Abstract
In this study we sought to explore how experience with specific mental and somatic practices is associated with wisdom, using self-report measures of experience and wisdom. We administered standard surveys to measure wisdom and experience among four groups of practitioners of mental and somatic practices, namely, meditators, practitioners of the Alexander Technique, practitioners of the Feldenkrais Method, and classical ballet dancers. We additionally administered surveys of trait anxiety and empathy to all participants to explore possible mediating relationships of experience and wisdom by characteristics thought to be components of wisdom. Wisdom was higher on average among meditation practitioners, and lowest among ballet dancers, and this difference held when controlling for differences in age between practices, supporting the view that meditation is linked to wisdom and that ballet is not. However, we found that increased experience with meditation and ballet were both positively associated with wisdom, and that lowered trait anxiety mediated this positive association among meditation practitioners, and, non-significantly, among ballet dancers. These results suggest that not all practices that are purported to affect mental processing are related to wisdom to the same degree and different kinds of experience appear to relate to wisdom in different ways, suggesting different mechanisms that might underlie the development of wisdom with experience.
Introduction
Although wisdom is considered the pinnacle of human cognition and has played a role in religion and philosophy reaching far back into human history, the scientific study of wisdom is a relatively recent phenomenon. As the study of wisdom has grown, a variety of ways to define the construct have emerged. Common themes include the skillful use of knowledge acquired through life experience, lowered anxiety in the face of difficult life decisions, careful reflection on the mental states of oneself and others, and action based in compassion and prosocial behavior [1]. The wisdom literature generally distinguishes between two types of wisdom: general wisdom, which represents insight into the pragmatics of life from a decentered third-person point of view, and personal wisdom, which an individual may acquire and cultivate through insight into daily life [2]. For the purposes of this study, we are interested in how individuals may cultivate personal wisdom through specific mental and somatic practices.
We conceptualize wisdom as a unified construct composed of interrelated cognitive, reflective, and affective characteristics [3]. In this model, wisdom is characterized as a deep and accurate perception of reality, in which insight into human nature and a diminished self-centeredness are acquired through life experience and practice in perspective taking. If wisdom exists as a set of cognitive, reflective, and affective characteristics, each susceptible to change over the lifespan, it is an open question whether experience with structured mental and somatic practices that cultivate these characteristics is associated with greater wisdom.
Mental and somatic practices may increase wisdom by providing what Glück and Bluck [4] refer to as positive general life resources, which affect the events an individual is likely to encounter in life, how such encounters are perceived and appraised, and how challenging experiences are integrated into a person’s life story. Experimental research into the malleability of wisdom suggests that wisdom is affected by training specific strategies for gaining knowledge, inferring insight from personal experience, and viewing difficult situations from a distanced perspective [5–8]. Certain structured mental and somatic practices that aim to affect these processes may therefore have positive effects on wisdom over time. In the current study, we provide a preliminary investigation into how specific types of life experiences may lead to the cultivation of wisdom and wisdom-related characteristics. We do this by surveying wisdom in individuals with varying levels of experience with four different mental and somatic practices: meditation, the Alexander Technique (AT), the Feldenkrais Method (FM), and classical ballet.
Meditation is a practice long associated with the development of wisdom in Buddhist and Taoist traditions [9,10]. Meditation may influence wisdom in multiple ways, for example by increasing interpersonal skills and by decreasing general anxiety through increased emotional self-regulation. Certain styles of meditation have been linked experimentally with increased compassion and prosocial behavior. For example, kindness-based meditation is associated with increased connectivity to and positive regard for others [11]. Highly experienced Buddhist meditators show greater self–other integration than non-religious controls [12]. Moreover, expert meditators (compared to novices) exhibit greater grey matter volume in regions of the brain associated with affective regulation [13], suggesting that increased social-connectedness, and subsequently increased compassion and prosocial behavior, are related to durable effects of meditation practice over time.
Wisdom is often characterized by the ability to face difficult situations with lowered stress and anxiety, and meditation may train the sort of emotional self-regulation that leads to this quiescent mental state. In experimental settings, brief meditation training has been associated with increased optimism and reduced recall of negative words [14,15], suggesting that meditation influences affect by reducing the impact of negative thoughts and stimuli. The results of a recent study indicate that, following brief meditation training, participants down-regulate thoughts emphasizing negativity but not those emphasizing positivity [16]. Though these studies used only brief meditation training with meditation-naïve participants, the results suggest that practicing emotional regulation in the course of meditation training leads to a decreased focus on negative thoughts and stimuli.
While the link between meditation practice and wisdom has been theorized since ancient times, somatic practices have been largely overlooked as potential means to foster wisdom. To our knowledge, no studies have examined whether physical practices of any kind are linked to the cultivation of personal wisdom, nor have they theorized that this association might exist. This may be due in part to the long-standing dualism that has dominated Western culture and scientific thought since Plato considered the human soul to be separate from the body and the body a distraction in intellectual life. Recent advances in embodied cognition, psychophysiology, and somatic therapies challenge these age-old conceptualizations and propose that our bodies have an important influence on the way we think and feel.
While embodied cognition has many different meanings (see Wilson,[17] for a review), they all hold that cognitive processes are deeply rooted in the body’s interactions with the world. One view is that thinking involves the reactivation of modality-specific brain areas involved in sensation and perception [18,19]. Even when we are not moving our bodies, our cognitive processes incorporate physical information from past experience. Since our bodies are an ever-present part of the context in which we use our minds, they influence the representations we form [20]. This view is supported by Kontra and colleagues [21], who found that specific bodily experiences bolster student learning of concepts in the physics classroom. Instructing children and adults to produce hand gestures has also been shown to enhance the learning of new material [22–26]. Additionally, movement patterns, such as changes in posture, can have a profound impact on mood and behavior. An open, expansive posture has been shown to increase feelings of power and tolerance of risk, decrease cortisol, and elevate testosterone levels [27]. Alternatively, hunched postures elicit more depressed feelings [28], and tilting the head upward induces pride [29]. To the extent that somatic training alters movement patterns (e.g., reinforces a more upright and open posture), this sort of bodily experience may contribute to psychological changes associated with wisdom.
While mental practices like meditation are often undertaken with the explicit goal of becoming more wise, somatic methods like AT and FM are typically practiced as a form of physical therapy, to achieve physical goals, or to enhance artistic performance. However, research suggests that these practices also have cognitive and emotional benefits related to components of wisdom. AT for example leads to increased emotional homeostasis [30], which may in turn lead to increased wisdom over time. The mind–body unity emphasized in AT greatly influenced philosopher John Dewey, who credited the practice with increased mental flexibility [31]. Research suggests that FM leads to both physical and mental improvements, including increased muscle length and flexibility [32], improvements in posture and muscle relaxation [33], reduced anxiety [34], as well as elevated mood, enhanced ability to learn, and increased clarity of thought [35]. Given the postulate that one’s mental state is reflected in and connected to the body, and based on the relative dearth of research into the connection between wisdom and somatic practices, we investigated the association between wisdom and experience with AT and FM.
In comparison to AT and FM, we also investigated classical ballet—a somatic practice that has also been associated with increased mental faculties. Since ballet has not been empirically linked with changes in many wisdom-related characteristics, we did not expect experience to be associated with personal wisdom. Research into the psychology of dancers suggests that the increased cognitive capabilities developed through ballet training are mainly limited to perception and memory for movements, though this limitation may be due to a lack of research into possible effects of dance experience on non-dance related cognition. Ballet dancers form and rely on increased body awareness to improve their physical performance [36], and experts are better able than novices to hold images of body-based movements in memory [37]. Furthermore, dancers show increased perceptual sensitivity to subtle changes in the movement of others [38]. As part of their training, dancers spend extensive periods of time observing their own and others’ bodies [39]. Ballet dancers also develop coping strategies to deal with performance anxiety and the pressures of professional success in the face of competition [40]. Given the nature of the changes observed in psychological studies of dance (i.e., they appear grounded in domain-specific cognition and affect), we did not hypothesize a link between ballet training and wisdom.
In the present study, we asked whether experience with mental and somatic practices is associated with wisdom. Previous research has connected wisdom with adverse life events, but we explore whether certain types of experience or training are associated with wisdom. We measured wisdom and several of its purported components with self-report questionnaires; these components included emotional self-regulation in the form of lowered trait anxiety as well as inter- and intrapersonal reactivity in the form of cognitive and affective empathy. We tested an hypothesized association between wisdom and experience with candidate mental and somatic practices—Meditation, Alexander Technique, and Feldenkrais Method, in comparison to another physical practice of classical ballet—including the mediating role of wisdom-related characteristics. By asking whether mental and somatic practices are associated with increased wisdom and exploring the relationship between the psychological construct of wisdom and its components, we aimed to lay the groundwork for a deeper understanding of the development of wisdom and to stimulate future work in the field. By examining both mental and somatic practices, it may be possible to identify aspects of life experiences that are relevant to the development of wisdom.
Method
Participants
Participants gave consent to participate in the study by agreeing to complete the online survey. This study was approved for online administration by the University of Chicago Institutional Review Board (IRB12-1920). Participants were recruited via online solicitations to respective companies and schools across the United States who had email addresses available online, as well as through email list-servs at the University of Chicago. Participants gave consent by agreeing to complete the online survey. Each participant was compensated with a $3 online gift card, with a chance to win a $100 online gift card at the conclusion of the study. Across all practice types, 452 individuals responded to the email and other solicitations. Of these 303 completed the survey in full, and an additional 6 participants were removed because their reported years of experience was greater than 2.5 standard deviations from the mean of their respective group, leaving 298 participants in the final set of analyses.
Participants from the AT sample ranged in age from 24 to 80 (mean = 50.49, sd = 13.76), Ballet dancers ranged in age from 11 to 62 (mean = 25.35, sd = 7.37), FM participants ranged in age from 27 to 76 (mean = 52.8, sd = 11.68), and meditation participants ranged in age from 18 to 68 (mean = 50.49, sd = 13.76). See Table 1 for a breakdown of group level descriptive statistics.
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of wisdom, its components, and predictor variables.
AT | Ballet | FM | Meditation | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Tot wisdom | 3.90 | .33 | 3.59 | .37 | 3.92 | .39 | 4.01 | .36 |
Cog Wisdom | 4.25 | .45 | 4.00 | .57 | 4.20 | .58 | 4.30 | .35 |
Refl Wisdom | 3.66 | .36 | 3.31 | .47 | 3.83 | .47 | 3.88 | .46 |
Aff Wisdom | 3.79 | .47 | 3.47 | .45 | 3.74 | .44 | 3.85 | .53 |
Years | 19 | 12.07 | 13.44 | 9.69 | 14.05 | 8.74 | 9.56 | 7.81 |
Age | 50.49 | 13.76 | 24.74 | 6.02 | 52.80 | 11.68 | 41.61 | 14.55 |
TA | 1.81 | .41 | 2.18 | .48 | 1.84 | .46 | 1.88 | .46 |
CE | 3.22 | .41 | 3.09 | .40 | 3.17 | .40 | 3.13 | .41 |
AE | 2.42 | .24 | 2.50 | .21 | 2.41 | .27 | 2.46 | .25 |
Materials and procedure
The survey was administered via the SurveyMonkey website. Participants first gave consent and then read a statement indicating the purpose of the survey—to gather information about experience with and possible benefits of a particular practice. The experience section of the survey included questions regarding experience as a student and/or teacher of meditation, AT, FM, or ballet. Experience with each practice was operationalized as the self-reported number of years of practice (continuous), as well as estimated hours of practice (categorical). The psychological questionnaires included the Three-Dimensional Wisdom Scale [41], the Questionnaire for Cognitive and Affective Empathy (QCAE [42]), and the Trait Anxiety portion of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults (STAI-Y [43]). Participants lastly completed a demographic questionnaire that asked for information on age, income, and gender.
Measures
Three-Dimensional Wisdom Scale. The 3D-WS is a measure of cognitive, reflective, and affective dimensions of wisdom [41,44]. The 3D-WS has adequate construct, content, predictive, discriminant, and convergent validity, among older populations [45] and is a valid measure of wisdom among young adults of varying cultural backgrounds [46–49]. Examples from the individual subscales include: cognitive dimension—A person either knows the answer to a question or he/she doesn’t (reverse scored); reflective dimension—I try to look at everybody’s side of a disagreement before I make a decision; and affective dimension—It’s not really my problem if others are in trouble and need help (reverse scored).
Questionnaire for Cognitive and Affective Empathy. The QCAE [42] measures cognitive empathy using the Perspective Taking and Online Simulation subscales, and affective empathy using the Emotional Contagion, Proximal Responsivity, and Peripheral Responsivity subscales. Perspective Taking refers to the ability to take on the perspective of another person, while Online Simulation involves imagining what another is feeling. Emotional Contagion is a measure of the automatic mirroring of another’s emotions. Proximal Responsivity is ones’ response when witnessing the mood of someone else in a close social context, while Peripheral Responsivity refers to the same type of response but in a detached social context. The cognitive and affective portions of the QCAE show convergence with the Basic Empathy Scale (BES [50]). For this study, four items that overlap with the 3D-WS were removed from the Online Simulation subscale of cognitive empathy, leaving 5 of the original 9 items.
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—Trait Anxiety. The Trait Anxiety portion of the STAI Form Y contains 20 items related to typical daily experiences of stress, discomfort and worry. Respondents rate each item (e.g., “I worry too much over something that doesn’t really matter”) on a 4-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). Considerable evidence attests to the construct and concurrent validity of the scale [43,51]. Internal consistency coefficients for the scale have ranged from .86 to .95 and test-retest reliability coefficients have ranged from .65 to .75 over a 2-month interval [43].
Data Analyses
Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) were performed to investigate the differences between groups in wisdom, cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and anxiety, followed by a similar analysis after partialling out variance due to individual differences in age. Ordinary least squares (OLS) regression models were constructed to test whether experience with each practice is associated with individual differences in wisdom, controlling for covariates age, gender, and income. Though we used years of experience as the dependent measure, in a separate analysis OLS models were constructed using either years of experience (continuous, self-report, ranging from 1 to 44 years) or hours of experience (categorical, self-report, ranging from 0–5 hours to greater than 40,000 hours) to ensure that using one or the other dependent measure did not significantly alter the pattern of results. For groups in which significant variance in wisdom was associated with experience, steps for determining mediation were performed separately for cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and trait anxiety as mediators [52], after partialling out covariance from age, gender, and income. Mediation analyses were tested for significance using the Sobel test for standard error [53].
Results
Wisdom and wisdom related characteristics between groups
An ANOVA revealed no significant differences between groups for measures of cognitive empathy (CE) or affective empathy (AE), and a significant difference between groups in trait anxiety (TA; F(3,290) = 11.06, p < .001). Posthoc analyses using Tukey's HSD test revealed significantly higher TA among ballet dancers (M = 2.18, SD = 0.48) than other groups combined (M = 1.84, SD = 0.44, p < .001). There were no significant differences in average TA between AT, FM, or meditation practitioners. An ANOVA comparing practice types on income indicated that ballet dancers are in a lower income level than other practices (ballet: $35,000 –$49,999, other practices: $50,000–74,999, p < .05).
An ANOVA with wisdom as the dependent variable and practice type (regardless of amount of practice) as the quasi-independent variable revealed significant differences in wisdom between groups (F(3,290) = 18.49, p < .001). Posthoc analysis using Tukeys HSD test revealed that ballet dancers reported significantly lower average wisdom scores (M = 3.59, SD = 0.37) than the other groups combined (M = 3.94, SD = 0.36, p < .001). There were no significant differences in average wisdom scores between AT, FM, or meditation practitioners. Because ballet dancers were much younger than other groups (ballet mean = 24.74, other practices mean = 49.26, t = 21.15, p < .001), and because age is traditionally associated with differences in wisdom, this analysis was repeated after variation accounted for by differences in age were statistically removed from the dependent variable wisdom. An ANOVA with the residual of wisdom (after statistically removing variation due to age) as the dependent variable and practice type as the quasi-independent variable revealed significant differences in wisdom between groups (F(3,290) = 7.13, p < .001) with meditation having the largest and ballet the smallest residual wisdom score. Posthoc analyses using Tukeys HSD indicate that significant differences occurred only between meditation (resid. M = .17, SD = .38) and FM (resid. M = .004, SD = .39, p < .05), between meditation and ballet (resid M. = -0.13, SD = .37, p < .001), and a trending difference between meditation and AT (resid. M = .004, SD = .32, p = .06).
The association between wisdom and practice
Two sets of ordinary least squares (OLS) regression models were constructed to investigate the association between wisdom and experience with mental and somatic practices, controlling for covariation in age, income, and gender, using either self-reported years of experience or self-reported hours of experience. See Table 2 for a breakdown of average years of experience and frequency counts of hours of experience across practice types. For all practice groups, there was not a significant difference in the models that use years of experience compared to the models that use hours of experience. Because years of experience had a higher level of granularity—ranging from a single year up to 44 years (in the case of the Alexander Technique)—we chose to use this measure over hours of experience—where participants chose from eight categories ranging from less than five hours, up to greater than 40,000 hours.
Table 2. Experience across practice types.
Hours | N | AT | Ballet | FM | Meditation |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Average years experiencea | - | 19.0 | 13.4 | 14.1 | 9.6 |
Fewer than 5 hoursb | 10 | 0 | 6 | 1 | 3 |
6–10 hours | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
11–100 hours | 14 | 3 | 4 | 2 | 5 |
101–1,000 hours | 64 | 6 | 23 | 17 | 18 |
1,001–10,000 hours | 107 | 34 | 14 | 40 | 19 |
10,001–20,000 hours | 47 | 14 | 16 | 13 | 4 |
20,001–40,000 hours | 21 | 6 | 5 | 8 | 2 |
Greater than 40,000 hours | 31 | 2 | 14 | 13 | 2 |
aAverage years taken from self-reported years of experience.
bFrequency of hours taken from 8-level categorical self-reported experience.
The association between meditation practice and wisdom was significant (R2 (adj) = 0.28, F(9,44) = 3.3, p < .01), as was the association between wisdom and experience with ballet dance (R2 (adj) = 0.24, F(9,71) = 3.8, p < .001), controlling for age, gender, and income. There was no significant association between AT experience and wisdom (R2 (adj) = -0.01, F(9,55) = 0.96, n.s.), nor was there a significant association between FM experience and wisdom (R2 (adj) = -0.03, F(9,84) = 0.72, n.s.), controlling for age, gender, and income. For a full description of the simple correlations between experience with each practice and wisdom, it’s subcomponents, and individual characteristics, see Table 3.
Table 3. Correlations among wisdom, its subcomponents, and predictor variables.
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Alexander Technique (N = 65, 56 females) | |||||||||
1. Tot wis | - | ||||||||
2. Cog wis | .77*** | - | |||||||
3. Refl wis | .68*** | .25* | - | ||||||
4. Aff wis | .83*** | .45*** | .40*** | - | |||||
5. Years | .05 | -.03 | -.05 | .17 | - | ||||
6. Age | .22 | .21 | .08 | .19 | .58*** | - | |||
7. TA | -.49*** | -.33** | -.49*** | -.32** | -.14 | -.25* | - | ||
8. CE | .44*** | .23 | .41*** | .39** | -.12 | -.06 | -.20 | - | |
9. AE | .40** | -.25* | -.41*** | -.28* | .07 | -.11 | .25* | -.27* | - |
Ballet (N = 81, 76 females) | |||||||||
1. Tot wis | - | ||||||||
2. Cog wis | .82*** | - | |||||||
3. Refl wis | .64*** | .22* | - | ||||||
4. Aff wis | .78*** | .54*** | .26* | - | |||||
5. Years | .45*** | .42*** | .20 | .38*** | - | ||||
6. Age | .11 | .18 | -.07 | .13 | .46*** | - | |||
7. TA | -.31** | -.32** | -.16 | -.20 | -.31** | -.11 | - | ||
8. CE | .30** | .26* | .14 | .27* | .05 | .16 | -.10 | - | |
9. AE | .08 | .13 | -.09 | .12 | -.01 | .10 | .09 | .12 | - |
Feldenkrais Method (N = 94, 72 females) | |||||||||
1. Tot wis | - | ||||||||
2. Cog wis | .84*** | - | |||||||
3. Refl wis | .74*** | .41*** | - | ||||||
4. Aff wis | .76*** | .48*** | .36*** | - | |||||
5. Years | .14 | .15 | .14 | .02 | - | ||||
6. Age | .04 | .16 | .09 | -.18 | .41*** | - | |||
7. TA | -.33** | -.20* | -.35*** | -.24* | -.04 | -.10 | - | ||
8. CE | .51*** | .39*** | .42*** | .39*** | .11 | -.04 | -.15 | - | |
9. AE | .06 | .14 | -.02 | -.01 | -.05 | .04 | 27** | -.26* | - |
Meditation (N = 54, 29 females) | |||||||||
1. Tot wis | - | ||||||||
2. Cog wis | .75*** | - | |||||||
3. Refl wis | .80*** | .42* | - | ||||||
4. Aff wis | .86*** | .52*** | .49*** | - | |||||
5. Years | .33** | .27* | .34* | .21 | - | ||||
6. Age | -.01 | .02 | -.03 | -.02 | .62*** | - | |||
7. TA | -.50*** | -.29* | -.57*** | -.34** | -.52*** | -.27* | - | ||
8. CE | .53*** | .35** | .59*** | .35** | .17 | -.04 | -.24 | - | |
9. AE | -.20 | .00 | -.27* | -.17 | -.18 | -.15 | .22 | -.23 | - |
* p < .05,
** p < .01,
*** p < .001.
Meditation styles were heterogeneous across respondents, with most reporting multiple styles to the open-ended question what style(s) of meditation do you have experience with? Across the meditation sample 29.49% of respondents reported practicing vipassana, 23.08% reported practicing mindfulness, and 14.10% reported practicing Buddhist meditation. Table 4 displays the top ten styles by frequency and percentage of use in the meditation sample. Meditation styles are not mutually exclusive, and so percentages add up to more than 100.
Table 4. Frequency of mediation practice type.
Meditation typea | Frequency | Percentage |
---|---|---|
Vipassana | 23 | 29.49 |
Mindfulness | 18 | 23.08 |
Buddhist | 11 | 14.10 |
Tibetan | 10 | 12.82 |
Zen | 9 | 11.54 |
Loving-Kindness | 8 | 10.26 |
Mantra | 7 | 8.97 |
Insight | 6 | 7.69 |
Moving | 6 | 7.69 |
Shamatha | 6 | 7.69 |
aMeditation types overlap within participants and so percentages do not add up to 100.
Mediation of the relationship between practice and wisdom
To investigate possible mediating effects of wisdom related characteristics on the relationship between practice and wisdom, OLS regression models were created in three steps. For these steps, as well as for Sobel tests, the covariates age, gender, and income were partialled out of the dependent variable wisdom. First the direct effect of the predictor variable experience was tested against the dependent variable wisdom. Secondly, each hypothesized mediating variable CE, AE, or TA was regressed against the dependent variable. Finally the predictor variable was regressed on the dependent variable, controlling for each mediating variable (in separate models). If when controlling for a mediator, the direct effect of the predictor on the dependent variable is no longer significant, then mediation may be assumed. This mediation was tested for statistical significance using the Sobel test of indirect effects, also known as the Sobel test for standard error [53].
Initial OLS analyses revealed a statistically significant positive association between meditation experience and wisdom (β = 0.34, p = 0.01). Among meditation practitioners, additional analyses revealed a significant negative association between experience and TA (β = -0.52, p < .001), though meditation experience was not significantly associated with CE or AE. Additional analyses revealed a significant negative association between TA and wisdom (β = -0.44, p < .001), indicating a potential mediating relationship of TA in the association between meditation experience and wisdom. There was a significant positive association between CE and wisdom (β = 0.45, p < .001) for people practicing meditation, and no significant association between AE and wisdom. Further analysis indicated that when TA is included in the model regressing meditation experience on wisdom, the relationship between practice and wisdom is no longer significant (β = 0.15, p = 0.3). Sobel's test revealed that this mediating effect is significant (p < .05).
As indicated by previous analyses, OLS regression revealed a significant positive association between ballet experience and wisdom (β = 0.38, p < .001). Further analyses among ballet dancers revealed a significant negative association between experience and TA (β = -0.31, p < .001), while experience with ballet dance was not significantly associated with CE or AE. In our ballet sample, there was a significant negative association between TA and wisdom (β = -0.24, p = 0.03), a significant positive association between CE and wisdom (β = 0.23, p = 0.04), and no significant relationship between AE and wisdom. When TA was included in a model regressing ballet experience on wisdom, the relationship between experience and wisdom decreased in magnitude (β = 0.33, p < .01), and Sobel's test of the indirect effect was not significant (p = 0.25), indicating that the relationship between ballet training and wisdom may be only partially mediated by TA.
The mean age of ballet dancers was significantly lower than for the three other practice groups (ballet mean = 24.74, sd = 6.02, other mean = 49.26, sd = 13.82; t(286.97) = 21.15, p < .001). To further investigate the association between experience and wisdom among ballet dancers and to see if experience and wisdom associations may have been driven by very experienced older dancers, we split the ballet group at the median of age and ran OLS analyses separately on the low and high age groups, with wisdom as the dependent variable and years of experience as the predictor, controlling for income and gender. Though the relationship between experience and wisdom was significant in both the lower and upper age groups (lower quantile: β = 0.59, p < .001; upper quantile: β = 0.33, p = .05), the association was stronger among the lower age group than the upper. Trait anxiety was significantly and negatively associated with wisdom among the older quantile (β = -0.36, p < .05), but not the lower age quantile (β = -0.22, n.s.), and it did not mediate the relationship between experience and wisdom in either group, mirroring the results in the group as a whole.
Discussion
The study explored the relationship between mental and somatic practices and wisdom by measuring self-reported wisdom and several of its purported components to test an hypothesized association between wisdom and experience with candidate mental and somatic practices. We found that on average, controlling for differences in age, meditation practitioners reported higher wisdom than practitioners of the Alexander Technique (AT), the Feldenkrais Method (FM), or ballet dancers, with ballet dancers showing the lowest average levels of reported wisdom. Additionally, regression analyses revealed that wisdom is positively associated with years of experience in both meditation and ballet, but not with years of experience with AT or FM. Finally, the association between experience and wisdom is completely mediated by lowered trait anxiety among meditators, though this mediating relationship is not significant among ballet dancers. Taken together, these results suggest first that the practice of meditation is related to increased wisdom, and that this relationship may be due to an effect of prolonged meditation on lowered everyday anxiety. Second, though not associated with heightened wisdom on average, prolonged ballet experience may be linked to increased wisdom, though the current study does not indicate any potential causal mechanism for this association. As a caveat, the relationship between experience and ballet appears to be driven by the participants in the lower ranges of experience, suggesting that ballet may increase the resources needed to develop wisdom in early years of practice.
Before discussing our findings, we must first acknowledge the limitations of our self-report questionnaire and our cross-sectional design. Researchers lack a consensus on the best way to define and measure personal wisdom and while some behavioral measures exist [54,55], their administration and coding is time-intensive, and their operationalization of wisdom differs from the definition we chose for the present study. Despite the strong construct, content, predictive, discriminant, and convergent validity of the 3D-WS [45], the scale has the typical limitations of self-report methodology, including differences in participants’ introspective ability, interpretation of rating scales, potential dishonesty, demand characteristics, and desire for image management. Another concern with assessing wisdom through self-report is that wise individuals might temper their responses due to their humility, even though their increased reflective ability and self-knowledge should counteract their desire to provide humble responses (cf. Tiberius [56]). However, it is interesting that our sample of ballet dancers reported significantly lower wisdom on average than meditators, after partialling out effects of age between groups. Future research is needed to see if this finding can be replicated, or if it was due to demographic or other differences between our ballet and other practice samples.
The cross-sectional nature of our survey study, compared to longitudinal alternatives, prevents us from making any causal claims about meditation or ballet experience leading to increases in wisdom. It is possible that wise individuals are more likely to stick with a given practice over time. However, such a view of perseverance or grit [57] should predict that all four groups show the same association between years of practice and wisdom. However, while ballet dancers scored significantly lower on wisdom than meditators and people drawn to AT and FM, both the meditators and ballet dancers show systematic changes in wisdom with years of practice, and this is not seen for the other two groups. Longitudinal intervention studies, in which people with no prior meditation or ballet experience receive training over time, will be needed to determine if differences in the difficulty and nature of these practices leads to the development of wisdom and over what time frame.
Despite these limitations, the present results provide the first demonstration of an association between mental and somatic practices and wisdom, and they suggest that trait anxiety might mediate this relationship in some circumstances. While past research has shown that mindfulness meditation and dance training can lead to decreases in trait anxiety [58–61], no prior research has explored this outcome in connection to wisdom. In alignment with past research, our results suggest a negative relationship between trait anxiety and both meditation and ballet experience. Furthermore trait anxiety mediated the association between meditation practice and wisdom in our sample, and partially mediated the relationship between ballet practice and wisdom, suggesting one way in which meditation and wisdom may be linked. The negative relationship between trait anxiety and wisdom was seen only with the younger half of the surveyed ballet dancers, so that the change in the magnitude of the relationship with age may help explain the difference in associations between meditation, ballet, wisdom, and anxiety. Although we cannot determine causality from our data, the negative relationship between trait anxiety and wisdom suggests the possibility that meditation and ballet training may contribute to wisdom by training a person to avoid, manage, or overcome personal anxiety or anxieties inherent in each practice and in life in general.
Our findings suggest that training in meditation and ballet may relate to wisdom through helping people develop wisdom related resources, such as resilience to adversity. The ability to deal successfully with hardship correlates with an increase in psychological health for elders identified as wise and may be a prerequisite for the development of wisdom [3]. Meditation practice in general, and mindfulness practice in particular, is associated with improvements in psychological well-being as evidenced by improvements across a variety of psychiatric disorders, such as depression, anxiety, and addiction [62]. By improving psychological health, it is possible that practicing meditation helps people to deal with hardship in a more successful and wise manner by arming them with the resources needed to handle the challenge rather than viewing it as a threat. To the degree that practicing ballet improves psychological health and requires persistence, dancing could similarly help people cope with hardship, particularly in early stages of practice, even though most people do not begin ballet training with this purpose in mind.
In identifying relationships between mental and somatic practices and wisdom, the present study suggests several potential avenues for future research. Training studies should determine if meditation and ballet directly contribute to the development of wisdom and should examine additional wisdom-related characteristics developed through mental and somatic practices that might mediate the link between practice and wisdom. In addition, our findings should be replicated with multiple measures of wisdom to see if the relationships hold up with behavioral or other self-report assessments (see Glück et al [4]). If mental and somatic practices can indeed help cultivate wisdom, their applications should be explored across settings such as in the classroom or workplace with the goal of creating not only wiser people but also a wiser society that may benefit future generations.
Supporting Information
Data Availability
Data are available within the Supporting Information file.
Funding Statement
This study was supported by Templeton Grant ID#: 37775 from the John Templeton Foundation (www.templeton.org). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Ferrari M, Weststrate N. The scientific study of personal wisdom: From contemplative traditions to neuroscience. New York: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Staudinger UM. The need to distinguish personal from general wisdom: A short history and empirical evidence In Ferrari M, Weststrate N, editors. The scientific study of personal wisdom. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ardelt M. Social crisis and individual growth: The long-term effects of the great depression. Journal of Aging Studies. 1998;12: 291–314. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Glück J, Bluck S. 2013. The MORE Life Experience Model: A theory of the development of personal wisdom In Ferrari M, Weststrate N, editors. The scientific study of personal wisdom. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 75–98. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Staudinger UM, Baltes PB. Interactive minds: A facilitative setting for wisdom-related performance? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71: 746–762. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Böhmig-Krumhaar SA, Staudinger UM, Baltes PB. Mehr Toleranz tut Not: Lässt sich wert-relativierendes Wissen und Urteilen mit Hilfe einer wissensaktivierenden Gedächtnisstrategie verbessern? Z Entwicklungspsychol Pädagog Psychol. 2002;34: 30–43. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Staudinger UM, Kessler EM, Doerner J. Wisdom in social context In Schaie KW, Carstensen L, editors. Social structures, aging, and self-regulation in the elderly. New York: Springer; 2006. pp. 33–54. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kross E, Grossman I. Boosting wisdom: Distance from the self enhances wise reasoning, attitudes and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2011;14: 43–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bodhidharma. The Zen Teaching of Bodhidharma Red Pine translation. New York: North Point Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jeste DV, Harris JC. Wisdom-A neuroscience perspective. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2010;304: 1602–1603. 10.1001/jama.2010.1458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hutcherson CA, Seppala EM, Gross JJ. Loving-kindness meditation increases social connectedness. Emotion. 2008;8: 720–724. 10.1037/a0013237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Colzato LS, Zech H, Hommel B, Verdonschot R, van den Wildenberg W, Hsieh S. Loving-kindness brings loving-kindness: The impact of Buddhism on cognitive self-other integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2012;19: 541–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Leung M, Chan CCH, Yin J, Lee C, So K, Lee TMC. Increased grey matter volume in the right angular and posterior parahippocampal gyri in loving-kindness meditators. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;8: 34–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kiken LG, Shook NJ. Looking up: Mindfulness increases positive judgments and reduces negativity bias. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2011;2: 425–431. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Alberts HJEM, Thewissen R. The effect of a brief mindfulness intervention on memory for positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Mindfulness. 2011;2: 73–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kiken LG, Shook NJ. Does mindfulness attenuate thoughts emphasizing negativity, but not positivity? Journal of Research in Personality. 2014;53: 22–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wilson M. Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2001;9: 625–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Barsalou LW. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999;22: 577–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermuller F. Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron. 2004;41: 301–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Casasanto D. Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2009;138: 351–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kontra C, Lyons DJ, Fischer SM, Beilock SL. Physical experience enhances science learning. Psychological science. 2015;26: 737–749. 10.1177/0956797615569355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Broaders SC, Cook SW, Mitchell Z, Goldin-Meadow S. Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2007;136: 539–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Goldin-Meadow S, Cook SW, Mitchell ZA. Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science. 2009;20: 267–272. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02297.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Novack MA, Congdon EL, Hemani-Lopez N, & Goldin-Meadow S. From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math. Psychological Science. 2014;25: 903–910. 10.1177/0956797613518351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ping R, Goldin-Meadow S. Hands in the air: Using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44: 1277 10.1037/0012-1649.44.5.1277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ping R, Decatur M, Larson SW, Zinchenko E, Goldin-Meadow S. Unpacking the gestures of chemistry learners: What the hands can tell us about correct and incorrect conceptions of stereochemistry. under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Carney DR, Cuddy AJ, Yap AJ. Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science. 2010;21: 1363–1368. 10.1177/0956797610383437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Riskind JH, Gotay CC. Physical posture: Could it have regulatory or feedback effects on motivation and emotion? Motivation and Emotion. 1982;6: 273–298. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Stepper S, Strack F. Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;64: 211–220. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stallibrass C, Sissons P, Chalmers C. Randomized controlled trial of the Alexander Technique for idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Rehabilitation. 2002;16: 695–708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jones FP. Freedom to change: the development and science of the Alexander technique. London: Mouritz; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hopper C, Kolt GS, McConville JC. The effects of Feldenkrais awareness through movement on hamstring length, flexibility and perceived exertion. Journal of Bodywork Movement Therapies. 1999;3: 238–247. [Google Scholar]
- 33.James ML, Kolt GS, Hopper C, McConville JC, Bate P. The effect of a Feldenkrais program and relaxation procedures on hamstring length. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy. 1998;44: 49–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Soloway E. Mary Jane: Basket weaver with back & shoulder pain. International Feldenkrais Federation Journal. 1996;3: 22–25. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lake B. Acute back pain: Treatment by the application of Feldenkrais principles. Australian Family Physician. 1985;14: 1175–1178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jola C, Davis A, Haggard P. Proprioceptive integration and body representation: Insights into dancers’ expertise. Experimental Brain Research. 2011;213: 257–265. 10.1007/s00221-011-2743-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bläsing B, Tenenbaum G, Shack T. The cognitive structure of movements in classical dance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2009;10: 350–360. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grèzes J, Passingham RE, Haggard P. Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15: 1243–1249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kleiner S. Thinking with the mind, syncing with the body: Ballet as symbolic and nonsymbolic interaction. Symbolic Interaction. 2009;32: 236–259. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Barrell G, Terry P. Trait anxiety and coping strategies among ballet dancers. Medical Problems of Performing Arts. 2013;18: 59–64. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ardelt M. Wisdom as expert knowledge system: A critical review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept. Human Development. 2004;47: 257–285. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Reniers RL, Corcroan R, Drake R, Shryane NM, Völlm BA. The QCAE: A questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. Journal of Personality Assessment, 2011;93: 84–95. 10.1080/00223891.2010.528484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bergsma A, Ardelt M. Self-reported wisdom and happiness: An empirical investigation. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2012;13: 481–499. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ardelt M. Development and empirical assessment of a three-dimensional wisdom scale. Research on Aging. 2003;25: 275–324. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Bailey A, Russel KC. Psycho-social benefits of a service-learning experience. Journal of Unconventional Parks, Tourism & Recreational Research. 2008;1: 9–16. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ardelt M. Are older adults wiser than college students? A comparison of two age cohorts. Journal of Adult Development. 2010;17: 193–207. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Benedikovičová J, Ardelt M. The Three-Dimensional Wisdom Scale in cross-cultural context: A comparison between American and Slovak college students. Studia Psychologica. 2008;50: 179–190. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ferrari M, Kahn A, Benayon M, Nero J. Phronesis, sophia, and hochma: Developing wisdom in Islam and Judaism. Research in Human Development. 2011;8: 128–148. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. Journal of Adolescence. 2006;29: 589–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Spielberger CD. State–Trait Anxiety Inventory: A comprehensive bibliography. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51: 1173–1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Sobel ME. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models In Leinhardt S, editor. Sociological Methodology. Washington DC: American Sociological Association; 1982. Pp. 290–312. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist. 2000;55: 122–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kitchener KS, Brenner HG. Wisdom and Reflective Judgment: Knowing in the Face of Uncertainty In Sternberg RJ, editor. Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 212–229. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tiberius V. The reflective life: Living wisely with our limits. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Duckworth AL, Peterson C, Matthews DM, Kelley DR. Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92: 1087–1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Menezes CB, Bizarro L. Effects of Focused Meditation on Difficulties in Emotion Regulation and Trait Anxiety. Psychology & Neuroscience. Advance online publication. 2015. Available: 10.1037/pne0000015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Shapiro SL, Brown K, Biegel G. Self-care for health care professionals: effects of MBSR on mental well being of counseling psychology students. Training and Education in Professional Psychology. 2007;1: 105–115. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bergen-Cico D, Cheon S. The mediating effects of mindfulness and self-compassion on trait anxiety. Mindfulness. 2014;5: 505–519. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lesté A, Rust J. Effects of dance on anxiety. American Journal of Dance Therapy. 1990;12: 19–25. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hofmann SG, Sawyer AT, Witt AA, Oh D. The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78: 169–183. 10.1037/a0018555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data are available within the Supporting Information file.