Abstract
Women who start college in one of the natural or physical sciences leave in greater proportions than their male peers. The reasons for this difference are complex, and one possible contributing factor is the social environment women experience in the classroom. Using social network analysis, we explore how gender influences the confidence that college-level biology students have in each other’s mastery of biology. Results reveal that males are more likely than females to be named by peers as being knowledgeable about the course content. This effect increases as the term progresses, and persists even after controlling for class performance and outspokenness. The bias in nominations is specifically due to males over-nominating their male peers relative to their performance. The over-nomination of male peers is commensurate with an overestimation of male grades by 0.57 points on a 4 point grade scale, indicating a strong male bias among males when assessing their classmates. Females, in contrast, nominated equitably based on student performance rather than gender, suggesting they lacked gender biases in filling out these surveys. These trends persist across eleven surveys taken in three different iterations of the same Biology course. In every class, the most renowned students are always male. This favoring of males by peers could influence student self-confidence, and thus persistence in this STEM discipline.
Introduction
Male faculty members outnumber female faculty members in every science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM) discipline [1]. The attrition of female STEM students from their disciplines can be seen in early stages of the progression to STEM careers including the transition into college and graduate school [2]. The experiences of women in STEM that may lead to this attrition can be subtle. It is generally no longer considered a matter of outright discrimination, but rather the accumulation of smaller experiences that determine whether a female student identifies with and persists in a scientific field [3–6]. One factor linked to persistence in STEM fields is self-confidence. This factor is heavily influenced by social interactions, particularly for women in historically male-dominated fields [7–9]. For example, a student whose abilities are endorsed by an influential person may experience increased performance and confidence; conversely, a student not receiving this affirmation experiences a decrease in performance and confidence [10,11]. While formative experiences like these can occur throughout one’s life, certain periods are more influential than others. Individuals are particularly attuned to cues confirming or discrediting their ability to succeed in a field during major transition periods [8,12]. Student experiences at one of those key transition points, introductory STEM courses, thus seem likely to influence current and future decisions to persist in STEM disciplines.
STEM faculty members provide some of the first professional feedback and interactions that students receive in their disciplines. Unfortunately, both male and female faculty members behave in ways that subtly favor males in STEM disciplines: (a) they are more likely to spend time mentoring males [13], (b) they are more likely to respond to emails from males [14], and (c) they are more likely to call on males in class [15]. These subtle yet consistent biases appear to cause at least some female STEM students to experience a lower sense of belonging or confidence in their discipline, resulting in an increased tendency to leave science [16].
In addition to interactions with faculty members, interactions with other students could impact a student’s sense of belonging and confidence in her discipline. In contrast to the work on gender biases among faculty, only limited research has been performed on the disposition of current college-age students (the “millennial” generation) towards women in STEM and how this disposition may impact their female peers (but see [17,18]). Such research would not only help us to measure one force that may be acting to decrease undergraduate females’ sense of belonging in STEM fields; it would also help us predict whether we can expect these implicit biases to persist in future STEM faculty.
In this paper we focus on the formative experience of nascent STEM professionals during an introductory college science course, a key transition period for the development of a STEM identity [19]. We explore this question in a biology classroom. We chose this field because females and males enroll equally in this discipline at the undergraduate level [20] and thus should represent a conservative case of the biases women in introductory STEM courses experience.
We explore the impact of gender on how students perceive their peers, as well as how students are perceived by their peers. It is important to note that the gender data used in this study come from the school registrar, and are thus defined by information given during student enrollment. The registrar constrains choice for gender identification to ‘male’ or ‘female’ choices. Given these complications, we choose to refer to student genders, but recognize that in some cases the data may not accurately reflect the true gender identity of each student.
To investigate how gender impacts peer perception, undergraduate students were asked to anonymously list class peers who they felt were “strong in their understanding of classroom material” at multiple time points throughout three iterations of a large introductory biology class. We employ longitudinal social network analyses of these data to (1) describe the distribution of nominations received between males and females, and (2) identify the factors that predict who a student will nominate as having mastered the content in their field. Finally, (3) we examine the characteristics of students receiving the most nominations in each class (to whom we refer to as “celebrities”). We focus on these students given our assumption that their ability to draw widespread acknowledgment of their excellence makes them among the most likely in the class to continue in the field beyond the undergraduate level.
Materials and Methods
Ethics statement
We obtained human subjects approval from the University of Washington Institutional Review Board (#44438). Because students were not asked to do anything outside of the normal class curriculum, an altered consent process was approved for use in this study. Subjects were informed that a research study was taking place and that their data would be analyzed as part of this study. Students were informed that they could opt out of the study at any time by filling out a form in a centralized office.
Classroom and classroom data
Data come from three different iterations of the same large introductory undergraduate biology class (n = 196, 759, and 760, corresponding to class A, B, and C, respectively) at a large American university that engages in very high research activity (an R1 university). The class of interest is the second in a series of three introductory Biology classes, where the first class in the series served as a pre-requisite. Because this was the second class, many students enter the class already knowing many of their peers. Student demographic information, including gender, was collected from the Office of the University Registrar and course grades from the Department of Biology.
All three iterations of this course included a lab section with a maximum capacity of 24 students that met once a week for several hours. Classes A, B, and C contained 9, 33, and 33 lab sections, respectively. The gender distribution within lab sections is approximately normal and mirrors that of the overall class (Mean = 57.4% female, SD = 0.11). The lecture portion of the course met for 50 minutes a day four days out of the week, and employed active learning techniques in all three iterations of the course. In all three cases, lectures were split into two sections with approximately 100 students in each for class A, and approximately 375 in each for Classes B and C; the instructor stayed consistent between lectures each class day to assure minimal differences between the two sections. Classes A and B were both taught by a male instructor, while Class C had three total instructors: two male instructors teaching 75% of class days and one female instructor teaching 25% of class days. All three iterations of the class included three exams spaced throughout the quarter, and a non-cumulative final exam that took place one week after the end of the quarter. Grades were not publically posted in any of the three classes.
A measurement of student outspokenness was collected by polling the course instructor of record immediately after the end of each course, and thus represents active participation as perceived by the instructor who was blind to the hypotheses being tested. Thus, a student who frequently offers an incorrect answer in class is considered equally outspoken as students who frequently offer the correct answers. Because measurements come from instructors, the list may be subject to each instructor’s own implicit biases.
All three classes consisted primarily of white and Asian students (40.5% and 29.9% of entire population across the three classes, respectively). Student ethnicity is not included in these analyses for two reasons. First, the diversity in each classroom is such that statistical power to understand the perception of minority students is lacking. Second, this issue is substantial enough to warrant its own separate analysis.
Student networks
All network surveys were administered via a confidential online survey. For Class A, students were given a class roster after the first and second exams and were asked to mark students they felt were particularly strong with class material. In Class B students were asked at the beginning of the class to list students by name who they felt would do particularly well in the course. After the first, second, and third exams, they were asked to list students they felt were particularly strong with class material. The same collection method was performed in Class C as Class B, but in addition students were surveyed again before the final exam of the course. Surveys in Class C distinguished between students who responded and didn’t know anyone they felt were knowledgeable and students who didn’t list anybody due to a non-response to the survey. Thus, Class C offers the most accurate means to calculate response rates. An average of 81.4% (SD = 0.02) of students responded across the five surveys in this class, with 82.8% of female students responding (SD = 0.02) and 79.9% of males responding (SD = 0.01). We have no reason to believe that Classes A or B differed in response rates, or that response rates were skewed by gender in any manner.
Analysis of nominations
To assess the hypotheses about nomination structure, we used exponential-family random graph models (ERGMs). This approach can be thought of as a kind of generalization of logistic regression to social networks–with the log-odds of a tie (here, a nomination) between two actors being dependent on a set of predictors of interest [21]. Those predictors may include characteristics of either or both nodes (e.g. their gender, class performance or outspokenness). However, it can also include structural factors involving the other ties in the network–e.g. the tendency for ties in a directed network to be mutual, or to form a triangle. When such structural terms are present, ties become conditionally dependent and estimation becomes more difficult, with Markov chain Monte Carlo-based methods the current state of the art for estimation [22]. Nevertheless, the coefficients may still be interpreted in terms of their contribution to the conditional log-odds of a tie, given all of the other ties in the network.
We specify two models, both of the general form:
where Yij represents the value of the tie from i to j, which equals 1 if i nominates j and 0 if they did not (we discuss missing data for these values in the SI). The quantity represents the complement of yij, i.e. the state of all of the ties in the network other than yij. The δ vector represents the amount by which the model statistics change when yij is toggled from 0 to 1, and the θ vector represents the coefficients on these statistics.
The first model contains seven model statistics (δ1 through δ7) and the second model contains nine (δ1 through δ9):
δ 1 = 1 for all dyads [the main effect or intercept];
δ 2 = 1 if j nominates i, and 0 otherwise [mutuality];
δ 3 = 1 if i is female and 0 otherwise [female nominator];
δ 4 = 1 if both i and j are female and 0 otherwise [female-female bias];
δ 5 = 1 if both i and j are male and 0 otherwise [male-male bias];
δ 6 = 1 if i and j are in the same lab section [lab homophily];
δ 7 = -1 if j has no nominations other than that from i [0-indegree];
δ 8 = j’s final grade in the class [grade of nominee];
δ 9 = 1 if j is outspoken, and 0 otherwise [outspokenness of nominee];
We use the R package network to process and store the data, and the R package ergm to estimate the θ coefficients for our two models for each survey wave [22,23]. The terms involving gender, grade, or outspokenness represent our core theoretical measures. We include mutuality since it is a basic phenomenon in directed networks (those where the relationship from i to j does not necessarily equal that from j to i), and we include lab homophily given that labs are a major structural element of the course. We include a unique propensity for individuals to have no nominations (called 0-indegree in network terminology) since this dramatically improved the fit of the model to the observed in-degree distribution, which is a condition for the statistical inference we later conduct (see S1 appendix for more information). Moreover, it is reasonable to expect that measures of renown such as that here would have more variation than expected by chance–that is, with more students who have either no or many nominations than otherwise expected. The δ on this term is negative given the unique condition that adding a tie reduces the statistic of interest (nodes without ties).
Results
Classroom data and student outspokenness
A summary of student data stratified by gender can be found in Table 1. All three classes consisted of a numerical female majority; classes A, B, and C were 56%, 55.4%, and 58.4% female, respectively. Males averaged higher course grades than females in all three classes; the differences in grade in class C, but not A and B, is significant (p = 0.0171; unpaired t-test).
Table 1. Student demographics from all three classes.
Class A | Class B | Class C | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Female | Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | |
Total students | 110 (56%) | 86 (44%) | 431 (55.4%) | 328 (44.6%) | 444 (58.4%) | 316 (41.6%) |
Mean class grade (out of 4.0) | 2.68 (1.01) | 2.93 (0.82) | 2.74 (0.83) | 2.86 (0.84) | 2.75 (0.82) | 2.89 (0.76) |
Number of students listed as outspoken | 16 (14.5%) | 16 (16.3%) | 64 (14.8%) | 52 (15.8%) | 98 (22.1%) | 95 (30.1%) |
Mean number nominations at S1 | - | - | 1.14 (1.50) | 1.20 (1.73) | 1.19 (1.52) | 1.13 (1.52) |
Mean number nominations at S2 | 1.05 (1.39) | 1.60 (2.81) | 0.98 (1.45) | 1.16 (2.25) | 1.01 (1.41) | 1.08 (1.58) |
Mean number nominations at S3 | 1.06 (1.55) | 1.69 (2.95) | 1.22 (1.55) | 1.48 (2.44) | 1.02 (1.43) | 1.17 (1.78) |
Mean number nominations at S4 | - | - | 1.12 (1.64) | 1.55 (3.63) | 1.23 (1.60) | 1.44 (1.92) |
Mean number nominations at S5 | - | - | - | - | 1.21 (1.55) | 1.36 (1.87) |
Proportionately, more males than females were listed as outspoken (p = 0.0258; Mantel-Haenszel test). While instructor bias causing this gender difference in outspoken status is something we cannot check, it is worth noting that any male bias in the assignment of outspoken status would make our estimates of male bias in peer perception more conservative than they actually are.
Males are over-nominated by peers as mastering biology
Across the 11 peer perception surveys, students received an average of 1.20 nominations with a standard deviation of 1.85; males averaged 1.31 nominations with a standard deviation of 2.23, while females averaged 1.12 nominations with a standard deviation of 1.51. Males consistently received more nominations than females in every survey, with the first survey in Class C as the only exception.
In all three classes, the number of nominations given to males increased throughout the course. This pattern was particularly strong in Classes B and C, where data were collected across a longer time span. No consistent longitudinal trend for females is visible in any of the three classes. Combined, these patterns result in a growing gender gap in the number of nominations received between males and females when comparing data from surveys early in the class to those taken later in the class (Fig 1; S1, S2 and S3 Figs).
To determine the significance of these results, we use Exponential Random Graph Models (ERGM). Our base model does not include grade or outspokenness in order to give an absolute sense of the gender differences in receiving nominations (S1 Table). In all 11 surveys, males show a significant bias toward nominating other males, with an absolute value greater than seen among females. In the last survey from Class A, females also show a bias towards nominating males, but show no significant bias either way in the remaining 10 surveys.
Class performance and outspokenness predict classroom wide recognition, but males still nominate more males after controlling for these
Over-representation of males in received nominations could be explained either by the higher frequency of outspokenness in males, or the higher average grades achieved by males compared to females, as both of these measures may indicate that, on average, males indeed know the material better or at least make their knowledge more visible to their peers. To test these explanations, we expanded our ERGM to include the class grade and outspokenness of the nominee as mediating factors (Table 2).
Table 2. Coefficients from exponential random graph models from all 11 networks across all three courses, demonstrating that female bias towards nominating other females is not significant in any survey, while male bias towards nominating males is significant in all 11 surveys.
Coefficient name | Course A | Course B | Course C | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S2 | S3 | S1 | S2 | S3 | S4 | S1 | S2 | S3 | S4 | S5 | |
Intercept | -7.42 (-8.18, -6.67) | -7.14 (-7.89, -6.40) | -8.27 (-8.66, -7.88) | -9.25 (-9.71, -8.78) | -8.86 (-9.26, -8.47) | -9.41 (-9.88, -8.93) | -8.11 (-8.51, -7.70) | -8.27 (-8.72, -7.82) | -8.26 (-8.68, -7.84) | -8.05 (-8.43, -7.66) | -8.18 (-8.56, -7.79) |
Mutuality | 3.66 (3.16, 4.15) | 4.43 (3.94, 4.92) | 5.60 (5.33, 5.86) | 5.52 (5.22, 5.81) | 5.70 (5.45, 5.95) | 5.50 (5.21, 5.78) | 5.81 (5.50, 6.12) | 5.39 (5.09, 5.70) | 5.47 (5.22, 5.72) | 5.26 (4.98, 5.53) | 5.46 (5.19, 5.72) |
Grade of nominee | 0.38 (0.19, 0.58) | 0.33 (0.15, 0.52) | 0.48 (0.38, 0.58) | 0.74 (0.62, 0.86) | 0.59 (0.49, 0.69) | 0.77 (0.65, 0.89) | 0.49 (0.39, 0.60) | 0.53 (0.42, 0.64) | 0.53 (0.42, 0.64) | 0.51 (0.40, 0.62) | 0.51 (0.41, 0.61) |
Outspokenness of nominee | 0.87 (0.59, 1.15) | 0.67 (0.42, 0.92) | 0.25 (0.12, 0.38) | 0.49 (0.34, 0.64) | 0.53 (0.39, 0.68) | 0.64 (0.50, 0.77) | 0.02 (-0.11, 0.15) | 0.14 (0.00, 0.27) | 0.15 (0.02, 0.28) | 0.20 (0.07, 0.32) | 0.15 (0.03, 0.27) |
Homophily on lab section | 1.44 (1.20, 1.68) | 1.06 (0.82, 1.30) | 0.73 (0.54, 0.92) | 1.08 (0.91, 1.25) | 1.20 (1.10, 1.29) | 1.20 (1.05, 1.35) | 1.12 (0.90, 1.34) | 1.20 (1.01, 1.39) | 1.29 (1.10, 1.47) | 1.41 (1.21, 1.61) | 1.41 (1.20, 1.62) |
0-indegree | 0.74 (0.23, 1.25) | 1.31 (0.80, 1.83) | 1.44 (1.18, 1.70) | 1.20 (0.93, 1.48) | 1.00 (0.75, 1.26) | 1.31 (1.03, 1.60) | 1.29 (0.99, 1.59) | 1.30 (0.99, 1.60) | 1.45 (1.16, 1.74) | 1.25 (0.97, 1.53) | 1.20 (0.91, 1.50) |
Female nominator | 0.84 (0.39, 1.28) | 1.13 (0.67, 1.60) | 0.53 (0.30, 0.77) | 0.43 (0.19, 0.67) | 0.59 (0.37, 0.81) | 0.71 (0.50, 0.92) | 0.25 (0.02, 0.48) | 0.27 (0.03, 0.51) | 0.29 (0.05, 0.54) | 0.16 (-0.05, 0.37) | 0.24 (0.00, 0.47) |
Female-female bias | -0.04 (-0.33, 0.25) | -0.21 (-0.49, 0.07) | 0.08 (-0.06, 0.22) | 0.01 (-0.15, 0.18) | -0.04 (-0.20, 0.11) | -0.11 (-0.26, 0.03) | 0.14 (-0.04, 0.32) | 0.12 (-0.05, 0.29) | 0.09 (-0.07, 0.26) | 0.11 (-0.04, 0.26) | 0.15 (0.00, 0.31) |
Male-male bias | 0.57 (0.15, 0.99) | 0.59 (0.17, 1.02) | 0.33 (0.11, 0.56) | 0.25 (0.02, 0.48) | 0.41 (0.22, 0.59) | 0.46 (0.26, 0.67) | 0.26 (0.04, 0.49) | 0.29 (0.06, 0.51) | 0.30 (0.09, 0.51) | 0.28 (0.10, 0.46) | 0.32 (0.14, 0.51) |
Cell entry = Point estimate (95% CI); S = survey number; Bold = significant (p < 0.05).
Note: for Course C Survey S5, the lower bound of the confidence interval is positive for female nominator and negative for female-female bias, although both round to 0 with only two decimal places.
Performance is a strong and significant predictor of receiving a nomination in every survey, indicating that students have an accurate sense of other students’ performance, despite not having any public way to view their peers’ grades. In addition, outspokenness has a significant effect in all but one case, indicating that students also nominate based on this trait. Being in the same lab section is also universally predictive of a nomination from one student to another. There is a significant tendency for there to be more students with no nominations than expected by chance given the overall nomination rates and the other terms in the model. The female nominator coefficient indicates that females make more nominations overall than males do, without considering the gender of those they nominate.
With performance and outspokenness in the model, females no longer show a bias toward nominating males in any of the 11 surveys; their nominations do not diverge from gender expectations in either direction in any survey. Males, on the other hand, continue to show a significant bias towards males in all 11 surveys; in each case the magnitude of the effect declined, but remains significant. Fig 2 shows the consequences of this inequity by simulating the nominations that would occur according to this model in a hypothetical classroom with a 1:1 gender ratio and equal mean class performance and outspokenness by gender.
Another way to understand the magnitude of the gender bias is to compare its coefficient to that for class grade point average (GPA), our best proxy for actual mastery of course material scored on a 4 point scale. Averaged across the 11 surveys, females give a boost to fellow females relative to males that is equivalent to an increase in GPA of 0.040; i.e. they would be equally likely to nominate an outspoken female with a 3.00 and an outspoken male with a 3.04. On the other hand, males give a boost to fellow males that is equivalent to a GPA increase of 0.765; for an outspoken female to be nominated by males at the same level as an outspoken male her performance would need to be over three-quarters of a GPA point higher than the male’s. On this scale, the male nominators’ gender bias is 19 times the size of the female nominators’.
Being male is a prerequisite for celebrity status
The three-to-four most nominated students in all classes examined were male. In each class, most students received very few nominations, while several students emerged over the course of the class as exceptionally well known; we refer to these students as “celebrities”. Several patterns are evident in the distribution of nominations in these classes (Fig 3). First, celebrity students tend to have high grades and speak up frequently in class. Second, with no exceptions, the biggest celebrity students in each network are male. While some females rank towards the top, the most well-known females are tied for 4th in two classes, and are 5th most well-known in the other. Third, male students at the top of the distribution tend to be considerably more well-known than any other student in the course. This is especially pronounced in Class B, where the most renowned male (52 nominations) received 5.78 times the nominations as the most renowned female (9 nominations). The most renowned male in Class A (16 nominations) has twice as many nominations than the most renowned female (8 nominations), while in Class C the most renowned male (13 nominations) has 1.63 times as many nominations than the most renowned female (8 nominations). These high nomination counts are notable, given the low average number of nominations seen across all 11 surveys (1.20).While the number of nominations achieved by celebrities in each class varies, the male biased pattern among the most frequently nominated peers holds.
The male majority among classroom celebrities could be explained if males were the only students who both achieved high grades and spoke up frequently in class. However, this is not the case. While male students on average scored slightly higher than female students and were more likely to be outspoken in every class, outspoken females with grades as high as these most renowned male students exist in every class (S4, S5 and S6 Figs). However, females achieving high grades and outspoken status never gain the same celebrity status as their male counterparts. It appears that being male is a prerequisite for students to achieve celebrity status within these classrooms.
Discussion
The underrepresentation of women in STEM is a complex and daunting problem. Increasing gender equity requires tackling both inequalities in students’ initial interest in STEM and the retention of women who have expressed that interest. While there is strong evidence that precollege factors influence a student’s initial decision to major in a STEM field [24], the causes of attrition after students initially declare a STEM major are less commonly explored. Studies on attrition of STEM-oriented women have found sense of belonging [16], decisions to start families [24], and confidence that one can succeed in one’s chosen profession (11) all influence a woman’s decision to leave STEM. In particular, professional confidence is lower for women in STEM than for men [12]. This confidence is influenced by many inputs, but one of the major ones, especially for women in STEM fields, is receiving verbal messages and encouragement from individuals with influence, such as teachers and peers [9]. Unfortunately, current faculty hold a gender bias that impacts the experiences of women in STEM [13] and could result in less support from faculty. Here we demonstrate that the peers of female students in introductory biology classes can also exhibit gender biases, adding to the list of subtle experiences that can lead to the attrition of females from STEM careers.
In three iterations of an undergraduate biology class, we found that even after controlling for actual course performance and outspokenness, male peers still disproportionately nominate males as being knowledgeable about biology while females nominate males and females equally. This indicates that males hold a bias against their female peers’ competence in biology. Our finding of peers as a second source of differential treatment by gender, beyond known biases of faculty, contributes to a more complete picture of the experiences of undergraduate women in STEM fields. The coalescence of subtle messages about their STEM abilities from both faculty and peers may undermine the self-confidence females have to persist in STEM fields beyond their undergraduate education [25].
The finding that a gender bias impacts the perception of millennial students may at first seem surprising, but is supported by work on implicit biases. Implicit biases are unconscious associations that people hold related to certain groups. Across many cultures, STEM is associated with males and not females [26]. Interestingly, male STEM majors in the US hold the strongest associations between maleness and science, while female STEM majors show some of the weakest implicit biases between gender and science [27]. These differences in the gender-STEM stereotypes held may explain why male undergraduate STEM majors nominate more males, but females do not demonstrate this bias. It also helps explain why male faculty demonstrate biases in hiring and mentoring, but many female STEM faculty do not [28].
One potential analytical concern for the current study is multiple comparisons. This occurs when statistical analyses involves multiple outcome measures, testing for an effect of multiple independent variables on a single outcome measure, or when the research design is repeated across several populations. In each case, the chance of finding a false positive is increased by adding another test. Because we repeated our study design three times and include multiple independent variables in our models, we are performing multiple tests, and thus have increased chances of a false positive. However, the repeated significance of our main result (that males over-nominate their male peers) across every survey gives us no reason to suspect that they are spurious due to multiple comparisons. It appears that males consistently hold a bias against their female peers’ competence in biology.
The classroom environment can influence student perceptions of their peers
Our work suggests that processes in the classroom may either be reinforcing pre-existing implicit biases over the quarter, or at least facilitating behaviors based on these biases. The end of every class term shows a stronger male bias than the beginning. This pattern is mediated by two class-related factors: 1) whether or not a student is outspoken in class and 2) level of achievement in the class. These factors, which seem to influence the opinions of both male and female peers, have previously been found to differ by gender in biology: males are more likely to be heard speaking in class and males slightly, but systematically, outperform women [15]. Instructors may be able to interrupt this process by equalizing the rate at which students of all genders speak up in class, closing the achievement gaps in their classrooms, or using more student-centered instruction in ways that do not rely primarily on whole class discussions (e.g. small group-work only).
We propose that the specific classroom environment can influence the effect size of the male bias, with some support for this hypothesis from Class C. In this term males did not behave differently than in previous years. Females, however, developed a stronger bias towards nominating other females than in the other two classes. Though this bias was not significant, it effectively lessens the overall magnitude of bias towards male students. Although we cannot specifically pinpoint why this was the case, this class differed from the other two in two critical ways. First, one of the three instructors in this course was female, whereas all instructors were male in the other two classes. Female instructors, when they are considered role models, have been shown to reduce the science-gender biases of female students, and this may have impacted the latter’s nomination patterns [29]. Second, during classroom discussions, all three instructors in Class C employed ‘random call,’ in which the instructor selects students to speak based on a randomized class list rather than by choice, more extensively than in the two other classes. Random call has been shown to eliminate the gender gap in class volunteers, leading to more females speaking up in class [15] and limiting the opportunity for one student to dominate classroom conversations and the instructors attention [30]. These differences in Class C seem to indicate that other factors in the classroom environment could mitigate the extent to which gender and renown are correlated. It is important to keep in mind that this mitigation seems to come from a larger female-female bias. This counteracting gender bias is likely undesirable compared to eliminating the male-male bias and achieving complete gender equity. Further research is needed to understand how to best achieve this equity in peer perception.
Biology is a conservative case; patterns may be more extreme across STEM
The context of this research on peer perceptions was an introductory biology classroom. We can only speculate on the peer biases present in other STEM fields, but we predict that the male bias observed in this study may be conservative relative to other STEM fields for three reasons. First, biology is thought to be the STEM field with the most gender equity: undergraduate enrollment is nearly equal in terms of males and females [31] and slightly more women than men earn degrees in the biological sciences. Thus females in biology do not have to contend with the biases associated with being the sole representative of their gender in a STEM classroom [32]. Second, there is also a perception that biology lacks a strong math basis, and does not invoke the math-gender stereotype as strongly [33]. Thus, stereotypes about women’s math ability may not be undermining how their peers perceive them to the same extent it might in more explicitly math-based fields like physics or computer science. Finally, biology is a field that people believe does not require “brilliance”, unlike other STEM fields [34]. This perception means that stereotypes that males are more intelligent may not impact peer perceptions as strongly as it does in fields that are considered to require brilliance, like physics and math. For these reasons, we argue that the gender inequities in peer perception in the classrooms presented in this paper are likely conservative compared to classrooms in other STEM fields. Further, this dynamic may exist beyond STEM fields. However, explicit tests are required to confirm these hypotheses.
Conclusion
Our findings have strong implications regarding the effectiveness of existing strategies to increase women in STEM fields. Without addressing social dynamics that perpetuate gender biases in the college classroom, simply increasing the number of young women entering STEM majors may not be enough. The patterns of uneven peer perceptions by gender shown in our student population suggest that future populations of academics may perpetuate the same gender stereotypes that have been illuminated among current faculty. This may not only be the case because the male students receiving high celebrity are reaffirmed in their abilities and are better able to advance through the STEM pipeline than women who do not receive this affirmation, but also because the existence of “celebrity” males and other individuals with distinction can impact and reaffirm the stereotypes held by others [35]. This gender biased pattern in celebrity was experienced by over 1,500 students in our analyses. This number is striking, but less worrisome than the millions of students who attend college STEM classes that may perpetuate the same biases described here. In addition to current impacts on the peers in their classes, the students in these classes are potential future faculty members. Although we cannot directly compare the magnitude of gender bias between current faculty and millennial students, our work implies that the chilly environment for women may not be going away any time soon.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Mary Pat Wenderoth, Scott Freeman, Erin Shortlidge, and Jim Collins for their thoughtful comments on this manuscript. We thank the many students for their participation in this study. Lastly, we thank Arielle Desure, Carrie Sjogren, Katherine Cook, and Sarah Davis for their help compiling the network data used in these analyses. We also thank Nicholas Horrocks and two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments.
Data Availability
Underlying data are subject to ethical restrictions as the data contain identifiable information, including gender and ethnicity, combined with sensitive information such as college grades. The limited, de-identified data can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author at grunny@uw.edu, pending ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board of the University of Washington.
Funding Statement
This research was supported in part by NSF DUE grant 1244847 (AJC). SMG was supported by R01-HD068395.
References
- 1.National Science Foundation NCfSaES (2013) Women, Minorities, and Persons with Disabilities in Science and Engineering: 2013. Special Report NSF 13–304.
- 2.Gillen A, Tanenbaum C (2014) Exploring Gender Imbalance Among STEM Doctoral Degree Recipients Washington, DC: American Institutes for Research. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Isbell LA, Young TP, Harcourt AH (2012) Stag parties linger: continued gender bias in a female-rich scientific discipline. PLoS One 7: e49682 10.1371/journal.pone.0049682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Settles IH, Cortina LM, Malley J, Stewart AJ (2006) The climate for women in academic science: The good, the bad, and the changeable. Psychology of Women Quarterly 30: 47–58. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Swim JK, Hyers LL, Cohen LL, Ferguson MJ (2001) Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from three daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues 57: 31–53. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eddy SL, Brownell SE (2015) Beneath the numbers: A review of gender disparities in undergraduate education across science, technology, engineering, and math disciplines. Physics Review: Special Topics Physics Education Research: Gender in Physics.
- 7.Robnett RD, Leaper C (2013) Friendship Groups, Personal Motivation, and Gender in Relation to High School Students' STEM Career Interest. Journal of Research on Adolescence 23: 652–664. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Usher EL, Pajares F (2008) Sources of self-efficacy in school: Critical review of the literature and future directions. Review of Educational Research 78: 751–796. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Zeldin AL, Pajares F (2000) Against the odds: Self-efficacy beliefs of women in mathematical, scientific, and technological careers. American Educational Research Journal 37: 215–246. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cohen GL, Steele CM, Ross LD (1999) The mentor’s dilemma: Providing critical feedback across the racial divide. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25: 1302–1318. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rosenthal L, London B, Levy SR, Lobel M (2011) The roles of perceived identity compatibility and social support for women in a single-sex STEM program at a co-educational university. Sex Roles 65: 725–736. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cech E, Rubineau B, Silbey S, Seron C (2011) Professional role confidence and gendered persistence in engineering. American Sociological Review 76: 641–666. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Moss-Racusin CA, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, Handelsman J (2012) Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 16474–16479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Milkman KL, Akinola M, Chugh D (2014) What happens before? A field experiment exploring how pay and representation differentially shape bias on the pathway into organizations. A Field Experiment Exploring How Pay and Representation Differentially Shape Bias on the Pathway into Organizations (July 11, 2014).
- 15.Eddy SL, Brownell SE, Wenderoth MP (2014) Gender Gaps in Achievement and Participation in Multiple Introductory Biology Classrooms. CBE-Life Sciences Education 13: 478–492. 10.1187/cbe.13-10-0204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Seymour E, Hewitt NM, Friend CM (1997) Talking about leaving: Why undergraduates leave the sciences: Westview Press Boulder, CO. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Eddy SL, Brownell SE, Thummaphan P, Lan M-C, Wenderoth MP (2015) Caution, Student Experience May Vary: Social Identities Impact a Student’s Experience in Peer Discussions. CBE-Life Sciences Education 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.O’Brien LT, Blodorn A, Adams G, Garcia DM, Hammer E (2015) Ethnic variation in gender-STEM stereotypes and STEM participation: An intersectional approach. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 21: 169 10.1037/a0037944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Olson S, Riordan DG (2012) Engage to Excel: Producing One Million Additional College Graduates with Degrees in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics. Report to the President. Executive Office of the President.
- 20.Wiens DJ, Depping DJ, Wallerich SR, Van Laar ES, Juhl AL (2003) Gender Matters: Factors Influencing Biology Interest among Undergraduate Majors. Journal of College Science Teaching 33: 32–36. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Robins G, Pattison P, Kalish Y, Lusher D (2007) An introduction to exponential random graph (p*) models for social networks. Social networks 29: 173–191. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hunter DR, Handcock MS, Butts CT, Goodreau SM, Morris M (2008) ergm: A package to fit, simulate and diagnose exponential-family models for networks. Journal of statistical software 24: nihpa54860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Butts CT (2008) network: a Package for Managing Relational Data in R. Journal of Statistical Software 24: 1–36. 18612375 [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ceci SJ, Williams WM (2011) Understanding current causes of women's underrepresentation in science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108: 3157–3162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Becker JR (1984) The pursuit of graduate education in mathematics: factors that influence women and men. J Educ Equit Leader 4: 39–53. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Nosek BA, Banaji M, Greenwald AG (2002) Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 6: 101. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nosek BA, Smyth FL (2011) Implicit social cognitions predict sex differences in math engagement and achievement. American Educational Research Journal 48: 1125–1156. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sheltzer JM, Smith JC (2014) Elite male faculty in the life sciences employ fewer women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111: 10107–10112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Young DM, Rudman LA, Buettner HM, McLean MC (2013) The influence of female role models on women’s implicit science cognitions. Psychology of Women Quarterly 37: 283–292. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Martin SN, Milne C, Scantlebury K (2006) Eye‐rollers, risk‐takers, and turn sharks: Target students in a professional science education program. Journal of research in science teaching 43: 819–851. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Board NS (2014) Science and Engineering Indicators. Arlington VA.
- 32.Thompson M, Sekaquaptewa D (2002) When being different is detrimental: Solo status and the performance of women and racial minorities. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy 2: 183–203. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Schmader T, Johns M, Barquissau M (2004) The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women's experience in the math domain. Sex Roles 50: 835–850. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Leslie S- J, Cimpian A, Meyer M, Freeland E (2015) Expectations of brilliance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science 347: 262–265. 10.1126/science.1261375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rothbart M, Fulero S, Jensen C, Howard J, Birrell P (1978) From individual to group impressions: Availability heuristics in stereotype formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 14: 237–255. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Underlying data are subject to ethical restrictions as the data contain identifiable information, including gender and ethnicity, combined with sensitive information such as college grades. The limited, de-identified data can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author at grunny@uw.edu, pending ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board of the University of Washington.