Abstract
Background
Exposure to elevated levels of inflammatory markers during pregnancy has been suggested as possible etiologic factor in the occurrence of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). In this study, we investigated the prospective relation between maternal C-reactive Protein (CRP) during early pregnancy and children's autistic traits in the general population.
Methods
In a large population-based cohort in the Netherlands, we measured maternal CRP levels before 18 weeks of gestation (N=4165). Parents reported on their children's autistic traits at age six years using the Social Responsiveness Scale, and the Pervasive Developmental Problem scale. Regression models were used to examine the relation between maternal CRP levels and autistic traits in children.
Results
Compared to the reference group (CRP <2.3 mg/L), elevated levels of CRP (>7.8 mg/L) in pregnant women were associated with higher Social Responsiveness Scale scores in children (B=0.055, 95% CI 0.033, 0.078); however, the effect was strongly attenuated after adjustment for several socioeconomic factors and in particular by maternal health-related factors including body mass index (fully adjusted model B=0.018, 95% CI -0.005, 0.042). We found no relation between maternal CRP levels and Pervasive Developmental Problem.
Conclusions
Our results suggest that the association between elevated levels of maternal CRP in pregnancy and autistic traits in children is confounded by maternal health-related and socioeconomic factors. Further studies are needed to explore whether other maternal inflammatory markers during pregnancy, as a response to maternal inflammation, are associated with the development of autistic traits in the offspring.
Keywords: C-reactive protein, pregnancy, autistic traits, longitudinal, children
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by impairments in social interactions and communication and repetitive behavior.1 Researchers have found high inheritance estimates for ASD, suggesting that ASD is largely caused by a genetic predisposition.2 However, the increase in the incidence of ASD in the general population – although partly due to the combination of better case definition and/or broadening of the diagnostic concept3– and heritability estimates of maximal 80%,2 highlight the importance of environmental risk factors in the etiology of ASD.
C-reactive protein (CRP) is an inflammatory marker that rises in response to infection or inflammation. Elevated levels of CRP are reported in patients with psychiatric disorders, for example schizophrenia.4-7 Therefore, it is suggested that raised CRP levels, as a result of an underlying inflammatory reaction, may have an etiological role in the development of psychiatric disorders. In addition, several epidemiological studies and animal models suggested an effect of maternal immune system activation during pregnancy on the risk of developing psychiatric diseases in offspring – for example, studies in humans showed a positive association of interferon gamma (IFN-γ), interleukin (IL)-4, IL-5, and IL-6 levels with the risk of developing ASD.8,9 In normal pregnancy, a mild systemic inflammatory reaction occurs in pregnant women which results in activation of the inflammatory network and therefore a raise of inflammatory markers. CRP is an inflammatory marker that is slightly elevated, up to 5.0 mg/L, during almost every normal pregnancy.10,11 Maternal CRP levels in pregnancy are increased as early as at four weeks of gestation, indicating that a maternal inflammatory response occurs when the implantation and development of the placenta takes place.11 However, maternal CRP values higher than 5.0 mg/L may reflect a maternal inflammatory reaction to an underlying infection or a low-grade systemic inflammation. A recent population-based study in a Finnish National Birth cohort by Brown et al. demonstrated an association between maternal CRP levels during early pregnancy and the diagnosis ASD in the offspring.12 They compared the data of 677 mother-child pairs with the diagnosis ASD to a matched control group and they showed that elevated maternal CRP levels (>58.4 mg/L) during the first trimester of pregnancy are related to the risk of developing ASD in the offspring. The authors concluded that a maternal immune reaction during pregnancy, which induces a release of inflammatory cytokines, leads to abnormal fetal brain maturation and, therefore, to the development of ASD in children.
ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder that represents the extreme end of the normal distribution of autistic traits in the general population.13 Autistic traits are defined as clinical deficits in communication, social interaction and repetitive behavior that do not meet the criteria for the diagnosis of ASD.13 Previous studies have shown that autistic traits in the general population are common, heritable, and continuously distributed.13,14 Environmental and genetic risk factors are consistent across the normal distribution of autistic traits, indicating an etiologic overlap between ASD at the end of the spectrum and subthreshold autistic traits.15 Furthermore, several studies have emphasized on the importance of establishing the course and neurobiology of the continuum of the autistic traits in the general population.16,17 To date, it is uncertain whether maternal low-grade inflammation during pregnancy is related to the development of autistic traits across the range of severity in the general population. Consequently, it is not known whether high levels of maternal inflammation markers during pregnancy are a precipitating factor for ASD in susceptible offspring or more generally determine the risk of autistic symptoms.
Against this background, we investigated the prospective relation between elevated levels of maternal high sensitivity CRP during early pregnancy and autistic traits in children at the age of 6 years. We hypothesized that elevated levels of maternal CRP during early pregnancy, as a marker of low-grade systemic inflammation and unspecific immune reaction in the mother, increases the risk of autistic-like traits across the spectrum in the general population. As several studies of CRP point to the importance of confounding,4,7,18,19 we carefully tested whether any relation in our study was confounded by socioeconomic status, life style factors and other health related outcomes.
Methods
Participants
This study was embedded within the Generation R Study, a population-based birth cohort in the Netherlands which follows children from fetal life onwards.20,21 Briefly, pregnant women living in Rotterdam with an expected delivery date between April 2002 and January 2006 were invited to participate. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the Erasmus Medical Center; and written informed consent was obtained from adult participants.
Of 7069 women enrolled in early pregnancy, blood samples were available in 6398 women and CRP was successfully measured in 5321 women. We excluded pregnant women with twin pregnancies (n = 114) due to possible differences in neurodevelopment of children born to singleton or twin pregnancies. This left a total of 5207 women. Of those, 4165 women (80%) reported autistic symptoms in their children at the age of 6 years. These mother-child pairs were included in the analyses.
We compared the 4165 mother-child pairs included in the analyses with the 1042 pairs excluded because of missing data on autistic traits. The maternal CRP levels were not different between mother-child pairs included in the study and those who were excluded. However, we found that the mothers excluded from analyses were younger (mean difference=-3.00 years, p<0.001), less educated (21.9% versus 52.5% higher education, p<0.001), and were mostly of a non-Western ethnic background (59.4% versus 26.7%, p<0.001) than the mothers included in the analyses. In addition, excluded mothers smoked more during pregnancy (26.3% versus 14.6%, p<0.001) and had a higher prenatal psychopathology score (mean difference=0.15, p<0.001) than those who were included. Excluded children were smaller at birth (mean difference=-111 grams in birthweight, p<0.001).
Measurements
Maternal venous blood samples were collected in early pregnancy [mean=13.4 (1.9), range=5.9-17.9 weeks] and were transported to the regional laboratory in Rotterdam, the Netherlands, for further processing and storage. Blood samples were stored at −80°C at one location. High sensitivity CRP concentrations were measured in EDTA plasma samples in one laboratory in 2009. CRP levels were analyzed using an immunoturbidimetric assay on the Architect System (Abbot Diagnostics B.V., Hoofddorp, the Netherlands).22 The total precision (interassay variation) for CRP was 0.9% at 12.9 mg/L and 1.3% at 39.9 mg/L. The lowest level of detection was 0.2 mg/L.
Parents scored autistic traits in their children at age 6 years with the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), and Pervasive Developmental Problem scale (PDP), subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL).
The SRS is a 65-item questionnaire that represents parental observations of a child's social behavior in a naturalistic setting, and provides a quantitative measure of autistic traits in children at the age of 4 to 18 years.23 Higher SRS scores indicate more autistic problems. Several studies investigated the relevance of the SRS as a valid and reliable instrument for assessment of autistic traits in the general population or in the clinical setting.13,23,24 They reported high sensitivity and specificity of the SRS for diagnosis of ASD and confirmed that the SRS is a valid quantitative measure of autistic traits in clinical settings and in large-scale research studies of autism spectrum conditions. To minimize subject burden, we used a short version of the SRS with 18 items. In a sample of 2719 children from the Interactive Autism Network25, the correlation between total scores derived from the 18-item SRS short-form and the complete SRS (65 items) was r=0.99 (p<0.001).
The CBCL is a 99-item questionnaire completed by parents based on their observations of a child's emotions and behavior in the preceding two months.26 The PDP subscale is one of the five scales that can be derived from the CBCL preschool, consistent with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edition (DSM-IV) diagnostic categories. It has a good predictive validity to identify preschoolers at risk of ASD.27 The PDP is a useful screening instrument for autistic problems when compared to the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic.28,29 The correlation between the CBCL PDP and the SRS scores in this study sample was r=0.6, P-value <0.001 (N=4165).
Information on parental age, parental education, maternal ethnic background, marital status, household income, maternal history of smoking and parity was collected during the first prenatal visit in pregnancy using questionnaires. Information on child's history of breast feeding was collected when the children were 6 months.Parental education was defined by the highest completed educational level. We created four categories of parental education level according to the classification of Statistics Netherlands: primary (no education or only primary school), secondary (lower or intermediate vocational education), higher education phase 1 (higher vocational training), and higher education phase 2 (higher academic education). Maternal ethnic background was defined based on the country of birth according to Statistics Netherlands. Household income was divided in three categories: low income (≤ 1200 euro per month), middle income (1200-2000 euro per month), and high income (≥ 2000 euro per month). Maternal smoking was assessed at enrollment and in mid and late pregnancy. We used the Brief Symptom Inventory, a validated self-report questionnaire with 53 items, to measure maternal psychopathology in early pregnancy.30 Brief Symptom Inventory encompasses eight subscales: depression, anxiety, hostility, psychoticism, paranoid ideation, interpersonal sensitivity and obsessive-compulsive. We used the Global Severity Index, an overall measure of maternal psychopathology. Higher scores indicated more problems. Maternal weight and length were measured at enrollment (first prenatal visit) and were used to calculate body mass index (BMI). Information on a child's gender and birthweight was obtained from midwives and hospital registries (at birth). Information on gestational age at birth was established using ultrasound examination during the first prenatal visit of pregnancy.
Statistical analysis
All children with data on maternal CRP in pregnancy and with SRS or PDP assessments were included in the analyses. In our study sample, missing values of the covariates ranged between 0 and 8.9%, except for maternal psychopathology (13.4%) and SRS scores (18.7%). Missing values were imputed using multiple imputations. Thirty copies of the original dataset were generated. In these copies, the missing values were replaced by values randomly generated from the predictive distribution on the basis of the association between the variable with missing values and other variables.
Maternal CRP during early pregnancy was the main determinant in all analyses. We performed the analyses with continuous and categorical CRP. In the continuous analyses, the values for CRP concentrations were divided by standard deviation in the sample, so that the association between CRP levels and autistic traits can be interpreted as standard deviation (SD) change in the determinant. We categorized maternal CRP levels in quartiles (corresponding CRP levels: 2.3, 4.3 and 7.8 mg/L) and CRP levels <2.3 mg/L were considered as the reference group in the analyses.
SRS and PDP scores were the outcomes in the analyses. The SRS scores were transformed using square root to satisfy the assumption of normality. After the transformation of the SRS scores, the residuals of the linear regression models had a normal distribution. To facilitate clinical interpretation, we used the 98th percentile of a Dutch norm group for the CBCL PDP scale as cut-off score to classify children with PDP within the clinical range versus those with scores in the non-clinical range (n=119).31 To examine the relation between maternal CRP and children's autistic traits, we used linear regression for SRS scores and logistic regression for CBCL PDP dichotomized scores.
Identification of the confounders was grounded on understanding of the causal network associated with maternal inflammation and child's autistic traits using a causal diagram.4,5,32 For example existing literature suggests that maternal body mass index is related to both maternal CRP levels32 and children's neurodevelopment33 and could potentially act as a confounder in the analyses. This method is widely used for selection of variables that could interfere with the causal effect of the investigated association in order to control for confounding. For the univariate association of the covariates with SRS and CRP, please see Supplementary Table 1 and 2. The association between maternal CRP and children's autistic traits was examined in five steps: Model 1 adjusted for children's gender and age at the time of assessment. Model 2 additionally adjusted for maternal age, maternal educational levels, maternal ethnic background, marital status, maternal history of smoking and maternal psychopathology in pregnancy. Model 3 additionally adjusted for paternal age, paternal educational levels and household income. Model 4 additionally adjusted for parity, time of blood sampling during pregnancy and maternal body mass index. Model 5 additionally adjusted for children's gestational age, birthweight and breast feeding at 6 months that could potentially mediate the relation between maternal CRP in pregnancy and children's autistic traits.
Gender differences are well established in ASD symptoms; in addition, evidence suggests gender-specific vulnerability to maternal inflammation in pregnancy.34 Therefore, we explored an interaction between children's gender and maternal CRP levels in relation to autistic traits.
Results
Characteristics of the study participants are presented in Table 1. In our sample, the mean age of women at enrollment was 30.7 years. Most of the women completed higher education (52.4%) and had a Dutch ethnic background (61.0%). In total, 14.5% of the mothers continued smoking during pregnancy and 6.1% of the parents were from low income families. Mean gestational age at birth was 40.0 weeks and mean birthweight of the children was 3454 grams. Mean CRP level during early pregnancy was 6.49 mg/L.
Table 1.
N for valid observation | Mean (SD) | |
---|---|---|
Maternal characteristics | ||
Age at enrollment, years | 4165 | 30.7 (4.7) |
Educational levels, % | 4027 | |
No education or primary school | 17.6 | |
Secondary school | 30.0 | |
Higher education phase 1 | 23.7 | |
Higher education phase 2 | 28.7 | |
Ethnicity, % | 4159 | |
Dutch | 61.0 | |
Other Western | 12.4 | |
Non Western | 26.6 | |
Marital Status, married/with partner, % | 3989 | 90.5 |
History of smoking | 4122 | |
Never | 77.1 | |
Quit when pregnancy known | 8.4 | |
Continued in pregnancy | 14.5 | |
Psychopathology in pregnancy, score | 3606 | 0.25 (0.33) |
Paternal characteristics | ||
Age at enrollment, years | 3786 | 33.4 (5.4) |
Educational level, % | 3158 | |
No education or primary school | 19.0 | |
Secondary school | 25.2 | |
Higher education phase 1 | 20.3 | |
Higher education phase 2 | 35.6 | |
Household income, % | 3797 | |
<€1200 | 6.1 | |
>€1200 and <€2000 | 15.0 | |
>€2000 | 78.9 | |
Maternal health characteristics | ||
Parity, primipara, % | 4147 | 59.7 |
Time of blood sampling in pregnancy, weeks | 4165 | 13.4 (1.9) |
Body mass index at enrollment | 4142 | 24.3 (4.2) |
Children's characteristics | ||
Age at time of assessment, months | 3440 | 74 (6) |
Gender, boy, % | 4165 | 49.8 |
Gestational age at birth, weeks | 4165 | 40.0 (1.7) |
Birthweight, grams | 4163 | 3454 (548) |
Breast feeding at 6 months, yes, % | 3331 | 32.9 |
Social Responsiveness Scale, score | 3388 | 0.22 (0.24) |
Pervasive Developmental Problem Scale, score | 4025 | 2.26 (2.54) |
Maternal C reactive protein, mg/L | 4165 | 6.49 (9.38) |
Numbers are mean (SD) unless otherwise indicated.
We found an association between maternal CRP levels and SRS scores in the basic model adjusted for child's gender and age at the time of assessment (CRP >7.8 mg/L; Table 2). However, this association was strongly attenuated after adjustment for confounding factors. Maternal socioeconomic factors including age, educational level, ethnic background, marital status, history of smoking and psychopathology changed the effect size to B= 0.029 (95% CI 0.008, 0.051) (Model 2). Model 3 shows that the paternal socioeconomic factors including age, educational level and household income, did not materially change the effect size in the association of continuous maternal CRP levels with SRS scores (B=0.028, 95% CI −0.006, 0.050). The maternal health related factors such as parity, time of blood sampling during pregnancy and body mass index, additionally attenuated the relation: B=0.019 (95% CI −0.005, 0.042) (Model 4). Model 5 shows that the child related factors including gestational age, birthweight and breast feeding at 6 months did not influence the observed association. Similar results and confounding patterns emerged from the analyses of maternal CRP levels in quintiles with the SRS scores.
Table 2.
Social Responsiveness Scale | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 11 | Model 22 | Model 33 | Model 44 | Model 55 | |
CRP mg/L | B (95% CI) | B (95% CI) | B (95% CI) | B (95% CI) | B (95% CI) |
Continuous6 | 0.009 (0.003, 0.015) | 0.005 (0.000, 0.011) | 0.005 (0.000, 0.011) | 0.004 (−0.002, 0.009) | 0.004 (−0.002, 0.009) |
≤2.3 | Reference | Reference | Reference | Reference | Reference |
2.4-4.3 | 0.008 (−0.013, 0.030) | 0.003 (−0.017, 0.024) | 0.001 (−0.020, 0.022) | −0.001 (−0.022, 0.021) | 0.000 (−0.021, 0.021) |
4.4-7.8 | 0.030 (0.008, 0.052) | 0.014 (−0.007, 0.035) | 0.013 (−0.008, 0.035) | 0.008 (−0.013, 0.030) | 0.008 (−0.014, 0.030) |
>7.8 | 0.055 (0.033, 0.078) | 0.029 (0.008, 0.051) | 0.028 (−0.006, 0.050) | 0.019 (−0.005, 0.042) | 0.018 (−0.005, 0.042) |
Basic model adjusted for children's age at the time of assessment and gender.
Additionally adjusted for maternal age, maternal educational levels, maternal ethnic background, marital status, maternal history of smoking and maternal psychopathology in pregnancy.
Additionally adjusted for paternal age, paternal educational levels and household income.
Additionally adjusted for parity, time of blood sampling during pregnancy and maternal body mass index.
Additionally adjusted for children's gestational age, birthweight and breast feeding at 6 months.
The values for continuous CRP concentrations were divided by standard deviation in the sample.
The B's are not interpretable since the mathematically transformed scores were used in the analyses.
We found no relation between maternal CRP levels and CBCL PDP in the clinical range (fully adjusted model OR 1.4, 95% CI 0.8, 2.4) (Table 3). There was no interaction between maternal CRP and the children's gender in the association with children's autistic traits at age 6 years.
Table 3.
Pervasive Developmental Problems In the Clinical Range | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 11 | Model 22 | Model 33 | Model 44 | Model 55 | |
CRP mg/L | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) |
Continuous6 | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) | 1.4 (0.9, 1.2) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) |
≤2.3 | 1.0 (Reference) | 1.0 (Reference) | 1.0 (Reference) | 1.0 (Reference) | 1.0 (Reference) |
2.4-4.3 | 0.9 (0.6, 1.1) | 0.8 (0.4, 1.4) | 0.8 (0.4, 1.4) | 0.7 (0.4, 1.4) | 0.8 (0.4, 1.4) |
4.4-7.8 | 1.3 (0.8, 2.3) | 1.2 (0.7, 2.1) | 1.2 (0.7, 2.1) | 1.1 (0.7, 2.0) | 1.1 (0.6, 2.0) |
>7.8 | 1.7 (1.1, 2.8) | 1.5 (0.9, 2.6) | 1.5 (0.9, 2.6) | 1.4 (0.8, 2.4) | 1.4 (0.8, 2.4) |
Pervasive Developmental Problems in the Clinical Range were defined as scores above the 98th percentile of a Dutch norm population
Basic model adjusted for children's age at the time of assessment and gender.
Additionally adjusted for maternal age, maternal educational levels, maternal ethnic background, marital status, maternal history of smoking and maternal psychopathology in pregnancy.
Additionally adjusted for paternal age, paternal educational levels and household income.
Additionally adjusted for parity, time of blood sampling during pregnancy and maternal body mass index.
Additionally adjusted for children's gestational age, birthweight and breast feeding at 6 months.
The values for continuous CRP concentrations were divided by standard deviation in the sample.
Pervasive developmental problems were defined as the scores in the highest 2% of the Child Behavior Checklist pervasive developmental problems scale (n=119)
Comment
In this large, population-based study, we found no relation between elevated levels of maternal CRP during early pregnancy and parent-reported autistic traits in children at the age of 6 years. Our findings suggest that the relation between maternal CRP levels and child autistic traits is strongly confounded by maternal socioeconomic and health-related factors such as educational level and body mass index.
Several studies examined the effect of elevated maternal inflammatory markers during pregnancy, as a response to a maternal infection or inflammation, on the risk of autism in children. For example, Goines et al., and Parker-Athill and colleagues examined the effect of high maternal cytokine profiles on the development of offspring psychiatric diseases and found an association between elevated maternal IL-4, IL-5, IL-6 and IFN-γ during pregnancy and the development of ASD in children.8,9 In addition, a population-based study in a national birth cohort by Brown et al. reported an association between elevated levels of maternal CRP in early pregnancy and the diagnosis of ASD in children.12 These results suggest an association between raised maternal inflammatory markers during pregnancy and the development of ASD in children. Therefore, we investigated the prospective relation between elevated levels of maternal high sensitivity CRP during early pregnancy, as a marker of low-grade systemic inflammation and unspecific immune reaction in the mother, and autistic traits across the whole spectrum in the children at the age of 6 years. However, after adjustment for confounding factors we did not found an association between maternal CRP levels during pregnancy and the risk of autistic traits in children.
Our results differ from the findings of the study by Brown and colleagues12 on the prospective association of maternal CRP levels during pregnancy and a diagnosis of ASD in children. There are two possible explanations for these different results. First, in this study, the outcome measure was autistic traits in children, while in the study by Brown et al. the outcome measure was the diagnosis ASD. Despite the fact that socio-economic and genetic risk factors are suggested to be associated with the continuum of autistic symptoms –and ASD at the extreme end of this continuum–, our findings on autistic traits in a non-clinical population-based sample might differ from the results found in a national cohort including children diagnosed with ASD. Second, Brown et al. considered several confounding factors in their study including child related factors (preterm birth and low birthweight), parental socioeconomic factors (maternal and paternal age, number of previous births, socioeconomic status and gestational time of maternal blood sampling) and parental history of psychiatric disorders. However, maternal educational level as socioeconomic factor and maternal body mass index as health related factor, were not included in the analyses. As the present study suggests that these factors are important confounders in studies investigating maternal CRP and children's autistic traits, residual confounding remains an alternative explanation for the inconsistencies between the studies.
Observational studies as the study by Brown et al. or the present study attempt to control confounding by conditioning the associations on a large number of factors associated with maternal CRP levels or children's neurodevelopment. Nevertheless, the association of interest may be further influenced by other unmeasured paternal factor or an unmeasured maternal health conditions that precede both the exposure and outcome. Furthermore, socioeconomic status or general health are factors with many levels and a model that uses a surrogate measure of socioeconomic status or health could possibly fail to completely block the confounding paths. Therefore, observational studies, even those considering a large number of socioeconomic and health-related confounding factors, cannot fully address the causality of the association between elevated levels of CRP and various health outcomes. To evaluate the possible causality of intrauterine influences, it has been proposed to compare the associations of maternal exposures (e.g. CRP levels) to those of the respective paternal exposures on behavioral outcomes in the offspring. Any similarity between the results would point to the confounding effect of familial or shared environmental factors, such as socioeconomic status. However, Mendialian randomization, in which natural assortment of genetic material at conception is used for randomization of exposed and non-exposed groups is probably the only approach to rule out that results are explained by residual confounding in studies on CRP.35
To our knowledge, the present study is one of the largest prospective population-based, non-clinical studies that examine the effect of elevated CRP levels in mothers during early pregnancy on autistic traits in children. However, the study has several limitations. First, we measured maternal CRP levels only at one time point in pregnancy. As a result of the relatively short half-life time of CRP (<12 hours), it was not possible to determine whether the elevated CRP levels were a result of an acute or chronic maternal inflammatory reaction. Only if data on repeated measurements of CRP are available during pregnancy, one is able to distinguish consequences of acute inflammation and chronic conditions in pregnant women. Second, there was no measure of maternal autistic traits available. Third, as in many observational studies we observed two types of non-response effect: non-response at baseline and loss to follow-up that could introduce bias in the association, and influence the generalizability of the findings. However, loss to follow-up was not associated with the CRP levels.
In conclusion, the findings of the present study suggest that the relation between maternal CRP levels and child autistic traits is strongly confounded by maternal socioeconomic and health-related factors. Residual confounding remains a major problem on associations with CRP levels. Further studies with more specific inflammatory markers are needed to find out whether a raised maternal inflammatory marker during pregnancy, as a response to maternal inflammation, predicts for the development of autistic traits in children.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgement
The general design of Generation R Study is made possible by financial support from the Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw 10.000.1003), the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport and the Ministry of Youth and Families. The authors gratefully acknowledge the contribution of general practitioners, hospitals, midwifes, and pharmacies in Rotterdam. The work of Dr. Tiemeier and Dr. Ghassabian was supported by a research grant sponsored by the European Community's 7th Framework Programme (FP7/2008–2013) under grant number 212652 (NUTRIMENTHE project, “The Effect of Diet on the Mental Performance of Children”). H. Tiemeier was also supported by the VIDI grant of ZonMw (2009–017.106.370). Also, A. Ghassabian was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD).
Footnotes
Financial Disclosure: Dr. Frank C. Verhulst is the contributing editor of the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment, from which he receives remuneration. For the other authors, no potentially competing financial interest is declared.
References
- 1.Levy SE, Mandell DS, Schultz RT. Autism. The Lancet. 2009;374:1627–1638. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61376-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chaste P, Leboyer M. Autism risk factors: genes, environment, and gene-environment interactions. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2012;14:281–292. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.3/pchaste. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schieve LA, Rice C, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Boyle CA, Kogan MD, Drews C, et al. Parent-reported prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in US-born children: an assessment of changes within birth cohorts from the 2003 to the 2007 National Survey of Children's Health. Matern and Child Health Journal. 2012;16(Suppl 1):S151–157. doi: 10.1007/s10995-012-1004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bjerkeset O, Romild U, Smith GD, Hveem K. The associations of high levels of C-reactive protein with depression and myocardial infarction in 9258 women and men from the HUNT population study. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:345–352. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dickerson F, Stallings C, Origoni A, Vaughan C, Khushalani S, Yolken R. Additive effects of elevated C-reactive protein and exposure to Herpes Simplex Virus type 1 on cognitive impairment in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2012;134:83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hedges DW, Farrer TJ, Brown BL. Association between C-reactive protein and cognitive deficits in elderly men and women: a meta-analysis. International Psychogeriatrics. 2012;24:1387–1392. doi: 10.1017/S1041610211002419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Liukkonen T, Rasanen P, Jokelainen J, Leinonen M, Jarvelin MR, Meyer-Rochow VB, et al. The association between anxiety and C-reactive protein (CRP) levels: results from the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort study. European Psychiatry. 2011. 26:363–369. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Parker-Athill EC, Tan J. Maternal immune activation and autism spectrum disorder: interleukin-6 signaling as a key mechanistic pathway. Neurosignals. 2010;18:113–128. doi: 10.1159/000319828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Goines PE, Croen LA, Braunschweig D, Yoshida CK, Grether J, Hansen R, et al. Increased midgestational IFN-gamma, IL-4 and IL-5 in women bearing a child with autism: A case-control study. Molecular Autism. 2011;2:13–24. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-2-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sacks GP, Seyani L, Lavery S, Trew G. Maternal C-reactive protein levels are raised at 4 weeks gestation. Human Reproduction. 2004;19:1025–1030. doi: 10.1093/humrep/deh179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Watts DH, Krohn MA, Wener MH, Eschenbach DA. C-reactive protein in normal pregnancy. Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1991;77:176–180. doi: 10.1097/00006250-199102000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Brown AS, Sourander A, Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki S, McKeague IW, Sundvall J, Surcel HM. Elevated maternal C-reactive protein and autism in a national birth cohort. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014;19:259–264. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:524–530. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ronald A, Hoekstra RA. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2011;156B:255–274. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Robinson EB, Koenen KC, McCormick MC, et al. Evidence that autistic traits show the same etiology in the general population and at the quantitative extremes (5%, 2.5%, and 1%). Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68:1113–1121. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Blanken LM, Mous SE, Ghassabian A, Muetzel RL, Schoemaker NK, El Marroun H, et al. Cortical morphology in 6- to 10-year old children with autistic traits: a population-based neuroimaging study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2015;172:479–486. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Whitehouse AJ, Hickey M, Ronald A. Are autistic traits in the general population stable across development? PLoS One. 2011;6:e23029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Davey Smith G, Lawlor DA, Harbord R, Timpson N, Rumley A, Lowe GD, et al. Association of C-reactive protein with blood pressure and hypertension: life course confounding and mendelian randomization tests of causality. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology. 2005;25:1051–1056. doi: 10.1161/01.ATV.0000160351.95181.d0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wensley F, Gao P, Burgess S, Kaptoge S, Di Angelantonio E, Shah T, et al. Association between C reactive protein and coronary heart disease: mendelian randomisation analysis based on individual participant data. British Medical Journal. 2011;342:d548. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tiemeier H, Velders FP, Szekely E, Roza SJ, Dieleman G, Jaddoe VW, et al. The Generation R Study: A review of design, findings to date, and a study of the 5-HTTLPR by environmental interaction from fetal life onward. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51:1119–1135. e1117. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jaddoe VW, van Duijn CM, Franco OH, van der Heijden AJ, van Iizendoorn MH, de Jongste JC, et al. The Generation R Study: design and cohort update 2012. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2012;27:739–756. doi: 10.1007/s10654-012-9735-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.de Jonge LL, Steegers EA, Ernst GD, Lindemans J, Russcher H, Hofman A, et al. C-reactive protein levels, blood pressure and the risks of gestational hypertensive complications: the Generation R Study. Journal of Hypertension. 2011;29:2413–2421. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0b013e32834c58e5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Constantino JN, Davis SA, Todd RD, Schindler MK, Gross MM, Brophy SL, et al. Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33:427–433. doi: 10.1023/a:1025014929212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bölte S, Westerwald E, Holtmann M, Freitag C, Poustka F. Autistic traits and autism spectrum disorders: The clinical validity of two measures presuming a continuum of social communication skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011;41:66–72. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1024-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Daniels AM, Rosenberg RE, Anderson C, Law JK, Marvin AR, Law PA. Verification of parent-report of child autism spectrum disorder diagnosis to a web-based autism registry. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disordors. 2012;42:257–265. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1236-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; Burlington, VT: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Muratori F, Narzisi A, Tancredi R, Cosenza A, Calugi S, Saviozzi I, et al. The CBCL 1.5-5 and the identification of preschoolers with autism in Italy. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences. 2011;20:329–338. doi: 10.1017/s204579601100045x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rescorla L, Kim YA, Oh KJ. Screening for ASD with the Korean CBCL/1(1/2)-5. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disordors. 2015;45(12):4039–50. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2255-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sikora DM, Hall TA, Hartley SL, Gerrard-Morris AE, Cagle S. Does parent report of behavior differ across ADOS-G classifications: analysis of scores from the CBCL and GARS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disordors. 2008;38:440–448. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0407-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.de Beurs E. Brief Symptom Inventory, handleiding. PITS; Leiden, the Netherlands: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tick NT, van der Ende J, Koot HM, Verhulst FC. 14-year changes in emotional and behavioral problems of very young Dutch children. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46:1333–1340. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3181337532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ernst GDS, de Jonge LL, Hofman A, Lindemans J, Russcher H, Steegers EAP, et al. C-reactive protein levels in early pregnancy, fetal growth patterns, and the risk for neonatal complications: the Generation R Study. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2011;205:132, e131–132, e112. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2011.03.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jo H, Schieve LA, Sharma AJ, Hinkle SN, Li R, Lind JN. Maternal prepregnancy body mass index and child psychosocial development at 6 years of age. Pediatrics. 2015;135:e1198–1209. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bronson SL, Bale TL. Prenatal stress-induced increases in placental inflammation and offspring hyperactivity are male-specific and ameliorated by maternal antiinflammatory treatment. Endocrinology. 2014;155:2635–2646. doi: 10.1210/en.2014-1040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Thanassoulis G. Mendelian randomization: how genetics is pushing the boundaries of epidemiology to identify new causes of heart disease. Canadian Journal of Cardiology. 2013;29:30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cjca.2012.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.