Skip to main content
Comparative Medicine logoLink to Comparative Medicine
. 2016 Feb;66(1):4–9.

Subregional Expression of Hippocampal Glutamatergic and GABAergic Genes in F344 Rats with Social Isolation after Weaning

Hisaya Iwata 1, Yutaka Yamamuro 1,*
PMCID: PMC4752030  PMID: 26884404

Abstract

Many studies have shown that postweaning social isolation (pwSI) alters various behavioral phenotypes, including hippocampus-dependent tasks. Here, we report the comprehensive analysis of the expression of glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission-related genes in the distinct hippocampal subregions of pwSI rats. Male F344 rats (age, 4 wk) experienced either pwSI or group housing (controls). At 7 wk of age, the hippocampus of each rat was removed and laser-microdissected into the CA1 and CA3 layers of pyramidal cells and the granule cell layer of the dentate gyrus. Subsequently, the expression of glutamatergic- and GABAergic-related genes was analyzed by quantitative RT-PCR. In the CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers, 18 of 24 glutamate receptor subunit genes were at least 1.5-fold increased in expression after pwSI. In particular, the expression of several N-methyl-D-aspartate and kainate receptors (for example, Grin2a in CA1, Grik4 in CA3) was significantly increased after pwSI. In contrast, pwSI tended to decrease the expression of GABAA receptor subunit genes, and Gabra1, Gabra2, Gabra4, Gabra5, Gabrb2, Gabrg1, and Gabrg2 were all significantly decreased in expression compared with the levels in the group-housed rats. These results indicate a subregion-specific increase of glutamate receptors and reduction of GABAA receptors, suggesting that the hippocampal circuits of pwSI rats may be in more excitable states than those of group-housed rats.

Abbreviations: AMPA, α-amino-2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-3-oxo-4-isoxazolepropanoic acid; LMD, laser microdissection; NMDA;N-methyl-D-aspartic acid; pwSI, postweaning social isolation; DG;dentate gyrus


Behavioral phenotypes differ among strains of laboratory animals, including mice and rats. The acquisition of strain-specific behavioral traits is governed not only by the genetic background but also by the postnatal rearing environment. The level of maternal care is a representative environmental factor. For example, inbred Fischer 344 pups raised by Wistar dams showed Wistar-like behavioral traits in adulthood in socially interactive situations.35 Furthermore, the effect of environmental factors is not limited to neonatal periods. In animals that form a society among conspecifics, such as mice and rats, postweaning social environments have a marked effect on behavioral traits. Many studies have reported that postweaning social isolation (pwSI) alters various strain-specific behavioral phenotypes, including aggressiveness,15,32,34 novelty preference,22 locomotor activity,29 anxiety-like behavior,20 and learning and memory.13,15,20,31 In other words, pwSI means the deprivation of several social interactions among conspecifics. The expression of social contact begins fundamentally in the postweaning periods, its frequency increases to a peak at 4 to 5 wk of age, and it declines thereafter until sexual maturity.2,17,30 Therefore, rats were isolated from their conspecifics to deprive them of social contact during the current study.

In the brain, the hippocampus is necessary for the acquisition of episodic and spatial memory,28 but the influences of pwSI on hippocampal functions remain largely unclear. The hippocampus has a lamellar organization of neurons, and the intrinsic neuronal circuit of the hippocampus, termed the trisynaptic circuit, consists of 3 topographically and morphologically distinct neuronal layers: the pyramidal cell layer in subfields CA1 and CA3 and the granule cell layer in the dentate gyrus (DG). Sensory information is carried first to the DG by a perforant pathway that originates in the entorhinal cortex. DG granule cells project to the apical dendrites of the CA3 pyramidal cells through mossy fibers. In turn, CA3 pyramidal cells project to the CA1 layer through Schaffer collaterals.1,18 Glutamate is a key excitatory neurotransmitter in the hippocampus and plays a central role in the activation of the trisynaptic circuit, whereas the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA modulates the activated circuit. This excitatory–inhibitory balance is critical for the appropriate functioning of the hippocampal circuit. Here, we comprehensively investigated the effect of pwSI on the expression of glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission-related genes in the 3 hippocampal subregions of inbred F344 rats.

Materials and Methods

All experiments were performed in accordance with the Guidelines for Animal Experiments (College of Bioresource Sciences, Nihon University) and Nihon University's Animal Care and Use Committees. Male F344/DuCrlCrlj rats (age, 3 wk; SPF, viral antigen-free) were purchased from Charles River Laboratories Japan (Yokohama, Japan). Rats were housed in opaque polypropylene cages (345 × 403 × 177 mm) and allowed free access to food (Labo MR Stock, Nosan, Kanagawa, Japan) and water. The rats were housed in an animal room maintained at 22 ± 1 °C with a humidity of 555 ± 10% and a 14:10-h light:dark cycle (lights on, 0600). When rats had reached 4 wk of age, they were allocated to 1 of 2 rearing conditions: isolated (pwSI; 1 rat per cage, n = 3) or group-housed (3 rats / cage, n = 3, with 1 rat randomly obtained from each cage) rearing. Rats were euthanized at 7 wk of age by CO2, and whole brains were removed. The trimmed brains were embedded in OCT compound and stored at –80 °C.

To separate the 3 hippocampal subregions, we used a laser microdissection system (LMD; model LMD7000-4, Leica Microsystems, Tokyo, Japan). First, coronal sections including the hippocampus (30 µm) were cut by using a cryostat (HM550; Thermo Fischer Scientific, Yokohama, Japan) at –10 °C and mounted on membrane slides (Membrane Slides Nuclease and Human Nucleic Acid Free PEN-Membrane, Leica Microsystems). According to the manufacturer's (Leica Microsystems) recommendations, the sections were fixed in a mixture of acetate and ethanol (1:19, v/v), and briefly stained with toluidine blue. The slides were air-dried and set within the LMD system. The CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers and DG granule cell layers were cut according to the Allen Brain Atlas http://mouse.brain-map.org/static/atlas) and collected separately into the caps of 0.2-mL microtubes (BT-02LC; Ina-Optica, Osaka, Japan) supplied with the LMD system.

The quantitative RT-PCR protocol was modified from previous methods.36 The collected specimens were completely dissolved in 9.5 mL of the lysis buffer provided with CellAmp Direct RNA Prep Kit (Takara Bio, Ohtsu, Japan) supplemented with proteinase K (0.3 U, Takara Bio). DNA in the lysate was digested by adding 1 mL of DNase provided with the kit and incubating at 37 °C for 5 min. Then, distilled water (UltraPure DNase/RNase-Free Distilled Water; Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA) was added to the lysate, which was incubated at 75 °C for 5 min to inactivate DNase. The cDNA was synthesized by using the iScript cDNA Synthesis Kit (BioRad, Hercules, CA) according to the manufacturer's instructions. Real-time PCR was performed by using Mini Opticon (BioRad) with SsoFast EvaGreen qPCR SuperMix (BioRad). Gene-specific primers were designed by using Primer Blast (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/tools/primer-blast/), and all primer sequences are listed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sequences of the primers used in quantitative RT-PCR

Primer Forward (5′→3′) Reverse (5′→3′) Accession no.
Gria1 CTTCATGCAGCAAGGATGTG TGGCTGTGTACGAGGAGATG NM_031608
Gria2 AGGTGGCAAAGAATCCACAG GCCTTGCTCCTCATGACATC NM_017261
Gria3 GGGAAGCATTCAAGAGGCTA CTGCTGAACCATTGGGTTTT NM_032990
Gria4 TTTGCAGGCAGATTGTCTTG TGGTCTGTGTTTCGGATGAA NM_017263
Grin1 TCCAGCTTCAAGAGACGTAGG CTCTCCCTATGACGGGAACA NM_017010
Grin2a CTGGAAGAGGCAGATTGACC TCTTCTCGTTGTGGCAGATG NM_012573
Grin2b GCATGCCTACATGGGAAAGT GTTGAGCACAGCTGCATCAT NM_012574
Grin2c GACTTCCTGCTGGCTTTCAG CTGCAGCATCTTCAGCACAT NM_012575
Grin2d TCTTCGCCACCACTGGTTAT ATTGTGGCAGATCCCTGAAA NM_022797
Grin3a GAAGAAAAGCAGCCACGTTC GGTTTTGTCCTTCCTCGTCA NM_138546
Grin3b AGGACCTGGCCTTTGACTTT TTGATGCTGAAGCTGGTCAC NM_133308
Grik1 TGGCAAATATGGAGCACAGA GTCATGAAGGGCTTGGAGAA NM_017241
Grik2 TGGAGGATGGGAAATATGGA CGATGACCTTCTCACGAACA NM_019309
Grik3 CGGAGTCTGGTTTGGGAATA CGTCCGCGTACTCAAAGATT NM_181373
Grik4 GCTCCAGCATGACCTTCTTC CCCTCCTCTGTGCTCTTCAC NM_012572
Grik5 TTCTCCTTCCTGGACCCTTT GAAGACACGGGTGAGGGTTA NM_031508
Grm1 GGTCCCTTCTGACACTTTGC CATTCCACTCTCGCCGTAAT NM_017011
Grm2 GGTAACTCACGGGGACAGAA TCACCAGCCCACTGATGTAA NM_001105711
Grm3 CCAAGCTCTGTGATGCAATG CATCCCGTCTCCAAAAGTGT NM_001105712
Grm4 TCAAGAAGGGAAGCCACATC CACAGCGTCAATCACGAACT NM_022666
Grm5 GTTTGCACAGGAGAACAGCA AGGGACATCTGCATATTGTGG NM_017012
Grm6 GAGGCCTTTGTGCAGATTTC CCGGATCACTTTGTGGAACT NM_022920
Grm7 CCAGGGCGTTATGACATCTT AGGATGGGATCTCTCGGACT NM_031040
Grm8 CGAGCAAGGGAAGAAATCTG CCACGACAAACCACACAAAC NM_022202
Slc17a6 TCAACCAGCTTATTTCGAGGA ATTGTCATGACCAGGTGTGG NM_053427
Slc17a7 CTGGTGGTCGGATACTCACA ATTGGAAATGCCCATCAAGA NM_053859
Slc17a8 ACGGGATGTGGAGTAAGTGG TACTCCTGCAAGGGGCATAG NM_153725
Slc1a1 TTCATAGTCGTGCGGAAGAA CACAGCGGAATGTAACTGGA NM_013032
Slc1a6 CATGCTGGTGGTGTGTTACC AAGAGGTACCCATGGCTGTG NM_032065
Gls CTTGACAAGATGGGCAACAG TCCTTCTCTCCGAGGATCAA NM_012569
Gls2 AACAAGACCGTGGTGAACCT TGCGGGAATCATAGTCCTTC NM_138904
Gabra1 TCTGAGCACTCTCTCGGGAA AGTTACACGCTCTCCCAAGC NM_183326
Gabra2 CGTATGGTTTCCGCTGCTTG GTAATGCTGTCTCCCAGTCCT NM_001135779
Gabra3 CCAAGGGGAGTCAAGACGAC AGTCACTGCATCTCCAAGCC NM_017069
Gabra4 TGTGCCTGGCGACTTGTTTA CCAAATCCAGGACGCAGTCT NM_080587
Gabra5 TGTCCGACACGGAAATGGAAT TGTTGAGAGGGAGACGTTGC NM_017295
Gabra6 AACGCTGACTGTCCGATGAG TCGGATAGGCATAGCTCCCA NM_021841
Gabrb1 ACAACAGGGGCATATCCACG CAGTGTGGAGGGCATGTAGG NM_012956
Gabrb2 TGTCAACAAGATGGACCCACA ATGCTGGAGGCATCATAGGC NM_012957
Gabrb3 CTGTACGGGCTCAGGATCAC CGATCCTTTCCACGCCAGTA NM_017065
Gabrg1 AAACATGGGTCTTGGCACCT ATTACTGTGGGTCTCACGCC NM_080586
Gabrg2 GAAAAACCCTGCCCCTACCA TGCGAATGTGTATCCTCCCG NM_183327
Gabrg3 GAGGCTCACTGGATCACCAC GGGGTAGCCATAGCTAGAGA NM_024370
Gabrd GCGCCAGAGCAATGAATGAC GCGTAGCCCTCCATTAGTCC NM_017289
Gabbr1 CTCTCGGGCTGGATGGTTAC GGCTCTAGGGTCTTCCTCCA NM_031028
Gabbr2 CAGGGAAGACTCCACAGCAG AGGCGTACCCATGGAACTTG NM_031802
Slc32a1 CCGTCGAGGGAGACATTCATT CACGAACATGCCCTGAATGG NM_031782
Slc6a1 ACAGCCAGTTCTGTACCGTG CACACGGCAGCAATGAAGAG NM_024371
Gad1 GATGGTTTTCGATGGT CCATGGTTGTTCCTGA NM_008077
Gad2 GTGTTCGATGGGAAGCCTCA TAATCACTGGCGCCACCTTT NM_012563
Gapdh ATGACTCTACCCACGGCAAG TACTCAGCACCAGCATCACC NM_017008

Statistical significance was determined by using 2-way ANOVA, and differences between control and pwSI groups when comparing the same region were tested in subsequent planned comparisons, contrasting the mean comparisons of selected levels of a factor, by using the software package Super ANOVA (Abacus Concepts, Berkeley, CA). The criterion for significance was a P value less than 0.05 in all cases.

Results

The glutamatergic neurotransmission-related genes measured in the present study were α-amino-2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-3-oxo-4-isoxazolepropanoic acid (AMPA) receptor subunits (Gria1Gria4), NMDA receptor subunits (Grin1Grin3b), kainate receptor subunits (Grik1Grik5), metabotropic receptor subunits (Grm1Grm8), vesicular glutamate transporters (Slc17a6–Slc17a8), membrane glutamate transporters (Slc1a1, Slc1a6), and glutamate synthases (Gls, Gls2). These results are shown in Table 2.

Table 2.

Effect of pwSI on glutamatergic neuron-related gene expressions in distinct hippocampal regions of F344 rats

CA1
CA3
DG
Group pwSI Group pwSI Group pwSI
Gria1 1.7E+00 ± 3.0E–01 1.8E+00 ± 3.0E–01 5.3E–01 ± 4.9E–02 7.5E–01 ± 1.2E–02 1.6E+00 ± 2.7E–01 1.9E+00 ± 3.5E–01
Gria2 6.1E–02 ± 1.5E–02 5.9E–02 ± 8.6E–03 1.7E–02 ± 1.3E–03 1.9E–02 ± 5.7E–04 7.2E–02 ± 5.6E–03 5.5E–02 ± 5.7E–03
Gria3 4.8E–02 ± 9.5E–03 3.1E–02 ± 7.1E–03a 1.2E–02 ± 1.1E–03 1.1E–02 ± 4.9E–04 3.4E–02 ± 5.7E–03 4.3E–02 ± 4.1E–03
Gria4 3.4E–01 ± 7.5E–02 4.7E–01 ± 8.8E–02 9.8E–02 ± 1.1E–02 1.6E–01 ± 2.2E–02 6.4E–01 ± 1.7E–01 8.1E–01 ± 1.6E–01
Grin1 6.6E–02 ± 1.4E–02 8.7E–02 ± 1.9E–02 3.4E–02 ± 5.6E–03 4.7E–02 ± 1.5E–03 6.9E–02 ± 1.0E–02 6.7E–02 ± 4.7E–03
Grin2a 3.0E+00 ± 1.4E–02 5.0E+00 ± 4.7E–01c 9.6E–01 ± 1.6E–01 1.5E+00 ± 8.4E–02 2.5E+00 ± 3.4E–01 2.4E+00 ± 3.3E–01
Grin2b 3.1E+00 ± 7.8E–01 3.9E+00 ± 7.3E–01 1.1E+00 ± 4.9E–02 1.7E+00 ± 2.5E–01 3.4E+00 ± 4.1E–01 3.7E+00 ± 2.5E–01
Grin2c 9.2E–02 ± 1.6E–02 1.1E–01 ± 1.5E–02 3.6E–02 ± 6.4E–03 4.3E–02 ± 5.1E–03 6.4E–02 ± 8.2E–03 8.4E–02 ± 2.4E–02
Grin2d 5.1E–03 ± 4.7E–04 9.1E–03 ± 1.6E–03 3.3E–03 ± 4.1E–04 3.3E–03 ± 4.0E–04 1.3E–02 ± 2.7E–03 1.2E–02 ± 1.4E–03
Grin3a 9.0E–03 ± 2.7E–03 1.3E–02 ± 1.3E–03 2.5E–03 ± 3.8E–04 2.6E–03 ± 2.3E–04 1.0E–02 ± 1.5E–03 6.3E–03 ± 4.7E–04
Grin3b nd nd nd nd nd nd
Grik1 1.6E–01 ± 7.1E–02 2.8E–01 ± 7.6E–02 6.8E–02 ± 5.6E–03 1.9E–01 ± 2.7E–02 4.0E–01 ± 7.6E–02 5.9E–01 ± 8.6E–02
Grik2 4.9E–01 ± 8.0E–02 7.5E–01 ± 9.4E–02 4.9E–01 ± 7.4E–02 8.5E–01 ± 2.0E–01 2.6E+00 ± 3.8E–01 2.6E+00 ± 3.8E–01
Grik3 5.3E–04 ± 1.2E–04 6.3E–04 ± 1.2E–04 2.9E–04 ± 1.8E–05 3.7E–04 ± 4.2E–05 2.9E–03 ± 3.7E–04 2.8E–03 ± 3.5E–04
Grik4 4.7E–02 ± 1.7E–02 4.6E–02 ± 5.3E–03 1.1E+00 ± 9.4E–02 1.7E+00 ± 2.2E–01b 7.3E–01 ± 1.6E–01 1.0E+00 ± 2.6E–01
Grik5 1.1E+01 ± 2.2E+00 1.3E+01 ± 2.3E+00 6.5E+00 ± 2.8E–01 1.0E+01 ± 1.0E+00 1.3E+01 ± 2.0E+00 1.7E+01 ± 1.4E+00a
Grm1 1.4E–03 ± 2.7E–04 2.3E–03 ± 3.7E–04 1.3E–02 ± 1.4E–04 2.2E–02 ± 3.1E–03 6.4E–02 ± 7.2E–03 7.9E–02 ± 1.6E–03a
Grm2 nd nd nd nd 9.2E–03 ± 1.1E–03 7.6E–03 ± 3.0E–04
Grm3 1.4E–02 ± 1.2E–03 1.4E–02 ± 4.0E–03 6.7E–03 ± 8.0E–04 7.7E–03 ± 7.3E–04 2.6E–02 ± 4.1E–03 3.6E–02 ± 7.9E–03
Grm4 6.8E–03 ± 3.0E–03 1.1E–02 ± 3.5E–03 1.3E–02 ± 1.0E–03 1.5E–02 ± 5.1E–03 1.6E–02 ± 3.5E–03 1.7E–02 ± 1.4E–03
Grm5 1.1E–01 ± 7.9E–03 2.6E–01 ± 2.2E–01 1.3E–02 ± 1.6E–03 4.9E–02 ± 1.9E–02 1.9E–02 ± 5.7E–04 2.2E–02 ± 3.0E–03
Grm6 2.9E–03 ± 1.3E–03 4.4E–03 ± 2.3E–03 1.0E–03 ± 5.0E–04 1.8E–03 ± 1.8E–04 nd nd
Grm7 1.2E–02 ± 1.7E–03 1.5E–02 ± 3.1E–03 9.3E–03 ± 1.9E–04 1.2E–02 ± 2.0E–03 2.2E–02 ± 4.4E–03 2.8E–02 ± 2.1E–03
Grm8 nd nd nd nd 2.5E–03 ± 6.9E–04 5.1E–03 ± 1.1E–03
Slc17a6 2.4E–03 ± 3.4E–04 2.4E–03 ± 7.0E–04 7.6E–04 ± 1.5E–04 4.3E–04 ± 1.0E–04 nd nd
Slc17a7 5.9E–02 ± 6.1E–03 5.9E–02 ± 6.1E–03 5.5E–02 ± 3.4E–03 4.2E–02 ± 4.3E–03 4.4E–02 ± 3.7E–03 6.0E–02 ± 6.4E–03
Slc17a8 2.1E–04 ± 6.8E–05 4.1E–04 ± 1.7E–04 2.9E–04 ± 1.4E–04 4.3E–04 ± 2.5E–04 5.6E–04 ± 2.1E–04 9.4E–04 ± 2.1E–04
Slc1a1 3.4E–02 ± 3.2E–03 4.4E–02 ± 6.8E–03 1.5E–02 ± 3.8E–04 1.4E–02 ± 2.8E–03 3.1E–02 ± 3.8E–03 4.7E–02 ± 1.0E–02a
Slc1a6 nd nd nd nd 1.2E–03 ± 1.8E–04 2.1E–03 ± 5.9E–04
Gls 1.7E–02 ± 8.2E–04 1.1E–02 ± 1.7E–03a 1.9E–02 ± 2.8E–03 1.8E–02 ± 3.2E–03 1.4E–02 ± 1.4E–03 2.1E–02 ± 2.7E–03
Gls2 1.6E–02 ± 3.1E–03 2.2E–02 ± 6.7E–03 7.4E–03 ± 8.0E–04 1.0E–02 ± 2.1E–03 1.1E–02 ± 5.1E–03 1.1E–02 ± 7.3E–04

nd, not detected

Data are shown as the mean ± SEM (n = 3) expression level relative to that of Gapdh.

Bold values indicate that the mean of pwSI/group > 1.5

Italicized values indicate that the mean of pwSI/group < 0.7

Values differ significantly (aP < 0.05, bP < 0.01, and P < 0.001) between group-housed and pwSI rats.

In CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers, 18 of the 24 glutamate receptor subunit genes evaluated were at least 1.5-fold increased (P < 0.05) in expression in pwSI rats compared with group-housed rats, and only Gria3 was decreased significantly (P < 0.05) by pwSI. In particular, Grin2a (P = 0.0003) in CA1 and Grik4 (P = 0.0065) in CA3 were significantly (P < 0.05) increased by pwSI. In contrast, pwSI had little effect on the expression of the glutamate receptor subunits in the DG granule cell layer, whereas the vesicular transporters, membrane transporters, and synthases tended to increase. The GABAergic neurotransmission-related genes analyzed were: GABAA receptor α subunits (Gabra1Gabra6), β subunits (Gabrb1Gabrb3), γ subunits (Gabrg1Gabrg3), δ subunit (Gabrd), GABAB receptor subunits (Gabbr1, Gabbr2), vesicular GABA transporter (Slc32a1), membrane GABA transporter (Slc6a1), and GABA synthases (Gad1, Gad2), and the results are shown in Table 3. Compared with group housing, pwSI had little effect on the expressions of these genes in the CA1 and CA3 regions. In contrast to its effect in the pyramidal cell layers, pwSI tended to decrease the expression of 8 of the 13 GABAA receptor subunit genes, especially 4 of the 5 α-subunit genes and 3 of 3 γ-subunit genes. Among them, the expression of Gabra1, Gabra2, Gabra4, Gabra5, Gabrb3, Gabrg1, and Gabrg2 was significantly (P < 0.05) lower in the DG of pwSI rats than in their group-housed counterparts.

Table 3.

Effect of pwSI on GABAergic neuron-related gene expression in distinct hippocampal regions of F344 rats

CA1
CA3
DG
Group pwSI Group pwSI Group pwSI
Gabra1 1.4E-02 ± 1.7E-03 1.4E-02 ± 2.2E-03 4.3E-03 ± 4.2E-04 3.9E-03 ± 3.2E-04 1.3E-02 ± 9.6E-04 9.0E-03± 3.2E-04c
Gabra2 4.4E-03 ± 8.0E-04 4.0E-03 ± 4.3E-04 3.8E-03 ± 2.2E-04 3.7E-03 ± 2.0E-04 9.6E-03 ± 9.2E-04 6.7E-03 ± 7.8E-04b
Gabra3 nd nd 9.7E-05 ± 1.7E-05 1.6E-04 ± 8.9E-06 3.0E-04 ± 3.0E-05 2.6E-04 ± 3.6E-05
Gabra4 1.1E-02 ± 2.5E-03 1.1E-02 ± 3.2E-03 3.1E-03 ± 3.3E-04 2.8E-03 ± 7.2E-05 2.6E-02 ± 1.4E-03 1.9E-02 ± 9.2E-04c
Gabra5 4.1E-02 ± 8.9E-03 3.9E-02 ± 1.3E-02 2.7E-02 ± 3.1E-03 3.4E-02 ± 3.9E-03 3.5E-02 ± 1.1E-03 2.7E-02 ± 2.4E-03a
Gabra6 nd nd nd nd nd nd
Gabrb1 1.1E-02 ± 2.4E-03 1.2E-02 ± 2.6E-03 6.5E-03 ± 6.3E-04 8.0E-03 ± 6.1E-04 1.8E-02 ± 1.9E-04 1.5E-02 ± 1.7E-03
Gabrb2 1.2E-02 ± 3.0E-03 1.1E-02 ± 3.7E-03 3.0E-03 ± 1.1E-04 2.9E-03 ± 1.5E-04 1.2E-02 ± 1.4E-03 8.8E-03 ± 6.9E-04
Gabrb3 4.7E-03 ± 8.8E-04 5.4E-03 ± 9.7E-04 2.7E-03 ± 7.6E-05 2.6E-03 ± 3.5E-04 1.1E-02 ± 1.6E-03 7.6E-03 ± 5.9E-04a
Gabrg1 3.1E-03 ± 8.5E-04 2.9E-03 ± 7.7E-04 1.4E-03 ± 1.5E-04 1.2E-03 ± 2.0E-04 4.4E-03 ± 6.2E-04 2.5E-03 ± 2.6E-04b
Gabrg2 2.9E-03 ± 9.0E-04 2.3E-03 ± 6.0E-04 1.1E-03 ± 2.0E-05 1.1E-03 ± 7.4E-05 4.8E-03 ± 1.2E-03 3.1E-03 ± 2.8E-05a
Gabrg3 2.0E-04 ± 5.1E-05 2.4E-04 ± 2.1E-05 1.2E-04 ± 3.3E-05 8.0E-05 ± 1.4E-05 5.9E-04 ± 1.7E-04 3.3E-04 ± 4.8E-05
Gabrd 2.6E-03 ± 5.1E-04 2.5E-03 ± 9.0E-04 4.0E-04 ± 8.7E-05 3.3E-04 ± 1.5E-05 1.9E-02 ± 2.4E-03 1.4E-02 ± 1.7E-03
Gabbr1 2.1E-02 ± 2.3E-03 2.9E-02 ± 6.3E-03 7.9E-03 ± 8.6E-04 1.0E-02 ± 9.8E-04 2.1E-02 ± 1.9E-03 1.8E-02 ± 2.1E-04
Gabbr2 2.5E-02 ± 3.9E-03 2.5E-02 ± 4.5E-03 2.2E-02 ± 2.6E-03 2.6E-02 ± 1.1E-03 4.9E-02 ± 2.6E-03 4.3E-02 ± 3.1E-03
Slc32a1 4.5E-04 ± 1.0E-04 4.7E-04 ± 1.4E-04 2.8E-04 ± 4.9E-06 2.3E-04 ± 1.4E-05 4.1E-04 ± 4.2E-05 6.8E-04 ± 1.2E-04
Slc6a1 4.1E-03 ± 1.3E-03 5.3E-03 ± 2.1E-03 2.3E-03 ± 2.9E-04 3.4E-03 ± 6.6E-04 7.5E-03 ± 1.1E-03 1.0E-02 ± 1.6E-03
Gad1 3.1E-03 ± 5.6E-04 2.9E-03 ± 7.2E-04 1.1E-03 ± 4.6E-05 1.1E-03 ± 1.2E-04 5.6E-03 ± 3.7E-04 6.4E-03 ± 5.2E-04
Gad2 2.8E-03 ± 6.5E-04 4.0E-03 ± 1.4E-03 1.2E-03 ± 9.9E-06 1.6E-03 ± 2.7E-04 5.2E-03 ± 9.4E-04 5.6E-03 ± 5.6E-04

nd, not detected

Data are shown as the mean ± SEM (n = 3) expression level relative to that of Gapdh.

Bold values indicate that the mean of pwSI/group >1.5

Italicized values indicate that the mean of pwSI/group < 0.7

Values differ significantly (aP < 0.05, bP < 0.01, and P < 0.001) between group-housed and pwSI rat.

Discussion

The present study showed that the gene expression of GABAA receptor subunits, except for the GABAB receptor, in the DG granule cell layer of pwSI rats largely tended to be lower than that in group-housed rats. The GABAA receptor is a ligand-operated chloride channel composed of 5 subunits. Sixteen subunit isoforms have been identified in mammalian brains:26 6 α subunits, 3 β subunits, 3 γ subunits, and single δ, ϵ, θ, and π subunits. The predominant GABAA receptors consist of 2 α, 2 β, and a single γ or δ subunit.25,31 The physiologic properties and intracellular localization of GABAA receptors are governed by the subunit composition of the pentamer.24 For example, GABAA receptors containing α1 subunits, which were reduced in expression by pwSI in the present study, show postsynaptic localization and mediate phasic synaptic inhibition.12 In contrast, the receptors that contain α4 or α5, which also showed decreased expression in pwSI rats, are extrasynaptic and induce tonic conductance.10 Irrespective of these effects, pwSI might reduce the expression of functional GABAA receptors in the DG granule cell layer. Moreover, our finding that the α6 subunit was not expressed in either the pyramidal cell layer or the granule cell layer is consistent with a previous report.20 Several reports describe the functional consequences of the various GABAA receptors. A study using sections from adult rat brain revealed that GABAA receptors containing the α5 subunit are the predominant extrasynaptic inhibitory receptor.3 Rendering this receptor dysfunctional rescues the learning impairment induced by anesthetics5 and enhances the learning ability analyzed by a variety of behavioral tasks, including fear conditioning.4,6,9 Therefore, the functional consequence of GABAA receptors continues to become clear.

The expression of NMDA and kainate receptors, rather than AMPA receptors, tended to increase in CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers. In particular, the expression of Grin2a in CA1, and of Grik4 in CA3 was markedly increased by pwSI. Seven NMDA receptor subunits are known: Grin1, 4 Grin2 isoforms (Grin2a, Grin2b, Grin2c, and Grin2d), and 2 Grin3 isoforms (Grin3a, Grin3b). The functional NMDA receptors are either di- or triheteromeric assemblies, and their physiologic properties are dependent on the subunit composition.19 In particular, the NMDA receptors that contain GRIN2A subunits play an important role in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. For example, mice lacking Grin2a showed attenuated long-term potentiation in CA3–CA1 and impairments in spatial learning and contextual fear conditioning.23,27 Unlike AMPA and NMDA receptors, which are predominantly involved with excitatory neurotransmission, the kainate receptors are thought to function mainly as modulators of excitatory signaling.7 In fact, administration of the selective GRIK1 inhibitor UBP304 attenuates coordinated neuronal activity, that is, θ oscillations, in the hippocampus.13 Therefore, the increased expression of glutamate receptors in CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers may be involved in the behavioral alterations by pwSI, particularly in hippocampus-dependent tasks.

Taken together, the present results suggest that pwSI reduced GABAA receptor expression in the DG granule cell layer and increased the gene expression of several NMDA and kainate receptors in the CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers. Although both hippocampal pyramidal cells and DG granule cells receive projections from inhibitory GABAergic interneurons,11 the GABAergic inputs to DG granule cells are more important for filtering excitatory inputs from the entorhinal cortex to hippocampal circuit.8 Therefore, the decreased expression of GABAA receptor genes in DG by pwSI could contribute to attenuated susceptibility to inhibitory inputs in granule cells and to enhanced excitability in downstream circuits. In addition, the expression of synthases and transporters that participate in glutamatergic neurotransmission in presynaptic cells was increased in DG, and glutamate receptor expression downstream of the circuits, that is, in CA3 and CA1, was upregulated, although the expression of GABA receptors was unaltered in both layers of pyramidal cells. In addition, these alterations in the pyramidal cell layer might contribute to enhanced hippocampal excitability. Therefore, the hippocampal circuits of pwSI rats may be in a more excitable state than that of group-housed rats.

References

  • 1.Andersen P, Bliss TVP, Skrede KK. 1971. Lamellar organization of hippocampal excitatory pathways. Exp Brain Res 13:222–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baenninger LP. 1967. Comparison of behavioural development in socially isolated and grouped rats. Anim Behav 15:312–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brünig I, Scotti E, Sidler C, Fritschy JM. 2001. Intact sorting, targeting, and clustering of γ-aminobutyric acid A receptor subtypes in hippocampal neurons in vitro. J Comp Neurol 443:43–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chambers MS, Atack JR, Broughton HB, Collinson N, Cook S, Dawson GR, Hobbs SC, Marshall G, Maubach KA, Pillai GV, Reeve AJ, MacLeod AM. 2003. Identification of a novel, selective GABAA α5 receptor inverse agonist which enhances cognition. J Med Chem 46:2227–2240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cheng VY, Martin LJ, Elliott EM, Kim JH, Mount HT, Taverna FA, Roder JC, Macdonald JF, Bhambri A, Collinson N, Wafford KA, Orser BA. 2006. α5GABAA receptors mediate the amnestic but not sedative–hypnotic effects of the general anesthetic etomidate. J Neurosci 26:3713–3720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Collinson N, Kuenzi FM, Jarolimek W, Maubach KA, Cothliff R, Sur C, Smith A, Otu FM, Howell O, Atack JR, McKernan RM, Seabrook GR, Dawson GR, Whiting PJ, Rosahl TW. 2002. Enhanced learning and memory and altered GABAergic synaptic transmission in mice lacking the α5 subunit of the GABAA receptor. J Neurosci 22:5572–5580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Contractor A, Mulle C, Swanson GT. 2011. Kainate receptors coming of age: milestones of 2 decades of research. Trends Neurosci 34:154–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Coulter DA, Carlson GC. 2007. Functional regulation of the dentate gyrus by GABA-mediated inhibition. Prog Brain Res 163:235–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Crestani F, Keist R, Fritschy JM, Benke D, Vogt K, Prut L, Blüthmann H, Möhler H, Rudolph U. 2002. Trace fear conditioning involves hippocampal α5 GABAA receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:8980–8985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Farrant M, Nusser Z. 2005. Variations on an inhibitory theme: phasic and tonic activation of GABAA receptors. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:215–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Freund TF, Buzsáki G. 1998. Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 6:347–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goldstein PA, Elsen FP, Ying SW, Ferguson C, Homanics GE, Harrison NL. 2002. Prolongation of hippocampal miniature inhibitory postsynaptic currents in mice lacking the GABAA receptor α1 subunit. J Neurol Neurophysiol 88:3206–3217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Huxter JR, Zinyuk LE, Roloff EV, Clarke VR, Dolman NP, More JC, Jane DE, Collingridge GL, Muller RU. 2007. Inhibition of kainate receptors reduces the frequency of hippocampal θ oscillations. J Neurosci 27:2212–2223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ibi D, Takuma K, Koike H, Mizoguchi H, Tsuritani K, Kuwahara Y, Kamei H, Nagai T, Yoneda Y, Nabeshima T, Yamada K. 2007. Social isolation rearing-induced impairment of the hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with deficits in spatial memory and emotion-related behaviors in juvenile mice. J Neurochem 105:921–932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Koike H, Ibi D, Mizoguchi H, Nagai T, Nitta A, Takuma K, Nabeshima T, Yoneda Y, Yamada K. 2009. Behavioral abnormality and pharmacologic response in social isolation-reared mice. Behav Brain Res 202:114–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lu L, Bao G, Chen H, Xia P, Fan X, Zhang J, Pei G, Ma L. 2003. Modification of hippocampal neurogenesis and neuroplasticity by social environments. Exp Neurol 183:600–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Meaney MJ, Stewart J. 1981. A descriptive study of social development in the rat (Rattus norvegicus). Anim Behav 29:34–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Neves G, Cooke SF, Bliss TVP. 2008. Synaptic plasticity, memory, and the hippocampus: a neural network approach to causality. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:65–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Paoletti P, Bellone C, Zhou Q. 2013. NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity, and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:383–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Parker V, Morinan A. 1986. The socially isolated rat as a model for anxiety. Neuropharmacology 25:663–664. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Quan MN, Tian YT, Xu KH, Zhang T, Yang Z. 2010. Postweaning social isolation influences spatial cognition, prefrontal cortical synaptic plasticity, and hippocampal potassium ion channels in Wistar rats. Neuroscience 169:214–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Iversen SD. 1977. The effects of isolation rearing on exploration in the rat. Anim Learn Behav 5:193–198. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sakimura K, Kutsuwada T, Ito I, Manabe T, Takayama C, Kushiya E, Yagi T, Aizawa S, Inoue Y, Sugiyama H, Mishina M. 1995. Reduced hippocampal LTP and spatial learning in mice lacking NMDA receptor ϵ1 subunit. Nature 373:151–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sieghart W, Sperk G. 2002. Subunit composition, distribution and function of GABA-A receptor subtypes. Curr Top Med Chem 2:795–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sigel E, Steinmann ME. 2012. Structure, function, and modulation of GABAA receptors. J Biol Chem 287:40224–40231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sperk G, Schwarzer C, Tsunashima K, Fuchs K, Sieghart W. 1997. GABAA receptor subunits in the rat hippocampus I: immunocytochemical distribution of 13 subunits. Neuroscience 80:987–1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sprengel R, Suchanek B, Amico C, Brusa R, Burnashev N, Rozov A, Hvalby O, Jensen V, Paulsen O, Andersen P, Kim JJ, Thompson RF, Sun W, Webster LC, Grant SG, Eilers J, Konnerth A, Li J, McNamara JO, Seeburg PH. 1998. Importance of the intracellular domain of NR2 subunits for NMDA receptor function in vivo. Cell 92:279–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Squire LR. 1992. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 99:195–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Syme LA. 1973. Social isolation at weaning: some effects on 2 measures of activity. Anim Learn Behav 1:161–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Thor DH, Holloway WR., Jr 1984. Social play in juvenile rats: a decade of methodological and experimental research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 8:455–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tretter V, Moss SJ. 2008. GABAA receptor dynamics and constructing GABAergic synapses. Front Mol Neurosci 1:7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Toth M, Mikics E, Tulogdi A, Aliczki M, Haller J. 2011. Postweaning social isolation induces abnormal forms of aggression in conjunction with increased glucocorticoid and autonomic stress responses. Horm Behav 60:28–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Võikar V, Polus A, Vasar E, Rauvala H. 2004. Long-term individual housing in C57BL/6J and DBA/2 mice: assessment of behavioral consequences. Genes Brain Behav 4:240–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wongwitdecha N, Marsden CA. 1996. Social isolation increases aggressive behaviour and alters the effects of diazepam in the rat social interaction test. Behav Brain Res 75:27–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yamamuro Y. 2008. Maternal environment alters social interactive traits but not open-field behavior in Fischer 344 rats. Exp Anim 57:439–446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Yoshioka W, Endo N, Kurashige A, Haijima A, Endo T, Shibata T, Nishiyama R, Kakeyama M, Tohyama C. 2012. Fluorescence laser microdissection reveals a distinct pattern of gene activation in the mouse hippocampal region. Sci Rep 2:1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Comparative Medicine are provided here courtesy of American Association for Laboratory Animal Science

RESOURCES