Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Youth Serv Rev. 2016 Feb;61:109–116. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.12.004

Promoting Birth Parents’ Relationships with their Toddlers upon Reunification: Results from Promoting First Relationships® Home Visiting Program

Monica L Oxford 1, Maureen Marcenko 2, Charles B Fleming 3, Mary Jane Lohr 4, Susan J Spieker 5
PMCID: PMC4746016  NIHMSID: NIHMS754345  PMID: 26869740

Abstract

Birth parents, once reunified with their child after a foster care placement, are in need of in-home support services to prevent reoccurrence of maltreatment and reentry into foster care, establish a strong relationship with their child, and enhance child well-being. Few studies have addressed the efficacy of home visiting services for reunified birth parents of toddlers. This study reports on the findings from a randomized control trial of a 10-week home visiting program, Promoting First Relationships® (Kelly, Sandoval, Zuckerman, & Buehlman, 2008), for a subsample of 43 reunified birth parents that were part of the larger trial. We describe how the elements of the intervention align with the needs of parents and children in child welfare. Although the sample size was small and most of the estimates of intervention effects were not statistically significant, the effect sizes and the pattern of results suggest that the intervention may have improved both observed parenting sensitivity and observed child behaviors as well as decreased parent report of child behavior problems. Implications are that providing in-home services soon after a reunification may be efficacious in strengthening birth parents’ capacity to respond sensitively to their children as well as improving child social and emotional outcomes and well-being.

Keywords: Reunified Birth Parents, Home Visiting Intervention, Promoting First Relationships, Child Welfare, Foster Care, Parental Sensitivity

1. Introduction

Young children are especially vulnerable to child maltreatment and subsequent child welfare removal, both as an entry cohort and post-reunification. Children birth to three years constitute one-third of first entries into out-of-home placement in child welfare, a rate higher than any other age group (Administration on Children, Youth and Families, 2013). Among those reunified, young children are at greatest risk of recurring maltreatment resulting in reentry to care, exposing them to further trauma and disruptions in primary nurturing relationships. It is estimated that 40% (Jonson-Reid, 2003) to 50% (Fuller, 2005) of young children will be maltreated post-reunification and 20% to 30% will again experience removal from their parents’ care (Shaw, 2006; Wulczyn, 2004).

The three aims of child welfare in the U.S. are safety, permanency, and well-being. Although historically U.S. policy has focused on the first two, recent priorities have integrated well-being. In an April 2012 information memorandum, the Administration for Children and Families (Administration on Children and Families, 2012) explained the enhanced priority of promoting social and emotional well-being for children and youth receiving child welfare services. The memo acknowledged that “while ensuring safety and achieving permanency are necessary to well-being, they are not sufficient” (p. 2). That same year, Title IV-E demonstration programs and discretionary grants were designed in part to develop capacity to provide evidence-based parenting support to caregivers so they could provide their children with the secure relationships required for social and emotional well-being.

Stability and continuity of attachment relationships are critical to the well-being of children. In child welfare, all reunified toddlers, by definition, have experienced a disruption in their primary attachment relationship. Attachment disruptions further exacerbate social and emotional development that has already been adversely affected by maltreatment, and can result in toddler behavior that is challenging for the reunified caregiver (Newton, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 2000; Rubin, O’Reilly, Luan, & Localio, 2007). If the relationship further deteriorates as a result, well-being, safety and permanency can be jeopardized again.

Many of the risk factors associated with recurrence and re-entry are not modifiable (age, race, maltreatment type) or are difficult to address in the near term (parent mental health, education, income) and are therefore outside the control of the child welfare system (Hindley, Ramchandani, & Jones, 2006; Kimberlin, Anthony, & Austin, 2009). However, parenting and relationship quality constitute modifiable factors well within child welfare’s purview and purpose. Unfortunately, most parenting programs provided to families in child welfare do not focus on relationship quality and do not use strategies with empirical support (Horwitz, Chamberlain, Landsverk, & Mullican, 2010).

On behalf of this argument for relationship-based parenting support for reunified parents, we describe a 10-week home visiting program, Promoting First Relationships®, (PFR; Kelly, Sandoval, Zuckerman, & Buehlman, 2008), that is strengths-based and grounded in attachment theory. We present results of a study that is part of a larger community-based randomized control trial in which PFR was assessed for its effectiveness (Spieker, Oxford, Kelly, Nelson, & Fleming, 2012a). The original aims of the parent study, called the Fostering Families Project, were to assess if PFR improved parenting and child outcomes for foster parents of toddlers recently placed in their care. However, because the program was designed as a community based participatory research project, engagement of the community led to alterations in the original design. The regional child welfare department stressed the importance of including reunified birth parents in the study. This change was accepted as feasible and implemented. Ultimately, 27% of the 210 caregivers in the Fostering Families Project were birth parents who were reunited with their children after a foster care separation.

1.1 Families Experiencing Removals: A Vulnerable Population

Young children who are removed from their parent’s care due to maltreatment typically face risks that began in the prenatal period and potentially extend through early childhood, including low birth weight, birth abnormalities, lack of prenatal care, and exposure to drugs, alcohol, and other teratogens (Needell & Barth, 1998; Rosenfeld, Wasserman, & Pilowsky, 1998; Wulczyn, 1994). The resulting complex interaction between genetic and environmental factors (Tomalski & Johnson, 2010) compromises the infants’ regulatory capacities, which can lead to problems in mood regulation, sensory integration, motor control, sleep, and behavioral control (Degangi, Breinbauer, Doussard, Porges, & Greenspan, 2000), and to adverse health and mental health outcomes through the life course (McEwen & Gianaros, 2010; Shonkoff et al., 2012). The key formative experience for children in foster care is the experience of significant risk in their birth family without adult protection, which resulted in placement into the foster care system. Maltreating parents can create an irresolvable paradox for their children; they can be simultaneously frightening and the only source of caregiving (van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1999). This paradox alters the behavioral and stress reactive systems of infants, becoming one of the mechanisms that lead to impaired behavioral, emotional, and health outcomes (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001; De Bellis, 2005; Twardosz & Lutzker, 2010).

While child removal from the family provides protection from maltreatment, it also exposes children to placement instability and relationship disruptions. Despite the risks associated with multiple relationship disruptions (multiple changes in primary caregivers) in the first years of life (Goldsmith, Oppenheim, & Wanlass, 2004), placement changes are the norm and may put the child on a trajectory of increasing placement instability (Webster, Barth, & Needell, 2000). Toddler participants in the Fostering Families Project experienced an average of 2.7 caregiver changes from birth to 18 months (Spieker et al., 2012a). Multiple placements and episodic foster care are linked to increased probability of mental health problems (James, Landsverk, Slymen, & Leslie, 2004; Lewis, Dozier, Ackerman, & Sepulveda-Kozakowski, 2007; Rubin et al., 2004), as well as a diminished ability to develop secure attachments (Penzerro & Lein, 1995).

Following serious maltreatment, children who have been placed in out-of-home care can continue to experience relationship disruptions and insecure attachments to subsequent caregivers. Foster children often adapt to relationship disruptions by turning away from new caregivers when they are feeling distressed (Dozier, Zeanah, & Bernard, 2013). This behavior may fail to elicit responsiveness from caregivers (Stovall-McClough & Dozier, 2004), which in turn may increase their risk for a new insecure attachment and even further maltreatment. Although infants placed at younger ages and those fortunate enough to have parents who are themselves secure can subsequently develop more secure, coherent behavior and less avoidant behavior (Stovall-McClough & Dozier, 2004), most reunified toddlers are returning to parents who very likely have insecure attachment representations themselves (Marcenko, Newby, Mienko, & Courtney, 2011).

Child welfare involved parents are, like their children, a vulnerable population. Their histories contain reports of childhood trauma and abuse, with resulting consequences associated with exposure to maltreatment. In a study of 408 reunified birth parents in Washington, it was documented that 55% had mental health diagnoses, 38% were experiencing domestic violence, 36% had substance abuse problems, 55% had annual incomes of less than $10,000, and 60% had less than a high school education (Marcenko et al., 2011). In this context of multiple vulnerabilities, parenting is scrutinized and judged. Not surprisingly, parents express a range of emotions including guilt, fear, anger and outrage, along with profound stigma (Scholte et al., 1999). Even when parents successfully navigate the child welfare system and are reunified with their children, they often feel fragile and uncertain in their parenting role (Carlson, Matto, Smith, & Eversman, 2006). Interventions that attend to the unique needs of the child/parent dyad in ways that are empathic and supportive are essential to creating a relationship that overcomes the negative experience of maltreatment and separation.

1.2 Reunified Dyads’ Need for Services

Family reunification is a time of intense emotions, both positive and negative. Joy at being together as a family can be tinged by parental shame and guilt related to prior maltreatment and difficulties re-establishing a relationship with children who may exhibit distress and challenging behaviors (Carlson et al., 2006). Unfortunately for children in foster care, reunification does not automatically create a safe, secure, and emotionally supportive parent-child relationship. Birth parents face many challenges in re-reestablishing their relationship with their child after reunification. Very young children who have experienced the loss of one or more primary attachment figures through multiple placements may be emotionally dysregulated and show levels of distress that are overwhelming to a caregiver (Dozier, Higley, Albus, & Nutter, 2002).

Birth parents are typically only required to take parenting classes prior to being reunified with their child. These classes are “…generally delivered in an ad hoc way and often characterized by uninformed practices with very little, if any attention to whether actual parenting practices change…with one-size-fits-all curriculum” (p. 4) (Beckmann, Knitzer, Cooper, & Dicker, 2010). The classes are generally psychoeducational, and not individualized to the parent-child relationship. However, both members of the reunified dyad may have needs for services (Heller, Smyke, & Boris, 2002). Intervention services aimed at birth parent-child relationships are critical to increase the likelihood of a healthy reunion and diminish the reoccurrence of maltreatment. In dyads that have had a history of maladaptation, the “real patient” is the parent-child relationship (Sameroff, 2004), which is why it is ineffective to only provide parenting classes prior to reunification. If the relationship is the real patient, then the parent and child must have an opportunity to interact in “real life” before parenting improvements begin (Beckmann et al., 2010).

Despite the need for evidence-based interventions for reunified families, in a recent survey of 46 states, “…most states reported a greater availability of post-permanency supports for adoptive parents compared to birth parents upon reunification…” (Child Trends and Zero to Three, 2013, p. 24). As noted by Barth (2012), there was only one program, Child Parent Psychotherapy (CPP; Lieberman, Van Horn, & Ghosh Ippen, 2005) identified by the California Evidence-Based Clearinghouse for Child Welfare (CEBC) for children birth-to-three that has the second highest endorsement, “well-supported by research.” CPP takes 52-weeks of service and is “hardly a good fit with the much shorter period of services typical for child welfare–involved families” (Barth, 2012, p. 28). Since Barth’s commentary in 2012, only one additional program has been added in this category, Attachment & Biobehavioral Catch-up (ABC; Dozier et al., 2009). Other relationship-focused programs that are shorter in duration have not been evaluated on samples of child welfare dyads and thus little is known about the generalizability to this population (Barth, 2012).

Services to support birth-to-three children who have been reunified with their birth parent must work in the “real world,” which means they should be short-term, efficient, and feasible. The results of a comprehensive meta-analysis (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van Ijzendoorn, & Juffer, 2003) indicated that home visiting services, which are the most feasible for reunified birth families given the demands of full-time parenting, the need to participate in mandated services and transportation barriers, are most efficient when there are fewer than 16 sessions, and when the service uses video feedback.

1.3 PFR Community Based Home Visiting Program

PFR, a home visiting program designed to support families, was adapted to address the social and emotional needs of families with toddlers for the Fostering Families Project. PFR is a brief 10- session intervention program that uses a manualized but flexible curriculum combined with video feedback, worksheets and handouts, and a strengths-based orientation to promote more sensitive parenting. Reflective consultation for PFR providers is also an essential element of the program. We believe that the use of video feedback is particularly helpful for parents because they are able to step outside of the interaction and observe the child’s cues and communications. Research indicates that video feedback is more effective than psychoeducational approaches in altering parental sensitivity (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2003), and it has the capacity to increase insight and reflective functioning, which in turn supports sensitive caregiving (Fukkink, 2008).

1.4 Conceptual Model

The four main aims of PFR are 1) to increase the parents’ understanding of the needs and feelings of their toddler and understand how they as parents are important to their child; 2) to help parents recognize their child’s communications and cues and respond to the emotional content of those communications and cues with sensitivity; 3) to increase the parents’ own sense of confidence and competence that they are capable of responding to their child’s needs and feelings; and 4) to increase parents’ awareness of their own feelings and needs as parents and how their feelings and needs impact their relationship with their child. The PFR training program instructs providers to use a strengths-based approach in their interactions with caregivers. Table 1 identifies risks that reunified birth families may have and how elements of PFR specifically can meet the needs of birth families in very personal ways. Taken together these elements are woven in with the use of video feedback, handouts, and “thoughts for the week.” The provider and parent enter into a partnership of inquiry that is based on positive respect for parents and their relationship with their child. These methods, grounded firmly in the strengths-based model, support parents’ capacity to learn more about themselves as parents and their child’s emotional needs that give rise to certain behaviors.

TABLE 1.

Parental Risk Factors Addressed by PFR

Risk Factor PFR Element
Low parental self-efficacy
  • Provider use of positive instructive feedback during video feedback identifies positive shared moments and what the parent is doing well to support the child.

  • Provider engagement that is respectful, contingent, and positive supports the parent’s sense of self as worthy.

Misattribution of meaning of toddler challenging behavior
  • Parent learns to observe child’s behavior via video feedback.

  • Parent learns to reflect on the underlying social emotional needs via video feedback, provider’s use of reflective questions and comments, and handouts all focused on identifying and labeling underlying social and emotional needs of children.

  • Baby Cue (NCAST.org) cards and videos help parent learn how children communicate nonverbally.

  • Reflective questions help parent “enter the mind of the child” and discuss what the child might be needing or feeling.

Difficulty regulating self
while distressed; difficulty
regulating child’s distress
  • Handouts and exercises that specifically address distress of parent and child and methods for calming.

  • Video feedback during times of distress; reflective questions about internal states of parent and child and how needs can be met for both.

  • “Intervention Worksheet” handout takes the parent through steps in the process of understanding challenging behavior: 1) defining the behavior, 2) identifying underlying feelings of parent and child, 3) identifying the underlying needs of parent and child, 4) thinking about ways to meet the needs of parent and child.

Difficulty being present,
calm, and appropriately
responsive to child fear,
anger, distress
  • Handouts to demonstrate attachment-based concepts and help parent understand the child’s need for a secure base from which to explore and a safe haven from which to receive comfort, protection, and support.

  • Video reflection to help parent “see” attachment-based behavior and needs.

  • Providing parent with a supportive presence to enable them to feel safe and secure enough to help the child.

Difficulty following the
Child’s lead in play
  • Positive instructive feedback during video feedback to identify times when the parent successfully follows the child’s lead.

  • Handouts on play and teaching which discuss the importance of following the child’s lead.

Underestimating how
important the parent is to the
child
  • During video feedback, provider points out how child is watching and responding to the parent’s words and behaviors.

  • Celebrating moments of shared joy during video feedback and other times.

Child’s trauma history
leading to behaviors that do
not elicit nurturing care
  • Video observation to help caregiver see the world from the child’s experience.

  • Reflective questions help the caregiver think about the child’s underlying social and emotional needs.

1.5 The Current Study

The rationale behind a focused examination of the subsample of reunified parent-child dyads became evident during service delivery in the Fostering Families Project. The needs and the context of parenting for reunified birth parents were different from that of kin and foster care providers. Birth parents had significantly lower incomes, were predominantly single parents with less than a high school education, and had poorer health. Consequently, we examined outcomes in the reunified subsample using the same approach as the main effects study (Spieker et al., 2012a). In our review of the literature, we were unable to find similar studies that evaluated relationship based, home-visiting programs specifically for birth parents of children birth-to-three during the post-reunification period. As states move toward provision of more post-reunification services, they will be searching for evidence-based services for this population (Sciamanna, 2013). Closer examination of this small but important subgroup yields a pilot study of the efficacy of PFR for child welfare involved parents and their young children.

We hypothesized that relative to the comparison condition, parents who received PFR would demonstrate improvements in their self-report of understanding toddler’s behavior, reductions in perceiving the child as difficult and reporting a dysfunctional parent-child relationship; improvements in observed parental sensitivity and parental support; and a reduction of parental report of child behavior problems. We also hypothesized an increase in observed child self-regulation, exploration of novel stimuli, engagement with parent, and secure based behavior among the children in the families who received PFR, relative to the comparison group.

2. Method

2.1 Participants

Two hundred and ten toddlers and their caregivers were recruited into the Fostering Families Project (FFP) (Spieker et al., 2012a), between April of 2007 and March of 2010. The primary recruitment area was a single, metropolitan county in Washington State. All study procedures and participant consent forms were reviewed and approved by the state IRB. Using state Department of Social and Health Services (DSHS) records, a DSHS social worker identified all children in state dependency between the ages of 10 and 24 months who had experienced a court-ordered placement that resulted in a change in primary caregiver within the prior 7 weeks. Toddler-caregiver dyads who were eligible and agreed to participate were randomly assigned to receive either the PFR intervention or a psychoeducational program developed for this study called Early Education Support (EES). Eligible caregivers spoke English and could be foster parents, birth parents, or adult kin. The consenting caregiver and child were assessed at baseline, received intervention services, and then assessed immediately post-intervention and six months later if they were still in the same household.

At enrollment there were 56 biological parents and their recently reunified toddlers. Forty-three dyads (18 in PFR and 25 in the comparison condition) completed all three research visits. Demographic and background characteristics of these 43 families are shown in Table 2. There were no statistically significant (p < .05) differences between experimental conditions in any demographic or background variables.

TABLE 2.

Baseline Characteristics by Intervention Condition

EES (n=25)
n (%)
PFR (n=18)
n (%)
Child male 11 (44) 9 (50)
Child Hispanic 3 (12) 0 (0)
Child race
    Native American/Alaskan Native 1 (4) 0 (0)
    Black 1 (4) 3 (17)
    Mixed race 2 (8) 5 (28)
    Unable to determine 2 (8) 0 (0)
    White 19 (76) 10 (56)
Multiple removals 1 (4) 5 (28)
Household income <$20,000 per year 14 (56) 12 (67)
Caregiver male 4 (16) 1 (6)

M (SD) M (SD)

Child age in months 18.15 (4.79) 18.29 (5.32)
Number of caregiver changes prior to
enrollment
3.04 (1.14) 2.94 (1.16)
Child age in months at first removal 8.59 (6.78) 7.23 (6.86)

Note: M = mean, SD = standard deviation.

2.2 Intervention Exposure

The PFR intervention consisted of ten weekly 60- to 75-minute in-home visits by trained providers from community mental health agencies. Of the parents in the analysis sample of the current study, 12 (67%) in the PFR condition received all ten sessions. Details about the intervention are provided by Kelly and colleagues (Kelly et al., 2008) and (Spieker et al., 2012a, b).

Families in the EES condition received three monthly 90-minute, in-home sessions delivered by an early education specialist. The sessions consisted of instruction in early childhood developmental issues as well as referral to service programs such as child care, housing, and mental health. Of the parents in the analysis sample who were in the EES condition, 24 (96%) parents received all sessions.

2.3 Data Collection Procedure

Infants and their caregivers were assessed in two-hour, blinded research home visits at enrollment in the project (baseline), and two times following the intervention (post-intervention and six-month follow-up). Because the PFR intervention took longer to complete than the EES intervention, the post-intervention assessment was closer to baseline in the EES condition than in the PFR condition, EES: M (SD) = 2.88 (0.93) months; PFR: M (SD) = 4.12 (1.36); t (173) = 7.05, p < .001. Also, months between baseline and the six-month follow-up differed by condition, EES: M (SD) = 8.85 (0.97); PFR: M (SD) = 10.05 (1.59); t (127) = 5.25, p < .001).

2.4 Measures

Measures used in the present study are the same as those examined in the prior report of the larger FFP sample. Information on internal consistency and inter-rater reliability of scales was derived from data on the original sample of 210 for all three research visits.

2.4.1 Parent report of caregiver outcomes

Understanding of toddlers was measured by Raising a Baby (RAB; Kelly, Korfmacher, & Buehlman, 2008), assessing caregiver knowledge of infant and toddler social emotional needs and developmentally appropriate expectations. Items such as “The best way to help a toddler through his tantrum is to ignore him” and “A toddler’s misbehavior is a sign they need help from his/her parent” were rated on a 4-point scale (strongly agree to strongly disagree) (16 items; Alphas =.73 – .77).

Parenting stress associated with the perception of having a difficult child or a dysfunctional parent-child relationship was measured with the Parenting Stress Index Short Form (PSI-SF; Abidin, 1995). The Difficult Child (12 items) and Parent-Child Dysfunction (11 items) subscales had alphas ranging from .87 to .89. Sample items include: “My child smiles at me much less than I expected”; “Sometimes I feel my child doesn’t like me and doesn’t want to be close to me”, and were rated on 5-point scale (strongly agree to strongly disagree).

2.4.2 Observed caregiver outcomes

Parent sensitivity was measured by a modified total score of the Nursing Child Assessment Teaching Scale (NCATS; Barnard, 1994), a videotaped interaction of the caregiver teaching the child to do an activity. Cronbach’s alpha for the 21 item sensitivity scale ranged from .71 to .79. A single coder was trained to reliability by a certified NCATS instructor and passed quarterly reliability checks. Sample items include: “Caregiver laughs or smiles at the child during the teaching interaction”; “Caregiver avoids making critical or negative comments about the child’s task performance”. Items were scored yes or no, and yes scores were summed.

An observational measure of parent support (15 items; alphas = .76 – .84) was the Indicator of Parent-Child Interaction (IPCI; Baggett, Carta, & Horn, 2009). Caregiver support was rated by two coders and averaged across three activities: free play, book reading, and a distraction task. Each activity was rated on five four-point scales (never, rarely, sometimes, or often): acceptance/warmth; descriptive language; follows child’s lead; extends/maintains child’s focus; and stress reducing strategies. Inter-rater reliability was assessed on 34% of coded episodes, and ranged from r = .80 to r = .84.

2.4.3 Parent report of child outcomes

Competence (11 items; Alphas = .69 – .70) and problem behavior (31 items; Alphas =.77 = .79) were based on the Brief Infant Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA; Briggs-Gowan & Carter, 2002). Behaviors in the last month were rated on a 3-point scale (not true/rarely; somewhat true/sometimes; very true/often). At the six-month follow-up only, caregivers completed the Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1½ −5 (CBC; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) to rate child behavior. The children were too young for caregivers to complete the CBC at previous assessments. Descriptions of behavior in the last two months were rated on a 3-point scale (not true; somewhat true/sometimes; very true/often). Raw scores on the internalizing (36 items; Alpha = .80) and externalizing (24 items; Alpha = .90) scales were used.

2.4.4 Observed child outcomes

Data collectors used the Bayley Behavior Rating Scale (BRS; Bayley, 1993) to rate the child’s behavior during administration of the Bayley-III Screening Test (Bayley, 2005) at baseline and the six-month follow-up. Seven items comprise the emotional regulation scale and capture how well the child adapts to challenging stimuli and frustration. Examples include: “degree of negative affect in response to test materials, tester, or parent”, “hypersensitivity to test materials” and “adaptation to test materials change or transitions”; alphas = .87 at both time points. Exploration consists of six items rated for exploratory behavior in the testing situation. Examples include “degree of positive affect” and “animation and energy level in exploration of objects or environment”; alphas = .74 and .76. Two research visitors were trained to rate the BRS scales; inter-rater reliability was assessed on 10% of visits and ranged from r =.67 to r = .70.

Secure base behavior was measured with the Toddler Attachment Sort-45 (TAS45; Kirkland, Bimler, Drawneek, McKim, & Schölmerich, 2004), which was scored immediately after each research home visit. The TAS45 is a 45-item modified version of the Attachment Q-Sort (AQS; Waters, 1987), a gold standard attachment measure which has been extensively validated (van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Riksen-Walraven, 2004). We used a sorting technique that the developers of the TAS45 termed trilemmas, in which the 45 descriptive statements are presented in specific sets of three. The three items in a sample trilemma are: “Child wants to be at the center of mother’s attention”; “Child is very independent”; “Child will go towards mother to give her toys, but does not touch nor look at her”. The observer decides which one of the three statements in the set is most like and which one is least like the child’s behavior during the observation. Each of the 45 statements appears in two trilemmas; there are 30 trilemmas in all. The scoring results in an overall secure base behavior score. Two research visitors were trained to administer the TAS45 by the first author; in 16% of visits the TAS45 was coded by the two raters on-site. Inter-rater reliability was r = .92.

Two coders rated child engagement with parent from the three videotaped IPCI activities (9 items, alphas = .79 – .82) (Baggett et al., 2009). Three 4-point scales, positive feedback to parent, sustained engagement to parent and non-social stimuli, and follows parent’s lead, were rated for each activity. Ratings were combined to create a total score. Reliability was assessed with the IPCI trainer on 34% of coded episodes, and coder-trainer agreement ranged from r = .80 to r = .84.

2.5 Analysis

ANCOVA models were estimated to assess differences by experimental condition in caregiver and child variables at baseline, the immediate post-test, and six-month follow-up time points. Covariates included age of child, whether a child had experienced multiple removals from the birth parent home prior to enrollment, and months between baseline and the given follow-up assessment. Baseline score on the given measure (when available) was also included as a covariate. Estimates of effect size are based on differences in adjusted means divided by the square root of mean square errors of the ANCOVA models. Positive effect sizes indicate that the difference between PFR and EES favored the PFR condition. According to Cohen’s conventions (1992), d = .20 is considered small, d = .50 medium, and d = .80 large.

3. Results

We hypothesized that parents who received PFR would show improvements in self-report of understanding toddler’s behavior, reductions in perceiving the child as difficult and reporting a dysfunctional parent-child relationship; improvements in observed parent sensitivity and parental support; and a reduction of parental report of child behavior problems. We also hypothesized an increase in observed child self-regulation, exploration of novel stimuli, engagement with parent, and secure base behavior among the children in the families who received PFR.

None of the group differences in scores on measures at baseline were statistically significant. Adjusted means (based on results from the ANCOVA models) and standard deviations at the immediate post-test and the six-month follow-up are shown by condition in Table 3. Table 3 also includes results of tests for differences at post-test and six-month follow-up by condition, and the effect size for these differences.

TABLE 3.

Means and Standard Deviations of Post-Intervention and Six-Month Outcomes by Experimental Condition; Test Statistic, Significance Level, and Effect Size for PFR

Post-intervention Six-month follow-up
EES
(n=25)
PFR
(n=18)
EES
(n=25)
PFR
(n=18)
Adj M (SD) Adj M (SD) F p d Adj M (SD) Adj M (SD) F p d
Parent Report Outcomes
    RAB Understanding
Toddlers
51.57 (3.58) 51.22 (4.84) 0.06 .814 −.10 51.18 (5.09) 52.02 (4.95) 0.29 .594 0.22
    PSI Difficult child 8.67 (3.95) 9.36 (4.84) 0.16 .691 −.16 9.95 (4.16) 7.96 (5.79) 1.29 .263 0.44
    PSI Parent-child
dysfunction
3.96 (3.90) 5.70 (4.11) 1.28 .265 −.46 4.47 (4.51) 4.96 (5.32) 0.12 .731 −0.13
Observed Parent Outcomes
    NCATS Parent sensitivity 11.46 (4.52) 12.58 (4.88) 0.55 .464 .30 13.27 (3.92) 14.64 (2.99) 1.37 .249 0.45
    IPCI Parent support 2.04 (0.36) 2.05 (0.34) 0.01 .934 .03 2.05 (0.40) 2.36 (0.40) 5.14 .029 0.87
Parent Report Child Outcomes
    BITSEA Competence 16.77 (2.87) 17.72 (2.74) 0.94 .339 .41 18.05 (2.24) 18.52 (2.82) 0.32 .573 0.22
    BITSEA Problem behavior 9.23 (4.46) 10.29 (6.54) 0.37 .549 −.26 10.59 (5.89) 7.75 (5.21) 2.07 .159 0.57
    CBCL Internalizing 6.92 (5.16) 6.50 (3.61) 0.05 .818 0.09
    CBCL Externalizing 15.54 (7.53) 11.48 (7.65) 2.06 .159 0.55
Observed Child Outcomes
    BRS Emotional regulation 3.92 (0.65) 4.19 (0.75) 1.30 .261 0.44
    BRS Exploration 4.39 (0.58) 4.42(0.47) 0.02 .884 0.06
    TAS45 Secure base
behavior
0.15 (0.06) 0.13 (0.06) 0.59 .446 −.31 0.12 (0.07) 0.14 (0.10) 0.39 .537 0.23
    IPCI Engagement with
parent
2.14 (0.52) 2.01 (0.49) 0.42 .52 −.26 2.32 (0.53) 2.45 (0.38) 1.50 .228 0.29

Notes: Adj M = mean adjusted for ANCOVA model covariates, SD = standard deviation, d = effect size (difference in adjusted mean/square root of mean square error) with a positive effect size indicating the difference favors the PFR condition. ANCOVA models adjust for baseline score, age of child, whether multiple removals, and time between baseline and the given follow-up assessment.

Of the nine immediate post-intervention outcomes examined, there were no significant differences between parents and children in the PFR condition and the EES condition. Four of the five parent outcomes were more problematic for the PFR group, with the largest differences being medium effect sizes (one favoring PFR and one favoring EES) for the PSI parent-child dysfunction scale (d = −.46), and the BITSEA competency scale (d = .41). The two parenting observations had positive effect sizes, with the NCATS parent sensitivity score having a small effect (d = .30).

Of the 13 six-month follow-up outcomes examined, one showed a difference by condition that was statistically significant at the p < .05 level. Parents in the PFR condition were observed to provide more parent support in their interactions with their child than were parents in the EES condition; the effect size was d = .87. The direction of all but one of the differences at six-month follow-up favored the PFR condition.

4. Discussion

The purpose of the study reported here was two-fold: to describe how the elements of PFR align with the identified needs of parents and children who have experienced a child welfare placement and subsequent reunification, and to test PFR compared to a three-session intervention consisting of psychoeducation and referral services. This study is part of a larger randomized trial that included foster parents and kin providers, in addition to birth parents. Strengths of the study include blinded outcome assessments, multiple observational measures coded by trained and reliable raters, and a strong comparison group which received in-home psychoeducation and referral services.

Results indicate that PFR produced positive results in this small sample of reunified birth families at the six-month follow-up, although not evident at the immediate post-test. These results, compared to results reported for the full sample including foster, kin, and birth parents (Spieker et al., 2012a), indicate that the effect sizes at the immediate post-test were more negative for the birth parent sample. In contrast, the positive effect sizes at the six-month follow-up reported here for birth parents are larger than they were for the full sample. These results hint at a longer time required for the intervention to effect change for the birth parent subsample.

PFR aims to help parents interpret their young child’s cues and respond sensitively, particularly around times of child distress. That parents exposed to PFR reported a decrease in problematic behavior as well as an increase in child competence lends support to the ability of the PFR to achieve this central goal. These results may reflect a change in parent perception or actual improvements in child behavior, or both effects could be operating. Regardless, when parents grasp the concept that “behavior has meaning” they are better positioned to reframe challenging behavior as resulting from the child’s unmet social or emotional needs. The response then becomes one of helping the child with difficult feelings rather than “punishing” as defiant behavior. Clearly, this would be a desired modification in parenting response for a child welfare population.

We want to underscore the importance of parental capacity to reframe behavior, or at the least to see the child in a more positive light. Once parent perceptions start to shift, the dyad will become more positively engaged, the relationship will be punctuated with more positive, mutually enjoyable interactions, and the parent will have greater empathy and understanding of the child’s needs. These changes create an iterative shift in the relationship toward a more positive trajectory. We theorize that the skill of reframing the meaning of challenging behavior is not only relevant for parents of toddlers. The importance of parents’ ability to understand and reflect on the underlying meaning of behavior applies across all stages of development.

4.1 Implications

When the child welfare system removes a child from the family home due to abuse or neglect, the goal of child welfare policy is safety, permanency, and child well-being (ACF, 2012). While reunification is the desired goal when feasible, it can be accompanied by conflicting emotions and distinct challenges. Parents and their children typically transition from limited time together in the context of supervised visitation, to becoming a full-time family with greatly increased interaction around all of the nuances of daily life. Parents report this as a time of joy mixed with guilt, anxiety, fear of a failed reunification, and loss, and also anger at the perceived lack of support from the child welfare system (Malet, McSherry, Larkin, Kelly, & Robinson, 2010).

In addition to the emotional aspects of reunification, the reunified parents in our study had many other obligations and service requirements during the time they were participating in PFR. These numerous and rigid compliance tasks meant it often took 16 weeks to complete the 10-week PFR program. The fact that parents had to juggle multiple demands during the intervention phase may account in part for the “sleeper effect” of positive outcomes emerging only six months post-intervention, after a period of practice and consolidation. Being flexible in scheduling and rescheduling home visits was important to support the families’ successful program completions. Beginning PFR before reunification, during visitation times, and then continuing with PFR as a support post-reunification is a model that could be effective. Visitation visits are often a missed opportunity for a therapeutic focus on the parent-child relationship and to scaffold parenting confidence and competence (Haight, Sokolec, Budde, & Poertner, 2001).

Reunifications fail for complex reasons, not all of which are addressed by a parenting program like PFR. Miller, Fisher, Fetrow, and Fordan (2006), however, found that parenting skill was an important positive contributing factor for successful reunifications, along with case management directed toward parental needs. PFR would be a good addition to a menu of services that should be offered by state agencies to strengthen the parent-child relationship. While in this sub-sample we showed improvement in parenting and child outcomes six months post-intervention, our earlier work with the full sample showed that receiving PFR did not reduce the rate of reentry into foster care after two years of follow up data, although PFR did improve permanency outcomes for those in foster care and kin care (Spieker, Oxford, & Fleming, 2014). Future research should identify the modifiable factors that contribute to re-entry for reunified birth families.

4.2 Limitations

There are important limitations to be noted for this study. The primary limitation is the small sample size. With groups of 18 and 25, power to detect statistically significant (p < .05) differences between groups was very limited, with type II error rates under .20 only when effect sizes are large (d > .80). The power to detect a medium effect (d = .5) is only .35. We can only speculate why positive outcomes emerged only at the six-month follow-up. Many of the effects in this pilot study are medium in size; with such low power due to sample size we were unable to demonstrate statistical significance. However, with a larger sample such as that in the parent study, medium effect sizes were statistically significant because we had sufficient power (Spieker et al., 2012a). Finally, the participants in this study may differ from the general population of reunified birth families. Parents consenting to participate in this research might represent a particularly motivated subset of reunified parents.

4.3 Conclusions

The results of this pilot study are promising and a first step in understanding the types of services that will alter the trajectory of children after foster care. Parents receiving the PFR intervention eventually perceived their child in a more positive light and demonstrated improved parenting behavior, as observed by blinded coders. These gains may contribute to longer term improvements in the parent-child relationship and enhanced overall child well-being.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants R01 MH077329 from the National Institute of Mental Health and U54HD083091 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Monica L. Oxford, Family & Child Nursing, University of Washington

Maureen Marcenko, School of Social Work, University of Washington.

Charles B. Fleming, School of Social Work, University of Washington

Mary Jane Lohr, Family & Child Nursing, University of Washington.

Susan J. Spieker, Family & Child Nursing, University of Washington

References

  1. Abidin RA. Parenting Stress Index: Short Form (PSI-SF) Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach T, Rescorla L. Child Behavior Checklist for ages 1½-5. Burlington, VT: ASEBA, University of Vermont; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  3. Administration for Children and Families (ACF) Promoting social and emotional well-being for children and youth receiving child welfare services. (ACYF-CB-IM-12-04) Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration on Children, Youth and Families; 2012. Apr 17, Retrieved from https://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/cb/im1204.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  4. Administration on Children, Youth and Families (ACYF) Office of Data Analysis, Research, and Evaluation. Recent demographic trends in foster care. Washington, DC: 2013. Sep, Retrieved from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/cb/data_brief_foster_care_trends1.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baggett KM, Carta JJ, Horn EM. Indicator of Parent-Child Interaction (IPCI) user’s manual. Lawrence, KS: Special Education, University of Kansas; 2009. Retrieved from http://www.igdi.ku.edu/training/IPCI_training/IPCI_admin_instructions.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(2):195–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barnard KE. What the Teaching Scale measures. In: Sumner GS, Spietz A, editors. NCAST: Caregiver/parent-child interaction teaching manual. Seattle, WA: University of Washington NCAST Publications; 1994. pp. 98–121. [Google Scholar]
  8. Barth RP. Progress in developing responsive parenting programs for Child Welfare-involved infants: Commentary on Spieker, Oxford, Kelly, Nelson, and Fleming (2012) Child Maltreatment. 2012;17(4):287–290. doi: 10.1177/1077559512466586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley-III) screening test. 3rd. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  11. Beckmann KA, Knitzer J, Cooper J, Dicker S. Supporting parents of young children in the child welfare system. New York: The National Center for Children in Poverty, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University; 2010. Feb, Retrieved from http://www.nccp.org/publications/pub_924.html. [Google Scholar]
  12. Briggs-Gowan M, Carter AS. Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) manual, version 2.0. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  13. Carlson BE, Matto H, Smith CA, Eversman M. A pilot study of reunification following drug abuse treatment: Recovering the mother role. Journal of Drug Issues. 2006;36(4):877–902. [Google Scholar]
  14. Child Trends and Zero to Three. Changing the course for infants and toddlers: A survey of child welfare policies and iniatives. Washington, DC: Author; 2013. Sep, Retrieved from http://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/09/changing-the-course-for-infants-and-toddlers-FINAL.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. The impact of child maltreatment and psychopathology on neuroendocrine functioning. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13(4):783–804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112(1):155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. De Bellis MD. The psychobiology of neglect. Child Maltreatment. 2005;10(2):150–172. doi: 10.1177/1077559505275116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Degangi GA, Breinbauer C, Doussard RJ, Porges S, Greenspan S. Prediction of childhood problems at three years in children experiencing disorders of regulation during infancy. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2000;21(3):156–175. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dozier M, Higley E, Albus KE, Nutter A. Intervening with foster infants’ caregivers: Targeting three critical needs. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2002;23(5):541–554. [Google Scholar]
  20. Dozier M, Lindhiem O, Lewis E, Bick J, Bernard K, Peloso E. Effects of a foster parent training program on children’s attachment behaviors: Preliminary evidence from a randomized clinical trial. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal. 2009;26(4):321–332. doi: 10.1007/s10560-009-0165-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dozier M, Zeanah CH, Bernard K. Infants and toddlers in foster care. Child Development Perspectives. 2013;7(3):166–171. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fukkink RG. Video feedback in widescreen: A meta-analysis of family programs. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28(6):904–916. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fuller TL. Child safety at reunification: A case-control study of maltreatment recurrence following return home from substitute care. Children and Youth Services Review. 2005;27(12):1293–1306. [Google Scholar]
  24. Goldsmith DF, Oppenheim D, Wanlass J. Separation and reunification: Using attachment theory and research to inform decisions affecting the placements of children in foster care. Juvenile and Family Court Journal. 2004;55(2):1–14. [Google Scholar]
  25. Haight W, Sokolec J, Budde S, Poertner J. Conducting parent-child visits: Research integration document. Champaign: Children and Family Research Center, School of Social Work: University of Illinois at Urbana; 2001. Mar, [Google Scholar]
  26. Heller SS, Smyke AT, Boris NW. Very young foster children and foster families: Clinical challenges and interventions. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2002;23(5):555–575. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hindley N, Ramchandani PG, Jones DP. Risk factors for recurrence of maltreatment: A systematic review. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2006;91(9):744–752. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.085639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Horwitz SM, Chamberlain P, Landsverk J, Mullican C. Improving the mental health of children in child welfare through the implementation of evidence-based parenting interventions. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research. 2010;37(1–2):27–39. doi: 10.1007/s10488-010-0274-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. James S, Landsverk J, Slymen DJ, Leslie LK. Predictors of outpatient mental health service use—The role of foster care placement change. Mental Health Services Research. 2004;6(3):127–141. doi: 10.1023/b:mhsr.0000036487.39001.51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jonson-Reid M. Foster care and future risk of maltreatment. Children and Youth Services Review. 2003;25(4):271–294. [Google Scholar]
  31. Kelly JF, Korfmacher J, Buehlman K. Ways of being with children. Unpublished manuscript. University of Washington; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kelly J, Sandoval D, Zuckerman TG, Buehlman K. Promoting First Relationships: A program for service providers to help parents and other caregivers nurture young children’s social and emotional development. 2nd. Seattle, WA: NCAST Programs; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  33. Kimberlin SE, Anthony EK, Austin MJ. Re-entering foster care: Trends, evidence, and implications. Children and Youth Services Review. 2009;31(4):471–481. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kirkland J, Bimler D, Drawneek A, McKim M, Schölmerich A. An alternative approach for the analyses and interpretation of attachment sort items. Early Child Development and Care. 2004;174(7–8):701–719. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lewis EE, Dozier M, Ackerman J, Sepulveda-Kozakowski S. The effects of placement instability on adopted children’s inhibitory control abilities and oppositional behavior. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(6):1415–1427. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lieberman A, Van Horn P, Ghosh Ippen C. Toward evidence based treatment: Child-parent psychotherapy with preschoolers exposed to marital violence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44(12):1241–1248. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000181047.59702.58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Malet MF, McSherry D, Larkin E, Kelly G, Robinson C. Young children returning home from care: The birth parents’ perspective. Child & Family Social Work. 2010;10:77–86. [Google Scholar]
  38. Marcenko M, Newby M, Mienko J, Courtney M. Discussion Paper. 5. Vol. 1. Seattle, WA: Partners for Our Children, University of Washington; 2011. Aug, Family reunification in Washington state: Which children go home and how long does it take? Retrieved from http://partnersforourchildren.org/sites/default/files/publications/family_reunification_discussion_brief.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  39. McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: Links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Science. 2010;1186:190–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Miller KA, Fisher PA, Fetrow B, Fordan K. Trouble on the journey home: Reunification failures in foster care. Children and Youth Services Review. 2006;28(3):260–274. [Google Scholar]
  41. Needell B, Barth RP. Infants entering foster care compared to other infants using birth status indicators. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1998;22(12):1179–1187. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(98)00096-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Newton RR, Litrownik AJ, Landsverk JA. Children and youth in foster care: Disentangling the relationship between problem behaviors and number of placements. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2000;24(10):1363–1374. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(00)00189-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Penzerro RM, Lein L. Burning their bridges: Disordered attachment and foster care discharge. Child Welfare: Journal of Policy, Practice, and Program. 1995;74(2):351–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rosenfeld A, Wasserman S, Pilowsky DJ. Psychiatry and children in the child welfare system. Child & Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 1998;7(3):515–536. viii. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rubin DM, Alessandrini EA, Feudtner C, Mandell DS, Localio AR, Hadley T. Placement stability and mental health costs for children in foster care. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5):1336–1341. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.5.1336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rubin DM, O’Reilly AL, Luan X, Localio AR. The impact of placement stability on behavioral well-being for children in foster care. Pediatrics. 2007;119(2):336–344. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sameroff A. Ports of entry and the dynamics of mother-infant interventions. In: Sameroff A, McDonough S, Rosenblum K, editors. Treating parent-infant relationship problems. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. pp. 243–264. [Google Scholar]
  48. Scholte EM, Colton M, Casas F, Drakeford M, Roberts S, Williams M. Perceptions of stigma and user involvement in child welfare services. British Journal of Social Work. 1999;29(3):373–391. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sciamanna J. Reunification of foster children with their families: The first permanency outcome. State Policy Advoccy and Reform Center (SPARC); 2013. Oct, Retrieved from http://www.nrcpfc.org/is/downloads/Reunification_FChildren_w.Fam.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  50. Shaw TV. Reentry into the foster care system after reunification. Children and Youth Services Review. 2006;28(11):1375–1390. [Google Scholar]
  51. Shonkoff J, Garner A Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health; Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care; & Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129(1):e232–e246. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Spieker S, Oxford M, Fleming C. Permanency outcomes for toddlers in child welfare two years after a randomized trial of a parenting intervention. Children and Youth Services Review. 2014;44:201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Spieker S, Oxford M, Kelly J, Nelson E, Fleming C. Promoting First Relationships: Randomized trial of a relationship-based intervention for toddlers in child welfare. Child Maltreatment. 2012a;17(4):271–286. doi: 10.1177/1077559512458176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Spieker S, Oxford M, Kelly J, Nelson E, Fleming C. Response to the Barth commentary. Child Maltreatment. 2012b;17(4):291–294. doi: 10.1177/1077559512467396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Stovall-McClough K, Dozier M. Forming attachments in foster care: Infant attachment behaviors during the first 2 months of placement. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16(2):253–272. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tomalski P, Johnson MH. The effects of early adversity on the adult and developing brain. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2010;23(3):233–238. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283387a8c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Twardosz S, Lutzker JR. Child maltreatment and the developing brain: A review of neuroscience perspectives. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2010;15(1):59–68. [Google Scholar]
  58. van IJzendoorn M, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg M. Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11(2):225–250. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. van IJzendoorn M, Vereijken C, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, Riksen-Walraven J. Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development. 2004;75:1188–1213. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Waters E. Attachment behavior Q-Set (revision 3.0) Stony Brook: State University of New York at Stony Brook, Department of Psychology; 1987. Retrieved from http://www.psychology.sunysb.edu/attachment/measures/content/aqs_items.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  61. Webster D, Barth RP, Needell B. Placement stability for children in out-of-home care: A longitudinal analysis. Child Welfare. 2000;79(5):614–632. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wulczyn F. Infant placements in New York City. In: Barth RP, Berrick JD, Gilbert N, editors. Child welfare research review. New York: Columbia University Press; 1994. pp. 99–111. [Google Scholar]
  63. Wulczyn F. Family reunification. The Future of Children. 2004;14(1):95–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES