Abstract
GABAB receptors are the G-protein-coupled receptors for GABA, the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in the central nervous system. Pharmacological activation of GABAB receptors regulates neurotransmission and neuronal excitability at pre- and postsynaptic sites. Electrophysiological activation of GABAB receptors in brain slices generally requires strong stimulus intensities. This raises the question as to whether behavioral stimuli are strong enough to activate GABAB receptors. Here we show that GABAB1a-/- mice, which constitutively lack presynaptic GABAB receptors at glutamatergic synapses, are impaired in their ability to acquire an operant learning task. In vivo recordings during the operant conditioning reveal a deficit in learning-dependent increases in synaptic strength at CA3-CA1 synapses. Moreover, GABAB1a-/- mice fail to synchronize neuronal activity in the CA1 area during the acquisition process. Our results support that activation of presynaptic hippocampal GABAB receptors is important for acquisition of a learning task and for learning-associated synaptic changes and network dynamics.
Introduction
GABAB receptors regulate neuronal excitability and synaptic transmission in the brain. Axonal GABAB receptors inhibit voltage-activated Ca2+-channels to reduce the release of GABA, glutamate, and other neurotransmitters [1]-[2]. Dendritic GABAB receptors activate Kir3 channels and inhibit neuronal activity by local shunting and by generating hyperpolarizing postsynaptic potentials. Consistent with the remote location of GABAB receptors in relation to the sites of GABA release [3], electrophysiological activation of GABAB receptors in brain slices typically requires strong stimulus intensities and pooling of synaptically released GABA from many interneurons [4]-[7]. This suggests that GABAB receptors are primarily activated during rhythmic cortical or hippocampal network activity, when ensembles of GABAergic neurons fire in synchrony [5]-[7]. It is still poorly understood whether GABAB receptors in the brain are mostly involved in homeostatic processes or whether they are activated in response to sensory inputs and behavioral tasks [8].
GABAB receptors are composed of principal and auxiliary subunits [2]. The principal subunits GABAB1a, GABAB1b, and GABAB2 form fully functional heteromeric GABAB(1a,2) and GABAB(1b,2) receptors with indistinguishable properties in heterologous cells. The subunit isoform GABAB1a contains a targeting motif in its primary sequence that traffics receptors to axon terminals [9]. In contrast, the subunit isoform GABAB1b traffics receptors to dendritic sites. The auxiliary subunits KCTD8, -12, -12b and -16 are cytoplasmic proteins that constitutively bind to GABAB2 and modulate kinetic properties of the receptor response [10]-[11]. The in vivo roles of GABAB(1a,2) and GABAB(1b,2) receptors were addressed using GABAB1a-/- and GABAB1b-/- mice that lack one or the other subunit isoform [12]. These mice exhibit distinct behavioral phenotypes, consistent with GABAB(1a,2) and GABAB(1b,2) receptors conveying non-redundant pre- and postsynaptic functions [2], [13]. GABAB1a-/- mice exhibited hippocampus-dependent cognitive deficits [12], impaired emotional learning [14], fragmented sleep [15], and infrequent seizures at an advanced age [15]. These phenotypes revealed that the genetic absence of presynaptic GABAB(1a,2) receptors interferes with mnemonic processes and eventually precipitates seizures. However, whether GABAB(1a,2) receptors are involved in the regulation of synaptic strength and network dynamics during the acquisition and performance of a learning task is still unclear.
Here, we used GABAB1a-/- mice to address whether the lack of presynaptic GABAB receptors influences changes in synaptic strength and network synchronizations during a learning task. Specifically, we recorded changes in synaptic strength [16]-[17] and oscillatory dynamics [17] at hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses by continuously recording evoked field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) during the acquisition and performance of an operant conditioning task. Operant conditioning is a type of associative learning involving the activity of many cortical circuits, including the hippocampus [17]-[18]. We found that learning-dependent changes in synaptic strength and network synchronizations in behaving GABAB1a-/- mice are impaired, while basic synaptic properties are normal. Our results support that learning processes and learning-associated network synchronizations depend on the presence of presynaptic GABAB receptors in the hippocampus.
Methods
Experimental Animals
All experiments were carried out with male GABAB1a-/- and wild-type (WT) littermate BALB/c mice [12]. Mice were 4 ± 1 months old at the beginning of the experiments and were kept on a 12-h light/dark cycle at a constant temperature (21 ± 1°C) and humidity (50 ± 7%) until surgery. Mice had ad libitum access to chow and water.
Ethics Statement
The experiments were performed during the light cycle according to European Union Council (2010/63/EU) and Spanish (BOE 34/11370-421, 2013) guidelines. All experiments were approved by the local Ethics Committee (Permit Number 01/11) of the Pablo de Olavide University (Seville, Spain).
Surgery
Mice were anesthetized with 0.8–3% halothane delivered from a calibrated Fluotec 5 (Fluotec-Ohmeda, Tewksbury, MA) vaporizer at a flow rate of 1–2 L/min oxygen. Mice were implanted with bipolar stimulating electrodes aimed at the right Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway of the dorsal hippocampus (2 mm lateral and 1.5 mm posterior to bregma, depth from brain surface 1.0–1.5 mm) [19] and with two recording electrodes aimed at the ipsilateral stratum radiatum underneath the CA1 area (1.2 mm lateral and 2.2 mm posterior to bregma; depth from brain surface 1.0–1.5 mm) [19]. The location of the electrodes was verified histologically (see below) and from the profiles of collected fEPSPs. This showed that most electrodes were located in the stratum radiatum and few electrodes near the pyramidal cell layer. From each animal we selected the recording electrode providing better defined fEPSP responses. These electrodes were made from Teflon-coated tungsten wire (50 μm, Advent Research Materials Ltd., Eynsham, England). The final position of hippocampal electrodes was determined using as a guide the field potential depth profile evoked by paired (40 ms of interval) pulses presented to the Schaffer collateral pathway. A bare silver wire (0.1 mm) was affixed to the skull as a ground. The four wires were connected to a 4-pin socket and the latter was fixed to the skull with the help of two small screws and dental cement [16]. Ten mice were used per experimental group.
Recording and Stimulation Procedures
fEPSPs were recorded with Grass P511 differential amplifiers through a high-impedance probe (2 × 1012 Ω, 10 pF). Electrical stimuli presented to Schaffer collaterals consisted of 100 μs, square, biphasic pulses presented alone, paired, or in trains. Stimulus intensities ranged from 0.02 mA to 0.4 mA for the construction of the input/output curves. For paired-pulse facilitation, the stimulus intensity was set well below the threshold for evoking a population spike, usually 35% of the intensity necessary for evoking a maximum fEPSP response [20]. Paired pulses were presented at six (10, 20, 40, 100, 200, and 500 ms) different pulse intervals.
For LTP induction, the stimulus intensity was set at 35% of the intensity necessary to evoke a maximum fEPSP response. An additional criterion for selecting the stimulus intensity for LTP induction was that a second stimulus, presented 40 ms after a conditioning pulse, evoked a larger (> 20%) synaptic field potential than the first stimulus [21]. After 15 min of baseline recording at 0.05 Hz, each animal was presented with a HFS protocol consisting of five trains (200 Hz, 100 ms) of pulses at a rate of 1/s. This protocol was applied 6 times at intervals of 1 min. The evolution of fEPSPs after the HFS protocol was followed for 60 min at 0.05 Hz. Additional recording sessions (30 min) were carried out during the following four days. Further experimental details can be found elsewhere [16], [22].
Operant Conditioning
GABAB1a-/- mice at the age of operant conditioning show normal locomotor activity and have normal body weight, indicating the absence of overt motor deficits or motivational changes that could influence performance [2], [14]. Training and testing took place in basic Skinner box modules (n = 3) measuring 12.5 cm × 13.5 cm × 18.5 cm (MED Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). The operant chambers were housed within a sound-attenuating chamber (90 cm × 55 cm × 60 cm), which was constantly illuminated (19 W lamp) and exposed to a 45 dB white noise (Cibertec, S.A., Madrid, Spain). Each Skinner box was equipped with a food dispenser from which pellets (MLabRodent Tablet, 20 mg; Test Diet, Richmond, IN, USA) could be delivered by pressing a lever. Before training, mice were handled daily for 7 days and food-deprived to 75–85% of their free-feeding weight [23]-[24]. Training took place for 20 min during successive days, in which mice were shaped to press the lever to receive pellets from the food tray using a fixed-ratio (1:1) schedule. The start and end of each session was indicated by a tone (2 kHz, 200 ms, 70 dB) provided by a loudspeaker located in the recording chamber. Animals were maintained on the 1:1 schedule until they reached the criterion and obtained ≥ 20 pellets in each of 2 successive sessions. WT mice typically reached this criterion after 4–6 days of training [24].
We recorded fEPSPs evoked at the CA3-CA1 synapse across the training (fixed ratio 1:1) sessions. Baseline recordings were collected at session 0 with the animal placed in the same Skinner box but in the absence of the lever and the feeder. Here again, the stimulus intensity was set at 35% of the intensity necessary for evoking a maximum fEPSP response. LFP analyses were carried out using recordings collected in the absence of Schaffer-collateral stimulation [17].
Mice that reached the criterion for the fixed-ratio 1:1 schedule in ≤ 6 sessions were further conditioned using a light/dark protocol for 6 additional days. In this protocol, only lever presses during the light period (20 s) were reinforced with a pellet. Lever presses performed during the dark period (20 ± 10 s) were not reinforced and restarted the dark protocol for an additional random (1–10 s) time. The light/dark coefficient was calculated as follows: (number of lever presses during the light period—number of lever presses during the dark period) / total number of lever presses. In this case, the criterion was to reach a 0 performance (i.e., an equal number of lever presses during the light and the dark periods) for 2 successive sessions.
Conditioning programs, lever presses, and delivered reinforcements were controlled and recorded by a computer, using a MED-PC program (MED Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). All operant sessions including brain stimulation and/or LFP and fEPSP recordings were filmed with a synchronized video capture system (Sony HDR-SR12E, Tokyo, Japan).
Drugs
To study the propensity of WT and GABAB1a-/- mice to generate convulsive seizures, we injected them (i.p.) with the AMPA/kainate receptor agonist kainate (8 mg/kg; Sigma, Saint Louis, MO, USA) dissolved in 0.1 M phosphate buffered saline (PBS) pH = 7.4 [25]. The electrocorticographic activity of the hippocampal pyramidal CA1 area was recorded before and up to 1 h after the injection. Animals were presented with a stimulus session (five 100-millisecond trains of pulses at a rate of 200 Hz) 30 min after the injection.
Histology
Once experiments were finished, mice were deeply re-anesthetized (sodium pentobarbital, 50 mg/kg) and perfused transcardially with saline and 4% phosphate-buffered paraformaldehyde. Their brains were removed, postfixed overnight at 4°C, and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in PBS. Sections were obtained in a microtome (Leica, Wetzlar, Germany) at 50 μm. Selected sections including the dorsal hippocampus were mounted on gelatinized glass slides and stained using the Nissl technique with 0.1% toluidine blue to determine the location of stimulating and recording electrodes.
Data Collection and Analysis
LFP, fEPSP, and 1-volt rectangular pulses corresponding to lever presses, pellet delivery, and brain stimulation, were stored digitally on a computer through an analog/digital converter (CED 1401 Plus, Cambridge, England). Data were analyzed off-line for quantification of each animal's performance in the Skinner box, LFP, and fEPSP with the Spike 2 (CED) program. The slope of evoked fEPSPs was computed as the first derivative (volts/s) of fEPSP recordings (volts). Five successive fEPSPs were averaged, and the mean value of the slope during the rise-time period (i.e., the period of the slope between the initial 10% and the final 10% of the fEPSP) was determined. Computed results were processed for statistical analysis using the IBM SPSS Statistics 18.0 software (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA).
For LFP analysis we used data collected across learning sessions until animals reached the criterion in the fixed-ratio (1:1) schedule or from selective recordings carried out to determine the kainate effects. As described by some of us [17], the analytical procedures, including the frequency domain (using the fast Fourier transform) and analyses of the LFP recordings, as well as the quantification and representation scripts of the power spectral density (PSD plots), were developed with the help of MATLAB routines (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). The purpose of estimating the PSD was to detect any periodicity in the LFP data, by observing peaks at the frequencies corresponding to these periodicities. We selected the following frequency bands: delta, 1–3 Hz; low theta, 3–8 Hz; high theta 8–12 Hz; beta, 12–30 Hz; and HFS, 30–100 Hz. The algorithm included the analysis of mean values of the spectral powers between the different frequency bands inside each epoch, and the analysis of mean values of the spectral powers for the same frequency band between the different epochs. The same procedure was carried out for the peak values of the spectral powers inside each frequency band [17].
Data are always represented as the mean ± SEM. Statistical significance of differences between groups was inferred by one-way ANOVA and ANOVA for repeated measures (data by groups), with a contrast analysis (Dunnett's Post Hoc test) for a further study of significant differences. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.
Results
GABAB1a-/- Mice Exhibit Deficits in Operant Conditioning and Learning-Dependent Changes in Synaptic Strength
For in vivo electrophysiological analysis of behaving mice we chronically implanted stimulating and recording electrodes in the Schaffer collaterals/commissural pathway and in the hippocampal CA1 area, respectively (Fig 1A). The mice were then trained in a Skinner box to obtain a food pellet every time they press a lever in daily sessions of 20 min (fixed ratio of 1:1, Fig 1B). The mice successfully completed the task when pressing the lever ≥ 20 times in 2 successive sessions. It is established that the hippocampus participates in the acquisition and storage of this type of associative learning [17], [22], [24]. Littermate WT mice successfully completed the task in significantly fewer sessions than GABAB1a-/- mice [F(1,14) = 168.633; P < 0.001; Fig 1C, left]. All WT mice completed the task within 6 sessions while only 40% of the GABAB1a-/- mice were successful (Fig 1C, right). In accordance with a previous study [17] and in order to study changes in synaptic strength at the CA3-CA1 synapse during the acquisition process, we evoked fEPSPs (≤ 3 times/min) at the moment the animal was approaching the lever (Fig 1D). The analysis of evoked fEPSPs at the CA3-CA1 synapse indicated that WT but not GABAB1a-/- mice exhibited a significant increase in synaptic strength during the acquisition process [F(1,14) = 1.131; P = 0.027; Fig 1D]. A separate analysis of GABAB1a-/- mice that did or did not reach the selected criterion indicates that both subgroups did not exhibit a significant increase (P = 0.457) in fEPSP slopes during the acquisition sessions. Therefore, successful completion of the learning task by the GABAB1a-/- mice does not predict an increase in synaptic strength.
Mice that successfully reached the above criterion within 6 days were subjected to a more complex operant conditioning task. Pressing the lever was rewarded with a food pellet only during periods of 20 s in which a light bulb above the lever was switched on (light/dark, Fig 1B). All WT littermate mice successfully reached the selected criterion [see Methods; F(1,15) = 7.740; P = 0.014] by the 4th conditioning session, while GABAB1a-/- mice were unsuccessful in completing the task (Fig 1E). No significant differences were observed in the slopes of fEPSPs in the two groups of mice during the training sessions (not illustrated). A possible explanation for the absence of plasticity changes during the light/dark test is that, as previously reported [17], changes in synaptic strength for appetitive behaviors (such as rewarded lever presses) during operant conditioning only take place during the early phase of the acquisition process.
Since GABAB1a-/- mice did not exhibit any overt motor or motivational impairment [2] the above findings cannot be ascribed to any specific difficulty to move around in the Skinner box or to any evident hyperactivity or motor inactivity. In conclusion, these data show that the lack of presynaptic GABAB receptors results in a significant deficit in the ability to learn an operant task and prevents learning-dependent changes in synaptic strength at CA3-CA1 synapses.
GABAB1a-/- Mice Fail to Synchronize Neuronal Activity in the CA1 Area during Operant Conditioning
We analyzed the electrical activity of the CA1 pyramidal layer during the first 6 training sessions of the operant conditioning task (1:1 ratio). We initially collected electroencephalographic epochs that each lasted 1 s in the absence of electrical stimulation of Schaffer collaterals. The averaged (n = 20 times) spectral power of the local field potential (LFP) activity recorded during the 6 sessions is shown in Fig 2A. The spectral power of LFPs collected from WT mice increased in magnitude across the training for most spectral bands (3–8 Hz, 8–12 Hz, 12–30 Hz, and 30–100 Hz; Fig 2C–2F), as previously observed with a similar operant conditioning task [17]. In contrast, GABAB1a-/- mice did not exhibit any increase in spectral power (8–12 Hz, 12–30 Hz, and 30–100 Hz; Fig 2D–2F) or showed a significant decrease (P < 0.05) for some spectral bands (1–3 Hz and 3–8 Hz; Fig 2B and 2C). Thus, deficits of GABAB1a-/- mice in the acquisition of the operant conditioning task are paralleled by a deficit in the synchronization of neuronal activity in the hippocampal CA1 area during operant conditioning.
Input/Output Curves and Short-Term Plasticity at CA3-CA1 Synapses of GABAB1a-/- Mice Appear Normal
To study whether the above deficits of GABAB1a-/- mice in operant learning and network synchronization are caused by alterations in the basic electrophysiological properties of CA3-CA1 synapses we quantified input/output curves. These experiments were carried out in the same two groups of mice. Single pulses of increasing intensity delivered to the ipsilateral Schaffer collaterals evoked similar slope increases in CA1 fEPSPs in WT and GABAB1a-/- mice (Fig 3A). In both groups of mice the input/output relationships were best fitted by sigmoid curves (r ≥ 0.996; P < 0.0001), supporting that the CA3-CA1 synapse in alert GABAB1a-/- mice functions normally.
We additionally tested whether a typical short-term plasticity phenomenon [22] at the CA3-CA1 synapse, synaptic facilitation, is altered in GABAB1a-/- mice. Synaptic facilitation evoked by the presentation of a pair of pulses has been related to increased neurotransmitter release during the 2nd pulse [26]. WT and GABAB1a-/- mice both showed a typical and significant [F(1,17) = 254.284; P < 0.01] increase in the response to the 2nd pulse at short (10–200 ms) time intervals (Fig 3B). The increase in the response to the 2nd pulse had a tendency to be smaller with GABAB1a-/- than with WT mice but this tendency did not reach statistical significance at any of the selected intervals [F(1,17) = 0.956; P = 0.342]. It therefore appears that this form of short-term plasticity is normal in GABAB1a-/- mice.
Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) at CA3-CA1 Synapses Is Increased in GABAB1a-/- Mice
We next studied LTP induced by high-frequency stimulation (HFS) of the CA3-CA1 synapse in alert mice (Fig 3C). GABAB1a-/- and WT mice exhibited a robust LTP following HFS [F(1,12) = 169.519; P < 0.001]. Selectively the GABAB1a-/- mice also exhibited increased LTP during the next four days of recording. The LTP evoked in GABAB1a-/- mice was significantly larger and longer-lasting than that of WT animals [F(1,13) = 6.409; P ≤ 0.03] during the first recording day. A possible explanation for this finding is that disinhibited glutamate release in the GABAB1a-/- mice facilitates the development of LTP [12].
GABAB1a-/- and WT Littermate Mice Differ in Oscillatory Properties of Hippocampal LFPs
Presynaptic GABAB receptors contribute to spontaneous down-state transitions in the entorhinal cortex [7]. GABAB1a-/- mice of several months of age occasionally develop seizures [15]. It is therefore surprising that GABAB1a-/- mice fail to synchronize neuronal activity in the hippocampal CA1 area during operant conditioning. We decided to study the oscillatory properties of LFPs in behaving GABAB1a-/- mice to address whether these mice suffer from a general deficit in synchronizing network oscillations. Under baseline conditions, GABAB1a-/- and WT littermate mice presented LFPs in the CA1 area of similar spectral power. A brief HFS (five 200 Hz, 100 ms trains of pulses at a rate of 1/s) applied to the Schaffer collaterals evoked a slight but statistically non-significant increase in the spectral power compared to baseline in GABAB1a-/- mice, an effect that was barely detectable in WT mice (Fig 4A and 4B).
The proconvulsive drug kainate has been used to determine the propensity of cortical circuits to oscillate [25]. When we applied the same brief HFS protocol in the presence of a low dose (8 mg/kg) of kainate we observed a significant increase in spectral power with GABAB1a-/- but not with WT mice (Fig 4A and 4B). Analysis of frequency bands in the presence of kainate revealed a significantly increased spectral power in GABAB1a-/- mice across the entire LFP spectrum (Fig 4C–4G). Of note, HFS in the presence of kainate also induced a significant increase in spectral power in WT mice for the gamma band (30–100 Hz; Fig 4G).
Analysis of the PSD allowed us to determine the spectral power and periodicity of the LFP data by observing the power peak at the frequency corresponding to that periodicity (Fig 5A). PSD plots (Fig 5B–5G) for the LFP activity revealed that HFS, in particular in the presence of kainate, induced a significant increase of the PSD value (Fig 5H) and a shift of the dominant frequency towards lower frequency in the GABAB1a-/- mice (6.97 Hz before HFS, 6.36 Hz after HFS, and 5.54 Hz after kainate + HFS; Fig 5E–5G and 5I), a change that was absent in WT mice (6.56 Hz, 6.46 Hz, and 6.51 Hz respectively; Fig 5B–5D and 5I).
In summary, the GABAB1a-/- mice differ from WT mice in their propensity to synchronize electrocortical activities upon HFS in the presence of kainate. It is therefore counterintuitive that GABAB1a-/- mice are unable to appropriately synchronize network activity during associative learning tasks.
Discussion
Ultrastructural methods revealed that GABAB receptors in the hippocampus are most abundant at glutamatergic terminals [2], [27], [28]. We previously used pharmacological and physiological activation of GABAB receptors in acute slices to show that selectively GABAB(1a,2) receptors inhibit glutamate release at brain synapses, including the CA3-CA1 and mossy fiber-CA3 synapses [2], [12], [29]. Lack of GABAB(1a,2) receptors affects the expression of LTP and produces cognitive deficits [12], [30]. However, it remained unclear whether behavioral stimuli can activate GABAB(1a,2) receptors. GABA released from GABAergic terminals has to diffuse to neighboring synapses to activate GABAB(1a,2) receptors at glutamatergic terminals. Interneurons therefore have to fire in synchrony to release sufficient GABA to spill over to glutamatergic terminals. In keeping with a requirement for synchronous interneuron activity to engage GABAB(1a,2) receptors these receptors exert a powerful inhibitory influence over network oscillations in vitro [7]. Cortical neurons display synchronous fluctuations between periods of persistent activity ('UP states') and periods of relative quiescence ('DOWN states'). Presynaptic GABAB(1a,2) receptors contribute to spontaneous DOWN state transitions in slice preparations of the entorhinal cortex [7]. Consistent with a role for GABA(B1a,2) receptors in the control of network oscillations, GABAB1a-/- mice also have a propensity to develop seizures at an advanced age [15].
Thus far, only one study has addressed whether sensory stimuli can activate GABAB(1a,2) and/or GABAB(1b,2) receptors at identified synapses. Using anesthetized GABAB1b-/- mice, Palmer and colleagues found that postsynaptic GABAB receptors exert an inhibitory influence on cortical layer 5 pyramidal cells in response to hind paw stimulation [27]. This supports that salient sensory events recruit postsynaptic GABAB(1b,2) receptors.
Here we used behaving GABAB1a-/- mice with implanted electrodes to address whether presynaptic GABAB receptors in the hippocampus are recruited during an operant conditioning task. As already described, both the hippocampus [17] and prefrontal cortical areas [18] participate in the acquisition of this type of associative task. We found that GABAB1a-/- mice exhibit a deficit in learning-dependent increases in synaptic strength at hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses and a lack of network synchronization during the acquisition phase. Interestingly, the largest increase in fEPSP slopes observed with WT mice took place one session before reaching the criterion. The fEPSP slopes then remained above baseline values for the remaining sessions (3 to 6). In a previous report we observed the largest increase in fEPSP slopes one session after reaching the criterion [17]. A possible explanation for this discrepancy is the genetic background of the mice used in the two studies. The BALB/c mice used in the present study appear to learn faster than the C57BL/6 mice used in the earlier study [17], consistent with a report that compared the two mouse strains in learning tasks [31].
These findings were paralleled with impairment in the acquisition of the task. Of note, basic properties of CA3-CA1 synapses, such as paired-pulse facilitation and input-output relationships, were normal in behaving GABAB1a-/- mice, supporting that these synapses do not undergo major adaptive changes. Our results therefore support that activation of presynaptic GABAB receptors in the hippocampus is necessary during the acquisition of a learning task and for learning-dependent synaptic changes and network dynamics. A previous in vitro study in acute slices of 3-week old mice, revealed a deficit in LTP at CA3-CA1 synapses, which was explained by a saturation of LTP mechanisms due to uncontrolled glutamate release [12]. In contrast LTP at CA3-CA1 synapses in response to HFS was increased in behaving GABAB1a-/- mice. The present findings may be explained by differences in the LTP protocols (40 stimuli at 0.05 Hz paired with postsynaptic depolarization versus HFS) or homeostatic mechanisms that, in older mice, drift the synapse back into the dynamic range. It is also possible that the HFS protocol used here evokes NMDA receptor-independent LTP in the CA1 region [32], which is not the case with the LTP protocol used during in vitro experiments [12].
Increased LTP in response to HFS in GABAB1a-/- mice also contrasts with a lack of learning-dependent increases in synaptic strength. A similar dissociation between HFS-induced CA3-CA1 synaptic plasticity and a lack of changes in synaptic strength during associative learning has been described in transgenic mice overexpressing the neurotrophin receptor TrkC [33] or the transcription factor CREB [34]. This dissociation may be explained by a differential activation of interneurons during HFS and instrumental conditioning. GABAB1a-/- mice do not show the typical network synchronization during acquisition of the instrumental conditioning task. This cannot be explained by a general inability of behaving GABAB1a-/- mice to synchronize network oscillations in the hippocampus, since these mice show an increased propensity to synchronize their electrocortical activity in the presence of kainate after HFS. It appears that presynaptic GABAB(1a,2) receptors are necessary to induce learning-dependent changes in network dynamics while kainate more efficiently induces network synchronizations through postsynaptic glutamate receptors when presynaptic GABAB(1a,2) receptors are absent. This suggests that the (homeostatic) balance of excitation/inhibition at pre- and postsynaptic sites is crucial for inducing learning-dependent network synchronizations and/or to prevent the development of an excessive synchronization leading to the appearance of unwanted seizures [35]. In summary, our experiments show that deficits in presynaptic GABAB(1a,2) receptor signaling lead to perturbed network activity, which not only impairs normal learning but ultimately also manifests as epilepsy [15].
Acknowledgments
The authors thank María Sánchez-Enciso for technical assistance.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper.
Funding Statement
This study was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (BFU2014-56692-R) to AG and JMD-G, and from the Swiss National Science Foundation (3100A0-117816) to BB, and a grant from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Program (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement N° 201714 (DEVANX) to AG, JMD-G, and BB.
References
- 1.Chalifoux JR, Carter AG (2011) GABAB receptor modulation of synaptic function. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21: 339–344. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gassmann M, Bettler B (2012) Regulation of neuronal GABAB receptor functions by subunit composition. Nat Rev Neurosci 13: 380–394. 10.1038/nrn3249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kulik A, Vida I, Luján R, Haas CA, López-Bendito G, et al. (2003) Subcellular localization of metabotropic GABAB receptor subunits GABAB1a/b and GABAB2 in the rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 23: 11026–11035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Davies CH, Collingridge GL (1996) Regulation of EPSPs by the synaptic activation of GABAB autoreceptors in rat hippocampus. J Physiol (Lond) 496: 451–470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Scanziani M (2000) GABA spillover activates postsynaptic GABAB receptors to control rhythmic hippocampal activity. Neuron 25: 673–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kohl MM, Paulsen O (2010) The roles of GABAB receptors in cortical network activity. Adv Pharmacol 58: 205–229. 10.1016/S1054-3589(10)58009-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Craig MT, Mayne EW, Bettler B, Paulsen O, McBain CJ (2013) Distinct roles of GABAB1a- and GABAB1b-containing GABAB receptors in spontaneous and evoked termination of persistent cortical activity. J Physiol (Lond) 591: 835–843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Vertkin I, Styr B, Slomowitz E, Ofir N, Shapira I, et al. (2015) GABAB receptor deficiency causes failure of neuronal homeostasis in hippocampal networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112: E3291–3299. 10.1073/pnas.1424810112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Biermann B, Ivankova-Susankova K, Bradaia A, Abdel Aziz S, Besseyrias V, et al. (2010) The Sushi domains of GABAB receptors function as axonal targeting signals. J Neurosci 30: 1385–1394. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3172-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Schwenk J, Metz M, Zolles G, Turecek R, Fritzius T, et al. (2010) Native GABAB receptors are heteromultimers with a family of auxiliary subunits. Nature 465: 231–235. 10.1038/nature08964 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Turecek R, Schwenk J, Fritzius T, Ivankova K, Zolles G, et al. (2014) Auxiliary GABAB receptor subunits uncouple G protein βγ subunits from effector channels to induce desensitization. Neuron 82: 1032–1044. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Vigot R, Barbieri S, Bräuner-Osborne H, Turecek R, Shigemoto R, et al. (2006) Differential compartmentalization and distinct functions of GABAB receptor variants. Neuron 50: 589–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pérez-Garci E, Gassmann M, Bettler B, Larkum ME (2006) The GABAB1b isoform mediates long-lasting inhibition of dendritic Ca2+ spikes in layer 5 somatosensory pyramidal neurons. Neuron 50: 603–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jacobson LH, Kelly PH, Bettler B, Kaupmann K, Cryan JF (2006) GABAB1 receptor isoforms differentially mediate the acquisition and extinction of aversive taste memories. J Neurosci 26: 8800–8803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Vienne J, Bettler B, Franken P, Tafti M (2010) Differential effects of GABAB receptor subtypes, γ-hydroxybutyric acid, and baclofen on EEG activity and sleep regulation. J Neurosci 30: 14194–14204. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3145-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gruart A, Muñoz MD, Delgado-García JM (2006) Involvement of the CA3-CA1 synapse in the acquisition of associative learning in behaving mice. J Neurosci 26: 1077–1087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jurado-Parras MT, Sánchez-Campusano R, Castellanos NP, del-Pozo F, Gruart A, et al. (2013) Differential contribution of hippocampal circuits to appetitive and consummatory behaviors during operant conditioning of behaving mice. J Neurosci 33: 2293–2304. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1013-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hasan MT, Hernández-González S, Dogbevia G, Treviño M, Bertocchi I, et al. (2013) Role of motor cortex NMDA receptors in learning-dependent synaptic plasticity of behaving mice. Nat Commun 4: 2258 10.1038/ncomms3258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Paxinos G, Franklin KBG (2001) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. London: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gureviciene I, Ikonen S, Gurevicius K, Sarkaki A, van Groen T, et al. (2004) Normal induction but accelerated decay of LTP in APP + PS1 transgenic mice. Neurobiol Dis 15: 188–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bliss TV, Gardner-Medwin AR (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanaestetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol (Lond) 232: 357–374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Madroñal N, Delgado-García JM, Gruart A (2007) Differential effects of long-term potentiation evoked at the CA3-CA1 synapse before, during, and after the acquisition of classical eyeblink conditioning in behaving mice. J Neurosci 27: 12139–12146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gottlieb M, Leal-Campanario R, Campos-Esparza MR, Sánchez-Gómez MV, Alberdi E, et al. (2006) Neuroprotection by two polyphenols following excitotoxicity and experimental ischemia. Neurobiol Dis 23: 374–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Madroñal N, López-Aracil C, Rangel A, del Río JA, Delgado-García JM, et al. (2010) Effects of enriched physical and social environments on motor performance, associative learning, and hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. PloS One 5(6): e11130 10.1371/journal.pone.0011130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Valles-Ortega J, Duran J, Garcia-Rocha M, Bosch C, Saez I, et al. (2011) Neurodegeneration and functional impairments associated with glycogen synthase accumulation in a mouse model of Lafora disease. EMBO Mol Med 3: 667–681. 10.1002/emmm.201100174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zucker RS, Regehr WG (2002) Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Physiol 64: 355–405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Palmer LM, Schulz JM, Murphy SC, Ledergerber D, Murayama M, et al. (2012) The cellular basis of GABAB-mediated interhemispheric inhibition. Science 335: 989–993. 10.1126/science.1217276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.López-Bendito G, Shigemoto R, Kulik A, Vida I, Fairén A, et al. (2004) Distribution of metabotropic GABA receptor subunits GABAB1a/b and GABAB2 in the rat hippocampus during prenatal and postnatal development. Hippocampus 14: 836–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Guetg N, Seddik R, Vigot R, Turecek R, Gassmann M, et al. (2009) The GABAB1a isoform mediates heterosynaptic depression at hippocampal mossy fiber synapses. J Neurosci 29: 1414–1423. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3697-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Shaban H, Humeau Y, Herry C, Cassasus G, Shigemoto R, et al. (2006) Generalization of amygdala LTP and conditioned fear in the absence of presynaptic inhibition. Nat Neurosci 9: 1028–1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Johnson JM, Bailey JM, Johnson JE, Newland MCh (2010) Performance of BALB/c and C57BL/6 mice under an incremental repeated acquisition of behavioral chains procedure. Behav Processes 84: 705–714. 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Moosmang S, Haider N, Klugbauer N, Adelsberger H, Langwieser N, et al. (2005) Role of hippocampal Cav1.2Ca2+ channels in NMDA receptor-independent synaptic plasticity and spatial memory. J Neurosci 25: 9883–9892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sahún I, Delgado-García JM, Amador-Arjona A, Giralt A, Alberch J, et al. (2007) Dissociation between CA3-CA1 synaptic plasticity and associative learning in TgNTRK3 transgenic mice. J Neurosci 27: 2253–2260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gruart A, Benito E, Delgado-García JM, Barco A (2012) Enhanced cAMP response element-binding protein activity increases neuronal excitability, hippocampal long-term potentiation, and classical eyeblink conditioning in alert behaving mice. J Neurosci 32: 17431–17441. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4339-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schwaller B, Tetko IV, Tandon P, Silveira DC, Vreugdenhil M, et al. (2004) Parvalbumin deficiency affects network properties resulting in increased susceptibility to epileptic seizures. Mol Cell Neurosci 25: 650–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper.