Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2015 Dec 7;4:e11386. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11386

Striatal dynamics explain duration judgments

Thiago S Gouvêa 1,, Tiago Monteiro 1,, Asma Motiwala 1, Sofia Soares 1, Christian Machens 1, Joseph J Paton 1,*
Editor: Timothy Behrens2
PMCID: PMC4721960  PMID: 26641377

Abstract

The striatum is an input structure of the basal ganglia implicated in several time-dependent functions including reinforcement learning, decision making, and interval timing. To determine whether striatal ensembles drive subjects' judgments of duration, we manipulated and recorded from striatal neurons in rats performing a duration categorization psychophysical task. We found that the dynamics of striatal neurons predicted duration judgments, and that simultaneously recorded ensembles could judge duration as well as the animal. Furthermore, striatal neurons were necessary for duration judgments, as muscimol infusions produced a specific impairment in animals' duration sensitivity. Lastly, we show that time as encoded by striatal populations ran faster or slower when rats judged a duration as longer or shorter, respectively. These results demonstrate that the speed with which striatal population state changes supports the fundamental ability of animals to judge the passage of time.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.001

Research Organism: Rat

eLife digest

You know someone is a good cook from their rice - grains must be well cooked, but not to the point of being mushy. Despite consistently using the same pot and stove, we, however, will sometimes overcook it. It is as if our inner sense of time itself is variable. What is it about the brain that explains this variability in time estimation and indeed our ability to estimate time in the first place?

One issue the brain must confront in order to estimate time is that individual brain cells typically fire in bursts that last for tens of milliseconds. So how does the brain use this short-lived activity to track minutes and hours? One possibility is that individual neurons in a given brain region are programmed to fire at different points in time. The overall firing pattern of a group of neurons will therefore change in a predictable way as time passes.

Gouvêa, Monteiro et al. found such predictably changing patterns of activity in the striatum of rats trained to estimate and categorize the duration of time intervals as longer or shorter than 1.5 seconds. Interestingly, when rats mistakenly categorized a short interval as a long one, population activity had travelled farther down its path than it would normally (and vice-versa for long intervals incorrectly categorized as short), suggesting that variability in subjective estimates of the passage of time might arise from variability in the speed of a changing pattern of activity across groups of neurons.

As further evidence for the involvement of the striatum, inactivating the structure impaired the rats’ ability to correctly classify even the longest and shortest interval durations.

The next challenge is to determine exactly how the striatum generates these time-keeping signals, at which stage variability originates, and how the brain regions that the striatum signals to use them to control an animal’s behavior.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.002

Introduction

Time, like space, is a fundamental dimension of the environment, yet how it is processed in the brain is poorly understood. A number of recent studies have identified dynamics that allow for robust representation of time by populations of neurons in multiple areas including the hippocampus (Pastalkova et al., 2008; MacDonald et al., 2011), prefrontal (Kim et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2014), parietal (Leon and Shadlen, 2003; Janssen and Shadlen, 2005) and motor (Lebedev et al., 2008) cortices, cerebellum (Mauk and Buonomano, 2004), and the striatum (Matell et al., 2003; Jin et al., 2009; Adler, 2012; Mello et al., 2015). However, any dynamics that result in a continuously-evolving and non-repeating population state can be used to encode time (Buonomano, 2014), and it is not known whether such temporal representations would inform subjects’ judgments of duration or merely covary with elapsing time. The striatum, a brain structure known to be involved in reinforcement learning and decision making (Lau and Glimcher, 2008; Samejima et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2015), has been implicated in interval timing by several lines of evidence (Hinton and Meck, 2004; Harrington et al., 2009; Wencil et al., 2010; Malapani, 1998; Meck, 2006). However, whether dynamics in striatal activity can explain the perceptual performance of behaving subjects is unknown. To determine whether striatal ensembles drive subjects’ judgments of duration, we manipulated and recorded from striatal neurons in rats performing a duration categorization psychophysical task.

Results

To measure the duration sensitivity of subjects’ timing judgments, we trained rats to judge whether time intervals belonged to a long or short category (Gouvêa et al., 2014) (see Materials and methods; Figure 1a). At each self-initiated trial, two brief tones (interval onset, offset) were presented separated in time by an interval randomly selected from the set I = {0.6, 1.05, 1.26, 1.38, 1.62, 1.74, 1.95, 2.4} seconds. Judgments about interval duration were reported at two laterally located nose ports: choosing the left side was reinforced with water after intervals longer than 1.5 seconds (long stimuli), and the right side otherwise (short stimuli, Figure 1b). Animals were required to withhold choice until interval offset. Animals made virtually no errors when categorizing the easiest (i.e. shortest and longest) intervals, but categorization performance declined as intervals approached the 1.5 second categorical boundary (Figure 1c).

Figure 1. Rats judged interval durations as either long or short.

(a) Rats triggered interval stimuli (i.e. two brief auditory tones separated by a silent interval of random duration) by inserting their snout into a central port. Following interval offset, animals reported their long vs short judgment at two lateral choice ports. Correct trials yielded a water reward, while incorrect or premature responses produced a white noise sound and a time out. Top view, high-speed video was acquired throughout task performance. (b) Reward contingency. (c) Averaged psychometric curves following bilateral muscimol or saline injections in dorsal striatum (mean ± standard deviation across session means, and logistic fit; n = 3 rats, 4 sessions each). Inset: slope of psychometric curves on consecutive saline and muscimol sessions. All raw data for Figure 1 can be found in Figure 1—source data 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.003

Figure 1—source data 1. The. txt file contains trial by trials stimulus (Interval), choice (choiceLong), animal (Name), treatment (MuscimolDose) and session (Date).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11386.004

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Histological confirmation of cannula placements.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Histology slices and schematic representation of the location of saline and muscimol injections. Coronal slices at intermediate anterior posterior (AP) positions are shown for reference at +0.84mm (left, rat Albert), +1.68 mm (center, rat Yuri) and + 0.60 mm (right, rat Zack) from Bregma. Vertical grey bars represent the location of the cannula placements. Yellow asterisks (*) show the approximate dorsal-ventral (DV) position from where the injectors extended 1.5 mm bellow the cannulae.

We recorded action potentials during task performance (see Materials and methods), from populations of single striatal neurons targeting dorsal-central striatum, an area where manipulations produced timing deficits (Meck, 2006) (Figure 2a. For a reconstruction of striatal recording sites see Figure 2—figure supplement 1.). We observed that striatal neurons displayed diverse firing patterns, with different units firing at different times within the interval period (Figure 2b–d). Can such firing patterns support duration judgments? To determine whether and the degree to which individual neurons could contribute to duration judgments, for each trial, we counted spikes in the last 500 ms of the interval period and compared spike count distributions of short vs long stimulus trials using a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis (see Materials and methods). We found that the majority of neurons (~57%) preferred either short or long stimuli (Figure 2e; short-preferring: n = 159/433, 36.7%; long-preferring: n = 87/433, 20.1%; permutation test, p<0.05). As expected, short-preferring neurons displayed higher firing on average prior to the 1.5 s category boundary, after which long-preferring neurons displayed higher firing (Figure 2f). These averaged activity patterns resemble the likelihood of receiving reward on a moment-by-moment by basis should the animal choose short or long (compare with reward contingency in Figure 1b). Such signals, previously observed in the parietal cortex of monkeys performing a similar timing task (Leon and Shadlen, 2003) and in the striatum in a value based decision task (Lau and Glimcher, 2008), are potentially useful for guiding choice. However, were animals’ judgments indeed guided by such signals, it should be possible to predict choices reported later in the trial using neural activity collected during interval stimuli. Indeed, in trials wherein a near boundary interval was judged as long, firing of the short (long) preferring subpopulation dropped (rose) faster, so that the two curves crossed before the 1.5 s boundary (Figure 2g). Conversely, in trials wherein the same interval was judged as short, the two curves evolved more slowly so that at the time of interval offset the short preferring subpopulation was still firing at a higher level and a crossing point had not yet been reached (Figure 2h).

Figure 2. Dynamics of striatal subpopulations predict duration judgments.

(a) Psychometric function for neural recording sessions (mean ± standard deviation across sessions and logistic fit, n = 37 sessions from 3 rats). (b,c) Raster plot and peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) of two example cells for trials in which the longest stimulus interval (2.4 s) was presented. Time = 0 corresponds to stimulus onset. (d) Normalized PSTHs of all neurons for trials in which the longest stimulus interval was presented. Arrowheads indicate cells shown in (b,c). Blue and red ticks indicate cells with significant short and long preferences, respectively. (e) Histogram of preference indices. Blue and red outlines indicate subpopulations with significant short and long preferences, respectively. (f) Averaged, normalized PSTH of the two subpopulations outlined in (e) for trials in which the longest stimulus interval was presented (mean ± SEM). (g) Same as in (f), for trials in which a near-boundary stimulus interval (1.62 s) was judged as long. For comparison, curves shown in (f) are reproduced as a watermark. (h) same as (g) for trials in which the stimulus was judged as short. For single subjects, see Figure 2—figure supplement 2 . Behavior and neural spike count data for Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplements 1 and 2 can be found in Figure 2—source data 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.006

Figure 2—source data 1. Folder with raw data for Figures 24.
Each file in the source data folder is named “‘<animal>_<session>"’, and contains an 8x2 cell "‘spikeCounts"’. For each of the eight stimuli and two choices, "‘spikeCounts"’ contains a matrix of spike counts with dimensions:number of neurons x number of trials x number of time steps. The stimuli (delay to the second tone in seconds) and choices are ordered as follows:STIM_SET = [0.60 1.05 1.26 1.38 1.62 1.74 1.95 2.4];CHOICES = ['incorrect' 'correct'];The spike counts are given from 500 ms prior to trial initiation up to 500 ms after the time of the second tone and were binned in 2 ms wide bins.This folder also includes underlying data for Figures 3 and 4.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11386.007

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Electrophysiological recordings in dorsal striatum.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a) Movable microwire bundle array (Innovative Neurophysiology) used for all neural recordings. (b) Histogram of firing rates for all selected cells (bin size 1 spike/s). (c) Schematic representation of the striatal recording sites. Coronal slices at intermediate AP positions are shown for reference (left to right, rats Bertrand, Edgar and Fernando). Colored rectangles show the approximate DV position of the wire bundles across recording sessions and horizontal black lines represent session by session recording sites, for 10, 9 and 18 recording sessions, respectively.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Dynamics of striatal subpopulations predict duration judgments.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(a,f,k) Psychometric functions for the recording sessions of rats Bertrand (a), Edgar (f) and Fernando (k) (mean ± standard deviation across sessions and logistic fit). (b,g,l) Histograms of preference indices for the same individual animals. Blue and red outlines indicate subpopulations with significant short and long preferences, respectively. (c,h,m) Averaged, normalized PSTHs of the two subpopulations outlined in (b,g,l) for trials in which the a near-boundary stimulus interval (1.62 s) was judged as long (mean ± SEM). (d,i,n) same as in (c,h,m) for short judgment trials. (e,j,o) Normalized PSTHs of all neurons for each animal for trials in which the longest stimulus interval was presented. Blue and red ticks indicate cells with significant short and long preferences, respectively.

The observation of large proportions of short- and long-preferring neurons whose dynamics predicted choice is evidence that duration judgments are guided by the state of striatal populations. Might the information afforded by ensembles of striatal neurons account for the pattern of subjects’ judgments across all stimuli? To test this hypothesis, we compared session to session fluctuations in behavioral performance with the separability of activity states of simultaneously recorded ensembles at the offset of short as compared to long intervals. Briefly, for each trial in a session we characterized neural population state as a vector r = (r1, r2,... , rN), where rn is the number of spikes fired by neuron n ∈ [1,N] within the last 500 ms of the interval period. Next, for each session we found the linear discriminant that best separated population state vectors according to whether they came from a long or a short interval trial (Figure 3a; see Materials and methods). A threshold placed along the linear discriminant was then used as a decision rule (black line in Figure 3a) to generate a ‘neural duration judgment’ for each trial. This procedure allowed us to obtain, for each session, a quantitative description of how well simultaneously recorded neurons could categorize stimuli, i.e., a neurometric function comparable to the behavioral psychometric function (Figure 3b). Consistent with duration information being encoded at the population level, we found that for sessions in which greater numbers of neurons were recorded simultaneously (i.e. upper tercile of sessions with regard to population size) psychometric and neurometric performances were similar and strongly correlated (r2 = 0.76, p<0.001; Figure 3c). These results demonstrate that a read out of stimulus category from even modestly-sized ensembles of striatal neurons was in many cases sufficient to explain the pattern of duration judgments produced by behaving subjects.

Figure 3. Simultaneously recorded population state at interval offset can explain behavioral performance.

(a) Low dimensional representation of population state at interval offset for one example session. Black line is the decision rule (see text). (b) Example psychometric, neurometric and videometric curves for the same session as in (a). (c) Slopes of psychometric and neurometric curves for all sessions. Color indicates terciles of population size. Highlighted data point corresponds to the session in (a-b). Inset: regression slope of neurometric and videometric curves for sessions in the upper tercile. See Figure 3—figure supplement 2 for psychometric-videometric comparison at interval offset and choice. (d) Performance of an ideal observer analysis in predicting stimulus category, applied to neural (orange) and video (blue) data obtained at different times relative to interval offset. Thin lines corresponds to individual sessions. Thick lines are averages. (e) The orange and blue curves (thin lines in panel (d)) for corresponding sessions were regressed against each other at different time shifts. The regression R2 values for each session are shown in thin grey lines. The average over all sessions is shown in black. Sizes of black squares indicate the number of sessions with significant positive correlations (largest squares at 200 and 100 ms correspond to 5 sessions and smallest one at -200 ms to 1, out of a total of 8 sessions). (f) Psychometric curves constructed from trials separated according to whether the population state at stimulus offset had advanced more or less along the mean trajectory. Color indicates terciles shown in (g). (g) Distributions of projection on normalized mean trajectory for all trials for each stimulus are shown (stimuli color coded as in [a]). The equal area bins shown correspond to the groups of trials used for constructing the three psychometric curves shown in panel (f). Data in f-g are from rat Bertrand. See Figure 3—figure supplement 3 for the remaining two subjects.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.010

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Image frames at the end of the neural analysis window do not show a consistent separation between short and long stimulus trials.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(a) Example frames for a 0.60 and 2.40 s stimulus trials (top and bottom, respectively) at stimulus offset. (b) Superimposed frames (thresholded and background subtracted) for each stimulus type (same color conventions as in Figure 3a). (c) sum of all frames shown in (b). (d) Same as in (c) but excluding the easiest short (0.60 s) and long (2.40 s) stimulus trials. The data show are from the same session presented in Figure 3b.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Behavior at the end of the neural analysis window did not explain the categorization performance of neural populations.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(a) Neurometric (orange data points) or videometric (purple data points) logistic slope plotted against the psychometric slope for each session in the upper tercile with respect to simultaneously recorded population size. (b) Videometric slope plotted against the psychometric slope where the videometric curve was built using image frames taken at the time that animals expressed their choice.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Population state at interval offset can explain behavioral performance.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(a) Psychometric curves constructed from trials separated according to whether the population state at stimulus offset had advanced more or less along the mean trajectory for rats Edgar (top) and Fernando (bottom) (b) Distributions of projection on normalized mean trajectory for all trials for each stimulus are shown for rats Edgar (left) and Fernando (right). The equal area bins shown correspond to the groups of trials used for constructing the three psychometric curves shown in (a).

It has been previously reported that duration judgments could be predicted by animals’ ongoing behavior during the interval period (Gouvêa et al., 2014; Matthews and Lerer, 1987; Killeen and Fetterman, 1988; Fetterman et al., 1998; Machado, 1997; Machado and Keen, 2003). In addition, it is well known that striatal neurons can fire around movements (Alexander and Crutcher, 1990; Jin and Costa, 2010). Could the categorization performance of striatal ensembles reflect activity related to movements the animal might be making during the task? To test to what degree ongoing behavior could explain the categorization performance of striatal neural activity, we applied an analogous classification analysis to video images taken of the animal just before interval offset (see Materials and methods). We found that our ability to categorize intervals using video frames was consistently poorer as compared to neural data collected at the analogous time periods during the task (Figure 3b, inset in Figure 3c, Video 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 2a). In contrast, we were able to categorize stimuli as well as the animal using video frames taken at the point when animals expressed their choice at one of the reward ports (Figure 3—figure supplement 2b). Furthermore, movement related responses in the striatum are known to occur both pre- and post-movement onset, much later than in other motor areas such as pre-motor and motor cortex (Alexander and Crutcher, 1990). Thus, if purely movement-related activity were responsible for the categorization performance of striatal ensembles, we would expect ensemble performance to display a similar time course to that of video frames. Applying the same analyses at multiple points in time ranging from 500 ms preceding to 500 ms following stimulus offset revealed a strikingly different profile of categorization performance for video frames as compared to neural ensembles (Figure 3d–e). Specifically, the time course of duration categorization by neural ensembles was best correlated with the duration categorization by video frames when using spikes collected between 400 ms and 200 ms preceding a reference video frame. These indicate that the categorization performance of striatal neurons was not simply related to the immediate sensorimotor state of the animal, and instead likely reflects that striatal neurons encode an internal neural representation of the state of animals’ categorical decisions.

Video 1. Video clips from the entire stimulus period do not show a consistent separation between short and long stimulus trials.

Download video file (665.7KB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11386.014

Superimposed video clips (thresholded and background subtracted) for each stimulus type and for the corresponding stimulus duration (same color conventions as in Figure 3a and Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Red stop marks signal the end of each video and the corresponding stimulus duration.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.014

We have shown thus far that categorical information about interval duration contained in the firing of striatal populations at the time of stimulus offset can explain the precision of animals’ judgments about duration. However, in the task employed here, categorical judgments must be derived from a continuously evolving decision variable that represents how much time has elapsed since the onset of the stimulus. As indicated by the diversity of firing patterns (Figure 2d), the state of population activity evolved continuously during interval stimuli (Figure 3g, Figure 4a, Figure 3—figure supplement 3b), a feature not captured by binary classification. Might trial to trial variations in population state predict the apparent speed of animals’ internal representation of elapsed time? To test this possibility, we performed two additional analyses.

Figure 4. Smoothly changing population state encodes elapsing time in accordance with perceptual report for a long stimulus.

(a) Low dimensional representation of population state during entire interval period of correct trials. Line colors indicate interval duration (warmer colors are longer intervals, as in Figure 3a). Dots are placed at the interval offset end, and their color indicates judgment (blue: short; red: long). (b–g) Population state and decoded time for a single long, near boundary stimulus interval (1.62 s). (b) Yellow curve is same as in (a). Red dots are 6 time points evenly spaced between interval onset and offset. Blue dots are projections of population state during short judgment trials. Grey lines link population states at equivalent time points. (c) Average cumulative distance travelled in full neural space along trajectory represented in (b) on long versus short judgment trials. (d) Posterior probability of time given population state at the time points indicated in (b), averaged within trials of each judgment type. (e,f) Same as (d) for the entire interval period. (g) Difference between posteriors for long and short judgment trials. Arrowheads indicate same time points used in (b,d). n = 433 neurons from 3 rats. See Figure 4—figure supplement 1 for a different near boundary stimulus, and Figure 4—figure supplement 2 for data from individual subjects.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.015

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Smoothly changing population state encodes elapsing time in accordance with perceptual report for a short stimulus.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) Low dimensional representation of population state during entire interval period of correct trials. Line colors indicate interval duration (same color code as in Figures 3 and 4). Dots are placed at the interval offset end, and their color indicates judgment (blue: short; red: long). (b-g) Population state and decoded time for a single short, near boundary stimulus interval (1.38 s). (b) Green curve is the population state trajectory for long judgment trials. Red dots are 6 time points evenly spaced between interval onset and offset. Blue dots are projections of population state during short judgment trials. Grey lines link population states at equivalent time points. (c) Average cumulative distance travelled in full neural space along trajectory represented in (b) on long versus short judgment trials. (d) Posterior probability of time given population state at the time points indicated in (b), averaged within trials of each judgment type. (e,f) Same as (d) for the entire interval period. (g) Difference between posteriors for long and short judgment trials. Arrowheads indicate same time points used in (b,d).

Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Single subjects show smoothly changing population states that encode elapsing time in accordance with perceptual report.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(a,e,i) Low dimensional representation of population state during entire interval period of correct trials of rats Bertrand (a), Edgar (e) and Fernando (i). Line colors indicate interval duration (warmer colors are longer intervals, as in Figures 3 and 4). Dots are placed at the interval offset end, and their color indicate choice (blue: short; red: long). (b,f,j) Yellow/green line is same as in (a,e,i) for a single near boundary stimulus interval (1.62/1.38 s; stimulus of highest choice variance for each subject). Red dots are 6 time points evenly spaced between interval onset and offset. Blue dots are projections of population state during short judgment trials. Grey lines link population states at equivalent time points. (c,g,k) Average cumulative distance travelled in full neural space along trajectory represented in (b,f,j) on long versus short judgment trials. (d,h,l) Difference between posteriors for long and short judgment trials for rats Bertrand (d), Edgar (h) and Fernando (l).

First, we projected the state of simultaneously recorded neuronal populations at stimulus offset in individual trials onto the mean trajectory within each session, noted the fraction of the mean trajectory traversed for each trial, and pooled the data for each stimulus over all sessions within a given subject. Indeed, when population state at stimulus offset had advanced relatively more or less along the mean trajectory, animals were more likely to judge intervals as long or short respectively (Figure 3f–g, Figure 3—figure supplement 3). This effect was observed most consistently for interval stimuli that were closer to the category boundary, and thus variations in projected population state led to horizontal shifts in the psychometric curves (see Materials and methods). These data are consistent with striatal population state encoding a representation of elapsed time that is used by animals to determine categorical judgments. Indeed, such a pattern of population activity has been proposed as a suitable neural code for elapsing time (Buonomano, 2014; Buonomano and Merzenich, 1995).

However, if such a representation encodes elapsed time, and not only subjects’ judgments in this task, neural activity should continuously traverse a non-repeating trajectory in state space in a manner that predicts duration judgments during presentation of particular stimuli. Indeed, even in a low dimensional projection of population activity, we found that network state ran ahead or behind depending on whether the animal judged a near boundary stimulus as long or short (Figure 4b–c, Figure 4—figure supplement 1b,c, Figure 4—figure supplement 2b,f,j). The correspondence between population trajectory and duration judgments further suggests that striatal dynamics may form an internal representation of elapsed time that informed categorical decisions about duration. To directly test this hypothesis, we focused on stimuli near the category boundary and decoded time from the population using a naive Bayes decoder and asked whether such a representation correlated with animals’ judgments, exhibiting choice probability (Britten et al., 1996). We found that decoded estimates of time ran faster or slower when animals judged a given stimulus as long or short, respectively (Figure 4d–g, Figure 4—figure supplement 1d-g, cross validated naive Bayes decoder; see Materials and methods). This indicates that striatal activity provides information about elapsing time, the continuously varying decision variable necessary to inform judgments in the task. Furthermore, if this information were read out and used to guide judgments, those judgments would match those of the rats.

If the striatal activity we describe above directly supported task performance, manipulating the striatum should modify duration judgments. To test whether this was the case, we bilaterally injected the GABAa receptor agonist muscimol (see Materials and methods). As a result, the duration sensitivity of animals’ judgments dropped significantly as compared to interleaved saline control sessions (Figure 1c; psychometric slope on saline sessions = [1.03 1.20] vs on muscimol sessions = [0.28 0.67]; 95% confidence intervals), yet animals otherwise performed normally. These results, by demonstrating that duration categorization in this task was dependent on a normally functioning striatum, suggest that the neural signals we observed directly supported duration judgments. However, the possibility that muscimol infusions changed other functions important for task performance such as reward processing or memory for the mapping between time and choice can not be ruled out.

Discussion

Attempts to understand the neural mechanisms of time estimation have begun to focus on continuously evolving population dynamics as a general mechanism for time encoding across the brain (MacDonald et al., 2011; Mauk and Buonomano, 2004; Buonomano, 2014; Buonomano and Merzenich, 1995; Gershman et al., 2013). According to this view, time may be encoded by any reproducible pattern of activity across a population of neurons for as long as the pattern is continuously changing and non-repeating. However, no study to date has directly compared the speed of such “population clocks” with the duration judgments of the behaving subjects in which they are found. We show that as rats judged the duration of interval stimuli, striatal neurons displayed dynamics in firing rate that contained information about elapsed time. Furthermore, this information was sufficient to account for the animals’ perceptual decisions, and was not accompanied by systematic differences in outwardly expressed behavior over time. Combined with the observation that striatal inactivation caused a specific decrement in timing performance, these data suggest that striatal dynamics form a central neural representation of time that guides animals’ decisions about duration. Such a coding mechanism in the striatum is well situated to inform the appropriate selection of actions through downstream circuitry involving the globus pallidus, substantia nigra, and various extrinsic connections between the basal ganglia and brainstem, thalamic, and cortical motor areas (Steiner and Tseng, 2010). An intriguing question for future studies is how the striatal dynamics we observed during the interval discrimination task are generated. Neurons in multiple cortical layers spread across the entire cortical mantle, as well as thalamic, pallidal, and neuromodulatory populations provide input to the striatum. While time coding has been assessed in some of these populations, a careful analysis of simultaneously recorded populations that might uncover causal relationships between signals in multiple brain areas has not been carried out. Furthermore, local striatal circuitry may also play a role in shaping dynamics. However, the coding properties tested here could be tested in other brain areas where timing signals have been identified such as the hippocampus (Pastalkova et al., 2008; MacDonald et al., 2011), medial prefrontal (Kim et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2014), parietal (Leon and Shadlen, 2003; Janssen and Shadlen, 2005) and motor (Lebedev et al., 2008) cortices, and the cerebellum (Mauk and Buonomano, 2004), among others. By comparing the signals recorded simultaneously in multiple brain areas during time estimation tasks, it should be possible to identify signatures of functional interaction between brain areas where they exist. Such an approach promises to elucidate where and how time information encoded at the population level is used by the brain to support the myriad time-dependent functions we and other organisms rely on for survival.

Materials and methods

Subject

Six male Long-Evans hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus) between the ages of 6 and 24 months were used for this study. Three rats were used for neural recordings and three rats for pharmacological manipulations. All experiments were in accordance with the European Union Directive 86/609/EEC and approved by the Portuguese Veterinary General Board (Direcção-Geral de Veterinária, project approval 014303 - 0420/000/000/2011).

Behavior

Rats were trained to perform a previously described two-alternative forced-choice timing task (Gouvêa et al., 2014). Briefly, animals had to categorize time intervals as either long or short by making left/right choices. For each session the animals were placed in a custom made behavioral box containing 3 nose ports and a speaker. Each trial was self-initiated by entry into the central nose port and was followed by a pair of brief auditory tones (square pulses at 7,500 Hz, 150 ms) separated by an interval selected randomly out of 8 possible durations (0.6, 1.05, 1.26, 1.38, 1.62, 1.74, 1.95 and 2.4 s). Judgments were reported at two laterally located nose ports. Left responses were reinforced with a drop of water (solenoid valves, Lee Company) after intervals longer than 1.5 seconds, and right responses otherwise. Incorrect responses were punished with a brief white noise burst (150 ms) and a time out (10 s). High speed video (120 fps) was collected from above during task performance. Psychometric functions were fitted using the following two-parameter logistic function

f(x)=11+e-b(x-c)

where b controls the slope and c is the inflection point of the curve.

Electrophysiology

Rats were implanted with 32-channel tungsten microwire moveable array bundles (Figure 2—figure supplement 1a, Innovative Neurophysiology) under isoflurane anaesthesia. All recordings (Figure 2—figure supplement 1) targeted dorsal striatum with coordinates centred at +0.2 mm AP and ± 3 mm ML (rat Bertrand), and +0.84 mm AP and ± 2.5 mm ML (rats Edgar and Fernando), from Bregma. Rats were given a week of post-surgical recovery and array placements were confirmed with histology (Figure 2—figure supplement 1c). Neural signals were recorded at 30 kHz during behavior, amplified and band-pass filtered at 250–750 Hz (Cerebus - Blackrock Microsystems). Each independent bundle was moved 50-100 μm after every recording session to ensure that independent neural populations were sampled across recording sessions. Waveforms corresponding to action potentials from single neurons were sorted offline using principal component analysis (PCA) (offline sorter, Plexon). All remaining analysis were run in Matlab (Mathworks) software. We selected all isolated units with a mean session firing rate >0.5 Hz and from sessions with >70% correct performance (averaged across all stimuli) and a minimum of 250 trials (n=433 cells, 37 recording sessions, 3 animals; rat Bertrand: 136 units, 10 sessions; rat Edgar: 163 units, 9 sessions; rat Fernando: 134 units, 18 sessions). The general result was found in all subjects. Sample size was not computed during study design. To build PSTHs, spikes were counted in 2-ms bins and convolved with a Gaussian kernel with 25-bin standard deviation. PSTHs in Figure 2d were ordered by angular position in the space formed by the first 2 principal components describing firing dynamics (i.e., dimensions are all time bins within interval period, samples are each neuron’s mean PSTH). This method (Geffen et al., 2009) orders cells with respect to their dynamics while taking into consideration the full response profile over the relevant temporal window, and not just a single response feature such as peak response time.

Pharmacology

We implanted 3-mm 20-gauge stainless steel guide cannulae (Bilaney) bilaterally into the striatum of 3 rats [+0.84 mm anterior-posterior (AP), ± 2.5 mm medial-lateral (ML), from Bregma, and -3mm dorsal-ventral (DV, from cortex surface) under isoflurane anesthesia. After one week of post-surgical recovery and 4 days of training, rats were injected with either vehicle (saline, PBS 1x) or muscimol (GABA-A agonist, 100 mg/L (rats Albert and Yuri) and 300 mg/L (rat Zack), SigmaTM) solutions in four alternate days. Two 1-μL syringes (Hamilton), attached to an injection pump (Harvard Apparatus) through 20-gauge internal cannulae that extended 1.5 mm bellow the guide cannulae, injected 0.6 μL of solution per site during 2.5 min. The internal cannulae were left in place for an additional 1.5 min and the rats were given a 45-min recovery period in their home-cage before starting the task. Cannula placements were confirmed by histology (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). The general result was found in all sessions of all subjects. Sample size was not computed during study design.

Preference index

We counted spikes during the last 500 ms of the stimulus period, and built two separate spike count distributions for short and long judgment trials. Next, we used a ROC analysis to measure the separation between distributions (95% bootstrap confidence interval, 1000 iterations). We then transformed the area under the ROC curve (auROC ∈ [0,1]) into a preference index (k = 2*auROC - 1; k ∈ [−1,1]). We adopted the convention that neurons with positive preference indices fired preferentially for long stimuli (Figure 2e).

Low dimensional representations of population state

We refer to the vector describing instantaneous firing rates (measured within 500-ms wide, 10-ms apart, overlapping time bins) across a population of neurons as the population state. The population state vector is a high dimensional variable (i.e., it has as many dimensions as neurons). With the purpose of visualizing population state in 2d plots, we employed standard dimensionality reduction techniques. In Figure 3a, we chose to represent in the abscissa a direction that emphasizes the separability between short and long stimulus trials (i.e., the direction that maximizes variance between groups while minimizing variance within groups; Fisher’s linear discriminant; see below), and in the ordinate the axis of maximal variance that is also orthogonal to the abscissa (i.e. first principal component calculated in the null space of the linear discriminant). In Figure 4a–b, population state is represented in the space formed by the first 2 principal components describing population state, calculated during presentation of the interval for which choice variance is maximal (i.e. dimensions are neurons, samples are averaged spike counts for the time bins within that interval).

Neurometric curves

For each trial in a session we characterized neural population state as a vector r = (r1, r2,... , rN), where rn is the number of spikes fired by neuron n ∈ [1,N] within the last 500 ms of the interval period in that trial. Next, for all trials but one from each session (training set; leave-one-out cross-validation procedure), we found the linear discriminant that best separated population state vectors according to whether they came from long or short interval trials (Fisher’s linear discriminant analysis, LDA). The linear discriminant is given by

w =argmaxwTSBwwTSWw=SW-1(μ1-μ2)

where w is the vector of coefficients for the linear discriminant, SB is the between class covariance, SW the within class covariance and µ1 and µ2 are the means of all points in class 1 and class 2 respectively. A threshold placed along the linear discriminant was then used as a decision rule applied to neural data from the remaining trial (test set). Figure 3a depicts population vectors from an example session (projection 1: linear discriminant, no cross-validation; projection 2: first principal component of the orthogonal subspace; black line: decision rule). We iterated over this procedure until all trials had been tested, thus obtaining for each trial a ‘neural duration judgment’. In analogy with behavioral judgments, we used two parameter logistic fits to obtain a quantitative description of the performance of simultaneously recorded neurons in categorizing stimuli -the neurometric function (Figure 3b, orange curve).

Videometric curves

Full session videos (256x192 pixels resolution) were cut into 3-s long clips with Bonsai (Lopes et al., 2015). Individual frames from approximately 75 ms before interval offset were used for this analysis (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). This buffer was added to ensure that all frames used preceded stimulus offset. Images were first represented as vectors composed of individual pixel luminance values. Given that image sequences tend to lie on curved low dimensional manifolds in pixel space (Pless, 2003), any slight differences in behavioral state reflected in images collected at the offset of short and long interval categories are not necessarily expected to be linearly separable. Thus, we employed isomap (Tenenbaum and Silva, 2000), a non-linear dimensionality reduction method, to obtain an information rich yet low dimensional representation of animals’ ongoing behavior. This approach has the advantage over tracking methods that it does not make assumptions as to what part of the animals’ movements might provide information about stimulus category. The neighborhood size, used to compute the shortest paths between data points, was set to 25 frames to minimize, on average, the dimensionality at which the reconstruction error elbow occurred. In analogy with the neurometric curves, for each stimulus type, we then trained a linear discriminant (leave-one-out cross-validation procedure) to classify frames into those that were recorded during trials where a ‘short’ or ‘long’ stimulus interval was presented. The classification was performed in the reduced space determined by isomap. As a positive control for the method, we repeated the same analysis for frames captured at the moment animals expressed their judgment by inserting their snout at one of the two choice ports (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Here, the neighborhood size was chosen to be the minimum for which all frames (from a single session) could be included in a single embedding. This analysis was done for all usable videos (8 out of 11) of sessions in the upper tercile with regard to population size.

Time course of classification performance from neural and video data

To compare how the decoding performance using neural and video data evolved over time, the classification analyses described in Neurometric curves and Videometric curves was performed every 100 ms within a one second window centered around stimulus offset. Video frames at the each time point and neural data in a 200 ms time bin terminating at each time point were used for the analysis. This generated neural and video classification curves that described the ability of simultaneously recorded neural ensembles and video frames to correctly classify interval stimuli as long or short (Figure 3d). To determine the relative timing of classification ability in neural ensembles and behavior, we regressed the neural classification curve against the video classification curve for shifts ranging from −300 ms to 300 ms in 100 ms steps (Figure 3e).

Psychometric curves split by population state at interval offset

We projected neural activity (of populations composed of simultaneously recorded neurons) on individual trials in high dimensional neural space onto the mean trajectory of those neurons during the delay period for correct trials. Neural activity was defined as the vector of firing rates of the population obtained by convolving spike trains using a causal kernel given by gamma density function with parameters θ = 100 ms and k = 2. We normalized these projections by the length of the mean trajectory of that group of neurons for the longest interval. Pooling normalized projections over all sessions for each animal, we plotted, for each stimulus, distributions of normalized projections at interval offset. To test whether distance traversed along the mean trajectory is predictive of animals' perceptual report, we separated the distribution of pooled projections for each stimulus into 3 bins. Psychometric curves were constructed using trials from each bin. To quantify the key differences between each of these psychometric curves, we performed model comparison using the following 4 parameter logistic function

f(x)=d+a-d1+e-b(x-c)

where b controls the slope, c is the inflection point and a and d are the maximum and minimum values of the curve respectively. For two of three animals (Bertrand and Edgar), the model that best accounted for the differences between the three curves (based on Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) scores) was one with only horizontal shifts between the curves. In the third animal (Fernando), the model that best fit the data was one in which the fit to the three curves differed in both horizontal shift and slope.

A trial's projection on the mean trajectory can be interpreted as a method for decoding time from neural state. Hence, trials that are outliers in the distribution of projections on the mean could potentially correspond to trials where the animal was disengaged. To remove such trials we defined a fraction (60%) of normalized trajectory around the mode of the distribution of pooled projections for each stimulus and excluded trials with projections outside this window.

Population decoder

We decoded elapsed time from striatal population activity using a cross validated, flat prior naive Bayes decoder. For each neuron n ∈ [1,N], spike counts rn were observed in 500-ms wide, 10-ms apart, overlapping time bins within the interval period (time referring to the right edge of the bin). For a given rn, the probability that the current time is t was estimated as the likelihood of observing rspikes at time t:

P(t|rn)P(rn|t)

To obtain the likelihood term P(r| t), we estimated the joint distribution P(rn, t) by computing, for each time bin, a weighted histogram of spike counts across all correct trials. For trials in which stimulus interval i was presented, spike counts contributed to the histogram with weight widefined as the normalized choice variance associated with that interval, 

wi=P(CS|i)P(CL|i)

 where CS and CLindicate short and long choices respectively. As a result, near boundary interval trials had a greater influence on the estimate of the joint distribution. Histograms were then smoothed using local linear regression (lowess) and normalized to unit area. When decoding from correct trials, leave-one-out cross validation was implemented by computing the joint distribution from all correct trials but the decoded one; incorrect trials were decoded using an estimate of the joint distribution computed from all correct trials. Multi-session population state vectors r = (r1, r2,... , rN) were obtained by concatenating together data from trials of same stimulus and choice type. By assuming statistical independence between spike counts of different neurons in r, we could compute population estimates of t as the product of single neuron estimates:

P(t|r)n=1NP(rn|t)

Data presented is the average over 100 random concatenations.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Bassam Atallah, Brian Lau and Masayoshi Murakami for reading earlier versions of the manuscript, Gustavo Mello for help with surgeries, the Champalimaud Research Vivarium and Histology platforms, Serkan Sülün for pre-processing of video data, João Frazão and Gonçalo Lopes for help with Video 1, João Semedo for technical discussions regarding Isomap, and Bruno Ceña for logistic support.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Champalimaud Foundation to Thiago S Gouvêa, Tiago Monteiro, Asma Motiwala, Sofia Soares, Christian Machens, Joseph J Paton.

  • Simons Foundation SCGB #325476 to Tiago Monteiro, Joseph J Paton.

  • Bial Foundation BIAL-BIC-188/12 to Thiago S Gouvêa, Tiago Monteiro, Sofia Soares, Joseph J Paton.

  • Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology to Thiago S Gouvêa, Asma Motiwala, Sofia Soares.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

TSG, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

TM, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

AM, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

SS, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

CM, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

JJP, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were in accordance with the European Union Directive 86/609/EEC and approved by the Portuguese Veterinary General Board (Direcção-Geral de Veterinária, project approval 014303 - 0420/000/000/2011).

References

  1. Adler A, Katabi S, Finkes I, Israel Z, Prut Y, Bergman H. Temporal convergence of dynamic cell assemblies in the striato-pallidal network. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:2473–2484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4830-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Neural representations of the target (goal) of visually guided arm movements in three motor areas of the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1990;64:164–178. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.1.164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Britten KH, Newsome WT, Shadlen MN, Celebrini S, Movshon JA. A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT. Visual Neuroscience. 1996;13:87–100. doi: 10.1017/S095252380000715X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Buonomano D, Merzenich M. Temporal information transformed into a spatial code by a neural network with realistic properties. Science. 1995;267:1028–1030. doi: 10.1126/science.7863330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Buonomano DV. Neurobiology of Interval Timing. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2014. Neural Dynamics Based Timing in the Subsecond to Seconds Range; pp. 101–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Gregor Fetterman J, Killeen PR, Hall S. Watching the clock. Behavioural Processes. 1998;44:211–224. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(98)00050-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Geffen MN, Broome BM, Laurent G, Meister M. Neural encoding of rapidly fluctuating odors. Neuron. 2009;61:570–586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gershman SJ, Moustafa AA, Ludvig EA. Time representation in reinforcement learning models of the basal ganglia. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2013;7 doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gouvêa TS, Monteiro T, Soares S, Atallah BV, Paton JJ. Ongoing behavior predicts perceptual report of interval duration. Frontiers in Neurorobotics. 2014;8 doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2014.00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Harrington DL, Zimbelman JL, Hinton SC, Rao SM. Neural modulation of temporal encoding, maintenance, and decision processes. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;bhp194 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hinton SC, Meck WH. Frontal-striatal circuitry activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra-seconds range. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 2004;21:171–182. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Janssen P, Shadlen MN. A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:234–241. doi: 10.1038/nn1386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jin DZ, Fujii N, Graybiel AM. Neural representation of time in cortico-basal ganglia circuits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:19156–19161. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909881106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Jin X, Costa RM. Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature. 2010;466:457–462. doi: 10.1038/nature09263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Killeen PR, Fetterman JG. A behavioral theory of timing. Psychological Review. 1988;95:274–295. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kim J, Ghim JW, Lee JH, Jung MW. Neural correlates of interval timing in rodent prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:13834–13847. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lau B, Glimcher PW. Value representations in the primate striatum during matching behavior. Neuron. 2008;58:451–463. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lebedev MA, O'Doherty JE, Nicolelis MA. Decoding of temporal intervals from cortical ensemble activity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;99:166–186. doi: 10.1152/jn.00734.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lee AM, Tai L-H, Zador A, Wilbrecht L. Between the primate and ‘reptilian’ brain: rodent models demonstrate the role of corticostriatal circuits in decision making. Neuroscience. 2015;296:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Leon MI, Shadlen MN. Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron. 2003;38:317–327. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00185-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lopes Gonçalo, Bonacchi Niccolò, Frazão João, Neto JP, Atallah BV, Soares S, Moreira Luis, Matias S, Itskov PM, Correia Patrícia A, Medina RE, Calcaterra L, Dreosti E, Paton JJ, Kampff AR. Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streams. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fninf.2015.00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. MacDonald CJ, Lepage KQ, Eden UT, Eichenbaum H. Hippocampal "time cells" bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron. 2011;71:737–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Machado A. Learning the temporal dynamics of behavior. Psychological Review. 1997;104:241–265. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Machado A, Keen R. Temporal discrimination in a long operant chamber. Behavioural Processes. 2003;62:157–182. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00023-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Malapani C, Rakitin B, Levy R, Meck WH, Deweer B, Dubois B, Gibbon J. Coupled temporal memories in parkinson's disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1998;10:316–331. doi: 10.1162/089892998562762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Matell MS, Meck WH, Nicolelis MAL. Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;117:760–773. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.4.760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Matthews TJ, Lerer BE. Behavior patterns in pigeons during autoshaping with an incremental conditioned stimulus. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1987;15:69–75. doi: 10.3758/BF03204906. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mauk MD, Buonomano DV. The neural basis of temporal processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:307–340. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Meck WH. Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: a functional link between mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems. Brain Research. 2006;1109:93–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mello GBM, Soares S, Paton JJ. A scalable population code for time in the striatum. Current Biology. 2015;25:1113–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsáki G. Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:1322–1327. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pless R. Image spaces and video trajectories: using isomap to explore video sequences. 2003;3:1433–1440. [Google Scholar]
  33. Samejima K, Ueda Y, Doya K, Kimura M. Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science. 2005;310:1337–1340. doi: 10.1126/science.1115270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Steiner H, Tseng KY. Handbook of Basal Ganglia Structure and Function: A Decade of Progress 2010 [Google Scholar]
  35. Tenenbaum JB, de Silva V, Langford JC. A global geometric framework for nonlinear dimensionality reduction. Science. 2000;290:2319–2323. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5500.2319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Wencil EB, Coslett HB, Aguirre GK, Chatterjee A. Carving the clock at its component joints: neural bases for interval timing. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:160–168. doi: 10.1152/jn.00029.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Xu M, Zhang S-y, Dan Y, Poo M-m. Representation of interval timing by temporally scalable firing patterns in rat prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111:480–485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1321314111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2015 Dec 7;4:e11386. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11386.018

Decision letter

Editor: Timothy Behrens1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Striatal dynamics explain duration judgments" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Timothy Behrens as the Senior and Reviewing Editor.

The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal his identity: Patrick Simen (Reviewer).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This paper addresses an important and understudied problem: how does the brain encode time on the scale of a second or so? The authors use a behavioral task in which rats are trained to nose poke left or right depending on the interval between two auditory tones – specifically, whether the interval was short or long in relation to a 1.5 s reference. Striatal recordings from hundreds of cells revealed a robust temporal code: it was possible to determine elapsed time from the onset of the trial based on the current population vector with a high degree of accuracy. Furthermore, it was possible to predict the incorrect behavioral responses based on whether the population trajectory was "running early" or "running late". Together with timing-effects of muscimol infusions in striatum (although see concerns below), these data provide and important physiological link between striatal processing and sensory timing.

Essential revisions:

1) The reviewers raised serious concerns about the muscimol data.

First, the data are from two rats. The reviewers do not think it is easy to make interpretations from two animals and ask that you increase the number. Whilst it is eLife policy not to ask for major new data acquisition under revision, the reviewing editor agrees that this is important to solidify the results in this case and believe that you could collect the data within the two month framework. The reviewers think that a good benchmark for a muscimol experiment is 5-6 animals. Please let us know early if you will not be able to perform the extra data acquisition.

Second, the reviewers were concerned that it is very difficult to tell where the cannulae are from the graphic (they are many hundreds of microns wide) – they might very well be in the dorsolateral striatum. Actual histology would be better.

Lastly in this section, there were questions about the behavioural effects of muscimol.

Why didn't muscimol systematically retard or advance the timer if it is truly a clock? It appears that muscimol infusions just increased the variability of the choices, but that doesn't mean that the striatum isn't just listening to a "clock" somewhere else in the brain. I would love to see that a striatal clock could, for example be physiologically sped up or slowed down, as in the amphetamine studies of Meck and colleagues, but I don't think that's what's shown here. Given the role of striatum in reward-guided behavior, I could imagine that inactivating it just makes the task not worth paying attention to, or that the decision stage is disrupted while the "clock", wherever it is, is still intact.

2) With respect to the video analysis and controls for movement.

The reviewers thought that the video analysis likely did a good job controlling for motion effects, but did not think they were clearly presented and had some specific concerns (see point from reviewer directly below). In the discussion, one reviewer suggested including a video that could make the classification clearer. There were also suggestions that the claim could be strengthened by including basic motor control tasks (open-field, circling, paw-rearing) in the muscimol animals, but in the discussion it was agreed that whilst such data would be convincing if you have it, this is not essential.

Specific point from reviewer:

I don't understand the author's valiant effort to use classifiers to analyze movement. Traditionally, force-controlled or highly stereotyped motor tasks (see work by Whishaw) are required – in this domain, video assessing kinematics is useful. As presented, I'm not sure if we're looking at movement preparation, licking, reward expectation, etc. See relevant work by Cowan et al., and David Redish on the issue of 'embodied' movement. The animals' behavioral reports are most helpful; as these at least have some consistent events.

3) There were also questions pertaining to the analysis and interpretation of the neural data.

I don't think the evidence is particularly strong that time is specifically encoded in the striatum in the way proposed. It seems plausible to me that the striatal neurons are just encoding an output stage of the decision process, in which a short or long choice is preferred. Yes, it is possible to decode subjective time from it, but I would imagine you could do that with population activity recorded from many different brain areas (in particular, areas examined in several of the papers cited here).

and relatedly:

This paper provides nice support for the notion that time is encoded in the evolving population dynamics of neurons. The authors also provide some evidence that the striatum is directly involved in the task. Less clear, and admittedly far outside the scope of this paper or any other single paper, is whether the code/dynamics is generated within the striatum. A few sentences as to the authors' thoughts on this issue would be useful, and help guide future research.

With respect to the following comment from one reviewer – in the discussion the reviewers and editor agreed that the figures could be clarified, but leave it up to you whether you think this can be best achieved using a PCA.

The neuronal analyses are unclear to me. In Figure 2, The rasters are helpful, as are the averages, but I'd favor something like PCA to help explain the dataset, as it seems like all patterns are represented in their large population (Figure 2D). For Figure 3C is the color population size in terms of neurons? I think I don’t understand what is being plotted in Figure 3F? I'd like more detail on the 'neurometric' ideal observer – it'd be nice to see the predictions for a single session, and for single population, and even a single neuron as I'm not sure how much neuron population size matters (is that Figure 3C?), and I'm not sure what the 'density' in Figure 3G shows – or the corresponding plots in the supplement. At the end of the day, is it surprising that striatal neurons predict temporal durations? Also, could the authors define 'psychometric'? It's not clear what this means. Figure 3 likely needs to be greatly clarified for most audiences.

4) The reviewers were not clear on where in the striatum the authors are interested in. The author's electrodes span the whole striatum, and dorsolateral striatum is completely different than dorsomedial striatum. See work from Henry Yin. Some electrodes even seem to reach ventral striatum.

eLife. 2015 Dec 7;4:e11386. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11386.019

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The reviewers raised serious concerns about the muscimol data.First, the data are from two rats. The reviewers do not think it is easy to make interpretations from two animals and ask that you increase the number. Whilst it is eLife policy not to ask for major new data acquisition under revision, the reviewing editor agrees that this is important to solidify the results in this case and believe that you could collect the data within the two month framework. The reviewers think that a good benchmark for a muscimol experiment is 5-6 animals. Please let us know early if you will not be able to perform the extra data acquisition.

We have now included additional experiments involving the infusion of muscimol into the striatum during task performance. This brings the total numbers to 6 muscimol and 6 saline sessions performed on adjacent days in three different animals (2 saline and 2 muscimol infusions in each animal). In all individual sessions, muscimol infusions produced a significant decrease in the sensitivity of animals’ judgments to interval duration as compared to saline infusions performed on the previous day. It is important to point out that these are reversible inactivations, not permanent lesions, allowing us to collect multiple control and test conditions within each animal. Thus, we believe there should be no doubt that these results reflect a general effect of infusing muscimol on task performance. However, given the slow time course of pharmacology, interpretive issues do remain (see below).

Second, the reviewers were concerned that it is very difficult to tell where the cannulae are from the graphic (they are many hundreds of microns wide) – they might very well be in the dorsolateral striatum. Actual histology would be better.

We have now included histology and have updated our schematics in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 to reflect the width of the infusion cannulae above the striatum.

Lastly in this section, there were questions about the behavioural effects of muscimol.

Why didn't muscimol systematically retard or advance the timer if it is truly a clock? It appears that muscimol infusions just increased the variability of the choices, but that doesn't mean that the striatum isn't just listening to a "clock" somewhere else in the brain. I would love to see that a striatal clock could, for example be physiologically sped up or slowed down, as in the amphetamine studies of Meck and colleagues, but I don't think that's what's shown here. Given the role of striatum in reward-guided behavior, I could imagine that inactivating it just makes the task not worth paying attention to, or that the decision stage is disrupted while the "clock", wherever it is, is still intact.

The finding that striatal muscimol infusions reduced the sensitivity of animal’s judgments only shows that signals contributed by the striatum are necessary for normal task performance. Muscimol could have had an effect by changing reward processing, memory for a mapping between time and choice, and/or by making less reliable the representation of the decision variable, elapsed time, that is the major focus of the physiology in the paper. We have now included language explaining this interpretive issue (Results). The experiment that the reviewer seems to be asking for is one wherein we systematically speed or slow the spatiotemporal patterns of activity within striatal populations. Indeed we would love to do such an experiment, although accomplishing this is not trivial and furthermore we believe it is beyond the scope of this paper. Such an experiment would require transiently altering spatiotemporal population dynamics on a small fraction of trials so as to prevent the animal from adapting its choices to the reward feedback it might receive from altered patterns of judgment.

2) With respect to the video analysis and controls for movement.

The reviewers thought that the video analysis likely did a good job controlling for motion effects, but did not think they were clearly presented and had some specific concerns (see point from reviewer directly below). In the discussion, one reviewer suggested including a video that could make the classification clearer.

We have now included a supplementary figure and Video 1 to give readers a sense of ongoing behavior during the task.

There were also suggestions that the claim could be strengthened by including basic motor control tasks (open-field, circling, paw-rearing) in the muscimol animals, but in the discussion it was agreed that whilst such data would be convincing if you have it, this is not essential.

We did not collect such data, although we agree that it might be informative to collect in the future.

Specific point from reviewer:

I don't understand the author's valiant effort to use classifiers to analyze movement. Traditionally, force-controlled or highly stereotyped motor tasks (see work by Whishaw) are required – in this domain, video assessing kinematics is useful. As presented, I'm not sure if we're looking at movement preparation, licking, reward expectation, etc. See relevant work by Cowan et al., and David Redish on the issue of 'embodied' movement.

We are well aware of the possibility that embodied strategies could play an important role in many instances of timing as well as other cognitive processes. In fact we previously published a paper that examined snout position in relation to timing judgments. However, we were underwhelmed by the power of tracking approaches to detect more subtle differences in behavior that might explain variance in neural firing collected during our experiments. By treating individual images as points in the space defined by the luminance of all of the pixels in the frame, we avoid a pitfall of selective annotation of videos, computer vision, etc. which only consider a subset of the information contained in the image. This was our motivation for the approach to video analysis contained in the paper. We point out that animals cannot lick at the reward ports during the task epochs that we analyze, as this would result in an aborted trial.

The animals' behavioral reports are most helpful; as these at least have some consistent events.

We agree with the reviewer that the behavioral reports are helpful, indeed they play a central role in most of the analyses of neural data in relation to behavior currently contained in the paper.

3) There were also questions pertaining to the analysis and interpretation of the neural data.

I don't think the evidence is particularly strong that time is specifically encoded in the striatum in the way proposed. It seems plausible to me that the striatal neurons are just encoding an output stage of the decision process, in which a short or long choice is preferred.

Were striatal neurons just encoding the output of a decision, we would expect neural activity to occupy two states, with perhaps a transition period in between those two states. We do not observe such a pattern, as evidenced by the distribution of response profiles over time (Figure 2D, Figure 2—figure supplement 2E, J, O) and our ability to decode time continuously (Figure 4, Figure 4—figure supplements 1 and 2) from the population. That said, we do not claim that information about the animals’ decisions is not also contained in the population.

Yes, it is possible to decode subjective time from it, but I would imagine you could do that with population activity recorded from many different brain areas (in particular, areas examined in several of the papers cited here).

This sentence seems to directly conflict the previous one, wherein the reviewer proposes that the striatum only encodes the output of a decision process. That aside, we do not doubt that signals related to elapsed time could be recorded from other brain areas. Time is fundamental to many diverse functions that the brain is tasked with carrying out.

However, a representation of time is necessary for the assumed functionality of the basal ganglia in learning to associate stimuli and actions with reward. Our work is the first to demonstrate that information about elapsed time contained in the evolving population dynamics of striatal neurons reflects time information used by the animals’ to guide their duration judgments.

and relatedly:

This paper provides nice support for the notion that time is encoded in the evolving population dynamics of neurons. The authors also provide some evidence that the striatum is directly involved in the task. Less clear, and admittedly far outside the scope of this paper or any other single paper, is whether the code/dynamics is generated within the striatum. A few sentences as to the authors' thoughts on this issue would be useful, and help guide future research.

We agree with the reviewer that this issue could be discussed in greater depth and we have now added text in the Discussion to expand on this important point.

With respect to the following comment from one reviewer – in the discussion the reviewers and editor agreed that the figures could be clarified, but leave it up to you whether you think this can be best achieved using a PCA.

The neuronal analyses are unclear to me. In Figure 2, The rasters are helpful, as are the averages, but I'd favor something like PCA to help explain the dataset, as it seems like all patterns are represented in their large population (Figure 2D).

Indeed, Figure 4A, B and Figure 4—figure supplement 2A, E, I contain projections of the neural data onto the first two principal components of the data.

For Figure 3C is the color population size in terms of neurons?

Yes, population size refers to the size of the simultaneously recorded population on each session.

I think I don’t understand what is being plotted in Figure 3F? I'd like more detail on the 'neurometric' ideal observer –

We direct the reviewer’s attention to the section of the Materials and methods titled ‘neurometric curves’ for a detailed description of the ideal observer analysis.

it'd be nice to see the predictions for a single session, and for single population,

We agree that it is nice to see single session examples, which in this case are also single population examples since only simultaneously recorded populations are included in this analysis. Indeed we had included such a single session, single population, example in Figure 3A, B.

and even a single neuron as I'm not sure how much neuron population size matters (is that Figure 3C?),

The single neuron ideal observer analysis corresponds to the receiver operating characteristic analysis displayed in Figure 2 and extended Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

and I'm not sure what the 'density' in Figure 3G shows – or the corresponding plots in the supplement.

We have now updated the figure to label the axis in question as “Trial density” to make it more clear that density refers to the density of single trial population states as a function of the fraction of mean neural trajectory traversed.

At the end of the day, is it surprising that striatal neurons predict temporal durations?

We believe it is less surprising that striatal, or any other, neurons predict time than that they predict time in a manner that agrees with the trial to trial variations in temporal judgments exhibited by a behaving subject. Furthermore, many models for how time is encoded by neural systems exist, and our data provides support for a subset of these models. Hence, if the reviewer is asking whether our data is surprising in the sense that it provides information about how timing on the scale of seconds is accomplished by the brain, then yes, we believe these results are surprising and thus informative.

Also, could the authors define 'psychometric'? It's not clear what this means.

The psychometric function, a standard term in sensory-based decision tasks, is an “inferential model applied in detection and discrimination tasks. It models the relationship between a given feature of the physical stimulus, e.g. velocity, duration, brightness, weight etc., and the forced-choice responses of the subject.”

Figure 3 likely needs to be greatly clarified for most audiences.

Without more specific instructions, we are not sure how to proceed beyond the changes we list above.

4) The reviewers were not clear on where in the striatum the authors are interested in. The author's electrodes span the whole striatum, and dorsolateral striatum is completely different than dorsomedial striatum. See work from Henry Yin. Some electrodes even seem to reach ventral striatum.

We have now included text highlighting our interest in the dorso-central striatum, an area where lesions have been shown previously to produce timing deficits (Results, second paragraph).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. The. txt file contains trial by trials stimulus (Interval), choice (choiceLong), animal (Name), treatment (MuscimolDose) and session (Date).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.004

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11386.004
    Figure 2—source data 1. Folder with raw data for Figures 24.

    Each file in the source data folder is named “‘<animal>_<session>"’, and contains an 8x2 cell "‘spikeCounts"’. For each of the eight stimuli and two choices, "‘spikeCounts"’ contains a matrix of spike counts with dimensions:number of neurons x number of trials x number of time steps. The stimuli (delay to the second tone in seconds) and choices are ordered as follows:STIM_SET = [0.60 1.05 1.26 1.38 1.62 1.74 1.95 2.4];CHOICES = ['incorrect' 'correct'];The spike counts are given from 500 ms prior to trial initiation up to 500 ms after the time of the second tone and were binned in 2 ms wide bins.This folder also includes underlying data for Figures 3 and 4.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11386.007

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11386.007

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES