The American Thoracic Society (ATS) official statement defines dyspnea as “a subjective experience of breathing discomfort” (1). We and others have previously urged that healthcare workers should routinely assess and document dyspnea in the same manner as pain. Outpatients most often report experiencing dyspnea during exertion, which can severely limit their activities, but at least this dyspnea can be quickly escaped by ceasing the activity, and that is what patients do: “breathlessness makes you slow right down, like a car running out of gas and it makes you feel exhausted, one has a desire to take a deep breath but the body can't do it” (2). Patients who experience dyspnea in their hospital bed are in a different situation: they cannot escape, and it is up to us to relieve their suffering. Many clinicians, having never personally experienced such inescapable dyspnea, do not fully understand its effect. Listen to what patients have to say about it: “I often thought about death while I was attacked by dyspnea”; “I wondered what’s going on with my breathing I asked myself ‘will I die here?’”; “I did not have any preparation for those uncontrolled discomforts, and this made me fearful” (mechanically ventilated intensive care unit patients described in Reference 3). “[I]t is a frightened feeling where you don't think you'll get another breath . . . it is accompanied by fear and panic and feeling tight”; “when the shortness of breath was at its extreme, I thought I was going to die and saw a coffin beside me. . . . I did have thoughts about suicide and I envied the dead” (cancer outpatients described in Reference 2).
Our hospital recently began routine documentation of inpatients’ dyspnea on the same schedule as pain assessment, both at admission and on each nursing shift (4, 5). We interviewed nurses about the process, and there was wide agreement that the process was easy, quick, and important. We discovered, however, that on some occasions, nurses were not asking the patients to rate how they felt, but, rather, were inferring the intensity of dyspnea from observed signs. The ATS official statement strongly emphasizes that “dyspnea per se can only be perceived by the person experiencing it.” This statement derives from the definition of a symptom (sensations experienced or perceived by an individual) and provides the basis for distinguishing a symptom from a “sign” (an observed or elicited physical finding). Little evidence is available to refute or support the assertion that clinicians can accurately judge a patient’s current breathing discomfort based on observation of behaviors and signs. A seminal report on dyspnea during mechanical ventilation by Lush and colleagues produced “the serendipitous finding that a discrepancy appeared to exist between the patient’s perception of his or her own dyspnea and the nurse’s perception of the patient’s dyspnea” (6). A later study of 33 cancer outpatients found that physicians underestimated dyspnea, missing about half the cases of patients who reported moderate or severe dyspnea (7).
In this issue of the Journal, Haugdahl and colleagues (pp. 1440–1448) provide the best systematic study to date on the correspondence (or lack thereof) between clinicians’ estimates versus patients’ reports of acute dyspnea (8). They collected patients’ ratings paired with both physicians’ and nurses’ estimates of dyspnea for 100 intensive care unit patients undergoing spontaneous breathing trials. This is a good population to study because it is likely to provide ample dyspnea to estimate; at the same time, it is a challenging population because patients may not fully understand what is being asked of them, there may be crosstalk between ratings of dyspnea and other discomforts such as the endotracheal tube and the anxiety associated with the environment, and their responses may be affected by residual effects of analgesic and sedative medications and/or delirium associated with their acute illness. The findings are instructive: There was a very poor correspondence between clinician estimate and patient report; in fact, the relationship seems almost random.
So what is going on here? First, training and experience were not related to success in estimating dyspnea, according to Haugdahl and colleagues’ analysis. Thus, it would seem that dyspnea is inherently difficult to estimate on the basis of observations of behavior and physical findings. Second, observations of respiratory signs are probably not random: our analysis of Haugdal’s data suggests there was a much higher correlation between nurses and doctors than between either clinician group and patients (r2 = 0.4 vs. r2 < 0.2). Clinicians’ estimates of respiratory distress may be an independently useful measure, but they do not correspond to the patient’s discomfort. Unfortunately, clinicians usually underestimate discomfort, making it likely that the symptom is not addressed appropriately.
In this study, clinicians did not use a systematic rating scheme for signs; this reflects usual practice in most institutions. There is one instrument intended to deduce dyspnea from observed signs in patients who cannot communicate that computes a score from eight parameters; however, it does not show a particularly close relationship to patient report; r2 versus patient report was 0.2 (9). We hope to see future research on the cues used by those clinicians who are successful at estimating dyspnea from signs.
In our laboratory, about 85% of subjects show a good correlation between their rating of breathing discomfort and the intensity of controlled stimuli producing that discomfort (threshold for “good” is defined as r2 above 0.5, but the median r2 is greater than 0.7). People can therefore reliably rate respiratory discomfort under controlled conditions. But the signs visible from the outside may be subtle and confusing, even in the laboratory. Clinicians are often suspicious of “subjective” reports, particularly when the patient “appears” comfortable, because they are well aware of reporting biases (stoicism, somatization, secondary gain, difficulty in using number scales, etc.) that may influence the report, but the patient is the only person who actually knows what he or she is feeling, so it is worth asking.
Despite the distortions of reporting bias, patient reports of dyspnea are much more effective in predicting COPD mortality than pulmonary function tests (10). Dyspnea is also a strong predictor of coronary death (11) and cancer death (e.g., refs. 12–14). So a lot of information must be getting through, despite the distortion. A secondary outcome of the present study hints that dyspnea ratings may help predict weaning success. Prior work has already told us that weaning protocols are more accurate than the individual assessment of clinicians (15). The rapid shallow breathing index remains our best single predictive criterion. Whether the addition of patient ratings of dyspnea will enhance the accuracy of our assessment of patient readiness to breathe on their own remains to be determined.
In the cohort described by Haugdahl and colleagues, far more than half the patients experienced moderate to severe dyspnea during the spontaneous breathing trial (8). A previous report showed a high prevalence of moderate to severe dyspnea during mechanical ventilation (16). This frightening form of discomfort should be addressed to minimize suffering and subsequent stress disorder. The presence of dyspnea and pain are predictive of post–intensive care unit post-traumatic stress disorder (17). We need to follow the pathway developed for pain and ask patients what they feel. Dyspnea can alert us to pathophysiological problems, and we need to address those problems. Beyond this, there are pharmacological and nonpharmacological tools to provide relief from dyspnea (18, 19). Among these tools, comfort and reassurance can be effective during mechanical ventilation (20); simply counseling the patient on what to expect and showing your awareness of his or her situation during mechanical ventilation and weaning may help him or her deal with the discomfort he or she is experiencing.
“I appreciated what that nurse said, ‘I understood your discomforts. Try to follow the new breathing rhythm . . . yes, good, you’re doing better . . . concentrate on your new breathing, I’m here to watch you, don’t worry’” (intensive care unit patient describing successful nursing intervention).
We cannot know when to deploy symptom management tools unless we ask how the patient feels. Ask your patient about her breathing discomfort; don’t just look at her.
Footnotes
Supported by National Institutes of Health grant NR10006.
Author disclosures are available with the text of this article at www.atsjournals.org.
References
- 1.Parshall MB, Schwartzstein RM, Adams L, Banzett RB, Manning HL, Bourbeau J, Calverley PM, Gift AG, Harver A, Lareau SC, et al. An official American Thoracic Society statement: update on the mechanisms, assessment, and management of dyspnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2012;185:435–452. doi: 10.1164/rccm.201111-2042ST. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.O’Driscoll M, Corner J, Bailey C. The experience of breathlessness in lung cancer. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl) 1999;8:37–43. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2354.1999.00129.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Shih FJ, Chu SH. Comparisons of American-Chinese and Taiwanese patients’ perceptions of dyspnea and helpful nursing actions during the intensive care unit transition from cardiac surgery. Heart Lung. 1999;28:41–54. doi: 10.1016/s0147-9563(99)70042-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Baker K, Barsamian J, Leone D, Donovan BC, Williams D, Carnevale K, Lansing R, Banzett R. Routine dyspnea assessment on unit admission. Am J Nurs. 2013;113:42–49. doi: 10.1097/01.NAJ.0000437112.43059.a0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Baker K, Stevens J, Anderson L, Bernstein H, O’Donnell C, Howell M, Banzett R. Dyspnea assessed by nurses every shift: prevalence and risk prediction in a pilot study. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2013;189:A1787. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lush MT, Janson-Bjerklie S, Carrieri VK, Lovejoy N. Dyspnea in the ventilator-assisted patient. Heart Lung. 1988;17:528–535. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hayes AW, Philip J, Spruyt OW. Patient reporting and doctor recognition of dyspnoea in a comprehensive cancer centre. Intern Med J. 2006;36:381–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1445-5994.2006.01094.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Haugdahl HS, Storli SL, Meland B, Dybwik K, Romild U, Klepstad P.Underestimation of patient breathlessness by nurses and physicians during a spontaneous breathing trial Am J Respir Crit Care Med 20151921440–1448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Campbell ML, Templin TN. Intensity cut-points for the respiratory distress observation scale. Palliat Med. 2015;29:436–442. doi: 10.1177/0269216314564238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nishimura K, Izumi T, Tsukino M, Oga T. Dyspnea is a better predictor of 5-year survival than airway obstruction in patients with COPD. Chest. 2002;121:1434–1440. doi: 10.1378/chest.121.5.1434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Abidov A, Rozanski A, Hachamovitch R, Hayes SW, Aboul-Enein F, Cohen I, Friedman JD, Germano G, Berman DS. Prognostic significance of dyspnea in patients referred for cardiac stress testing. N Engl J Med. 2005;353:1889–1898. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa042741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Djärv T, Metcalfe C, Avery KN, Lagergren P, Blazeby JM. Prognostic value of changes in health-related quality of life scores during curative treatment for esophagogastric cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28:1666–1670. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2009.23.5143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Heyse-Moore LH, Ross V, Mullee MA. How much of a problem is dyspnoea in advanced cancer? Palliat Med. 1991;5:20–26. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Escalante CP, Martin CG, Elting LS, Cantor SB, Harle TS, Price KJ, Kish SK, Manzullo EF, Rubenstein EB. Dyspnea in cancer patients: etiology, resource utilization, and survival-implications in a managed care world. Cancer. 1996;78:1314–1319. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0142(19960915)78:6<1314::AID-CNCR21>3.0.CO;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.McConville JF, Kress JP. Weaning patients from the ventilator. N Engl J Med. 2012;367:2233–2239. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1203367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schmidt M, Demoule A, Polito A, Porchet R, Aboab J, Siami S, Morelot-Panzini C, Similowski T, Sharshar T. Dyspnea in mechanically ventilated critically ill patients. Crit Care Med. 2011;39:2059–2065. doi: 10.1097/CCM.0b013e31821e8779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Schelling G. Effects of stress hormones on traumatic memory formation and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder in critically ill patients. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;78:596–609. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bausewein C, Booth S, Gysels M, Higginson I. Non-pharmacological interventions for breathlessness in advanced stages of malignant and non-malignant diseases. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2008;(2):CD005623. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005623.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Booth S, Moosavi SH, Higginson IJ. The etiology and management of intractable breathlessness in patients with advanced cancer: a systematic review of pharmacological therapy. Nat Clin Pract Oncol. 2008;5:90–100. doi: 10.1038/ncponc1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Strøm T, Martinussen T, Toft P. A protocol of no sedation for critically ill patients receiving mechanical ventilation: a randomised trial. Lancet. 2010;375:475–480. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)62072-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]