethnic differences in the home food environment and parental food practices among families of low-income Hispanic and African-American preschoolers. J Immigr Minor Health 2012;14:1014–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Zajonc RB. Mere exposure: a gateway to the subliminal. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2001;10:224–8. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Harper LV, Sanders KM. The effect of adults’ eating on young children's acceptance of unfamiliar foods. J Exp Child Psychol 1975;20:206–14. [Google Scholar]
- 82.Rhee K. Childhood overweight and the relationship between parent behaviors, parenting style and family functioning. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci 2008;615:12–37. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Blissett J. Relationships between parenting style, feeding style and feeding practices and fruit and vegetable consumption in early childhood. Appetite 2011;57:826–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Baumrind D. Prototypical descriptions of 3 parenting styles. Psychology 1966;37:887–904. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Black MM, Aboud FE. Responsive feeding is embedded in a theoretical framework of responsive parenting. J Nutr 2011;141:490–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Satter EM. The feeding relationship. J Am Diet Assoc 1986;86:352–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.O'Connor TM, Hughes SO, Watson KB, Baranowski T, Nicklas TA, Fisher JO, Beltran A, Baranowski JC, Qu H, Shewchuk RM. Parenting practices are associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in pre-school children. Public Health Nutr 2010;13:91–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Kiefner-Burmeister AE, Hoffmann DA, Meers MR, Koball AM, Musher-Eizenman DR. Food consumption by young children: a function of parental feeding goals and practices. Appetite 2014;74:6–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Baranowski T, Chen TA, O'Connor T, Hughes S, Beltran A, Frankel L, Diep C, Baranowski JC. Dimensions of vegetable parenting practices among preschoolers. Appetite 2013;69:89–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Murashima M, Hoerr SL, Hughes SO, Kaplowitz SA. Feeding behaviors of low-income mothers: directive control relates to a lower BMI in children, and a nondirective control relates to a healthier diet in preschoolers. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;95:1031–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Rigal N, Chabanet C, Issanchou S, Monnery-Patris S. Links between maternal feeding practices and children's eating difficulties: validation of French tools. Appetite 2012;58:629–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Dave JM, Evans AE, Saunders RP, Watkins KW, Pfeiffer KA. Associations among food insecurity, acculturation, demographic factors, and fruit and vegetable intake at home in Hispanic children. J Am Diet Assoc 2009;109:697–701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Ebenegger V, Marques-Vidal PM, Nydegger A, Laimbacher J, Niederer I, Burgi F, Giusti V, Bodenmann P, Kriemler S, Puder JJ. Independent contribution of parental migrant status and educational level to adiposity and eating habits in preschool children. Eur J Clin Nutr 2011;65:210–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hurley KM, Black MM. Commercial baby food consumption and dietary variety in a statewide sample of infants receiving benefits from the special supplemental nutrition program for women, infants, and children. J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110:1537–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Does social class predict diet quality? Am J Clin Nutr 2008;87:1107–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Lorson BA, Melgar-Quinonez HR, Taylor CA. Correlates of fruit and vegetable intakes in US children. J Am Diet Assoc 2009;109:474–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Zenk SN, Odoms-Young AM, Dallas C, Hardy E, Watkins A, Hoskins-Wroten J, Holland L. "You have to hunt for the fruits, the vegetables": environmental barriers and adaptive strategies to acquire food in a low-income African American neighborhood. Health Educ Behav 2011;38:282–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.DiSantis KI, Grier SA, Odoms-Young A, Baskin ML, Carter-Edwards L, Young DR, Lassiter V, Kumanyika SK. What "price" means when buying food: insights from a multisite qualitative study with black Americans. Am J Public Health 2013;103:516–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Radnitz C, Byrne S, Goldman R, Sparks M, Gantshar M, Tung K. Food cues in children's television programs. Appetite 2009;52:230–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Powell LM, Wada R, Kumanyika SK. Racial/ethnic and income disparities in child and adolescent exposure to food and beverage television ads across the U.S. media markets. Health Place 2014;29:124–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Miller SA, Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Gillman MW. Association between television viewing and poor diet quality in young children. Int J Pediatr Obes 2008;3:168–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Manios Y, Kondaki K, Kourlaba G, Grammatikaki E, Birbilis M, Ioannou E. Television viewing and food habits in toddlers and preschoolers in Greece: the GENESIS study. Eur J Pediatr 2009;168:801–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA, Dubow E. Prospective associations between early childhood television exposure and academic, psychosocial, and physical well-being by middle childhood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2010;164:425–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Kraak VI, Story M, Wartella EA, Ginter J. Industry progress to market a healthful diet to American children and adolescents. Am J Prev Med 2011;41:322–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Cooke L. The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet 2007;20:294–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Aldridge V, Dovey TM, Halford JCG. The role of familiarity in dietary development. Dev Rev 2009;29:32–44. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P. Delayed introduction of lumpy foods to children during the complementary feeding period affects child's food acceptance and feeding at 7 years of age. Matern Child Nutr 2009;5:75–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Heath P, Houston-Price C, Kennedy OB. Increasing food familiarity without the tears: a role for visual exposure? Appetite 2011;57:832–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Kral TV, Hetherington MM. Variability in children's eating response to portion size: a biobehavioral perspective. Appetite 2015;88:5–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Howard AJ, Mallan KM, Byrne R, Magarey A, Daniels LA. Toddlers’ food preferences: the impact of novel food exposure, maternal preferences and food neophobia. Appetite 2012;59:818–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Ahern SM, Caton SJ, Blundell P, Hetherington MM. The root of the problem: increasing root vegetable intake in preschool children by repeated exposure and flavour-flavour learning. Appetite 2014;80:154–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.de Wild V, de Graaf C, Jager G. Efficacy of repeated exposure and flavour-flavour learning as mechanisms to increase preschooler's vegetable intake and acceptance. Pediatr Obes 2015;10:205–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.O'Connell ML, Henderson KE, Luedicke J, Schwartz MB. Repeated exposure in a natural setting: a preschool intervention to increase vegetable consumption. J Acad Nutr Diet 2012;112:230–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Keller KL. The use of repeated exposure and associative conditioning to increase vegetable acceptance in children: explaining the variability across studies. J Acad Nutr Diet 2014;114:1169–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Zajonc RB. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc Psychol Monograph Supplement 1968;9:1–27. [Google Scholar]
- 116.Branen L, Fletcher J, Hilbert L. Snack consumption and waste by preschool children served "cute" versus regular snacks. J Nutr Educ Behav 2002;34:279–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Correia DC, O'Connell M, Irwin ML, Henderson KE. Pairing vegetables with a liked food and visually appealing presentation: promising strategies for increasing vegetable consumption among preschoolers. Child Obes. 2014;10:72–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Fisher JO, Mennella JA, Hughes SO, Liu Y, Mendoza PM, Patrick H. Offering "dip" promotes intake of a moderately-liked raw vegetable among preschoolers with genetic sensitivity to bitterness. J Acad Nutr Diet 2012;112:235–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Havermans RC, Jansen AT. Increasing the efficacy of cue exposure treatment in preventing relapse of addictive behavior. Addict Behav 2003;28:989–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Sclafani A, Ackroff K. Role of gut nutrient sensing in stimulating appetite and conditioning food preferences. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2012;302:R1119–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Johnson SL, McPhee L, Birch LL. Conditioned preferences: young children prefer flavors associated with high dietary fat. Physiol Behav 1991;50:1245–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Bowman SA, Gortmaker SL, Ebbeling CB, Pereira MA, Ludwig DS. Effects of fast-food consumption on energy intake and diet quality among children in a national household survey. Pediatrics 2004;113:112–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Skinner JD, Ziegler P, Pac S, Devaney B. Meal and snack patterns of infants and toddlers. J Am Diet Assoc 2004;104(1, Suppl 1):s65–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Eating vegetables first: the use of portion size to increase vegetable intake in preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr 2010;91:1237–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Spill MK, Birch LL, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Hiding vegetables to reduce energy density: an effective strategy to increase children's vegetable intake and reduce energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 2011;94:735–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Kral TV, Kabay AC, Roe LS, Rolls BJ. Effects of doubling the portion size of fruit and vegetable side dishes on children's intake at a meal. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2010;18:521–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Johnson SL, Hughes SO, Cui X, Li X, Allison DB, Liu Y, Goodell LS, Nicklas T, Power TG, Vollrath K. Portion sizes for children are predicted by parental characteristics and the amounts parents serve themselves. Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99:763–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Johnson SL, Goodell LS, Williams K, Power TG, Hughes SO. Getting my child to eat the right amount. Mothers’ considerations when deciding how much food to offer their child at a meal. Appetite 2015;88:24–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Birch LL, Birch D, Marlin DW, Kramer L. Effects of instrumental consumption on children's food preference. Appetite 1982;3:125–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Birch LL, Marlin DW, Rotter J. Eating as the "means" activity in a contingency: effects on young children's food preference. Child Dev 1984;55:431–9. [Google Scholar]
- 131.Fildes A, van Jaarsveld CH, Wardle J, Cooke L. Parent-administered exposure to increase children's vegetable acceptance: a randomized controlled trial. J Acad Nutr Diet 2014;114:881–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Cooke LJ, Chambers LC, Anez EV, Wardle J. Facilitating or undermining? The effect of reward on food acceptance: a narrative review. Appetite 2011;57:493–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Alvarez AL, Booth AE. Motivated by meaning: testing the effect of knowledge-infused rewards on preschoolers’ persistence. Child Dev 2014;85:783–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Brownell CA, Zerwas S, Ramani GB. "So big": the development of body self-awareness in toddlers. Child Dev 2007;78:1426–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Dwyer JT, Butte NF, Deming DM, Siega-Riz AM, Reidy KC. Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study 2008: progress, continuing concerns, and implications. J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110(12, Suppl):S60–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Speirs KE, Bruan B, Zoumenou V, Anderson EA, Finkbeiner N. Grandmothers’ involvement in preschool-aged children's consumption of fruits and vegetables: an exploratory study. Infant Child Adolesc Nutr 2009;1:332–7. [Google Scholar]
- 137.US Department of Health and Human Services. Developing Healthy People 2020: objectives for public comment. 2015 [cited 2015 May 7]. Available from: http://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/default.