Abstract
Tinnitus, the perception of phantom sounds, is thought to arise from increased neural synchrony, which facilitates perceptual binding and creates salient sensory features in the absence of physical stimuli. In the auditory cortex, increased spontaneous cross-unit synchrony and single-unit bursting are de facto physiological correlates of tinnitus. However, it is unknown whether neurons in the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), the putative tinnitus-induction site, exhibit increased synchrony. Using a temporary-threshold shift model and gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle to assess tinnitus, we recorded spontaneous activity from fusiform cells, the principle neurons of the DCN, in normal hearing, tinnitus, and non-tinnitus guinea pigs. Synchrony and bursting, as well as spontaneous firing rate (SFR), correlated with behavioral evidence of tinnitus, and increased synchrony and bursting were associated with SFR elevation. The presence of increased synchrony and bursting in DCN fusiform cells suggests that a neural code for phantom sounds emerges in this brainstem location and likely contributes to the formation of the tinnitus percept.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Tinnitus, a phantom auditory percept, is encoded by pathological changes in the neural synchrony code of perceptual processing. Increased cross-unit synchrony and bursting have been linked to tinnitus in several higher auditory stations but not in fusiform cells of the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), key brainstem neurons in tinnitus generation. Here, we demonstrate increased synchrony and bursting of fusiform cell spontaneous firing, which correlate with frequency-specific behavioral measures of tinnitus. Thus, the neural representation of tinnitus emerges early in auditory processing and likely drives its pathophysiology in higher structures.
Keywords: bursting, dorsal cochlear nucleus, fusiform cells, neural synchrony, spontaneous activity, tinnitus
Introduction
Tinnitus, or auditory phantom perception, arises from alterations in sound encoding in the central auditory pathway (Eggermont, 2015). This pathological state is often, but not always, triggered by auditory insult (König et al., 2006) and manifests as increased resting-state neural activity and synchronization in the human primary auditory (A1) and associated cortices (Elgoyhen et al., 2015). Thus, hyperactivity and synchrony may encode sound representations in tinnitus (Weisz et al., 2007). Because features of an auditory stimulus are extracted by assemblies of neurons firing synchronously, a hypersynchronous neural ensemble could signal the presence of “auditory objects” even in the absence of external stimuli, giving rise to the tinnitus percept.
The substructure of neural synchronization is spontaneous cross-unit synchrony and single-unit bursting (Singer, 1993). In animal models of noise trauma (Noreña and Eggermont, 2003) and tinnitus (Engineer et al., 2011), correlates of increased synchrony and bursting have been confirmed in A1. However, lower stations also exhibit increased synchrony and bursting as correlates of tinnitus: bursting in the medial geniculate body (Kalappa et al., 2014) and synchrony and bursting in the inferior colliculus (IC; Bauer et al., 2008). The presence of increased synchrony and bursting in subcortical nuclei suggests that perceptual binding by means of synchronous neural activity is processed early and diffusely in the auditory pathway.
The dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), a brainstem nucleus that receives direct input from the auditory nerve, is essential for tinnitus induction (Kaltenbach, 2011). Fusiform cells, the principal output neurons of the DCN, exhibit increased spontaneous firing rates (SFRs) after noise trauma and tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2002; Kaltenbach et al., 2004; Dehmel et al., 2012), which are driven by homeostatic and timing-dependent plasticity (Koehler and Shore, 2013; Li et al., 2015). Increased SFR, an established tinnitus correlate, is unperturbed by transection of afferent or efferent input to the DCN (Zacharek et al., 2002; Zhang et al., 2006), whereas bilateral DCN ablation prevents the development of tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2002). However, despite evidence for their essential role in tinnitus induction, the presence of increased synchrony in fusiform cells remains unexplored. As increased synchrony encodes for tinnitus, it is plausible that the neural representation of tinnitus originates in fusiform cells.
Consistent with this hypothesis, we showed that synchrony, bursting, and SFRs of fusiform cells correlate with behavioral evidence of tinnitus in guinea pigs. In tinnitus animals, physically and tonotopically adjacent unit pairs showed increased cross-correlations associated with SFR, whereas synchrony first appeared in a group of unit pairs separated by larger distances. In addition, increased bursting was driven by the increased SFR and accompanied by increases in nonbursting spikes. These findings reveal that degraded perceptual processing occurs in DCN fusiform cells and likely influences tinnitus pathophysiology in higher-order structures.
Materials and Methods
Animals.
All animal procedures were performed in accordance with protocols established by the National Institutes of Health (Publication 80-23) and approved by the University Committee on Use and Care of Animals at the University of Michigan. Female, pigmented guinea pigs (n = 14) were obtained from Elm Hill Labs at 2–3 weeks of age.
Noise exposure and auditory brainstem responses.
Ten guinea pigs were exposed to narrow-band noise to produce a temporary threshold shift (TTS) for tinnitus induction (Dehmel et al., 2012). Four guinea pigs served as sham-exposed (anesthesia-only) controls. Noise exposures and auditory brainstem response (ABR) recordings to assess hearing thresholds were performed in a double-walled soundproof booth. Guinea pigs were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine [40 mg/kg ketamine (Putney); 10 mg/kg xylazine (Lloyd)]. Sound exposures were unilateral via microphone inserts (Brüel and Kjær) with a 7 kHz centered, half-octave noise at 97 dB SPL for 2 h. A second noise/sham exposure was performed after 4 weeks. ABRs (0–90 dB SPL tone bursts; 2 ms cos2 rise/fall times, 10 ms duration, 30 Hz presentation rate, 1024 repetitions in 10 dB steps; Tucker-Davis Technologies) were recorded at 8, 12, and 16 kHz, before, immediately after, and 1 week after each noise exposure.
Behavioral tinnitus assessment.
Gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle (GPIAS) was used to assess tinnitus (Turner et al., 2006). A constant background carrier (band limited at 8–10, 12–14, and 16–18 kHz) was presented at 65 dB SPL. Startle responses elicited by broadband noise pulses (2 ms rise/fall time, 20 ms) at 95 dB SPL were quantified by video tracking (Point Gray Research) of the Preyer reflex (modified from Berger et al., 2013). All animals showed stable startle reflex amplitudes over the duration of the study. Startle reflexes were inhibited by a 50 ms silent gap or 75 dB SPL prepulse (with equivalent 5 ms onset/offset time) 100 ms before the startle pulse. A normalized inhibition ratio (R) was computed by dividing the mean startle amplitude for the gap (or prepulse) trials by those for the startle-only, or no-gap, trials. R values were pooled over the 4 week, twice weekly sessions to establish a behavioral baseline. Four weeks after the second noise exposure, postexposure R values for each animal were calculated. In this study, R values for prepulse inhibition were unchanged before and after noise exposure. An animal was presumed to have tinnitus if the postexposure mean R value for gap inhibition was significantly greater than the baseline value (α = 0.05). The changes in gap R values from preexposure to postexposure were quantified by the standardized tinnitus index [(x − μ)/σ] (Kalappa et al., 2014), where x is the postexposure gap R value, and μ and σ are the mean and SD of the preexposure gap R value. A larger positive index indicates a higher degree of impaired gap detection (“more tinnitus”) after noise exposure.
Single-unit recordings.
Neurophysiological recordings were performed 8 weeks after the first noise exposure. Animals were anesthetized and secured in a stereotaxic frame with hollow ear bars (David Kopf Instruments). The surface of the cerebellum was exposed after a small craniotomy. A two-shank, 16-channel probe (NeuroNexus) was inserted stereotaxically through an intact cerebellum. Recorded voltages were digitized by a PZ2 preamplifier (Tucker-Davis Technologies) and bandpass filtered (0.3–3 kHz) for spike detection, with threshold set at 2.5 SDs above background noise. Broadband noise bursts were used to locate units in the DCN. Receptive fields (0.1–24 kHz; 0–90 dB) were recorded to determine thresholds and best frequencies (BFs). A suitable electrode location in the fusiform cell layer was confirmed by robust responses to BF tones with buildup or pauser-buildup temporal patterns typical of fusiform cells. Spontaneous activity was recorded for 153 s.
Neural synchrony analyses.
Recorded spikes were sorted manually into single units using the Plexon Offline Sorter and imported into MATLAB as timestamps. Spatial synchrony was calculated using paired cross-correlation (Voigt and Young, 1990; Noreña and Eggermont, 2003). Common spikes occurring within ±150 μs across different units were removed. Cross-correlation coefficients [p(τ)] were computed as a function of time lag for each pairwise combination of spike trains (Eq. 1):
where RAB(τ) is the unbiased cross-correlation of spike trains A and B, and NA and NB indicate spike counts in the respective spike trains. E is the expectancy of coincidence for Poisson-distributed data (Eq. 2), defined by the multiplication of NA and NB over the number of bins (n). Bin size was constant at 0.3 ms (Voigt and Young, 1990). A unit pair was considered synchronous when the peak p value was greater than ±4 SDs from the mean p(τ). In the present study, both significant positive and negative cross-correlations were observed, but only positive peak p values were considered in additional analyses.
Bursting was characterized using the Poisson surprise method (Noreña and Eggermont, 2003). The negative log-likelihood (S) measures how unlikely, or “surprising,” a series of spikes is within a Poisson-distributed spike train (Eq. 3):
where r is the mean SFR of the spike train, and T is a given time interval that contains n spikes. S values were computed for every three spikes occurring in interval T(n + 2) − T(n), and the triplet was considered a burst when its S value exceeded 10. When a burst was identified, additional spikes (T(n + 3), T(n + 4)…) were recruited to increase burst duration until the S value decreased. In addition, the first spike (n) in a burst was removed if the resulting new burst returned a higher S value. Each defined burst contains information regarding its structure: S value, on/off time, and number of spikes incorporated.
Statistics.
Statistical tests, including Pearson's linear correlation, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), and ANOVAs were used to determine statistical differences (α = 0.05). Post hoc analyses were performed using the Tukey–Kramer method.
Results
ABR thresholds (Fig. 1A) of noise-exposed animals exhibited 20–40 dB elevations immediately after exposure (Fig. 1B; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F(2,2) = 12.3, p = 0.0006) but recovered to the preexposure baseline within 1 week (post hoc, p > 0.05). A second noise exposure produced similar TTSs. Unexposed, control animals did not show ABR threshold shifts (Fig. 1C), and neither exposed nor control animals showed permanent threshold shifts. Eight weeks after the first exposure, six animals (60%) showed behavioral evidence of tinnitus with significantly higher tinnitus indices than unexposed controls and the other four animals (40%) without behavioral signs of tinnitus (Fig. 1D; two-way ANOVA, F(2,2) = 1.12, p = 0.0025 for tinnitus classification), predominantly at 12 and 16 kHz (post hoc, p < 0.05). There was no correlation between the amount of threshold shift and resulting tinnitus (Fig. 1E: F(2,1) = 2.52, p = 0.13; Fig. 1F: r(32)2 = 0.29, p = 0.10). To test for potential suprathreshold hearing impairment as a result of TTSs (Kujawa and Liberman, 2009), we measured ABR wave I amplitudes for suprathreshold intensities (Fig. 1A) in tinnitus and non-tinnitus animals. There was no significant wave I decrement after recovery from TTS in either group (Fig. 1G; three-way ANOVA, F(116,1,1) = 0.12, p = 0.73 for exposure time point, F(116,1,1) = 2.89, p = 0.12 for tinnitus classification). There was also no correlation between wave I amplitude (at 80 dB) and tinnitus index (Fig. 1H: F(2,2) = 0.02, p = 0.70; Fig. 1I: r(25)2 = 0.13, p = 0.52).
Physiological differences between tinnitus and non-tinnitus animals were apparent in fusiform cell spontaneous activity. Synchrony, quantified by the peak cross-correlation coefficient between fusiform cell pairs (Fig. 2A, inset), was elevated in tinnitus animals compared with the unexposed controls and non-tinnitus animals (Fig. 2A; two-way ANOVA, F(4,2) = 3.5, p = 0.03 for tinnitus classification) and was highest in tonotopic regions ∼1 octave above the frequency of noise-exposure peak intensity (post hoc, p < 0.05). Synchrony correlated with tinnitus behavior at corresponding 8, 12, and 16 kHz bins (r(374)2 = 0.15, p = 0.0048; Fig. 2B).
In addition to increased peak cross-correlation coefficients, both the Euclidean distance (physical distance between recording sites) and the BF difference between units (tonotopic distance) decreased in tinnitus animals (Fig. 2C: r(443)2 = −0.10, p = 0.042; Fig. 2D: r(452)2 = −0.09, p = 0.044). Plotting distance measurements to peak cross-correlation coefficients (Fig. 2E) revealed two patterns of increased synchrony in tinnitus animals: unit pairs with short physical and tonotopic distances showed increased cross-correlation strengths [peak cross-correlation coefficient; Fig. 2E, (a)], governing the anti-correlation with tinnitus index, whereas a distinct population of unit pairs with high tonotopic and physical distances (unsynchronized in normal and no-tinnitus animals) became synchronized in tinnitus animals [Fig. 2E, (b)].
Spontaneous bursting also increased in tinnitus animals (Fig. 3A; F(6,2) = 16.9, p = 6.2 × 10−8 for tinnitus classification; F(6,2) = 3.3, p = 9.3 × 10−5 for interaction). Significant increases in burst rate were observed in frequency bins (12–16 kHz) with high tinnitus indices (Fig. 1C). Interestingly, non-tinnitus units showed significantly lower burst rates than both the unexposed controls and tinnitus animals in this region (post hoc, p < 0.05). Burst rate and percentage bursting time correlated positively with tinnitus behavior (Fig. 3B: r(710)2 = 0.22, p = 6.3 × 10−9; Fig. 3C: r(797)2 = 0.14, p = 4.9 × 10−5), whereas average burst duration correlated negatively with tinnitus behavior (Fig. 3D; r(414)2 = −0.25, p = 2.3 × 10−7). Decreased burst duration combined with increased burst rate (Fig. 3E) suggests that the burst pattern is reorganized in tinnitus.
Because synchrony and bursting both correlate with tinnitus behavior, they may simply reflect the SFR elevation associated with tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2002; Kaltenbach et al., 2004; Dehmel et al., 2012). As expected, increased SFR was observed in tinnitus above the peak exposure frequency (F(7,2) = 20.0, p = 3.2 × 10−9 for tinnitus classification; F(7,2) = 2.1, p = 0.0088 for tinnitus class × frequency interaction; Fig. 4A) and correlated with tinnitus behavior (Fig. 4B; r(736)2 = 0.21, p = 1.7 × 10−8). However, synchrony did not correlate directly with SFR [Fig. 4C, (a); r(444)2 = 0.12, p = 0.07] because of the influence of unit pairs with increased distances but low cross-correlation strengths [Figs. 2E, (b), 4C, filled). When only short-distance unit pairs (<200 μm) were analyzed, a significant correlation with SFR was observed [Fig. 4C, (b), open; r(112)2 = 0.21, p = 0.026]. Similarly, burst rate showed a linear relationship with SFR (Fig. 4D; r(948)2 = 0.87, P → 0), although the linear slope for tinnitus units was significantly shallower (ANCOVA, F(2,898) = 119.3, p = 1.1 × 10−46), reflecting the altered burst pattern (Fig. 3E). Thus, although synchrony and bursting are linked to SFR, they contribute additional tinnitus phenotypes of lengthened synchrony distance and decreased burst duration.
Discussion
This study reveals that increased synchrony and bursting in fusiform cells correlate with behavioral evidence of tinnitus. The physiological correlates appear in tonotopic regions of the DCN that overlap the animal's tinnitus spectra, suggesting that the phantom percept is encoded in fusiform cell spontaneous activity. Although most studies focus on cortical synchrony, in which correlations between single-unit and EEG recordings are accessible (Singer, 1993), there is evidence from the somatosensory system that perceptual binding involves diffuse subcortical regions (Nicolelis et al., 1995). Thus, increased synchrony in fusiform cells likely also contributes to coding of the tinnitus percept. The present findings emphasize the role of DCN fusiform cells in tinnitus induction and suggest a bottom-up process of information transfer. SFR elevation in fusiform cells after noise trauma may influence their output targets in the IC (Manzoor et al., 2013) or thalamus (Malmierca et al., 2002; Kalappa et al., 2014). Likewise, neural synchrony may propagate across brain regions (Kumar et al., 2010) to institute increased synchrony and bursting in the IC (Bauer et al., 2008).
Neural synchrony and tinnitus induction in fusiform cells
Several mechanisms may regulate DCN synchrony. Fusiform cells receive granule cell–parallel fiber synapses on their apical dendrites. The noncochlear, excitatory projections to granule cells undergo homeostatic upregulation (Zeng et al., 2012) and contribute to long-term enhancement of fusiform cell excitability in tinnitus (Dehmel et al., 2012; Koehler and Shore, 2013). In the cerebellum, a structural analog of the DCN, principal neurons are synchronized by common granule cell inputs, an effect that decays as pairwise distance increases (highest synchrony < 300 μm; Heck et al., 2007). Consistent with the present study, in low-distance pairs, neural synchrony was dependent on firing rate (de la Rocha et al., 2007). Similarly, increased fusiform cell synchrony is likely driven by increased parallel fiber excitation [Fig. 4C, (b)]. However, increased parallel fiber activity can also potentiate a class of inhibitory interneurons, Golgi cells, to entrain additional parallel fibers into synchronized firing (Vos et al., 1999; Yaeger and Trussell, 2015). Consequently, this would widen the spatial distribution of a synchronized fusiform-cell network [Fig. 2E, (b)], because a single parallel fiber can traverse hundreds of microns (Mugnaini et al., 1980) and is unlikely to synapse onto nearby fusiform cells (Roberts and Trussell, 2010). In addition, parallel fiber–fusiform cell synapses exhibit spike timing-dependent plasticity (STDP; Tzounopoulos et al., 2004), which can also regulate neural synchrony (Nowotny et al., 2003). The recent report of inverted STDP in fusiform cells after noise-induced tinnitus (Koehler and Shore, 2013) suggests that increased synchrony can emerge from altered circuit plasticity.
Along with increased excitation and altered plasticity, downregulation of glycinergic synapses on fusiform cells (Wang et al., 2009), as well as intrinsic changes in membrane kinetics (Pilati et al., 2012; Li et al., 2015), drive fusiform cells to higher excitability. Increased bursting and altered burst duration have been associated with reduced high-voltage-activated potassium currents in fusiform cells (Pilati et al., 2012) and cerebellar principal neurons (McKay and Turner, 2004). Together, these reports suggest that tinnitus induction involves multiple processes: increased excitation (Dehmel et al., 2012), reduced inhibition (Wang et al., 2009), altered synaptic plasticity (Koehler and Shore, 2013), and intrinsic excitability (Li et al., 2015).
Tinnitus resilience: the non-tinnitus animals
Tinnitus does not always occur after auditory insult (König et al., 2006). In animal models, only 50–70% of noise-exposed animals develop behavioral evidence of tinnitus (Koehler and Shore, 2013; Kalappa et al., 2014; Li et al., 2015). Tinnitus and non-tinnitus animals receive the same amount of measurable auditory nerve damage, reflected by equivalent threshold shifts (Fig. 1E,F) and suprathreshold wave I amplitudes (Fig. 1H,I). Although it is unknown whether cellular differences exist in the cochlea (Kujawa and Liberman, 2009), physiological signatures of tinnitus and non-tinnitus are distinctive in fusiform cells: non-tinnitus animals exhibit significantly lower burst rates and SFRs compared with unexposed controls (Fig. 3A). This is consistent with the findings of Li et al. (2015) of more hyperpolarized fusiform cell resting membrane potentials in non-tinnitus compared with normal-hearing animals, thereby lowering spike incidence.
Few studies have focused on neural changes in non-tinnitus animals, but their opposing plasticity (lower burst rate and SFR) than both tinnitus and control animals suggests a mechanism of tinnitus resilience. Because non-tinnitus animals exhibit resilient phenotypes, there is an critical need to distinguish tinnitus from non-tinnitus animals. Many studies use noise exposure as a proxy for tinnitus, but, as demonstrated here, not all noise-exposed animals develop tinnitus, and those that do not develop tinnitus exhibit disparate physiological responses (Dehmel et al., 2012; Koehler and Shore, 2013; Li et al., 2015). Interpreting results from noise exposure studies that omit behavioral assessments may lead to erroneous conclusions.
Among various behavioral assessment techniques, the commonly used GPIAS provides a major advantage in its tinnitus spectrum estimation (Turner et al., 2006). However, GPIAS interpretation requires caution (Galazyuk and Hébert, 2015), because the fundamental assumption of how tinnitus affects gap and startle processing is currently under scrutiny (Fournier and Hébert, 2013). In the present study, accuracy of our behavioral outcomes was ensured by eliminating potential confounds: using a unilateral TTS model and performing prepulse inhibition to control for hearing impairment and ensuring stable startle reflexes to control for hyperacusis (Salloum et al., 2014). Moreover, physiological correlates including increased SFR (Brozoski et al., 2002; Kaltenbach et al., 2004; Dehmel et al., 2012), and as shown here, synchrony and bursting, can aid in ensuring consistency of GPIAS assessment across studies.
Footnotes
This study was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01-DC004825 (S.E.S.) and T32-DC00011 (C.W.) and the Wallace H. Coulter Translational Research Partnership. We thank James Wiler and Chris Ellinger for technical assistance and Michael Roberts, Sandy Bledsoe, and Amarins Heeringa for helpful comments.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Bauer CA, Turner JG, Caspary DM, Myers KS, Brozoski TJ. Tinnitus and inferior colliculus activity in chinchillas related to three distinct patterns of cochlear trauma. J Neurosci Res. 2008;86:2564–2578. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berger JI, Coomber B, Shackleton TM, Palmer AR, Wallace MN. A novel behavioural approach to detecting tinnitus in the guinea pig. J Neurosci Methods. 2013;213:188–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brozoski TJ, Bauer CA, Caspary DM. Elevated fusiform cell activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of chinchillas with psychophysical evidence of tinnitus. J Neurosci. 2002;22:2383–2390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02383.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Rocha J, Doiron B, Shea-Brown E, Josić K, Reyes A. Correlation between neural spike trains increases with firing rate. Nature. 2007;448:802–806. doi: 10.1038/nature06028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehmel S, Pradhan S, Koehler S, Bledsoe S, Shore S. Noise overexposure alters long-term somatosensory-auditory processing in the dorsal cochlear nucleus—possible basis for tinnitus-related hyperactivity? J Neurosci. 2012;32:1660–1671. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4608-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eggermont JJ. Animal models of spontaneous activity in the healthy and impaired auditory system. Front Neural Circuits. 2015;9:19. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgoyhen AB, Langguth B, De Ridder D, Vanneste S. Tinnitus: perspectives from human neuroimaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:632–642. doi: 10.1038/nrn4003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engineer ND, Riley JR, Seale JD, Vrana WA, Shetake JA, Sudanagunta SP, Borland MS, Kilgard MP. Reversing pathological neural activity using targeted plasticity. Nature. 2011;470:101–104. doi: 10.1038/nature09656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fournier P, Hébert S. Gap detection deficits in humans with tinnitus as assessed with the acoustic startle paradigm: does tinnitus fill in the gap? Hear Res. 2013;295:16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galazyuk A, Hébert S. Gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex (GPIAS) for tinnitus assessment: current status and future directions. Front Neurol. 2015;6:88. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heck DH, Thach WT, Keating JG. On-beam synchrony in the cerebellum as the mechanism for the timing and coordination of movement. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:7658–7663. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609966104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalappa BI, Brozoski TJ, Turner JG, Caspary DM. Single unit hyperactivity and bursting in the auditory thalamus of awake rats directly correlates with behavioural evidence of tinnitus. J Physiol. 2014;592:5065–5078. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.278572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaltenbach JA. Tinnitus: models and mechanisms. Hear Res. 2011;276:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaltenbach JA, Zacharek MA, Zhang J, Frederick S. Activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of hamsters previously tested for tinnitus following intense tone exposure. Neurosci Lett. 2004;355:121–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koehler SD, Shore SE. Stimulus timing-dependent plasticity in dorsal cochlear nucleus is altered in tinnitus. J Neurosci. 2013;33:19647–19656. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2788-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- König O, Schaette R, Kempter R, Gross M. Course of hearing loss and occurrence of tinnitus. Hear Res. 2006;221:59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujawa SG, Liberman MC. Adding insult to injury: cochlear nerve degeneration after “temporary” noise-induced hearing loss. J Neurosci. 2009;29:14077–14085. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2845-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar A, Rotter S, Aertsen A. Spiking activity propagation in neuronal networks: reconciling different perspectives on neural coding. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:615–627. doi: 10.1038/nrn2886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S, Kalappa BI, Tzounopoulos T. Noise-induced plasticity of KCNQ2/3 and HCN channels underlies vulnerability and resilience to tinnitus. Elife. 2015;4 doi: 10.7554/eLife.07242. 07242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malmierca MS, Merchán MA, Henkel CK, Oliver DL. Direct projections from cochlear nuclear complex to auditory thalamus in the rat. J Neurosci. 2002;22:10891–10897. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10891.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manzoor NF, Gao Y, Licari F, Kaltenbach JA. Comparison and contrast of noise-induced hyperactivity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus and inferior colliculus. Hear Res. 2013;295:114–123. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKay BE, Turner RW. Kv3 K+ channels enable burst output in rat cerebellar Purkinje cells. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:729–739. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03539.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mugnaini E, Warr WB, Osen KK. Distribution and light microscopic features of granule cells in the cochlear nuclei of cat, rat, and mouse. J Comp Neurol. 1980;191:581–606. doi: 10.1002/cne.901910406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA, Baccala LA, Lin RC, Chapin JK. Sensorimotor encoding by synchronous neural ensemble activity at multiple levels of the somatosensory system. Science. 1995;268:1353–1358. doi: 10.1126/science.7761855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noreña AJ, Eggermont JJ. Changes in spontaneous neural activity immediately after an acoustic trauma: implications for neural correlates of tinnitus. Hear Res. 2003;183:137–153. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00225-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nowotny T, Zhigulin VP, Selverston AI, Abarbanel HD, Rabinovich MI. Enhancement of synchronization in a hybrid neural circuit by spike-timing dependent plasticity. J Neurosci. 2003;23:9776–9785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-30-09776.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilati N, Large C, Forsythe ID, Hamann M. Acoustic over-exposure triggers burst firing in dorsal cochlear nucleus fusiform cells. Hear Res. 2012;283:98–106. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts MT, Trussell LO. Molecular layer inhibitory interneurons provide feedforward and lateral inhibition in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:2462–2473. doi: 10.1152/jn.00312.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salloum RH, Yurosko C, Santiago L, Sandridge SA, Kaltenbach JA. Induction of enhanced acoustic startle response by noise exposure: dependence on exposure conditions and testing parameters and possible relevance to hyperacusis. PLoS One. 2014;9:e111747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer W. Synchronization of cortical activity and its putative role in information processing and learning. Annu Rev Physiol. 1993;55:349–374. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.55.030193.002025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner JG, Brozoski TJ, Bauer CA, Parrish JL, Myers K, Hughes LF, Caspary DM. Gap detection deficits in rats with tinnitus: a potential novel screening tool. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:188–195. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzounopoulos T, Kim Y, Oertel D, Trussell LO. Cell-specific, spike timing-dependent plasticities in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:719–725. doi: 10.1038/nn1272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voigt HF, Young ED. Cross-correlation analysis of inhibitory interactions in dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 1990;64:1590–1610. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.5.1590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vos BP, Maex R, Volny-Luraghi A, De Schutter E. Parallel fibers synchronize spontaneous activity in cerebellar Golgi cells. J Neurosci. 1999;19 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-j0003.1999. RC6(1-5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H, Brozoski TJ, Turner JG, Ling L, Parrish JL, Hughes LF, Caspary DM. Plasticity at glycinergic synapses in dorsal cochlear nucleus of rats with behavioral evidence of tinnitus. Neuroscience. 2009;164:747–759. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisz N, Müller S, Schlee W, Dohrmann K, Hartmann T, Elbert T. The neural code of auditory phantom perception. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1479–1484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaeger DB, Trussell LO. Single granule cells excite Golgi cells and evoke feedback inhibition in the cochlear nucleus. J Neurosci. 2015;35:4741–4750. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3665-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zacharek MA, Kaltenbach JA, Mathog TA, Zhang J. Effects of cochlear ablation on noise induced hyperactivity in the hamster dorsal cochlear nucleus: implications for the origin of noise induced tinnitus. Hear Res. 2002;172:137–143. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00575-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeng C, Yang Z, Shreve L, Bledsoe S, Shore S. Somatosensory projections to cochlear nucleus are upregulated after unilateral deafness. J Neurosci. 2012;32:15791–15801. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2598-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang JS, Kaltenbach JA, Godfrey DA, Wang J. Origin of hyperactivity in the hamster dorsal cochlear nucleus following intense sound exposure. J Neurosci Res. 2006;84:819–831. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]