Abstract
The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans stops feeding and moving during a larval transition stage called lethargus and following exposure to cellular stressors. These behaviors have been termed “sleep-like states”. We argue that these behaviors should instead be called “sleep”. Sleep during lethargus is similar to sleep regulated by circadian timers in insects and mammals, and sleep in response to cellular stress is similar to sleep induced by sickness in other animals. Sleep in mammals and Drosophila shows molecular and functional conservation with C. elegans sleep. The simple neuroanatomy and powerful genetic tools of C. elegans have yielded insights into sleep regulation and hold great promise for future research into sleep regulation and function.
Keywords: sleep, quiescence, Caenorhabditis elegans, evolution, development, cellular stress
Behavioral and electrophysiological properties of sleep
The core function of sleep is a long-standing mystery. Theories for sleep function include roles in brain energetics [1], brain recovery [2], somatic functions and thermoregulation [3], biosynthesis [4], neural plasticity [5,6], and allocation of energetic resources [7]. A key challenge for sleep researchers is to determine whether identified sleep functions are particular to the organism of study, or if they represent core functions that led to the evolutionary maintenance of sleep states. Comparative physiology across phylogeny is a powerful approach to distill conserved sleep functions.
Until the mid-1930s, sleep in all animals was identified by three behavioral characteristics: decreased responsiveness to environmental stimuli, rapid reversibility to strong stimuli, and an increased threshold to arousal [8]. The development and use of the electroencephalogram (EEG) revealed that brain activity patterns during sleep are variable [9] and that sleep can be divided into physiologically distinct states based on electrical activity patterns [10]. The subsequent discovery of Rapid Eye Movement (REM) and non-REM sleep [11] demonstrated that these different sleep states are generated by distinct physiological processes [12]. Experiments using the EEG have also revealed that slow wave activity can serve as a biomarker for sleep need in certain settings [13,14].
In the 1960s, the ontogenetic hypothesis of sleep, which suggested sleep played an important role in the development of the nervous system specifically in young animals, came to prominence [15]. More recently, it was found that sleep can be triggered by both circadian [16] and non-circadian factors [17], such as bacterial infection [18], through at least partly distinct neural pathways [19,20]. Together, this indicates that there is additional heterogeneity in sleep. While the physiological differences between mechanisms generating REM and non-REM sleep are widely appreciated, the function and regulation of these states are incompletely understood [21]. Likewise, little is known about the physiological and functional differences between infection-induced sleep and circadian sleep, or between sleep at different developmental stages [22].
The past 15 years have witnessed the introduction of non-mammalian model organisms, such as Danio rerio, a zebrafish [23], and Drosophila melanogaster, a fruit fly [24,25], to sleep research, providing insights into the function and regulation of sleep. Because EEGs are not performed in non-mammals, sleep in such species must be defined by behavioral criteria so as to differentiate this state from quiet wakefulness or pathological quiescence. In addition to locomotion and feeding quiescence, rapid reversibility, and reduced sensory responsiveness, Tobler [26] and others [27,28] have proposed that a stereotypical body posture and a homeostatic response to sleep deprivation should be included in these criteria.
States that fulfill the behavioral criteria for sleep have been also identified in Caenorhabditis elegans, a nematode (Table 1). However, unlike in zebrafish and Drosophila, in C. elegans these behavioral states have been referred to as “sleep-like” states rather than “sleep” states [29,30]. We argue here that these nematode behavioral states should be referred to as sleep.
Table 1.
Mammals | Drosophila | C. elegans | References for C. elegans | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Reversible quiescent behavior | yes | yes | yes | [29,31,89] |
Increased arousal threshold | yes | yes | yes | [29,35] |
Stereotypical posture | yes | yes | yes | [32–34] |
Homeostatic response to deprivation | yes | yes | yes | [29,37,93] |
Sleep deprivation can be lethal | yes | yes | yes | [37] |
C. elegans sleeps
C. elegans has two states that fulfill all the behavioral criteria for sleep: developmentally timed sleep (DTS), or lethargus, and stress-induced sleep (SIS).
DTS occurs for two to three hours following each of the four larval stages [29,31], and is characterized by behavioral quiescence, stereotypical posture, decreased response to sensory stimuli, and homeostatic response to sleep deprivation [29,32]. Feeding and locomotion, which persist throughout the development of the worm (Video S1), cease during DTS (Video S2), and worms tend to assume a hockey stick-shaped posture [32–34]. The worms are more difficult to arouse during this time [29], partially due to decreases in evoked Ca2+ response in sensory neurons [35,36]. Deprivation of DTS, which can be lethal [37], is followed by a homeostatic rebound [29]. These bouts of immobility are interspersed with activity bouts, during which animals move but do not feed [32].
SIS occurs following exposure to environmental stimuli that cause cellular stress, and its duration depends on the severity of the stressor [30,38]. Like DTS, SIS is characterized by a similar cessation of feeding and locomotion and increased arousal threshold [30] (Video S3). Although a homeostatic response to deprivation of SIS has not been documented, impaired SIS results in increased mortality [30], demonstrating that this sleep state has an adaptive physiological function. SIS can occur during any developmental state, but the possibility that prolonged waking is stressful, and therefore causes SIS, has not been tested.
Satiety behavior is induced by feeding on high quality food or by refeeding after starvation and shares behavioral quiescence with SIS and DIS, but its other sleep properties have not yet been assessed [39,40].
The importance of nomenclature
Why is the correct nomenclature important? One could argue that the term “sleep-like” has sufficed to advance the field. However, “sleep-like” carries the connotation that C. elegans sleep is “like” sleep but somehow not quite the same, as if there is a universally accepted definition of sleep that is met by other organisms but not C. elegans. What is this universally accepted definition of sleep that C. elegans “sleep-like” states fail to meet? Should a qualifying “like” be added to all invertebrate analogs of mammalian behaviors, such as learning, feeding, and reproduction?
This importance of a proper definition is not new to science. In the middle of the 20th century, researchers debated the definition and existence of a circadian clock [41]. However, by the early 1960s, the circadian field came to consensus criteria expected of a circadian clock [42], which were subsequently used to define and then study circadian rhythms in diverse organisms ranging from single cell algae to humans. Such comparative studies have led to great insight into molecular mechanisms governing circadian rhythms, as both similarities and differences between mechanisms of circadian clocks across phylogeny have been informative [43].
Likewise, the Drosophila sleep field demonstrates the practical importance of using the term “sleep”. From 2000 to 2005, nearly all publications dealing with Drosophila sleep used the term “rest” or “sleep-like” to refer to fly sleep. In 2005, the journal Nature published a paper titled “Reduced sleep in Drosophila Shaker mutants” [44]. Following this publication, there was an explosion in the number of labs studying Drosophila sleep. It was arguably not new data revealing additional properties of this state, but rather the decision of a prominent journal to use the term “sleep” that led to wide acceptance of this term and expansion of the field of Drosophila sleep research.
Molecular conservation of C. elegans sleep
Unlike circadian sleep in mammals and Drosophila, DTS occurs with an ultradian periodicity (it occurs four times during development, with a period of 7–9 hours) and is linked to the molting cycle rather than the 24-hour light-dark cycle [31]. Despite this difference, many molecular pathways regulating sleep in other species also regulate C. elegans sleep. The molecular connection between DTS and circadian sleep was first made via the gene lin-42. LIN-42 is the C. elegans homolog of PERIOD [45], a protein that cycles its expression in a circadian fashion in Drosophila [46] and mammals [47,48]. The lin-42 gene product does not cycle its expression in phase with circadian time [49], but rather cycles in phase with molting time and DTS [50,51]. Moreover, lin-42 mutants show aberrant timing of molting and DTS behavior [52]. Thus, even though the C. elegans larval timer does not synchronize to circadian time, it has molecular homology with the circadian timer found in diurnal species.
Other signaling pathways are conserved between C. elegans, Drosophila and mammalian sleep, suggesting that the regulation of these states is broadly conserved (Table 2). Here, we discuss five of these conserved pathways in detail: the pigment dispersing factor (PDF), protein kinase A/cyclic adenosine monophosphate (PKA/cAMP), epithelial growth factor receptor (EGFR), dopamine, and protein kinase G (PKG) signaling pathways. We focus on these five pathways because they are the ones for which the most evidence has accumulated for similarities between nematodes and other species, but there are others that are likely conserved (e.g., serotonin [53] and glutamate [54,55]), which we do not discuss here.
Table 2.
Mammals | Drosophila | C. elegans | References for C. elegans | |
---|---|---|---|---|
PERIOD regulates timing | yes | yes | yes | [52] |
PDF signaling promotes wake | unknown | yes | yes | [59] |
EGF signaling promotes sleep | yes | yes | yes | [30,64] |
cAMP signaling promotes wake | yes | yes | yes | [29,32,68,53] |
Dopamine signaling promotes wake | yes | yes | yes | [53] |
PKG activity is associated with sleep or sleep intensity | yes | yes | yes | [29] |
The neuropeptide PDF was first implicated in the circadian regulation of behavior in Drosophila, as flies lacking PDF showed defects in the circadian timing of their locomotive activity [56]. Its effects on circadian behavior and sleep in flies have since been extensively documented [57]. PDF is a key output of two populations of circadian clock cells: when PDF is lost, these clock cells lose their synchronization [57]. PDF-expressing cells affect activity and other circadian behaviors most strongly in the morning, and flies mutant for PDF or its receptor PDFR display increased morning sleep [58]. In C. elegans, mutants for the PDF homolog pdf-1 or its receptor pdfr-1 have increased locomotion quiescence and decreased touch sensitivity during DTS [59], consistent with a wake-promoting effect of PDF. Release of C. elegans PDF-1 is greatly reduced during DTS [59] suggesting that activity of PDF-secreting neurons promotes arousal in C. elegans, as they do in Drosophila [60].
EGFR
Intracerebroventricular (ICV) injection of EGF promotes sleep in rabbits [61]. In hamsters, EGF and transforming growth factor alpha (TGF-α) can activate the EGF receptor (EGFR), and infusion of either ligand into the hamster ICV suppresses locomotion and feeding in an EGFR-dependent fashion [62]. Similarly, activation of the EGFR signaling pathway in Drosophila promotes sleep [63]. In C. elegans, overexpression of the EGF homolog LIN-3 causes cessation of feeding and locomotion [64]. EGF signaling appears primarily necessary for SIS, rather than DTS [30,64], suggesting that the EGFR signaling pathway may serve a similar role in other species. While reduced EGFR signaling in Drosophila has relatively minor effects on circadian sleep (shorter and more frequent sleep bouts) [63], its effects on stress-induced sleep in Drosophila and mammals have not yet been tested.
cAMP
In the hamster suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), activation of the cAMP-regulated binding protein (CREB) by the cAMP-dependent protein kinase (PKA) is controlled by the circadian clock [65]. CREB mutations also lead to decreased cortical arousal and increased sleep in mice [66], suggesting that the cAMP/PKA/CREB signaling pathway promotes wake. This signaling pathway plays a similar role in Drosophila, where duration of sleep is inversely related to cAMP levels and CREB expression [67]. C. elegans mutants have been identified with increased PKA signaling caused by increased cAMP levels (due to increased synthesis or reduced breakdown) or by reduced function of the PKA regulatory subunit. During DTS, these mutants are hyperactive [32,53,68] and have increased responsiveness to sensory stimuli [29,53,68].
Dopamine
Dopamine release is positively correlated with arousal [69] and compounds that promote dopamine release also promote wakefulness in mammals [70]. Clinical drugs that increase dopamine release, such as the amphetamines, strongly promote wake in humans. In Drosophila, endogenous dopamine promotes wake [71,72], an effect attributed to two dopaminergic neurons [73,74]. In C. elegans, mutations in the dopamine D1 receptor gene dop-1 cause increased quiescence, and loss of function mutations in the dopamine transporter gene dat-1 results in reduced quiescence [53], effects that parallel those observed in Drosophila.
PKG
PKG is a protein kinase activated by elevation of cyclic guanosine monophosphate (cGMP), and it promotes sleep or sleep drive across different species. Reduced activity of the Drosophila PKG encoded by the foraging locus is associated with decreased sleep [29,75]. In mice, brain-specific knockout of the PKG homolog PRKG1 is associated with reduced slow wave power in the cortical EEG, a finding supporting a reduced drive to sleep in these mutants [76]. Gain-of-function mutations in C. elegans pkg-1 (previously called egl-4) cause increased quiescence and arousal threshold, while loss of function mutations in pkg-1 cause decreased quiescence and decreased arousal threshold [29].
Putative sleep functions are conserved in C. elegans
In addition to the molecular conservation, there is also evidence that DTS and SIS serve functions similar to the proposed functions of mammalian sleep, including synaptic plasticity [5], increased anabolic metabolism [4], and stress response [18,77]. During DTS, GABAergic neuromuscular junctions are less active [78], consistent with the notion that sleep is a state that promotes synaptic plasticity. In addition, cuticle synthesis [37,51,79,80] and DNA synthesis [81] both occur during DTS, demonstrating anabolic/synthetic metabolism. As its name implies, SIS is important for recovery following exposure to cellular stressors and pathogens [30], a conserved process in Drosophila [82] and mammals [20]. State-dependent neural responses to sensory stimuli, a well-established feature of the mammalian visual system [83], have been demonstrated in mechanosensory and chemosensory neurons in C. elegans [35,54,59]. However, other putative sleep functions, such as a role in learning and memory, have not yet been addressed in C. elegans. Nevertheless, C. elegans sleep serves functions similar to those described in other animals, indicating that the study of sleep function in this animal model will be illuminating.
Prominent developmental sleep is seen across species
Implicit to its name, DTS occurs only during larval development and is not observed in adult animals. Interestingly, across all terrestrial animals sleep is most prominent during development, where it has long been thought to play an important role [15]. In Drosophila, sleep in young animals plays a role in the development of neural circuitry required for courtship behavior [84]. Sleep in neonatal rats has been described as distinct from both REM and non-REM sleep in adult animals, suggesting that developmental sleep may serve different functions than sleep in adults [22]. Thus, C. elegans DTS may prove to be a useful model for understanding the importance of sleep in growth and development. The apparent absence of natural sleep-wake cycles in adult worms (unless triggered by cellular stress) suggests that across phylogeny, developmental sleep is more fundamental and conserved than adult sleep.
New insights gained from studying C. elegans sleep
Though still in its nascent stages, C. elegans sleep research has already revealed a surprising degree of neural and molecular complexity.
Sleep regulating circuitry
Two types of peptidergic interneurons have been identified that are required for quiescence during DTS, the single GABAergic RIS interneuron and the paired glutamatergic RIA interneurons [85,86]. The RIS interneuron is active during DTS and is required for locomotion quiescence during this state. Optogenetic activation of RIS inhibits locomotion, an effect that requires neuropeptide processing but not GABA synthesis, suggesting that RIS releases an unidentified somnogenic neuropeptide [85]. The RIA interneurons express the gene encoding NLP-22, a neuropeptide that inhibits both feeding and locomotion during DTS [86]. However, the role of the RIA neurons is complex, since acute optogenetic activation of RIA does not induce quiescence during the active adult stage. Rather, RIA activation inhibits locomotion quiescence during DTS [86]. This complexity might be explained by regulation of NLP-22 release primarily by modulation of expression rather than by neural activity. Consistent with this idea, nlp-22 mRNA shows prominent cycling during larval development.
A third type of peptidergic interneuron, the single ALA neuron, is required for quiescence during SIS [30,64,87]. The ALA neuron releases neuropeptides encoded by the gene flp-13 in response to EGF signaling [30,64,87], and flp-13 mRNA levels are increased following exposure to cellular stress. Optogenetic depolarization of the ALA neuron causes cessation of feeding and locomotion in a flp-13-dependent manner [87].
The neuropeptide PDF-1 is expressed in C. elegans chemosensory neurons as well as interneurons [88]. Since sensory neuron activity is decreased during DTS [35,36] and restoration of pdf-1 in sensory neurons rescues the increased quiescence observed in pdf-1 mutants, PDF-1 is likely released from sensory neurons to promote arousal during wake. The PDF-1 receptor PDFR-1 promotes the touch sensitivity of mechanosensory neurons to stimulate locomotion during wake [59]. This role of PDF-1 offers molecular insight into the reduced sensitivity of sensory neurons, one mechanism of sensory gating during C. elegans sleep [35,36]. Sensory gating, a universal feature of sleep in all species, is understood better in C. elegans than any other animal [35,36,59], a testament to the simplicity of the nematode nervous system and the powerful genetic and optogenetic tools available in this system.
Distinct mechanisms of different sleep states
The FLP-13 neuropeptides released by the ALA neuron are not required for quiescence during DTS [87], and NLP-22 and RIS neuron function are not required for quiescence during SIS [89], indicating that these states are governed by at least partly distinct mechanisms. Despite behavioral similarities between DTS and SIS, the mechanisms of quiescence generation downstream of the FLP-13 and NLP-22 neuropeptides during these states are distinct [89]. During SIS, optogenetic stimulation of the cholinergic motor neurons of the pharynx, the C. elegans feeding organ, causes an increase in feeding rate similar to that observed following neuron stimulation during wake. In contrast, feeding rate during DTS is not increased by stimulation of pharyngeal cholinergic motor neurons, or even direct stimulation of pharyngeal muscle, demonstrating that pharyngeal muscle excitability is strongly decreased during DTS. Thus, despite being behaviorally indistinguishable, SIS and DTS are generated by distinct mechanisms. A comparison between DTS and SIS is shown in Table 3.
Table 3.
DTS | SIS | |
---|---|---|
Occurs during | development | any stage |
Trigger | larval timer | cellular stress |
Duration | 2–3 hours | depends on severity of stressor |
Feeding quiescence | yes | yes |
Locomotion quiescence | yes | yes |
Increased arousal threshold | yes | yes |
Homeostatic response to deprivation | yes | unknown |
Deprivation can be lethal | yes | yes |
Stereotypical posture | yes | unknown |
Peptidergic interneuron modulation | RIS, RIA | ALA |
Associated neuropeptides | NLP-22, others | FLP-13, others |
Mechanism of feeding quiescence | muscular | neuronal |
Mechanism of locomotion quiescence | neuronal* | neuronal* |
Putative functions | anabolic metabolism synaptic plasticity | recovery of cellular homeostasis |
the neuronal mechanism for locomotion quiescence differs between DTS and SIS
The demonstration of mechanistically distinct sleep states in a non-mammalian animal is relevant to results obtained in the study of other animals in which sleep is identified strictly by behavioral criteria. Recent studies have shown that Drosophila daytime sleep and nighttime sleep are regulated by at least partly distinct mechanisms [90,91], and that mechanisms regulating sleep in young flies are partially distinct from mechanisms regulating sleep in older flies [84]. Consistent with lessons learned in C. elegans, Drosophila mutations that impair stress-induced sleep have no effect on circadian sleep [92]. Together, these results demonstrate that though sleep appears behaviorally homogenous, invertebrates have physiologically distinct sleep states. Given the high degree of molecular and functional conservation of sleep between C. elegans, Drosophila, and mammals, it would be surprising if sleep in all species were not similarly complex.
Sleep homeostasis
C. elegans sleep research has also provided insights into the regulation of sleep homeostasis. A recent study demonstrated that distinct molecular mechanisms are required for different types of homeostatic responses to sleep deprivation during DTS: deprivation with a gentle stimulus has a short-lasting effect on sleep bout durations immediately following the stimulus and requires neuropeptide signaling through an NPY receptor homolog NPR-1 [93], while deprivation with a harsh stimulus has long-lasting effects on sleep and requires signaling via the FOXO transcription factor DAF-16 [37,93]. The notion that different types of wake-promoting perturbations can have distinct effects on the sleep homeostatic response was recently also shown in Drosophila [94]. A role for NPY in sleep regulation in humans and flies has been described [95,96], and a role for FOXO signaling in Drosophila sleep has recently been identified [91], suggesting the homeostatic roles for these signaling pathways are conserved in species beyond C. elegans.
Prospects for C. elegans sleep research
Sleep arises from networks of neurons rather than individual neurons, so the small number of neurons and known anatomical connectivity of the C. elegans nervous system provides an advantage both for identifying sleep circuits and for propelling our understanding of the neural basis of sleep. Whole brain calcium imaging can be used to detect activity patterns of neurons across behavioral states [97], and optogenetic techniques for manipulating individual neurons in vivo can be used to identify those that modulate sleep-related behaviors, such as feeding [98] and locomotion [99–101]. Further, the ease of manipulation of C. elegans allows the independent study of the neural subprograms of sleep, such as decreased sensory response [35,36,54,59], behavioral quiescence [89], and homeostasis [37,93], which will shed light into how these behaviors are coordinated.
In addition to defining and understanding sleep circuits, C. elegans research benefits from an advanced set of genetic tools and from rapid genetic analysis. These techniques can be used to determine how conserved genetic pathways, such as those discussed here, affect sleep behavior, and to discover novel pathways regulating sleep. The description and understanding of the genetic landscape of C. elegans sleep can then be translated to research in other animals. These genetic data can also be used to uncover functions of sleep in C. elegans and their relative importance for organismal fitness, which will shed light onto the core function of sleep. Wider acknowledgement of sleep states in this model organism will facilitate the leveraging of knowledge gained from the study of C. elegans into a better understanding of sleep in general.
Supplementary Material
Trends.
C. elegans has two states that meet the behavioral criteria for sleep, developmentally timed sleep (DTS) and stress-induces sleep (SIS).
DTS and SIS are regulated by the same signaling pathways that regulate mammalian sleep, including PDF, cAMP, EGFR, dopamine, and PKG signaling.
C. elegans sleep has proposed functions similar to those of mammalian sleep, including synaptic plasticity, anabolic metabolism, and stress response, and is similarly prominent during development.
Animals in DTS and SIS display behaviorally identical cessation of feeding and locomotion, but this quiescence is produced by different mechanisms in each state.
Homeostatic responses to disruption of DTS by gentle and harsh stimuli occurs through molecularly distinct pathways.
Acknowledgments
N.F.T. was supported by the National Institutes of Health (T32-HL-007953). D.M.R. was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01-NS-088432, and R21-NS-091500). For comments, we thank Cheryl Van Buskirk, Matthew Kayser, Matthew Nelson, Hilary DeBardeleben, Richard McCloskey, Michael Iannacone, and Jennifer Aleman.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Benington JH, Heller HC. Restoration of brain energy metabolism as the function of sleep. Progress in Neurobiology. 1995;45:347–360. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(94)00057-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mignot E. Why We Sleep: The Temporal Organization of Recovery. PLoS Biology. 2008;6:e106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rechtschaffen A. Current Perspectives on the Function of Sleep. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 1998;41:359–390. doi: 10.1353/pbm.1998.0051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mackiewicz M, et al. Macromolecule biosynthesis: a key function of sleep. Physiological Genomics. 2007;31:441–457. doi: 10.1152/physiolgenomics.00275.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron. 2014;81:12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Frank MG, Cantera R. Sleep, clocks, and synaptic plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences. 2014;37:491–501. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Schmidt MH. The energy allocation function of sleep: a unifying theory of sleep, torpor, and continuous wakefulness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:122–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kleitman N. Sleep. Physiol Rev. 1929;9:624–665. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Loomis AL, et al. Further Observations on the Potential Rhythms of the Cerebral Cortex during Sleep. Science. 1935;82:198–200. doi: 10.1126/science.82.2122.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Loomis AL, et al. Cerebral States During Sleep, As Studied By Human Brain Potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1937;21:127–144. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Aserinsky E, Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science. 1953;118:273–274. doi: 10.1126/science.118.3062.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dement WC. The occurrence of low voltage, fast, electroencephalogram patterns during behavioral sleep in the cat. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1958;10:291–296. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(58)90037-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Finelli LA, et al. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience. 2000;101:523–529. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00409-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Suzuki A, et al. Behavioral and biochemical dissociation of arousal and homeostatic sleep need influenced by prior wakeful experience in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:10288–10293. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308295110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Roffwarg HP, et al. Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science. 1966;152:604–619. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1982;1:195–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Krueger JM, et al. Sleep-promoting effects of muramyl peptides. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1982;79:6102–6106. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.19.6102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Toth LA, Krueger JM. Alteration of sleep in rabbits by Staphylococcus aureus infection. Infection and Immunity. 1988;56:1785–1791. doi: 10.1128/iai.56.7.1785-1791.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Saper CB, et al. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature. 2005;437:1257–1263. doi: 10.1038/nature04284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Imeri L, Opp MR. How (and why) the immune system makes us sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:199–210. doi: 10.1038/nrn2576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Siegel JM. Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature. 2005;437:1264–1271. doi: 10.1038/nature04285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Blumberg MS, et al. The development of sleep–wake rhythms and the search for elemental circuits in the infant brain. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;128:250–263. doi: 10.1037/a0035891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Zhdanova IV, et al. Melatonin promotes sleep-like state in zebrafish. Brain Res. 2001;903:263–268. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02444-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hendricks JC, et al. Rest in Drosophila is a sleep-like state. Neuron. 2000;25:129–138. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80877-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shaw PJ, et al. Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2000;287:1834–1837. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5459.1834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Campbell SS, Tobler I. Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across phylogeny. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1984;8:269–300. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(84)90054-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hendricks JC, et al. The need for a simple animal model to understand sleep. Progress in Neurobiology. 2000;61:339–351. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(99)00048-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Zimmerman JE, et al. Conservation of sleep: insights from non-mammalian model systems. Trends in Neurosciences. 2008;31:371–376. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Raizen DM, et al. Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like state. Nature. 2008;451:569–572. doi: 10.1038/nature06535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hill AJ, et al. Cellular Stress Induces a Protective Sleep-like State in C. elegans. Current Biology. 2014;24:2399–2405. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cassada RC, Russell RL. The dauerlarva, a post-embryonic developmental variant of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Developmental Biology. 1975;46:326–342. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(75)90109-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Iwanir S, et al. The Microarchitecture of C. elegans Behavior during Lethargus: Homeostatic Bout Dynamics, a Typical Body Posture, and Regulation by a Central Neuron. Sleep. 2013;36:385–395. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tramm N, et al. Why Do Sleeping Nematodes Adopt a Hockey-Stick-Like Posture? PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e101162. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schwarz J, et al. Reduced muscle contraction and a relaxed posture during sleep-like Lethargus. Worm. 2012;1:11–13. doi: 10.4161/worm.19499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cho JY, Sternberg PW. Multilevel Modulation of a Sensory Motor Circuit during C. elegans Sleep and Arousal. Cell. 2014;156:249–260. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Schwarz J, et al. Reduced activity of a sensory neuron during a sleep-like state in Caenorhabditis elegans. Current Biology. 2011;21:R983–R984. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Driver RJ, et al. DAF-16/FOXO Regulates Homeostasis of Essential Sleep-like Behavior during Larval Transitions in C. elegans. Current Biology. 2013;23:501–506. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jones D, Candido EP. Feeding is inhibited by sublethal concentrations of toxicants and by heat stress in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans: relationship to the cellular stress response. Journal of Experimental Zoology. 1999;284:147–157. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-010x(19990701)284:2<147::aid-jez4>3.3.co;2-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.You YJ, et al. Insulin, cGMP, and TGF-β Signals Regulate Food Intake and Quiescence in C. elegans: A Model for Satiety. Cell Metabolism. 2008;7:249–257. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gallagher T, et al. ASI Regulates Satiety Quiescence in C. elegans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:9716–9724. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4493-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pittendrigh CS. On temperature independence in the clock system controlling emergence time in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1954;40:1018–1029. doi: 10.1073/pnas.40.10.1018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pittendrigh CS. Circadian rhythms and the circadian organization of living systems. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. 1960;25:159–184. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1960.025.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Stanewsky R. Genetic analysis of the circadian system in Drosophila melanogaster and mammals. Journal of Neurobiology. 2002;54:111–147. doi: 10.1002/neu.10164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cirelli C, et al. Reduced sleep in Drosophila Shaker mutants. Nature. 2005;434:1087–1092. doi: 10.1038/nature03486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Konopka RJ, Benzer S. Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1971;68:2112–2116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.68.9.2112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hardin PE, et al. Feedback of the Drosophila period gene product on circadian cycling of its messenger RNA levels. Nature. 1990;343:536–540. doi: 10.1038/343536a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sun ZS, et al. RIGUI, a putative mammalian ortholog of the Drosophila period gene. Cell. 1997;90:1003–1011. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80366-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tei H, et al. Circadian oscillation of a mammalian homologue of the Drosophila period gene. Nature. 1997;389:512–516. doi: 10.1038/39086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.van der Linden AM, et al. Genome-Wide Analysis of Light- and Temperature-Entrained Circadian Transcripts in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Biology. 2010;8:e1000503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jeon M, et al. Similarity of the C. elegans developmental timing protein LIN-42 to circadian rhythm proteins. Science. 1999;286:1141–1146. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5442.1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Frand AR, et al. Functional Genomic Analysis of C. elegans Molting. PLoS Biology. 2005;3:e312. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Monsalve GC, et al. LIN-42/PERIOD Controls Cyclical and Developmental Progression of C. elegans Molts. Current Biology. 2011;21:2033–2045. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Singh K, et al. Deep conservation of genes required for both Drosphila melanogaster and Caenorhabditis elegans sleep includes a role for dopaminergic signaling. Sleep. 2014;37:1439–1451. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Choi S, et al. Sensory Neurons Arouse C. elegans Locomotion via Both Glutamate and Neuropeptide Release. PLoS Genet. 2015;11:e1005359. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1005359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tomita J, et al. The NMDA Receptor Promotes Sleep in the Fruit Fly, Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0128101. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Renn SC, et al. A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell. 1999;99:791–802. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81676-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Taghert PH, Nitabach MN. Peptide Neuromodulation in Invertebrate Model Systems. Neuron. 2012;76:82–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Parisky KM, et al. PDF Cells Are a GABA-Responsive Wake-Promoting Component of the Drosophila Sleep Circuit. Neuron. 2008;60:672–682. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Choi S, et al. Analysis of NPR-1 Reveals a Circuit Mechanism for Behavioral Quiescence in C. elegans. Neuron. 2013;78:869–880. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Sheeba V, et al. Large Ventral Lateral NeuronsModulate Arousal and Sleep in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2008;18:1537–1545. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kushikata T, et al. Epidermal growth factor enhances spontaneous sleep in rabbits. American Journal of Physiology. 1998;275:R509–14. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.2.R509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kramer A, et al. Regulation of daily locomotor activity and sleep by hypothalamic EGF receptor signaling. Science. 2001;294:2511–2515. doi: 10.1126/science.1067716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Foltenyi K, et al. Activation of EGFR and ERK by rhomboid signaling regulates the consolidation and maintenance of sleep in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1160–1167. doi: 10.1038/nn1957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Van Buskirk C, Sternberg PW. Epidermal growth factor signaling induces behavioral quiescence in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1300–1307. doi: 10.1038/nn1981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ginty DD, et al. Regulation of CREB phosphorylation in the suprachiasmatic nucleus by light and a circadian clock. Science. 1993;260:238–241. doi: 10.1126/science.8097062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Graves LA, et al. Genetic evidence for a role of CREB in sustained cortical arousal. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;90:1152–1159. doi: 10.1152/jn.00882.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hendricks JC, et al. A non-circadian role for cAMP signaling and CREB activity in Drosophila rest homeostasis. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:1108–1115. doi: 10.1038/nn743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Belfer SJ, et al. Caenorhabditis-in-drop array for monitoring C. elegans quiescent behavior. Sleep. 2013;36:689–698G. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Trulson ME. Simultaneous recording of substantia nigra neurons and voltammetric release of dopamine in the caudate of behaving cats. Brain Research Bulletin. 1985;15:221–223. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(85)90140-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wisor JP, et al. Dopaminergic role in stimulant-induced wakefulness. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:1787–1794. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01787.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Andretic R, et al. Dopaminergic Modulation of Arousal in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2005;15:1165–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kume K, et al. Dopamine is a regulator of arousal in the fruit fly. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:7377–7384. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2048-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Liu Q, et al. Two Dopaminergic NeuronsSignal to the Dorsal Fan-Shaped Body to Promote Wakefulness in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2012;22:2114–2123. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ueno T, et al. Identification of a dopamine pathway that regulates sleep and arousal in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1516–1523. doi: 10.1038/nn.3238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Donlea JM, et al. Foraging alters resilience/vulnerability to sleep disruption and starvation in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:2613–2618. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112623109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Langmesser S, et al. cGMP-Dependent Protein Kinase Type I Is Implicated in the Regulation of the Timing and Quality of Sleep and Wakefulness. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e4238. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Rampin C, et al. Immobilisation stress induces a paradoxical sleep rebound in rat. Neuroscience Letters. 1991;126:113–118. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90532-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Dabbish NS, Raizen DM. GABAergic Synaptic Plasticity during a Developmentally Regulated Sleep-Like State in C. elegans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:15932–15943. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Singh RN, Sulston JE. Some observations on moulting in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nematologica. 1978;24:63–71. [Google Scholar]
- 80.George-Raizen JB, et al. Dynamically-expressed prion-like proteins form a cuticle in the pharynx of Caenorhabditis elegans. Biology Open. 2014;3:1139–1149. doi: 10.1242/bio.20147500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Hedgecock EM, White JG. Polyploid tissues in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Developmental Biology. 1985;107:128–133. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(85)90381-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Williams JA, et al. Interaction between sleep and the immune response in Drosophila: a role for the NFkappaB relish. Sleep. 2007;30:389–400. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.4.389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Livingstone MS, Hubel DH. Effects of sleep and arousal on the processing of visual information in the cat. Nature. 1981;291:554–561. doi: 10.1038/291554a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Kayser MS, et al. A critical period of sleep for development of courtship circuitry and behavior in Drosophila. Science. 2014;344:269–274. doi: 10.1126/science.1250553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Turek M, et al. An AP2 Transcription Factor Is Required for a Sleep-Active Neuron to Induce Sleep-like Quiescence in C. elegans. Current Biology. 2013;23:2215–2223. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Nelson MD, et al. The neuropeptide NLP-22 regulates a sleep-like state in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Communications. 2013;4:2846. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Nelson MD, et al. FMRFamide-like FLP-13 Neuropeptides Promote Quiescence following Heat Stress in Caenorhabditis elegans. Current Biology. 2014;24:2406–2410. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Janssen T, et al. Discovery and characterization of a conserved pigment dispersing factor-like neuropeptide pathway in Caenorhabditis elegans. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2009;111:228–241. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06323.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Trojanowski NF, et al. Distinct Mechanisms Underlie Quiescence during Two Caenorhabditis elegans Sleep-Like States. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:14571–14584. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1369-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.van Alphen B, et al. A Dynamic Deep Sleep Stage in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:6917–6927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0061-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Metaxakis A, et al. Lowered Insulin Signalling Ameliorates Age-Related Sleep Fragmentation in Drosophila. PLoS Biology. 2014;12:e1001824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Lenz O, et al. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. Brain, Behaviour, and Immunity. 2015;47:141–148. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Nagy S, et al. Homeostasis in C. elegans sleep is characterized by two behaviorally and genetically distinct mechanisms. eLife. 2014;3:e04380. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Seidner G, et al. Identification of Neurons with a Privileged Role in Sleep Homeostasis in Drosophila melanogaster. Current Biology. 2015;25:2928–2938. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.He C, et al. Regulation of Sleep by Neuropeptide Y-Like System in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e74237. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Antonijevic IA, et al. Neuropeptide Y promotes sleep and inhibits ACTH and cortisol release in young men. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39:1474–1481. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(00)00057-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Kato S, et al. Global Brain Dynamics Embed the Motor Command Sequence of Caenorhabditis elegans. Cell. 2015;163:656–669. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.09.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Trojanowski NF, et al. Neural and genetic degeneracy underlies Caenorhabditis elegans feeding behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2014;112:951–961. doi: 10.1152/jn.00150.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Stirman JN, et al. Real-time multimodal optical control of neurons and muscles in freely behaving Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Methods. 2011;8:153–158. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Leifer AM, et al. Optogenetic manipulation of neural activity in freely moving Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Methods. 2011;8:147–152. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Kocabas A, et al. Controlling interneuron activity in Caenorhabditis elegans to evoke chemotactic behaviour. Nature. 2012;490:273–277. doi: 10.1038/nature11431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.