Abstract
It is now well appreciated that individual animals behave differently from one another and that individual differences in behaviors—personality differences—are maintained through time and across situations. Quantitative genetics has emerged as a conceptual basis for understanding the key ingredients of personality: (co)variation and plasticity. However, the results from quantitative genetic analyses are often divorced from underlying molecular or other proximate mechanisms. This disconnect has the potential to impede an integrated understanding of behavior and is a disconnect present throughout evolutionary ecology. Here we discuss some of the main conceptual connections between personality and quantitative genetics, the relationship of both with genomic tools, and areas that require integration. With its consideration of both trait variation and plasticity, the study of animal personality offers new opportunities to incorporate molecular mechanisms into both the trait partitioning and reaction norm frameworks provided by quantitative genetics.
Introduction
A thriving area of research in the study of animal behavior involves understanding consistent individual differences (‘animal personality’). There is growing appreciation that individual animals within natural populations behave differently from one another, and that they retain these behavioral differences through time and across situations. Consistent individual differences in behavior are interesting to animal behaviorists for at least three reasons. First, we want to know why individuals are different from one another; in other words, why is there variation? Second, we want to understand why behavioral traits are correlated in particular configurations (i.e. as components of behavioral syndromes). Why, for example, are boldness and aggressiveness correlated in some populations but not others? Third, we want to know why individuals have a behavioral type that they maintain over time—what limits behavioral plasticity? The key features of personality—among individual (co)variation and within individual consistency—are what distinguishes the study of personality from simply the study of behavior.
Interest regarding animal personalities increasingly integrates research across levels of analysis and combines different methodological approaches. In particular there is growing interest in evolutionary processes that can generate consistent individual differences and widespread appreciation that understanding the proximate mechanisms underlying personality can shed light on its causation and evolution.
Quantitative genetics provides a framework for understanding personality
Quantitative genetics provides a strong conceptual basis for considering the key ingredients of personality: (co)variation and plasticity [1]. In particular, quantitative genetics provides a statistical framework for partitioning trait variation, covariation within and among individuals, and can estimate population and individual level behavioral plasticity [1]. For example, mixed effects statistical models can estimate reaction norms—phenotypic plasticity—using the phenotypic equation [2]:
(equation 1) |
where yij is the behavioral response of individual j at instance i [3]. The different parts of the equation correspond to population level average behavioral responses (β0, assuming centering of predictor variables), the individual’s deviation from this average (ind0j, i.e. personality variation), and residual variation for that individual at that particular instance (e0ij). Plasticity enters via xij, which is the environment experienced by that individual at that instance. If the population as a whole shows behavioral plasticity, this can be estimated (β1) and individual level plasticity can also be estimated (ind1j). Individual average behavior as it differs from the population mean (ind0j) and individual plasticity as it differs from population level plasticity (ind1j) might also be correlated such that personality and plasticity are correlated (for further details and assumptions see [3]). The emergence of quantitative genetics as an organizational basis for the field of personality research has allowed the rapid translation of statistical methods to the field and highlighted evolutionary questions of potential importance (e.g. [4]).
The problem: How to incorporate molecular mechanisms?
However, while quantitative genetics provides a conceptual basis for understanding consistent individual differences, the results from quantitative genetic analyses are often statistical abstractions, divorced from underlying molecular or other proximate mechanisms. Indeed, the prevailing approaches for studying the genetic basis of traits tend to focus on either the quantitative genetic or molecular side of the equation, and the two approaches require very different tools. Studies of quantitative genetics and molecular mechanisms are also often carried out in isolation of each other and their results are difficult to integrate. For example, one study might compute a genetic correlation between two behaviors [5], while another study in the same species finds correlated patterns of genome-wide expression associated with these two behaviors [6,7]. Similarly, one study might estimate the slope of a behavioral reaction norm across environments, while another shows that gene expression changes across environments. Unfortunately the results from these approaches are not immediately comparable.
The gap between the statistical abstractions of quantitative genetics and the actual genetic or molecular mechanisms underlying them can be a problem because different mechanisms can produce the same outcome, and/or have different implications for evolution. For example, a relatively low estimate of heritability could reflect either low genetic variation or high non-genetic variation. Similarly, a genetic correlation between two behaviors could reflect either pleiotropy (when the same genes influence the two traits), physical linkage between loci or selection-induced linkage disequilibrium (when the traits are influenced by different, co-selected genes) [4,5,8]. Finally, phenotypic plasticity could reflect allelic sensitivity (different genes underlie the trait in different environments) or gene regulation (the same gene influences the trait in different environments, but the gene is regulated differently in the two environments [9]). Bridging this gap is an important priority for all of biology and emerging interest in personality offers an opportunity for combining molecular mechanisms and quantitative genetic approaches. Arguably, the advancement of personality research—with its concern for both variation among-individuals and for plasticity—necessitates this synthesis.
One strategy for bridging the divide between molecular mechanisms and traditional quantitative genetics is to identify the specific genetic variants that contribute to additive genetic variation and thus to phenotypic variation. Identification of such genetic variants can be achieved via quantitative trait loci (QTL) studies and genome-wide association studies (GWAS). While such approaches are currently limited to studying traits underlain by genes of large effect [10], finding such genes can be informative for understanding the proximate basis of traits, and can be used to address longstanding questions about the genetic basis of adaptation [11]. Identification of such genes also allows better understanding of the evolution of behavior and whether particular responses might be deeply conserved or novel. Finally, intensive public investment in GWAS was driven by concerns regarding human health, for which understanding how particular genes affect behavior can be highly relevant [12].
A number of recent papers have identified QTL associated with behavioral variation in natural populations, e.g. burrowing behavior in mice [13], schooling behavior in sticklebacks [14], aggressive behavior in Drosophila [15] and feeding behavior in C. elegans [16]. Importantly, such information from QTL studies can be integrated into the statistical framework provided by traditional quantitative genetics. For example, the proportion of phenotypic variation attributable to QTL variation can be framed in terms of heritability due to that QTL (but see [17]). Further, the phenotypic equation can be modified to include information about the influence of particular loci (e.g. QTL, expanding from [18]):
(equation 2) |
where the individual’s phenotype is now also determined by a known loci (m, e.g. a particular QTL) which has an estimable average contribution to the phenotype (G0m; as above this represents a deviation from the population level average, β0). Within this framework, the contribution of variation at specific loci and epistatic interactions among them can thereby be estimated. Moreover, this framework can also be extended to include not only directly estimable genetic influences on behavior but also inferred genetic influences when pedigree information is known and relatedness included (i.e. application of the “animal model”, [19]). However, it is likely to be challenging to apply this general framework to natural variation in behavioral traits with fitness consequences, because they are typically underlain by hundreds of genes of small effect and influenced by both genetic variation and the environment [20].
In principle, equation 2 can incorporate information about particular genes that might contribute to individual variation in plasticity, via the same manner by which individual plasticity was included:
(equation 3) |
where the individual’s known genotype (m) now has an estimable plastic effect on the behavior of the individual (G1m) based on the environment currently experienced (xij). However it is likely to be challenging to empirically obtain these estimates.
An alternative to QTL and GWAS that comes closer to capturing molecular mechanisms associated with plasticity involves measuring genome-wide gene expression (e.g. [21–23]). The appeal of measuring genome-wide expression, as opposed to focusing on fixed genetic variation, is that it can reveal which genes in the genome are responsive to the environment, and therefore likely to be related to phenotypic plasticity. For example, recent genome-wide transcription studies have revealed that roughly ~10% of all of the genes in the genome are differentially expressed in response to a mating opportunity [24–29], predation risk [7,30,31], or a territorial challenge [6,32,33]. Recent evidence suggests that transcriptional responses can be the result of a conserved genomic response to social challenges [34]. Genome-wide gene expression studies have also detected differences in brain gene expression between behavioral types, e.g. scouts vs nonscout honey bees [35] and alternative mating types [29,36,37]. A few particularly exciting recent studies have both compared gene expression between behavioral types and changes in gene expression in response to the environment [29,38]. This strategy can inform our understanding of the evolution of plasticity by identifying potential targets of selection on plasticity [39]. However, studies measuring gene expression in regards to behavior largely occur in isolation of studies aimed at quantifying repeatability, heritability and genetic correlations – the traditional province of quantitative genetics (but see eQTL [40]). Unfortunately, how results from genome-wide gene expression studies translate to the conceptual framework of quantitative genetics remains to be resolved. This is not a trivial problem and there are several specific (e.g. Box 1) and general questions that must be addressed. It is, however, an essential problem as our theoretical understanding of evolution is in large part tied to quantitative genetic theory.
Box 1. Sample challenges for integrating molecular mechanisms into quantitative genetics, as applied to personality.
Can genome-wide gene expression data be used to link the P and G matrix?
How to relate pleiotropy via a genetic correlation with patterns of gene expression?
How to relate I × E or G × E reaction norms estimated within a quantitative genetic framework with gene expression measured in different environments?
Is there a way to detect permanent environment, among-individual, within-individual and residual effects in gene expression data?
Do differences in brain gene expression between behavioral types reflect additive genetic variation, among-individual variation or within-individual variation?
Conclusions: Personality requires integration
Understanding personality—consistent individual differences—requires consideration of both variation and plasticity. The study of personality has prompted questions about how processes that can generate variation can coexist alongside processes that allow within-individual plasticity, and indeed, how those processes are related to each other [41]. The study of personality therefore offers an opportunity for integration and improved understanding of both individual differences and behavioral plasticity. The study of personality also prompts a comprehensive synthesis of molecular mechanisms into both the variance partitioning and reaction norm frameworks provided by quantitative genetics. This integration of stability and plasticity alongside the integration of molecular mechanisms and quantitative genetics represents the next frontier for personality research and will similarly advance not only the study of animal behavior but evolutionary ecology as a whole.
HIGHLIGHTS.
The study of animal personality is concerned with both variation and plasticity.
Quantitative genetics provides a conceptual framework for personality.
Recent studies have identified genes related to behavioral variation and plasticity.
A challenge is to integrate molecular mechanisms into quantitative genetic theory.
Acknowledgments
Work in AMB’s lab has been supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (IOS 1121980) and the National Institutes of Health (R01 GM082937).
Footnotes
Conflicts of interest
None declared
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References cited
- 1.Dingemanse NJ, Dochtermann NA. Individual behaviour: behavioural ecology meets quantitative genetics. In: Charmantier ADG, Kruuk LEB, editors. Quantitative genetics in the wild. Oxford University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Nussey DH, Wilson AJ, Brommer JE. The evolutionary ecology of individual phenotypic plasticity in wild populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2007;20:831–844. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01300.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3**.Dingemanse NJ, Dochtermann NA. Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2013;82:39–54. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12013. Provides a statistical framework for integrating individual variation in behavior and plasticity into quantitative genetic theory. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dochtermann NA, Dingemanse NJ. Behavioral syndromes as evolutionary constraints. Behavioral Ecology. 2013;24:806–811. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bell AM. Differences between individuals and populations of threespined stickleback. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2005;18:464–473. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00817.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sanogo YO, Band MA, Blatti C, Sinha S, Bell AM. Transcriptional regulation of brain gene expression in response to a territorial intrusion. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2012;279:4929–4938. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sanogo YO, Hankison S, Band M, Obregon A, Bell AM. Brain transcriptomic response of threespine sticklebacks to cues of a predator. Brain Behavior and Evolution. 2011;77:270–285. doi: 10.1159/000328221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sih A, Bell AM, Johnson JC, Ziemba R. Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. Quarterly Review of Biology. 2004;79:241–277. doi: 10.1086/422893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Schlichting CD, Pigliucci M. Gene regulation, quantitative genetics and the evolution of reaction norms. Evolutionary Ecology. 1995;9:154–168. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rockman MV. The QTN program and the alleles that matter for evolution: all that’s gold does not glitter. Evolution. 2012;66:1–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01486.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11*.Rausher MD, Delph LF. When does understanding phenotypic evolution require identification of the underlying genes? Evolution. doi: 10.1111/evo.12687. In press:n/a–n/a. Articulates when and why “knowing the genes” matters. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Visscher Peter M, Brown Matthew A, McCarthy Mark I, Yang J. Five Years of GWAS Discovery. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2012;90:7–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.11.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Weber JN, Peterson BK, Hoekstra HE. Discrete genetic modules are responsible for complex burrow evolution in Peromyscus mice. Nature. 2013;493:402–405. doi: 10.1038/nature11816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Greenwood AK, Wark AR, Yoshida K, Peichel CL. Genetic and neural modularity underlie the evolution of schooling behavior in threespine sticklebacks. Current Biology. 2013;23:1884–1888. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Edwards AC, Mackay TF. Quantitative trait loci for aggressive behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 2009;182:889–897. doi: 10.1534/genetics.109.101691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bendesky A, Tsunozaki M, Rockman MV, Kruglyak L, Bargmann CI. Catecholamine receptor polymorphisms affect decision-making in C. elegans. Nature. 2011;472:313–318. doi: 10.1038/nature09821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bogdan M, Doerge RW. Biased estimators of quantitative trait locus heritability and location in interval mapping. Heredity. 2005;95:476–484. doi: 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Walsh B. Quantitative genetics in the age of genomics. Theoretical Population Biology. 2001;59:175–184. doi: 10.1006/tpbi.2001.1512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wilson AJ, Reale D, Clements MN, Morrissey MM, Postma E, Walling CA, Kruuk LEB, Nussey DH. An ecologist’s guide to the animal model. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2010;79:13–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01639.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mackay TFC. The genetic architecture of behavior: Lessons from Drosophila. Genetica. 2009;136:295–302. doi: 10.1007/s10709-008-9310-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Harris RM, Hofmann HA. Neurogenomics of behavioral plasticity. Advances Experimental Medicine Biology. 2014;781:149–168. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-7347-9_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22*.Cardoso SD, Teles MC, Oliveira RF. Neurogenomic mechanisms of social plasticity. J Experimental Biology. 2015;218:140–149. doi: 10.1242/jeb.106997. Reviews molecular mechanisms likely to contribute to short- and long-term behavioral plasticity. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bell AM, Aubin-Horth N. What whole genome expression data can tell us about the ecology and evolution of personality in animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. 2010 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.McGraw LA, Clark AG, Wolfner MF. Post-mating gene expression profiles of female Drosophila melanogaster in response to time and to four male accessory gland proteins. Genetics. 2008;179:1395–1408. doi: 10.1534/genetics.108.086934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mack PD, Kapelnikov A, Heifetz Y, Bender M. Mating-responsive genes in reproductive tissues of female Drosophila melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:10358–10363. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604046103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lawniczak MKN, Begun DJ. A genome-wide analysis of courting and mating responses in Drosophila melanogaster females. Genome. 2004;47:900–910. doi: 10.1139/g04-050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cummings ME, Larkins-Ford J, Reilly CRL, Wong RY, Ramsey M, Hofmann HA. Sexual and social stimuli elicit rapid and contrasting genomic responses. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2008;275:393–402. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Carney GE. A rapid genome-wide response to Drosophila melanogaster social interactions. Bmc Genomics. 2007;8 doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-8-288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fraser BA, Janowitz I, Thairu M, Travis J, Hughes KA. Phenotypic and genomic plasticity of alternative male reproductive tactics in sailfin mollies*. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2014;281:20132310. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2310. Uses brain gene expression profiling to identify genes associated with behavioral variation and behavioral plasticity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lavergne SG, McGowan PO, Krebs CJ, Boonstra R. Impact of high predation risk on genome-wide hippocampal gene expression in snowshoe hares. Oecologia. 2014;176:613–624. doi: 10.1007/s00442-014-3053-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jansen M, Vergauwen L, Vandenbrouck T, Knapen D, Dom N, Spanier KI, Cielen A, De Meester L. Gene expression profiling of three different stressors in the water flea Daphnia magna. Ecotoxicology. 2013;22:900–914. doi: 10.1007/s10646-013-1072-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Alaux C, Sinha S, Hasadsri L, Hunt GJ, Guzman-Novoa E, DeGrandi-Hoffman G, Uribe-Rubio JL, Southey BR, Rodriguez-Zas S, Robinson GE. Honey bee aggression supports a link between gene regulation and behavioral evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:15400–15405. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907043106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33*.Rittschof CC, Robinson GE. Manipulation of colony environment modulates honey bee aggression and brain gene expression. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2013;12:802–811. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12087. Shows that some of the genes that are responsive to a social challenge are deeply conserved. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rittschof CC, Bukhari SA, Sloofman LG, Troy JM, Caetano-Anollés D, Cash-Ahmed A, Kent M, Lu X, Sanogo YO, Weisner PA, et al. Neuromolecular responses to social challenge: Common mechanisms across mouse, stickleback fish, and honey bee. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111:17929–17934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1420369111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Liang ZS, Nguyen T, Mattila HR, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Seeley TD, Robinson GE. Molecular determinants of scouting behavior in honey bees. Science. 2012;335:1225–1228. doi: 10.1126/science.1213962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Stiver KA, Harris RM, Townsend JP, Hofmann HA, Alonzo SH. Neural gene expression profiles and androgen levels underlie alternative reproductive tactics in the ocellated wrasse, Symphodus ocellatus. Ethology. 2015;121:152–167. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Aubin-Horth N, Landry CR, Letcher BH, Hofmann HA. Alternative life histories shape brain gene expression profiles in males of the same population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2005;272:1655–1662. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Alaux C, Sinha S, Hasadsri L, Hunt GJ, Guzmán-Novoa E, DeGrandi-Hoffman G, Uribe-Rubio JL, BRS, Rodriguez-Zas S, Robinson GE. Honey bee aggression supports a link between gene regulation and behavioral evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:15400–15405. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907043106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.West-Eberhard MJ. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gilad Y, Rifkin SA, Pritchard JK. Revealing the architecture of gene regulation: the promise of eQTL studies. Trends in Genetics. 2008;24:408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2008.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Stamps JA, Groothuis TGG. The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biological Reviews. 2010;85:301–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00103.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]