Abstract
Testosterone replacement for older men is increasingly common, with some observations suggesting a protective effect on cognitive function. We examined the association of endogenous testosterone with cognitive function among older men in a Mendelian randomization study using a separate-sample instrumental variable (SSIV) analysis estimator to minimize confounding and reverse causality. A genetic score predicting testosterone was developed in 289 young Chinese men from Hong Kong, based on selected testosterone-related single nucleotide polymorphisms (rs10046, rs1008805 and rs1256031). The association of genetically predicted testosterone with delayed 10-word recall score and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was assessed at baseline and follow-up using generalized estimating equation among 4,212 older Chinese men from the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Predicted testosterone was not associated with delayed 10-word recall score (−0.02 per nmol/L testosterone, 95% confidence interval (CI) −0.06–0.02) or MMSE score (0.06, 95% CI −0.002–0.12). These estimates were similar after additional adjustment for age, education, smoking, use of alcohol, body mass index and the Framingham score. Our findings do not corroborate observed protective effects of testosterone on cognitive function among older men.
Testosterone has attracted great interest as a therapy for older men in the last few years, especially in North America1. Animal experiments suggest testosterone improves spatial cognition in male rats2,3. The evidence in humans is inconsistent and unclear (Table 1). Some observational studies suggest testosterone is associated with better verbal memory or higher Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score4,5, although only among older men4 or only for MMSE at baseline5; whilst others suggest the association of testosterone with cognitive function is null6 or negative7. Observational studies are open to residual confounding and reverse causality8. Evidence from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of testosterone administration in older men is also inconsistent, suggesting no effect9,10, or potential benefits for verbal and spatial memory11 or MMSE12. In contrast, another RCT showed a decrease in verbal memory13, suggesting potential harm for cognitive function. Similarly, evidence from a recent meta-analysis of interventional studies also showed unclear effects of androgen deprivation therapy on cognitive function14. As such, the role of testosterone in cognition remains unclear, and improved cognition remains a potential benefit15.
Table 1. Main findings from epidemiological studies on testosterone and cognitive function in men.
Main references |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study design | Exposure | Cognitivefunction | Author, year | Samplesize(men) | Mean age(years) | Main findings |
Observational studies | Serum testosterone | ↑ in older people | Muller, 20054 | 400 | 60.2 | Curvilinear association with verbal memory, which disappears in age subgroups; only associated with better verbal memory in the oldest age group, no association in other age groups. No association with MMSE. |
↑↔ | Hogervorst, 20105 | 257 | 74.4 | Optimal TT associated with better MMSE at baseline, no association with MMSE change or verbal memory | ||
↔ | LeBlanc, 201063 | 1602 | NA | No association with modified mental state examination (3MS), which includes MMSE | ||
↓ | Martin, 20077 | 1195 | 54.3 | Associated with worse verbal memory | ||
RCTs | Testosterone administration | ↔ | Haren, 20059 | 76 | 68.5 | No effect on MMSE |
↔ | Sih, 199710 | 15 | 68 | No effect on verbal memory | ||
↓↔ | Maki, 200713 | 15 | 73.9 | Decreased short-delay verbal memory; no effect on composite verbal memory | ||
↑ | Cherrier, 200511 | 32 | 76 | Increased verbal and spatial memory | ||
↑ | Wahjoepramono, 201512 | 44 | NA | Increased MMSE | ||
Meta-analysis of interventional studies | Androgen deprivation therapy (ADT) | ↓↔ | McGinty, 201414 | 417 | 63.2–71.0 | Prostate cancer patients receiving ADT had worse performance on visuomotor tasks compared to noncancer control groups. No effect on other cognitive domains. |
TT, total testosterone; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; ADT, Androgen deprivation therapy; NA, not available currently.
↑ means a positive association with or an increase in cognitive function; ↓ means a negative association with or a decrease in cognitive function; ↔ means no association or no effect.
Using naturally occurring testosterone-related genetic variants in a Mendelian randomization (MR) study design provides a means of examining the causal effects of testosterone on cognitive function without any interventions. Genetic variants determined at conception, resulting in life-long differences in exposures, are unlikely to be associated with socioeconomic position or other confounders. Testosterone falls with age and ill-health16,17, and cognitive impairment has a long pre-clinical course, thus lifetime testosterone exposure may be more relevant than contemporaneous exposure in older men, however obtaining biomaterials indicating lifetime testosterone exposure is not currently feasible. An MR study with a separate-sample instrumental variable (SSIV) analysis using genetic instruments enables examination of the association of endogenous testosterone, unaffected by aging or ill-health, with cognitive function. Here, we used a separate-sample MR study design to assess the effect of endogenous peak testosterone on cognitive function in older men.
Results
Genetically predicted log testosterone was calculated as −0.07 × rs1008805 + 0.07 × rs10046 −0.07 × rs1256031 + 3.0, as previously18. The proportion of the variance in log testosterone explained by genetically predicted log testosterone was 4.1%, which is typical for MR studies19. An instrument explaining all the variance in serum testosterone would be invalid because it would be equivalent to serum testosterone, and so would not provide an unconfounded estimate. The F-statistic was 13.3, suggesting a reliable instrument.
Among the 8,450 men in all 3 phases of the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study (GBCS), DNA was extracted from blood samples and used for single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) testing for 4,262 men, with availability depending on phase of recruitment and other logistical concerns, but not on genetic profile or cognitive function. Men with SNPs tested had higher baseline delayed 10-word recall score (mean difference 0.22, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.14–0.30) than men without SNP testing, but were not different in baseline MMSE (mean difference −0.17, 95% CI −0.37–0.03), assessed from linear regression adjusted for age; small differences could occur by chance. Among these 4,262 men, 4,212 (98.8%) had complete information on the three SNPs. None of these SNPs deviated from Hardy-Weinburg equilibrium in GBCS. Among the 4,212 men, 4,160 had 10-word recall from baseline or follow-up (3,330 from both baseline and follow-up, 712 from baseline only and 118 from follow-up only). 4,122 had MMSE score from baseline or follow-up (891 from both baseline and follow-up, 628 from baseline only, and 2,603 from follow-up only). As would be expected, genetically predicted testosterone was not associated with age, education, smoking status or use of alcohol (Table 2).
Table 2. Genetically predicted testosterone by socio-demographic characteristics among 4,212 men (50+), Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study at baseline, 2003–2008.
Genetically predicted testosterone(nmol/L) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristic | No. | % | Mean | SD | ANOVA pvaluea |
Age group, years | 4212 | 0.94 | |||
50–54 | 8.1 | 18.05 | 1.10 | ||
55–59 | 20.2 | 18.01 | 1.22 | ||
60–64 | 25.4 | 18.01 | 1.24 | ||
65–69 | 24.1 | 18.01 | 1.27 | ||
70–74 | 16.5 | 17.99 | 1.19 | ||
75–79 | 4.2 | 17.95 | 1.28 | ||
≥80 | 1.5 | 18.15 | 1.52 | ||
Education | 4209 | 0.91 | |||
Less than primary school | 2.6 | 18.11 | 1.36 | ||
Primary school | 26.9 | 18.01 | 1.22 | ||
Junior middle school | 30.0 | 18.01 | 1.19 | ||
Senior middle school | 24.2 | 18.01 | 1.23 | ||
Junior college | 8.9 | 17.99 | 1.34 | ||
College | 7.4 | 17.96 | 1.24 | ||
Smoking status | 4192 | 0.59 | |||
Never smoker | 40.8 | 17.99 | 1.22 | ||
Ex-smoker | 27.9 | 18.02 | 1.25 | ||
Current smoker | 31.3 | 18.03 | 1.22 | ||
Use of alcohol | 4212 | 0.49 | |||
Never | 51.8 | 18.02 | 1.25 | ||
<1/month | 19.9 | 18.03 | 1.19 | ||
<1/week | 4.3 | 18.04 | 1.20 | ||
1–4/week | 6.0 | 18.03 | 1.30 | ||
5+/week | 11.9 | 17.90 | 1.19 | ||
Ex-drinker | 4.4 | 18.01 | 1.20 | ||
Unknown | 1.8 | 18.08 | 1.27 |
Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.
aGenetically predicted testosterone was not associated with age, socioeconomic position (education) or lifestyle, including smoking status and use of alcohol (ANOVA p value > 0.05).
Table 3 shows genetically predicted testosterone was not associated with delayed 10-word recall score. The estimates were close to the null. The estimate for the association of genetically predicted testosterone with MMSE score was positive but the 95% CI included the null. The estimates remained unchanged after adjustment for covariates (Models 2–5) and after excluding men selected for another cognition-related project (Model 6)20, although in model 3 genetically predicted testosterone was associated with higher MMSE score. The association of genetically predicted testosterone with delayed 10-word recall and MMSE did not vary with age (p-values for interaction 0.13 and 0.11).
Table 3. Effect of genetically predicted testosterone (nmol/L) on cognitive function among men (50+ years), Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study, recruitment 2003–2008 and follow up till Dec 31, 2012.
Outcome | Mean(SD) | n | Model | Beta-coefficienta | 95% CI | P value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Delayed 10-word | 5.5 | 4160 | 1 | −0.02 | −0.06–0.02 | 0.23 |
10-word recall | (1.9) | 4160 | 2 | −0.03 | −0.07–0.01 | 0.17 |
4129 | 3 | −0.02 | −0.06–0.02 | 0.27 | ||
4123 | 4 | −0.02 | −0.06–0.02 | 0.30 | ||
4093 | 5 | −0.02 | −0.06–0.02 | 0.30 | ||
3470 | 6 | −0.02 | −0.06–0.02 | 0.30 | ||
MMSE | 27.5 | 4122 | 1 | 0.06 | −0.002–0.12 | 0.06 |
(2.7) | 4122 | 2 | 0.05 | −0.007–0.11 | 0.08 | |
4083 | 3 | 0.06 | 0.0003–0.12 | 0.05 | ||
4078 | 4 | 0.06 | −0.0004–0.12 | 0.05 | ||
4048 | 5 | 0.06 | −0.001–0.12 | 0.06 | ||
3443 | 6 | 0.06 | −0.01–0.13 | 0.10 |
Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; CI, confidence interval.
Model 1 showed the association of genetically predicted testosterone with cognitive function without any covariates;
Model 2 adjusted for age;
Model 3 additionally adjusted for education, smoking, and use of alcohol;
Model 4 additionally adjusted for body mass index (BMI);
Model 5 additionally adjusted for Framingham score.
Model 6 replicated the analysis in Model 1 excluding men in another cognition project.
aBeta coefficient refers to the average change in cognitive function (the delayed 10-word recall score and MMSE score) with each unit (nmol/L) increase in genetically predicted testosterone.
Discussion
Using an MR study design to minimize confounding and reverse causality, we found no clear indication that endogenous testosterone was associated with improved cognitive function in older Chinese men, although we cannot rule out a small positive effect on MMSE. Our novel study provides limited support for any protective effects of endogenous testosterone on cognitive function.
The strengths of our study include using an MR study design with two separate samples (SSIV) to achieve an unbiased estimate. Simulations have shown MR with two separate samples is preferable to using the same sample for assessing the relations of the instrument with the exposure and the instrument with the outcome21. SSIV is useful and cost-efficient when the phenotype of interest was either not measured or was measured with substantial error in the sample with the outcome22,23. The MR design makes it feasible to use testosterone in early adulthood as a marker of lifetime exposure, when it is infeasible to obtain lifetime testosterone for older men. Thus, we avoid the imprecision in MR estimates that could arise from assessing the genetic association with testosterone in older age, when testosterone may reflect ill-health, inducing an underestimation of the genetic association with testosterone and inflating the MR estimates24. SSIV also remediates weak-instruments bias, reducing concerns about using multiple polymorphisms as instrument22. Any correlation of the genetic variants with unmeasured confounders in the sample with the phenotype is unlikely to be replicated in the sample with the outcome22. We also restricted the sample of young men to those with both parents and at least 3 grandparents born in Hong Kong or Guangdong province, to ensure genetic homogeneity, reflected by the similar allele frequencies of the genetic variants in the two samples18.
Although we used MR which can mimic the randomized treatment allocation in RCTs25, limitations exist. First, our SNP selection was limited by the lack of evidence from any genome-wide association study (GWAS) of testosterone in young Chinese men. Most GWAS of testosterone have been conducted in Caucasians26,27, but the SNPs identified have not all replicated successfully in Chinese men28. Moreover, one SNP that did replicate (rs2075230) in Chinese men is not suitable as a genetic instrument because of a pleiotropic association with sex hormone-binding globulin (SHBG)28. The GWAS are also conducted among older men, so the genetic associations might reflect accumulated age-related co-morbidities rather than testosterone29. We used a candidate gene approach rather than risk alleles from GWAS, given the relatively small sample of young men in Hong Kong, however, the requirement for a genetic instrument is to predict the exposure which may be based on functionality as well as statistical grounds30. The association of genetically predicted testosterone with lower high-density lipoprotein (HDL)-cholesterol18, as would be expected from meta-analysis of RCTs31, also suggests some validity of our genetic instrument. Second, the delayed 10-word recall test and MMSE might not be completely comprehensive. However, these standard tools have been validated as feasible, sensitive tools for screening mild cognitive impairment in population-based research in developing countries32,33, are widely used in epidemiological studies34 and have been previously used in this study19,35,36. Third, population stratification and canalization may affect MR. However, the participants were restricted to genetically homogeneous Chinese men. Moreover, it is generally assumed that a gene (and hence) a genetic score has the same effect on the phenotype across time and space37. No evidence suggests the association of the selected SNPs with testosterone varied by environmental factors. Fourth, MR is based on the assumption that the genetic instrument is associated with the exposure, is not associated with the outcome other than via the exposure (no pleiotropy) and no confounders of the association of the genetic instrument with the outcomes exist25. The selected SNPs would not be expected to affect cognitive function directly (pleiotropy) other than via testosterone, because the SNPs are from genes functionally relevant to testosterone. Some associations of genetically predicted testosterone with systemic inflammation, cardiovascular disease risk factors and electrocardiographic QT interval duration have been found in our previous MR studies18,38,39. Cardiovascular disease is associated with cognitive decline40, as such the association with cognitive function might be secondary to any associations with cardiovascular disease risk factors. However, additional adjustment for the Framingham score (Model 5) did not change the estimates. The genetic score was not associated with estradiol (data not shown) or the confounders (as shown in Table 2). Fifth, MR requires a large sample size. For the 10-word recall and MMSE scores, a sample size of ~4,000 has 0.8 power to detect a relatively small effect size of 0.2241, according to the formula for instrumental variable analysis sample size calculation given by Freeman et al.41, and consistent with simulation results given by Pierce et al.42. The null associations with delayed 10-word recall (−0.02, 95% CI −0.06–0.02) or MMSE (0.06, 95% CI −0.002–0.12) might be due to lack of power. However, the small estimates suggest little, if any, clinical benefits, for diseases, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD), given the rapid cognitive decline with disease progression. For example, delayed 10-word recall score declines with time at a slope of 0.12 in preclinical AD43 and MMSE score declines at the average annual rate of 3.3 in AD patients44. As such, although we cannot confirm the role of testosterone, our findings which are consistent with evidence from RCTs9,10, raise doubts about the observed beneficial association of testosterone with cognitive function4,5, with relevance to those who might be attracted globally by commercial promotion of testosterone45. Sixth, MR assumes linearity41, we obtained similar results assuming a log-linear association. Seventh, the result shows no clear association of genetically predicted testosterone with cognitive function, but might suggest a potential benefit for MMSE. One possible explanation is that MMSE reflects a comprehensive measure of cognitive function whilst 10-word recall reflects verbal memory. The positive association for MMSE might reflect a potential benefit of testosterone for a specific domain of cognition, such as spatial ability, despite only a small estimate for MMSE. Replication using a specific measure of spatial cognition would be desirable. Finally, correction for multiple testing might be appropriate when using the same study for a series of outcomes. Such correction is used in agnostic or exploratory studies, such as GWAS, to control for type 1 error (false positive)46. However, it may increase the risk of type 2 error (false negative)47,48. Given that our study aims at confirmation and our negative findings do not corroborate observed protective associations of testosterone with cognitive function among men, controlling for the type 2 error is important from a public health perspective.
Our study is inconsistent with some observational studies where testosterone is positively or negatively associated with cognitive function4,5,7. However, these studies cannot distinguish whether testosterone is associated with cognitive function as a cause, as a symptom of disease or as a marker of an underlying process affecting testosterone and cognitive function. As expected, our study is consistent with some small RCTs9,10, where testosterone therapy had no effect on cognitive function in older men, although intervention in late adulthood may not be the same as lifetime intervention. The null association in our study is also consistent with observations from “lay epidemiology”49 where no obvious difference in overall cognitive ability exists between men and women despite very different levels of testosterone, although slight differences might exist in some specific domains, such as greater spatial ability in men50.
Our novel study provides a feasible and less biased method to test the lifetime effect of testosterone on cognitive function in a short time frame without any intervention. Our study adds to the limited evidence on the role of testosterone in cognitive function, providing minimal support for a protective effect of endogenous testosterone. Given emerging concerns about the use of testosterone51, it is unclear that the potential cognitive benefit would outweigh the risks.
Methods
Ethics, consent and permissions
The methods were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines. The protocols were approved by the University of Hong Kong-Hospital Authority Hong Kong West Cluster Joint Institutional Review Board. The Guangzhou Medical Ethics Committee of the Chinese Medical Association approved GBCS, including the use of genetic data. Written, informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation.
Study design
A separate-sample MR design was used (Supplementary Fig. S1). First, a genetic score predicting serum testosterone was developed in young Chinese men from Hong Kong (sample 1), with mean age of 21.0 years (standard deviation (SD) 1.8 years), as described previously18. Second, we applied the genetic prediction rule in the sample of older men from GBCS (sample 2), with the same genetic origin as sample 1, to examine the association of genetically predicted testosterone, rather than serum testosterone, with cognitive function among older Chinese men.
Sources of data
Morning blood samples were collected from students recruited from the University of Hong Kong, restricted to those with both parents and at least three grandparents born in Hong Kong or Guangdong and not taking medication affecting hormones. Testosterone was assessed by competitive immunoassay on Vitros 3600 immunodiagnostic system (Ortho Clinical Diagnostics Inc, USA) with a detection limit of 0.17 nmol/L. The intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 4.9% and 5.7% at 4.4 nmol/L, 3.2% and 3.9% at 16.3 nmol/L, and 1.8% and 3.0% at 37.5 nmol/L, respectively. DNA was extracted and analyzed at the Centre for Genomic Sciences of the University of Hong Kong for selected SNPs from ESR1 (rs722208 and rs2175898), CYP19A1 (rs10046 and rs1008805) and ESR2 (rs1256030 and rs1256031) using a Mass ARRAY system (Sequenom, San Diego, California), for establishing the genetic prediction rule, following the flow chart (shown in Supplementary Fig. S2). A self-administered questionnaire was used to collect socioeconomic position and health status.
GBCS is an ongoing collaboration of Guangzhou Number 12 Hospital, the Universities of Hong Kong and Birmingham, UK52. Recruitment was in 3 phases. All participants were permanent residents of Guangzhou aged 50+ years and members of the “The Guangzhou Health and Happiness Association for the Respectable Elders” (GHHARE), a community social and welfare association unofficially aligned with the municipal government. Membership of GHHARE is open to older people for a monthly fee of 4 Yuan (50 US cents). About 7% of permanent Guangzhou residents aged 50+ years are members of GHHARE, of whom 11% (~10,000) enrolled for each of phases one, two and three. Inclusion criteria were that they were capable of consenting, ambulatory, and not receiving treatment modalities which if omitted may result in immediate life threatening risk, such as chemotherapy or radiotherapy for cancer, or dialysis for renal failure. Fasting blood samples were collected at recruitment in phase 3 and at follow-up for participants recruited in other phases. Samples were stored, as whole blood or as buffy coat and sera, at −80 °C for all apart from a subset of phase 3 participants whose DNA was extracted from fresh blood and stored at −80 °C. Selected SNPs were analyzed by a commercial company (Beijing CapitalBio Corporation) in Beijing using a Mass ARRAY system (Sequenom, San Diego, California).
Cognitive function test
Cognitive function was assessed from a test battery developed for the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD)53. In all 3 phases at baseline and follow-up we used the test of new learning ability (10-word list learning task). Four words (“arm”, “letter”, “ticket” and “grass”) were taken from the original CERAD English language test33. “Pole”, “shore”, “cabin” and “engine” were replaced with “corner”, “stone”, “book” and “stick” as in the adapted CERAD 10-word list learning task33. “Butter” and “queen” were replaced with “soy sauce” and “chairman” to be culturally appropriate. During the learning phase the 10-word list was read to the participants who were then asked to recall immediately. This was repeated 3 times giving immediate recall. After a 5-minute period of distraction, during which the interview was continued, the participants were asked to recall as many of the 10 words as possible. Delayed recall score is generally more predictive than immediate recall score and less affected by education33, so delayed rather than immediate recall score was used. The adapted Consortium 10-word list learning task has been validated as a culturally and educationally sensitive tool for identifying dementia in population-based research in developing countries33 and reported previously in this study19,35,36.
MMSE was used in phase 3 at baseline and follow-up for all phases. MMSE is the most commonly administered psychometric screening assessment of cognition54. MMSE is a general cognition test covering orientation, attention, memory, recall and language55, widely used as a relatively sensitive marker of cognitive impairment54,56,57. Three of 11 tasks in the original MMSE were amended to be more culturally appropriate. Orientation in place was adapted according to the geographical divisions of China and the screening setting to “country”, “province”, “city”, “hospital” and “floor”. In the 3-word registration and recall “table” and “penny” were replaced with “newspaper” and “train” to ensure all 3 words were frequently used 2-character Chinese words. The modified MMSE has the same scale as the original MMSE58 hence, the psychometric properties of the measures should be similar. The modified MMSE has been previously reported19,35,36.
Exposure
The exposure was genetically predicted testosterone, estimated as the anti-log of predicted log testosterone from the genetic score. Serum testosterone was not measured in older men in GBCS. Testosterone, instead of log testosterone, was used for ease of interpretation as it gave the same pattern of results.
Outcomes
The primary outcomes were delayed 10-word recall score (out of 10) and MMSE score (out of 30). The delayed 10-word recall score was normally distributed. The MMSE score was skewed, but the sample size is large59. Preliminary analysis showed similar results using logged MMSE, so the results in natural units are presented. Higher scores indicate better performance.
Statistical analysis
In the young men, after testing for Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium and linkage disequilibrium, rs2175898 was dropped because it deviated from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium, rs1256030 was dropped because it was in linkage disequilibrium with rs125603118. Stepwise linear regression with all the remaining candidate SNPs (rs722208, rs10046, rs1008805, rs1256031) was used to find a parsimonious set of SNPs which best predicted log testosterone, as described previously18. The F-statistic, calculated from the regression of log testosterone on genetically predicted log testosterone, was obtained; an F-statistic >10 suggests a reliable genetic instrument60.
In the older men from GBCS, a test for trend was used to compare genetically predicted testosterone by key characteristics. We used generalized estimating equation models to estimate the relation of genetically predicted testosterone with cognition using data from both baseline and follow-up to take into account any correlation between two measurements for the same man. Although the association should not be confounded by socioeconomic position, lifestyle, or health attributes, covariates were included to achieve more precise estimates61. Model 1 had no covariates. Model 2 adjusted for age. Model 3 additionally adjusted for education, smoking and use of alcohol. Model 4 additionally adjusted for body mass index (BMI). Model 5 additionally adjusted for the Framingham score, because it is associated with cognitive decline62. We assessed whether the association of genetically predicted testosterone with cognition varied with age4, from the relevant interaction term. GBCS is a large cohort for answering many research questions; 690 men with all 3 SNPs for genetically predicted testosterone had SNP testing for another project on cognitive decline20. In model 6, we repeated the analysis with these men excluded. The power calculation was conducted using the formula for an instrumental variable analysis sample size given by Freeman et al.41, the sample size for such studies is approximately the usual sample size for exposure on outcome divided by the R2 between instrument and exposure. We also compared the power calculation with the simulation results of Pierce et al.42. All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata 10.1 (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas).
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Zhao, J. V. et al. A Mendelian randomization study of testosterone and cognition in men. Sci. Rep. 6, 21306; doi: 10.1038/srep21306 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study investigators include: Guangzhou No. 12 Hospital–Dr. Zhang WS, Dr. Zhu T, Dr. Liu B, Prof. Jiang CQ (Co-Principal Investigator (PI)); University of Hong Kong-Dr. C. M. Schooling, Prof. SM McGhee SM, Prof. R Fielding, Prof. GM Leung, Prof. TH Lam (Co-PI); University of Birmingham--Dr. G. N. Thomas, Dr. P Adab, Prof. KK Cheng (Co-PI). The authors thank Drs Au Yeung SL and Sushma Kavikondala for convoking the student sample, Drs Garcia-Barcelo MM, So Man, Xu L and Zhu T for facilitating the DNA extraction.
This work was supported by the University of Hong Kong Foundation for Development and Research (Hong Kong, China); the University of Hong Kong University Research Committee Strategic Research Theme of Public Health (Hong Kong, China); Guangzhou Public Health Bureau (Guangzhou, China; Key technology collaboration project, [grant number 2012J5100041]), Guangzhou Science and Technology Bureau (Guangzhou, China) and the University of Birmingham (Birmingham, United Kingdom). This sub-study was funded by the Research Grant Council General Research Fund [grant number 769710], Research Grant Council of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, People’s Republic of China.
The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, the decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Author Contributions C.Q.J. is the corresponding author. C.M.S. designed this specific study. T.H.L., K.K.C. and C.Q.J. designed the original study and acquired the data with the help of G.M.L., W.S.Z., B.L. and S.S.C., J.Z. implemented the analysis, interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript with assistance from C.M.S. All authors revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content; and gave final approval of the version to be submitted.
References
- Handelsman D. J. Global trends in testosterone prescribing, 2000–2011: expanding the spectrum of prescription drug misuse. Med. J. Aust. 199, 548–551 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locklear M. N. & Kritzer M. F. Assessment of the effects of sex and sex hormones on spatial cognition in adult rats using the Barnes maze. Horm. Behav. 66, 298–308 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawley W. R. et al. Testosterone modulates spatial recognition memory in male rats. Horm. Behav. 63, 559–565 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller M., Aleman A., Grobbee D. E., de Haan E. H. & van der Schouw Y. T. Endogenous sex hormone levels and cognitive function in aging men: is there an optimal level? Neurology 64, 866–871 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogervorst E., Matthews F. E. & Brayne C. Are optimal levels of testosterone associated with better cognitive function in healthy older women and men? Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1800, 1145–1152 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf O. T. & Kirschbaum C. Endogenous estradiol and testosterone levels are associated with cognitive performance in older women and men. Horm. Behav. 41, 259–266 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin D. M., Wittert G., Burns N. R., Haren M. T. & Sugarman R. Testosterone and cognitive function in ageing men: data from the Florey Adelaide Male Ageing Study (FAMAS). Maturitas 57, 182–194 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan N. A., Didelez V., Burton P. R. & Tobin M. D. Mendelian randomisation and causal inference in observational epidemiology. PLoS Med. 5, e177 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haren M. T., Wittert G. A., Chapman I. M., Coates P. & Morley J. E. Effect of oral testosterone undecanoate on visuospatial cognition, mood and quality of life in elderly men with low-normal gonadal status. Maturitas 50, 124–133 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sih R. et al. Testosterone replacement in older hypogonadal men: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 82, 1661–1667 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherrier M. M. et al. Testosterone improves spatial memory in men with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 64, 2063–2068 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wahjoepramono E. J. et al. The Effects of Testosterone Supplementation on Cognitive Functioning in Older Men. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maki P. M. et al. Intramuscular testosterone treatment in elderly men: evidence of memory decline and altered brain function. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 92, 4107–4114 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinty H. L. et al. Cognitive functioning in men receiving androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Support. Care Cancer 22, 2271–2280 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland J., Bandelow S. & Hogervorst E. Testosterone levels and cognition in elderly men: a review. Maturitas 69, 322–337 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vermeulen A. Andropause. Maturitas 34, 5–15 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen N. E., Appleby P. N., Davey G. K. & Key T. J. Lifestyle and nutritional determinants of bioavailable androgens and related hormones in British men. Cancer Causes Control 13, 353–363 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao J. et al. Genetically predicted testosterone and cardiovascular risk factors in men: a Mendelian randomization analysis in the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 43, 140–148 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Au Yeung S. L. et al. Evaluation of moderate alcohol use and cognitive function among men using a Mendelian randomization design in the Guangzhou biobank cohort study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 175, 1021–1028 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gui H. et al. Influence of Alzheimer’s disease genes on cognitive decline: the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2422 e2423–2428 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor A. E. et al. Mendelian randomization in health research: Using appropriate genetic variants and avoiding biased estimates. Econ. Hum. Biol. 13, 99–106 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchetgen Tchetgen E. J., Walter S. & Glymour M. M. Commentary: building an evidence base for mendelian randomization studies: assessing the validity and strength of proposed genetic instrumental variables. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42, 328–331 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce B. L. & Burgess S. Efficient Design for Mendelian Randomization Studies: Subsample and 2-Sample Instrumental Variable Estimators. Am. J. Epidemiol. 178, 1177–1184 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schooling C. M., Au Yeung S. L. & Freeman G. Mendelian randomization estimates may be inflated. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 61, 1931–1931 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett D. A. An introduction to instrumental variables–part 2: Mendelian randomisation. Neuroepidemiology 35, 307–310 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin G. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies a new locus JMJD1C at 10q21 that may influence serum androgen levels in men. Hum. Mol. Genet. 21, 5222–5228 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohlsson C. et al. Genetic determinants of serum testosterone concentrations in men. PLoS Genet. 7, e1002313 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Z. et al. Genome-wide association study of sex hormones, gonadotropins and sex hormone-binding protein in Chinese men. J. Med. Genet. 50, 794–801 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sartorius G. et al. Serum testosterone, dihydrotestosterone and estradiol concentrations in older men self-reporting very good health: the healthy man study. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 77, 755–763 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawlor D. A., Harbord R. M., Sterne J. A., Timpson N. & Davey Smith G. Mendelian randomization: using genes as instruments for making causal inferences in epidemiology. Stat. Med. 27, 1133–1163 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez-Balsells M. M. et al. Clinical review 1: Adverse effects of testosterone therapy in adult men: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 95, 2560–2575 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doody R. S. et al. Identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment in primary care: a feasibility study. Clin. Drug Investig. 31, 483–491 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince M. et al. Dementia diagnosis in developing countries: a cross-cultural validation study. Lancet 361, 909–917 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeCarli C. Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence, prognosis, aetiology, and treatment. Lancet Neurol. 2, 15–21 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Au Yeung S. L. et al. Moderate alcohol use and cognitive function in an elderly Chinese cohort. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59, 172–174 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heys M. et al. Is childhood meat eating associated with better later adulthood cognition in a developing population? Eur. J. Epidemiol. 25, 507–516 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothman K. J., Gallacher J. E. & Hatch E. E. Why representativeness should be avoided. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42, 1012–1014 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao J. et al. Genetically predicted testosterone and systemic inflammation in men: a separate-sample mendelian randomization analysis in older chinese men. PLoS One 10, e0126442 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao J. et al. Genetically predicted testosterone and electrocardiographic QT interval duration in Chinese: a Mendelian randomization analysis in the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Int. J. Epidemiol. 44, 613–620 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haring B. et al. Cardiovascular disease and cognitive decline in postmenopausal women: results from the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2, e000369 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman G., Cowling B. J. & Schooling C. M. Power and sample size calculations for Mendelian randomization studies using one genetic instrument. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42, 1157–1163 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce B. L., Ahsan H. & Vanderweele T. J. Power and instrument strength requirements for Mendelian randomization studies using multiple genetic variants. Int. J. Epidemiol. 40, 740–752 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riley K. P. et al. Prediction of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: longitudinal rates of change in cognition. J. Alzheimers Dis. 25, 707–717 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han L., Cole M., Bellavance F., McCusker J. & Primeau F. Tracking cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease using the mini-mental state examination: a meta-analysis. Int. Psychogeriatr. 12, 231–247 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Low T Center. Symptoms of low testosterone in men. Available at: https://lowtcenter.com/symptoms/. (Accessed: 28th December 2015).
- Johnson R. C. et al. Accounting for multiple comparisons in a genome-wide association study (GWAS). BMC Genomics 11, 724 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perneger T. V. What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ 316, 1236–1238 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feise R. J. Do multiple outcome measures require p-value adjustment? BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2, 8 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankel S., Davison C. & Smith G. D. Lay epidemiology and the rationality of responses to health education. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 41, 428–430 (1991). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke A., Kandler A. & Good D. Women who know their place : sex-based differences in spatial abilities and their evolutionary significance. Hum. Nat. 23, 133–148 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schooling C. M. Could androgens be relevant to partly explain why men have lower life expectancy than women? J. Epidemiol. Community Health (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang C. et al. Cohort profile: The Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study, a Guangzhou-Hong Kong-Birmingham collaboration. Int. J. Epidemiol. 35, 844–852 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welsh K. A., Hoffman J. M., Earl N. L. & Hanson M. W. Neural correlates of dementia: regional brain metabolism (FDG-PET) and the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 9, 395–409 (1994). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Bryant S. E. et al. Detecting dementia with the mini-mental state examination in highly educated individuals. Arch. Neurol. 65, 963–967 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E. & McHugh P. R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198 (1975). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grut M., Fratiglioni L., Viitanen M. & Winblad B. Accuracy of the Mini-Mental Status Examination as a screening test for dementia in a Swedish elderly population. Acta Neurol. Scand. 87, 312–317 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvan J. R. & Cotter V. An evaluation of dementia screening in the primary care setting. J. Am. Acad. Nurse Pract. 18, 351–360 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu G. et al. Adapting mini-mental state examination for dementia screening among illiterate or minimally educated elderly Chinese. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 609–616 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X., Wong W., Lamoureux E. L. & Wong T. Y. Are linear regression techniques appropriate for analysis when the dependent (outcome) variable is not normally distributed? Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 53, 3082–3083 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stock J. H. & Yogo M. Testing for Weak Instruments in Linear IV Regression. (National Bureau of Economic Research, 2002). [Google Scholar]
- Burgess S., Thompson S. G. & Collaboration C. C. G. Avoiding bias from weak instruments in Mendelian randomization studies. Int. J. Epidemiol. 40, 755–764 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaffashian S. et al. Predicting cognitive decline: a dementia risk score vs. the Framingham vascular risk scores. Neurology 80, 1300–1306 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeBlanc E. S. et al. Association between sex steroids and cognition in elderly men. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 72, 393–403 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.