Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2015 Dec 23;4:e11814. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11814

Genetic variation in offspring indirectly influences the quality of maternal behaviour in mice

David George Ashbrook 1,*, Beatrice Gini 1, Reinmar Hager 1,*
Editor: Jonathan Flint2
PMCID: PMC4758942  PMID: 26701914

Abstract

Conflict over parental investment between parent and offspring is predicted to lead to selection on genes expressed in offspring for traits influencing maternal investment, and on parentally expressed genes affecting offspring behaviour. However, the specific genetic variants that indirectly modify maternal or offspring behaviour remain largely unknown. Using a cross-fostered population of mice, we map maternal behaviour in genetically uniform mothers as a function of genetic variation in offspring and identify loci on offspring chromosomes 5 and 7 that modify maternal behaviour. Conversely, we found that genetic variation among mothers influences offspring development, independent of offspring genotype. Offspring solicitation and maternal behaviour show signs of coadaptation as they are negatively correlated between mothers and their biological offspring, which may be linked to costs of increased solicitation on growth found in our study. Overall, our results show levels of parental provisioning and offspring solicitation are unique to specific genotypes.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.001

Research Organism: Mouse

eLife digest

Genes encode instructions that can influence the behaviour and physical traits of the individual that carries them. Individuals of the same species can carry different versions (or variants) of the same gene, leading to a variety of traits in the population. However, a gene expressed in one individual can also alter the traits of another individual. This is known as an indirect genetic effect. For example, a gene in a mother that affects her ability to provide care may influence how her offspring develop.

Researchers have predicted that offspring should be able to manipulate their mothers to try and gain more care than the mothers are willing to give. Furthermore, the offspring born to mothers who respond to this begging are predicted to save energy and beg less. However, few gene variants that indirectly modify the behaviour of mothers or offspring have so far been identified.

Ashbrook et al. used mice to test the idea that genetic variation in particular locations in the offspring’s genome can affect maternal behaviour, and vice versa. In the first experiment, mother mice with different gene variants fostered litters of mouse pups that were all genetically identical. In the other experiment, genetically identical mothers fostered litters of mouse pups with different gene variants. Ashbrook et al. identified several locations in the offspring’s genome that modified the behaviour of their foster mothers. Furthermore, the experiments also show that genetic variation among the mothers influenced the development of their offspring, independent of the genes carried by the offspring.

The next steps are to identify the specific genes underlying the changes in behaviour, and the molecular and genetic pathways by which they impact indirectly on the traits of other individuals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.002

Introduction

The close interaction between mother and offspring in mammals is fundamental to offspring development and fitness. However, parent and offspring are in conflict over how much parents should invest in their young where offspring typically demand more than is optimal for the parent (Trivers, 1974; Godfray, 1995), and the existence of this genetic conflict has been demonstrated in empirical research (Kölliker et al., 2015). The resulting selection pressures are predicted to lead to the evolution of traits in offspring that influence parental behaviour (and thus investment). Conversely, parental traits should be selected for their effects on offspring traits that influence parental behaviour indirectly (Kilner and Hinde, 2012). The correlation between parental and offspring traits has been the focus of coadaptation models where specific combinations of demand and provisioning are selectively favoured (Wolf and Brodie III, 1998; Kölliker et al., 2005). The fundamental assumption underlying predictions about the evolution of traits involved in parent-offspring interactions is that genetic variation in offspring exists for traits that indirectly influence maternal investment and vice versa. However, it remains to be shown whether specific genetic variants in offspring indirectly influence maternal behaviour. In an experimental mouse population, we demonstrate that genes expressed in offspring modify the quality of maternal behaviour and thus affect, indirectly, offspring fitness.

To investigate the genetics of parent-offspring interactions we conducted a cross-fostering experiment between genetically variable and genetically uniform mice, using the largest genetic reference panel in mammals, the BXD mouse population. We generated families of genetically variable mothers and genetically uniform offspring by cross-fostering C57BL/6J (B6) litters, in which no genetic variation occurs between animals of this strain, to mothers of a given BXD strain. Conversely, a BXD female’s litter was cross-fostered to B6 mothers (Figure 1). Thus, we can analyse the effects of genetic variation in mothers or offspring while controlling for genetic variation in the other. This cross-fostering design has been successfully utilized in previous studies on family interactions because it breaks the correlation between maternal and offspring traits. Here, different families, or naturally occurring variation of maternal and offspring trait combinations across different broods, are assumed to represent distinct evolved strategies (Agrawal et al., 2001; Hager and Johnstone, 2003; Meunier and Kölliker, 2012). From birth until weaning at 3 weeks of age we recorded offspring and maternal body weights and behaviour, following Hager and Johnstone (2003).

Figure 1. Experimental cross-foster design.

Figure 1.

Females of different lines of the BXD strain (light to grey mice) adopt B6 offspring (dark) and B6 females (dark) adopt offspring born to females of different BXD lines. A total of 42 BXD lines with three within-line repeats plus the corresponding B6 families were set up for the experiment.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.003

Results

We first investigated whether there is evidence for indirect genetic effects in offspring influencing maternal behaviour, keeping maternal genotype constant. To find out if maternal behaviour is modified by genes expressed in offspring, we mapped variation in maternal behaviour as a function of their adoptive BXD offspring genotype. We found that variation in offspring genotype affects maternal behaviour, which in turn influences offspring development and fitness. Throughout, we denote loci as either maternal or offspring, Mat or Osp, followed by whether it is an indirect effect (Ige) or a direct effect locus (Dge). We provide a summary of all loci in Table 1, and mapping details in Supplementary files 1A-D. During the first postnatal week we mapped a locus on offspring chromosome 7, OspIge7.1, modifying maternal nestbuidling on day 6 (Figure 2). At this locus, the D2 allele increases the trait value such that B6 mothers showed more nestbuilding activity when fostering BXD pups carrying the D2 allele at the locus. Nestbuilding is particularly important for offspring fitness as thermoregulation is underdeveloped and hypothermia is the primary cause of early death, even if milk is supplied (Lynch and Possidente, 1978). We detected a further locus on offspring distal chromosome 5 (OspIge5.1) that affects maternal behaviour on day 14, around the time we expect the weaning conflict to be highest (Figure 3). Here, mothers showed increased levels of maternal behaviour when fostering BXD offspring carrying the D2 allele. Conversely, we can look at how variation in maternal genotype influences offspring traits, keeping offspring genotype constant, mapping variation in offspring traits as a function of BXD genotype. Here, we found that offspring growth during the second week is affected by a locus on maternal chromosome 17, MatIge17.1, where the B6 allele increases the trait value (Figure 4). In addition to looking at indirect genetic effects we can analyse direct genetic effects, i.e. how an individual’s genotype influences its own traits. In mothers we found a direct genetic effect locus for maternal behaviour on proximal chromosome 10, and for nestbuilding behaviour on chromosome 1 (MatDge10.1 and MatDge1.1, respectively), where the B6 allele increases the trait value in both cases. We also detected a locus for offspring solicitation behaviour on chromosome 5 (OspDge5.1) with the D2 allele increasing the level of solicitation shown. Its location is at the opposite end on chromosome 5 from where OspIge5.1 is located.

Table 1.

Summary of direct and indirect genetic effects. We list the loci, followed by their position in Mb, the 1.5 LOD confidence interval (Dupuis and Siegmund, 1999), the genome-wide peak marker LRS and LOD score and associated p-value and the number of genes within the interval.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.010

Loci QTL position Confidence interval Max LRS Max LOD Max P Number of genes
MatDge1.1 168.32 165.24 - 172.06 17.03 3.70 0.069 68
OspDge5.1 23.827 17.82 - 24.62 17.85 3.88 0.046 73
OspIge5.1 146.68 145.20 - 147.65 18.65 4.05 0.038 40
OspIge7.1 53.68 47.76 - 56.65 17.85 3.88 0.039 232
81.49 76.12 - 90.92 16.06 3.49 0.087 133
MatDge10.1 19.09 18.61 - 21.83 22.37 4.86 0.008 30
MatIge17.1 23.32 11.48 - 31.17 19.02 4.13 0.022 422
33.02 31.32 - 40.65 18.57 4.03 0.028 321

Figure 2. Offspring indirect genetic effect modifying maternal nestbuidling behaviour (OspIge7.1).

Figure 2.

The figure shows an offspring genomescan of maternal nestbuilding behaviour on day 6. The blue line represents the genome scan, showing the likelihood ratio statistic (LRS) associated with each marker across the 19 autosomal and the X chromosome. The top, pink, line marks genome-wide significance, the lower, grey, line the suggestive significance threshold. The green or red line show the additive coefficient, with green showing that the DBA/2J alleles increase trait values and red that the C57BL/6J alleles increase trait values. The green axis on the right shows by how much the respective alleles increase trait values (the DBA/2J allele in offspring increases maternal nestbuilding by ~0.8).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.004

Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing the B6 maternal nestbuilding behaviour on day 6 per pup for the BXD lines.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.005

Figure 3. Offspring indirect genetic effect modifying maternal behaviour (OspIge5.1).

Figure 3.

The figure shows an offspring genomescan of maternal behaviour on day 14. The blue line represents the genome scan, showing the likelihood ratio statistic (LRS) associated with each marker across the 19 autosomal and the X chromosome. The top, pink, line marks genome-wide significance, the lower, grey, line the suggestive significance threshold. The green or red line show the additive coefficient, with green showing that the DBA/2J alleles increase trait values and red that the C57BL/6J alleles increase trait values. The green axis on the right shows by how much the respective alleles increase trait values (the DBA/2J allele in offspring increases maternal behaviour by ~2.5).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.006

Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing the B6 maternal behaviour on day 14 per pup for the BXD lines.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.007

Figure 4. Maternal indirect genetic effect modifying offspring growth (MatIge17).

Figure 4.

The figure shows a maternal genomescan of offspring growth in the second postnatal week. The blue line represents the genome scan, showing the likelihood ratio statistic (LRS) associated with each marker across the 19 autosomal and the X chromosome. The top, pink, line marks genome-wide significance, the lower, grey, line the suggestive significance threshold. The green or red line show the additive coefficient, with green showing that the DBA/2J alleles increase trait values and red that the C57BL/6J alleles increase trait values. The green axis on the right shows by how much the respective alleles increase trait values (the C57BL/6J allele in mothers increases offspring growth by ~0.25).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.008

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing the B6 offspring growth in the second postnatal week per pup for the BXD lines.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.009

Overall, our results show that IGEs, as well as DGEs, can be linked to specific loci that affect parent-offspring interaction, and it is thus possible that selection may occur on genes with indirect and direct effects on parental behaviour. Importantly, our result that variation in maternal behaviour is affected by genes expressed in offspring is also clearly borne out at the phenotypic level: offspring solicitation behaviour in genetically variable BXD pups is positively correlated with the level of maternal behaviour in their genetically uniform B6 adoptive mothers on all three days we measured behaviour with significant effects of maternal behaviour and day on offspring solicitation (GLM, F1,103 = 17.62, P < 0.001 and F2,103 = 14.22, P < 0.001, respectively; day 6: Pearsons’r = 0.56, P = 0.003; day 10: r = 0.63, P = 0.001, and day 14: r = 0.55, P = 0.008; Supplementary file 2, a). Similarly, we can investigate how traits in genetically uniform B6 offspring correlate with maternal traits in their genetically variable BXD mothers (i.e. within adoptive families). We found that maternal behaviour and day have a significant positive effect on offspring solicitation behaviour (GLM, F1,118 = 6.33, P = 0.013, and F2,118 = 6.33, P < 0.001; Supplementary file 2, b). While one might generally assume that mothers behave in response to offspring solicitation behaviour, these results show, perhaps surprisingly, that variation in maternal behaviour influences the level of solicitation: here, we need to remember that there is no variation in offspring genotype so we assume that across families differences in offspring behaviour are due to differences in the genotype of their adoptive mothers.

Coadaptation of parental and offspring traits

While in the previous section we have focused on analysing traits within foster families, we now turn to the correlation between traits of biological families, i.e. mothers and their biological offspring. This correlation has been analysed in coadaptation models, which make specific predictions about how parental and offspring traits are correlated, and in empirical work (Kölliker et al., 2000; Agrawal et al., 2001; Curley et al., 2004; Lock et al., 2004; Kölliker et al., 2005; Hinde et al., 2010; Meunier and Kölliker, 2012). Prior experimental studies have found both a positive (Parus major; Kölliker et al., 2000: Nicrophorus vespilloides; Lock et al., 2004) and negative correlation between offspring solicitation and parental traits (Sehirus cinctus; Agrawal et al., 2001). In our study, we found a negative correlation. When we measured short-term provisioning, we found a negative correlation between BXD offspring short-term weight gain and the corresponding provisioning of their biological (BXD) mothers on day 10, as well as a negative correlation between BXD offspring solicitation and the corresponding provisioning of their biological (BXD) mothers on day 14 (GLM, F1,32 = 4.77, P = 0.036; r = -0.34 and GLM, F1,28 = 8.046, P = 0.008, r = -0.48; Figure 5 and Supplementary file 2, d). Thus, our results suggest that mothers who are generous providers produce young that solicit less maternal resources than offspring born to less generous mothers. Such a negative correlation is predicted to occur when maternal traits are predominantly under selection as long as parents respond to offspring demand (which we have shown above; Kölliker et al., 2005). One scenario to explain this negative correlation might be that each BXD line, i.e. genotype, is characterized by a unique (to this line, everything else being equal) combination of offspring and maternal behaviours where higher maternal provisioning is correlated with lower offspring solicitation. This may be due to the cost of increased solicitation (reflected in reduced bodyweight for the effort expended) for which we found evidence in our study. Bodyweight is indeed negatively correlated with the level of offspring solicitation (GLM, F1,66 = 20.57, P < 0.001 e.g. day 10, r = -0.39, and day 14, r = -0.44; Figure 6 and Supplementary file 2, e).

Figure 5. Correlation between offspring and maternal traits in biological BXD families.

Figure 5.

The first panel shows the correlation between BXD offspring short-term weight change per pup and provisioning of their corresponding biological BXD mother on day 10 per pup. The second panel shows the correlation between the level of BXD offspring solicitation per pup on day 14 and their mother’s provisioning per pup.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.011

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing BXD offspring short-term weight change on day 10 per pup, BXD maternal provisioning on day 10, BXD offspring solicitation on day 14 per pup and BXD maternal provisioning on day 14 for the BXD lines.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.012

Figure 6. Correlation between per pup offspring solicitation and corresponding body weight in BXD lines on day 10 and day 14, respectively.

Figure 6.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.013

Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing BXD offspring solicitation on day 10 per pup, BXD offspring weight on day 10 per pup, BXD offspring solicitation on day 14 per pup and BXD offspring weight on day 14 per pup for the BXD lines.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.014

Discussion

Our study of the genetics underlying family interactions has revealed that genes expressed in offspring can indirectly influence the quality of maternal behaviour and thus offspring fitness. At the same time, we detected specific loci in maternal genotype that indirectly modify offspring traits, which shows that IGEs can be an important component of the genetic architecture of complex traits (Bijma and Wade, 2008). We note that while postnatal cross-fostering controls for postnatal maternal effects, prenatal maternal effects can only be addressed (to some degree) by embryo transfer (e.g. Cowley et al., 1989), a procedure that is impractical in genetics experiments. Potentially, this may strengthen or weaken, for example, effects of BXD genotype on B6 maternal phenotype. At the same time, pre-natal maternal effects may also contribute to a phenotypic correlation between parent and biological offspring traits, as reported above on coadaptation. In-utero effect variation due to differences in BXD genotype may therefore contribute to differences in BXD offspring behaviour, in turn affecting the behaviour of their adoptive mothers. We now need to investigate the candidates identified here and how their effects on parental and offspring traits are integrated into the gene networks determining individual development. By controlling for genetic variation in either mothers or offspring we have been able to show that levels of maternal provisioning and offspring solicitation are unique to specific genotypes (here each BXD line) and that solicitation is costly. The ability to conduct complex systems genetics analyses in experimental systems of parent offspring interactions will enable us to concentrate now on understanding the underlying pathways involved, and how they are modified by social environmental conditions that determine adult phenotypes and associated reproductive success.

Materials and methods

Animals and genetic analysis

We used mice of the BXD recombinant inbred population, which consists of experimentally tractable and genetically defined mouse lines capturing a large amount of naturally occurring genetic variation, which underlies variation at the phenotypic level (e.g. Chesler et al., 2005; Hayes et al., 2014). The BXD panel incorporates ~5 million segregating SNPs, 500,000 insertions and deletions, and 55,000 copy-number variants. These lines are used for complex systems genetics analyses integrating massive phenotype and gene expression data sets obtained across years and studies (e.g. Andreux et al., 2012; Ashbrook et al., 2014).

The 42 BXD strains used in this study (1, 11, 12, 14, 24, 32, 34, 38–40, 43–45, 48a, 49–51, 55, 56, 60–64, 66–71, 73a, 73b, 73–75, 83, 84, 87, 89, 90, 98, 102) were obtained from Professor Robert W. Williams at the University of Tennessee Health Science Centre, Memphis, TN. C57BL/6J (B6) mice were obtained from Charles River, UK. Three within-line repeats plus the corresponding 42 B6 families with three within-line repeats were set up for the experiment (Figure 1). Sample size was determined considering power analyses and logistical aspects. Mapping power is maximised with increasing number of lines whereas within-line repeats n increase confidence of line average phenotypes, which, however, rapidly diminishes as n exceeds four (Belknap, 1998). We have modelled power and effect sizes following (Belknap, 1998): n = (Zα + Zβ)2 / S2QTL / S2Res). Zα and Zβ are Z values for a given α and β; S2QTL is the phenotypic variance due to a QTL and S2Res is the residual variance. With power (1-β) of 80%, α of 0.05 we estimated that with 45 lines we can detect QTL at genome-wide significance explaining ~ 16% of trait variance, which is sufficient mapping power given effect sizes of prior work. We have modelled the relationship between power and number of replicates using qtlDesign (Sen et al., 2007). Everything else being equal power can be optimized by maximizing the number of genotypes (i.e. lines) and reducing replicates, even with varying degrees of heritability. Thus, we set up three replicates using 45 lines, and line averages are mapped, although for some traits and some lines this number may be lower due to lower breeding success. Outliers have been retained as they represent distinct genotypes, evinced by outliers for different traits being from the same line. Interval mapping (Haley and Knott, 1992) relies on 3795 informative SNP markers across all chromosomes, except Y, as implemented in GeneNetwork (GN) (Hager et al., 2012). The BXD strains were genotyped using the MUGA array in 2011, along with genotypes generated earlier using Affymetrix and Illumina platforms (Shifman et al., 2006), and mm9 is used. Loci are identified in GN by the computation of a likelihood statistic score and significance was determined using 2000 permutations of the phenotype data. Candidates were identified within the region defined by using GeneNetwork (http://www.genenetwork.org) and further information combined from QTLminer (Alberts and Schughart, 2010), Entrez genes (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/gene) and Mouse Genome Informatics (Eppig et al., 2015).

Behavioural protocols

Mice were maintained under standard laboratory conditions in the same room, exclusively used for the experiment in individually ventilated cages (IVC Tecniplast Green line), and given chow and water ad lib. Humidity ranges between 50% and 65% relative humidity, temperature between 20°C and 21°C. All animals were kept on a reverse dark light cycle with 12h red light (active phase) and 12h white light.

Cross-fostering of entire litters took place within 24h of birth of corresponding B6 and BXD females and analyses used trait values per pup to adjust for differences in adoptive litter size, which is a significant covariate for provisioning and solicitation as of course a mother nursing a large litter will overall provide more than a mother nursing a small litter. Both mothers and litters were weighed at birth and once weekly, for three weeks until weaning, i.e. at the end of week 1, week 2 and week 3, respectively, to enable the calculation of growth during these periods. In addition, we recorded maternal and offspring behaviour on postnatal days 6, 10 and 14 when we simulated maternal departure to standardize observation conditions (Hager and Johnstone, 2003, 2005). After a 4h separation, mother and litter were re-joined and maternal and offspring behaviours recorded simultaneously over 15 min, using scan sampling every 20 s (Martin and Bateson, 2007). Provisioning is measured using an established protocol (Hager and Johnstone, 2003, 2005, 2007) as maternal and offspring weight change after reunification with pups over the following two hours. Because rodents are nocturnal all observations occurred under red light, i.e. the active phase. Maternal behaviour was recorded as the sum of nursing, suckling and nest building. Nursing is defined as attending the litter, sitting on the nest and suckling up to half the litter while suckling refers to the entire litter being suckled at the same time. This distinction was used as sometimes it cannot be ascertained whether pups are suckling or not because of the position of the mothers in the nest. Nestbuilding behaviour is gathering nesting material and constructing a nest. Pup solicitation behaviour in mice is defined as pups attempting to suck and following the mother, but individual pups were not distinguished. All procedures were approved by the University of Manchester Ethics Committee.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Tucker Gilman, John Fitzpatrick, Casey Bergman and three anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on a previous draft of the manuscript. DGA and BG are supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC), UK and NERC grant NE/F013418/1.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Natural Environment Research Council NE/F013418/1 to Reinmar Hager.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council to David George Ashbrook.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

DGA, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

BG, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

RH, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All procedures were approved by the University of Manchester Ethics Committee.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1:

A) Functional and further details about the genes within the OspIge5.1 QTL for B6 maternal behaviour on day 14, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. Potential candidate genes in this region are Cyp3a16, Cyp3a44, Cyp3a11, Cyp3a25 and Cyp3a41a because of their involvement in steroid hormone biosynthesis. B) Functional and further details about the genes within the MatDge1.1 QTL for BXD nestbuilding on day 6, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. Potential candidate genes in this region include: Hsd17b7, as it is involved in steroid hormone synthesis, and hormonal regulation is needed to initiate nestbuilding behaviour (Gammie et al., 2007; Keisala et al., 2007; Bester-Meredith and Marler, 2012); Pou2f1, Lmx1a and Rgs4 as they are linked to activity related phenotypes; finally, Pbx1 and Ddr2 have been linked to craniofacial morphology, which may affect the ability to make nests (Schneider et al., 2012). C) Functional and further details about the genes within the MatDge10.1 QTL for BXD maternal behaviour on day 6, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. A potential candidate gene is Ifngr1 as it is related to depression-like behaviour, which in turn has been linked to reduced maternal care (Smith et al., 2004; Malkesman et al., 2008). D) Functional and further details about the genes within the OspDge5.1 QTL for BXD offspring solicitation on day 6, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. Cdk5 might be a good candidate as it is involved in several neuronal annotations (e.g. axonogenesis and synaptic transmission), and mutants have no suckling reflex (Ohshima et al., 1996).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.015

elife-11814-supp1.xlsx (101.5KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.015
Supplementary file 2: Details of correlation analyses.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.016

elife-11814-supp2.docx (765.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.016

References

  1. Agrawal AF, Brodie III ED, Brown J. Parent-offspring coadaptation and the dual genetic control of maternal care. Science. 2001;292:1710–1712. doi: 10.1126/science.1059910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alberts R, Schughart K. QTLminer: identifying genes regulating quantitative traits. BMC Bioinformatics. 2010;11:516. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-11-516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andreux PA, Williams EG, Koutnikova H, Houtkooper RH, Champy MF, Henry H, Schoonjans K, Williams RW, Auwerx J. Systems genetics of metabolism: the use of the BXD murine reference panel for multiscalar integration of traits. Cell. 2012;150:1287–1299. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashbrook DG, Williams RW, Lu L, Stein JL, Hibar DP, Nichols TE, Medland SE, Thompson PM, Hager R. Joint genetic analysis of hippocampal size in mouse and human identifies a novel gene linked to neurodegenerative disease. BMC Genomics. 2014;15:850. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-15-850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Belknap JK. Effect of within-strain sample size on QTL detection and mapping using recombinant inbred mouse strains. Behavior Genetics. 1998;28:29–38. doi: 10.1023/a:1021404714631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bester-Meredith JK, Marler CA. Naturally occurring variation in vasopressin immunoreactivity is associated with maternal behavior in female peromyscus mice. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2012;80:244–253. doi: 10.1159/000341899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bijma P, Wade MJ. The joint effects of kin, multilevel selection and indirect genetic effects on response to genetic selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2008;21:1175–1188. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01550.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chesler EJ, Lu L, Shou S, Qu Y, Gu J, Wang J, Hsu HC, Mountz JD, Baldwin NE, Langston MA, Threadgill DW, Manly KF, Williams RW. Complex trait analysis of gene expression uncovers polygenic and pleiotropic networks that modulate nervous system function. Nature Genetics. 2005;37:233–242. doi: 10.1038/ng1518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cowley DE, Pomp D, Atchley WR, Eisen EJ, Hawkins-Brown D. The impact of maternal uterine genotype on postnatal growth and adult body size in mice. Genetics. 1989;122:193–203. doi: 10.1093/genetics/122.1.193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Curley JP, Barton S, Surani A, Keverne EB. Coadaptation in mother and infant regulated by a paternally expressed imprinted gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2004;271:1303–1309. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dupuis J, Siegmund D. Statistical methods for mapping quantitative trait loci from a dense set of markers. Genetics. 1999;151:373–386. doi: 10.1093/genetics/151.1.373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eppig JT, Blake JA, Bult CJ, Kadin JA, Richardson JE, The Mouse Genome Database Group The mouse genome database (mGD): facilitating mouse as a model for human biology and disease. Nucleic Acids Research. 2015;43:D726–D736. doi: 10.1093/nar/gku967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gammie SC, Bethea ED, Stevenson SA. Altered maternal profiles in corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 deficient mice. BMC Neuroscience. 2007;8:17. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Godfray HCJ. Evolutionary theory of parent-offspring conflict. Nature. 1995;376:133–138. doi: 10.1038/376133a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hager R, Johnstone RA. The genetic basis of family conflict resolution in mice. Nature. 2003;421:533–535. doi: 10.1038/nature01239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hager R, Johnstone RA. Differential growth of own and alien pups in mixed litters of mice: a role for genomic imprinting? Ethology. 2005;111:705–714. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01097.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hager R, Johnstone RA. Maternal and offspring effects influence provisioning to mixed litters of own and alien young in mice. Animal Behaviour. 2007;74:1039–1045. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hager R, Lu L, Rosen GD, Williams RW. Genetic architecture supports mosaic brain evolution and independent brain–body size regulation. Nature Communications. 2012;3:1079. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Haley CS, Knott SA. A simple regression method for mapping quantitative trait loci in line crosses using flanking markers. Heredity. 1992;69:315–324. doi: 10.1038/hdy.1992.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hayes KS, Hager R, Grencis RK. Sex-dependent genetic effects on immune responses to a parasitic nematode. BMC Genomics. 2014;15:193. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-15-193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hinde CA, Johnstone RA, Kilner RM. Parent-offspring conflict and coadaptation. Science. 2010;327:1373–1376. doi: 10.1126/science.1186056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Keisala T, Minasyan A, Järvelin U, Wang J, Hämäläinen T, Kalueff AV, Tuohimaa P. Aberrant nest building and prolactin secretion in vitamin d receptor mutant mice. The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology. 2007;104:269–273. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2007.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kilner RM, Hinde CA. Parent–offspring conflict. In: Royle NJ, Smiseth PT, Kölliker M, editors. The Evolution of Parental Care. Oxford, UK:: Oxford University Press;; 2012. pp. 118––132. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kolliker M, Brinkhof MWG, Heeb P, Fitze PS, Richner H. The quantitative genetic basis of offspring solicitation and parental response in a passerine bird with biparental care. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2000;267:2127–2132. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kölliker M, Brodie III ED, Moore AJ. The coadaptation of parental supply and offspring demand. The American Naturalist. 2005;166:506–516. doi: 10.1086/491687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kölliker M, Boos S, Wong JW, Röllin L, Stucki D, Raveh S, Wu M, Meunier J. Parent-offspring conflict and the genetic trade-offs shaping parental investment. Nature Communications. 2015;6 doi: 10.1038/ncomms7850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lock JE, Smiseth PT, Moore AJ. Selection, inheritance, and the evolution of parent‐offspring interactions. The American Naturalist. 2004;164:13–24. doi: 10.1086/421444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lynch CB, Possidente BP. Relationships of maternal nesting to thermoregulatory nesting in house mice (mus musculus) at warm and cold temperatures. Animal Behaviour. 1978;26:1136–1143. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90103-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Malkesman O, Lavi-Avnon Y, Maayan R, Weizman A. A cross-fostering study in a genetic animal model of depression: maternal behavior and depression-like symptoms. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2008;91:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Martin P, Bateson P. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. 3rd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Meunier J, Kölliker M Parental antagonism and parent-offspring co-adaptation interact to shape family life. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;279:3981–3988. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ohshima T, Ward JM, Huh CG, Longenecker G, Veeranna, Pant HC, Brady RO, Martin LJ, Kulkarni AB. Targeted disruption of the cyclin-dependent kinase 5 gene results in abnormal corticogenesis, neuronal pathology and perinatal death. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1996;93:11173–11178. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.20.11173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schneider T, Skitt Z, Liu Y, Deacon RMJ, Flint J, Karmiloff-Smith A, Rawlins JNP, Tassabehji M. Anxious, hypoactive phenotype combined with motor deficits in Gtf2ird1 null mouse model relevant to williams syndrome. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012;233:458–473. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sen S, Satagopan JM, Broman KW, Churchill GA. R/qtlDesign: inbred line cross experimental design. Mammalian Genome. 2007;18:87–93. doi: 10.1007/s00335-006-0090-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Shifman S, Bell JT, Copley RR, Taylor MS, Williams RW, Mott R, Flint J, Gibbs R, Flint J. A high-resolution single nucleotide polymorphism genetic map of the mouse genome. PLoS Biology. 2006;4:e11814. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Smith JW, Seckl JR, Evans AT, Costall B, Smythe JW. Gestational stress induces post-partum depression-like behaviour and alters maternal care in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:227–244. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00025-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Trivers RL. Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist. 1974;14:249–264. doi: 10.1093/icb/14.1.249. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wolf JB, Brodie III ED. The coadaptation of parental and offspring characters. Evolution. 1998;52:299–308. doi: 10.2307/2411068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2015 Dec 23;4:e11814. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11814.017

Decision letter

Editor: Jonathan Flint1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Genetic variation in offspring indirectly influences the quality of maternal behaviour in mice" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Diethard Tautz (Senior editor) and three reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This paper demonstrates that indirect genetic effects can be identified and genetically mapped. The authors adopt a cross-fostering design so that in families either the mothers or the offspring are genetically variable (from the BXD recombinant inbred panel) while the corresponding offspring or mothers show no genetic variation (i.e. are from the B6 inbred strain). The authors identify three indirect genetic effects, and three direct genetic effects. Overall this study represents a substantial and well-designed effort to address an important question.

Essential revisions:

1) Design limitations need to be acknowledged and discussed. In their design the in-utero maternal effects are perfectly confounded with offspring genotypes. This means that the results of part 1 could be interpreted as "Variation in BXD mothers genotypes causes variation in in-utero environment, which causes variation in BXD offspring behaviour later in early life, which causes variation in B6 adoptive mothers provisioning". This would effectively be IGE from BXD mothers on B6 mothers.

The results of part 2 could be interpreted as "Variation in BXD mothers genotypes causes variation in in-utero environment, which causes variation in BxD offspring weight gain and solicitation in early life" and Variation in BXD mothers genotypes independently causes variation in BXD biological mothers provisioning, in which case there would not be coadaptation between BXD offspring phenotypes and BXD maternal behaviour.

This caveat is sufficiently important that its implications should be discussed in the Discussion. As the authors note in the Discussion however, overcoming this limitation would be extremely challenging.

2) The results are only just significant. Given that they have tested multiple behaviors, and both direct and indirect effects, the chances are that a proportion of the loci are likely false positives. The authors should assess a false discovery rate and provide results corrected for the total number of tests carried out.

3) The mapping approach is relatively un-sophisticated and in particular environmental effects receive scant attention. Cage effects should be accounted for in all the analyses. In addition there may be unacknowledged environmental effects, which might be captured if they included the time when the experiment was carried out as a covariate. The fact that results for the genetic effects only just achieve significance means that there is a concern that even slight contributions from covariates may be giving rise to false positives.

4) It wasn't clear why they used per pup values. Why not fit litter size should be fitted as a covariate? They should repeat the analyses with litter fitted as a covariate.

5) The phenotypic correlation observed between BXD offspring solicitation and B6 maternal behaviour could arise from environmental effects only as BXD offspring and B6 mothers share a cage. It is not strong evidence that BXD genotypes affect B6 maternal behaviour, and can only weakly be linked to the QTLs identified as no overlap was found between DGE QTLs for offspring solicitation and IGE QTLs for maternal behaviour. Genetic correlation should be computed from the individual measurements.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Genetic variation in offspring indirectly influences the quality of maternal behaviour in mice" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by Diethard Tautz (Senior editor) and three reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:

You state that you have "used the correct litter size to calculate per pup measures, namely adoptive litter size." That a litter size effect exists does not imply that per pup values are appropriate, i.e., it does not prove that per pup values are independent of litter size. This would only be correct if you could prove that total provisioning is exactly linear in litter size; the mere significance of a (linear) litter size effect is no proof thereof.

Intuitively, one would expect that provisioning per pup decreases with litter size, simply because there is some sort of limit to total provisioning that is feasible. If you can fit a litter size effect on total provisioning using linear models, then you can also do this on provisioning defined per pup.

While this may not be feasible in the Genenetwork software that you used for the mapping, it could be investigated in a prior analysis using simple linear models. Then the results should be reported in the manuscript, and if the litter size effect on per pup provisioning is significant in the prior analysis, then you should acknowledge in the manuscript that ideally a covariate for litter size would have been included in the mapping.

The correlation/covariance that may exist between direct and indirect environmental effects (IDE and IEE) should be modelled when calculating the correlation between mother and offspring phenotypes as it may increase or decrease the correlation reported in the manuscript.

Example of such a correlation between IDE and IEE: differences in noise across cages may lead to B6 mothers in noisy cages showing reduced maternal behaviour and BXD offspring in those same cages soliciting less (or more).

To account for this covariance, you would need to focus on the genetic component of the correlation and use the replicate structure of the data (3 replicates per genotype) to calculate something analogous to a genetic correlation but where phenotypes are measured in B6 mothers and BXD offspring (instead of in the same individual as is traditional) and the genotypes would be those of the BXD offspring.

Although a sentence was added in the Discussion with regard to in-utero effects, you still need to put the results of part 1 in perspective and address interpretation of the results of part 2 (co-adaptation).

eLife. 2015 Dec 23;4:e11814. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11814.018

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) Design limitations need to be acknowledged and discussed. In their design the in-utero maternal effects are perfectly confounded with offspring genotypes. This means that the results of part 1 could be interpreted as "Variation in BXD mothers genotypes causes variation in in-utero environment, which causes variation in BXD offspring behaviour later in early life, which causes variation in B6 adoptive mothers provisioning". This would effectively be IGE from BXD mothers on B6 mothers.

The results of part 2 could be interpreted as "Variation in BXD mothers genotypes causes variation in in-utero environment, which causes variation in BxD offspring weight gain and solicitation in early life" and Variation in BXD mothers genotypes independently causes variation in BXD biological mothers provisioning, in which case there would not be coadaptation between BXD offspring phenotypes and BXD maternal behaviour.

This caveat is sufficiently important that its implications should be discussed in the Discussion. As the authors note in the Discussion however, overcoming this limitation would be extremely challenging.

We agree, and have thus expanded the first paragraph of the Discussion following this suggestion though we note coadaptation may well be manifest in utero (see e.g. Wolf and Hager 2006 PLoS Biol 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040380, and 2009 in J Evol Biol 22:2519).

2) The results are only just significant. Given that they have tested multiple behaviors, and both direct and indirect effects, the chances are that a proportion of the loci are likely false positives. The authors should assess a false discovery rate and provide results corrected for the total number of tests carried out.

The experiment was designed to test the hypothesis that genetic variation in BXD offspring can affect trait variation in B6 maternal behaviour and vice versa in the context of family interactions. While we need protection against many false discoveries we need, at the same time, assurance we are not neglecting truly interesting locations (see e.g. Chen and Storey 2005 Genetics 173: 2371–2381). Traditionally, LOD scores above 3.3 are considered in interval mapping studies and we follow the convention of previous studies on maternal behaviour (e.g. Peripato et al. 2002 Genetics 162:1341) and other multiple complex traits (Cheverud et al. 2008 PNAS 105:4253, Wolf et al. 2008 PLoS Genet 4:e1000091, Hager et al. 2012 Nat Commun 3:1079) in reporting significance both at the genome-wide and suggestive level, which we exceed with five offspring and maternal traits for all loci (0.63/5 or 10,respectively).

In addition, our result that variation in maternal behaviour is affected by our identified (IGE) in offspring is further supported by the phenotypic correlation: “offspring solicitation behaviour in […] BXD pups is positively correlated with the level of maternal behaviour […] in B6 adoptive mothers on all three days we measured behaviour with significant effects of maternal behaviour and day on offspring solicitation[…]” (Results, second paragraph), notwithstanding the caveat added in the Discussion about in-utero effects.

3) The mapping approach is relatively un-sophisticated and in particular environmental effects receive scant attention. Cage effects should be accounted for in all the analyses. In addition there may be unacknowledged environmental effects, which might be captured if they included the time when the experiment was carried out as a covariate. The fact that results for the genetic effects only just achieve significance means that there is a concern that even slight contributions from covariates may be giving rise to false positives.

Please see next point.

4) It wasn't clear why they used per pup values. Why not fit litter size should be fitted as a covariate? They should repeat the analyses with litter fitted as a covariate.

In reply to points 3 and 4: First, we need to clarify the per pup measure used as it appears that this will answer several points raised. We did not record individual pup behaviour (where cage effects may appear) but rather for the entire litter and the per pup measure divides the score by litter size. This is because mothers may differ e.g. in their provisioning not because they have different genotypes but simply because they nurse litters of different sizes. Using linear models we found significant litter size effects. Thus, using a per pup measure is fully justified and appropriate, as well as supported by statistics, and we have used the correct litter size to calculate per pup measures, namely adoptive litter size. We have clarified this in the Methods section now. All our analyses are conduced within Genenetwork, the analytical tool kit for the BXD population (www.genenetwork.org) used in over 1000 papers, which at present does not allow running models with covariates but we understand this will be implemented in future.

While we unfortunately do not have the time of each recording we have experimentally ensured as much as possible to standardize motivation in mothers and offspring. This is a technique we have employed and developed over years (e.g. Hager and Johnstone 2003 Nature 421:533, 2005 Ethology 111:705, 2007 Anim Behav 74:139, Lyst et al. 2012 PLoS One 7:e47640) for the following reasons: consider the scenario of measuring nursing behaviour. Prior to the experiment on the day we have no information about whether mothers have just been nursing their pups or whether the last bout of nursing was several hours ago. Thus, we might see a difference in the pups’ motivation to suck, and in the mothers to nurse, not because of differences in genotypes but because of differences in the times that elapsed between the last feed. Therefore, we standardize the time prior to us measuring nursing.

5) The phenotypic correlation observed between BXD offspring solicitation and B6 maternal behaviour (part 1.2) could arise from environmental effects only as BXD offspring and B6 mothers share a cage. It is not strong evidence that BXD genotypes affect B6 maternal behaviour, and can only weakly be linked to the QTLs identified as no overlap was found between DGE QTLs for offspring solicitation and IGE QTLs for maternal behaviour. Genetic correlation should be computed from the individual measurements.

Cage effects may arise by a shared environment among multiple offspring that have been individually phenotyped but this is not how offspring traits were measured (see previous comment). Given that one family (i.e. one BXD genotype plus B6) occupied one cage we do not think that this effect applies here, and we have published correlations using the same protocol multiply before (e.g. Hager and Johnstone 2003 Nature 421:533, 2005 Ethology 111:705, 2006a,b Biol Lett 2:81 and 2:253). Further, our result that variation in maternal behaviour is affected by genes expressed in offspring is supported by phenotypic results: offspring solicitation behaviour in genetically variable BXD pups is positively correlated with the level of maternal behaviour in their genetically uniform B6 adoptive mothers on all three days we measured behaviour with significant effects of maternal behaviour and day on offspring solicitation. We appreciate the point the reviewer is making regarding an overlap of DGE QTL and IGE QTL, but no direct support is provided and we have no reason to assume that DGE QTL and IGE QTL have to overlap to support this argument.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Intuitively, one would expect that provisioning per pup decreases with litter size, simply because there is some sort of limit to total provisioning that is feasible. If you can fit a litter size effect on total provisioning using linear models, then you can also do this on provisioning defined per pup. While this may not be feasible in the Genenetwork software that you used for the mapping, it could be investigated in a prior analysis using simple linear models. Then the results should be reported in the manuscript, and if the litter size effect on per pup provisioning is significant in the prior analysis, then you should acknowledge in the manuscript that ideally a covariate for litter size would have been included in the mapping. We have run linear models as suggested and can confirm that litter size is a non-significant predictor for all our per pup provisioning traits.

The correlation/covariance that may exist between direct and indirect environmental effects (IDE and IEE) should be modelled when calculating the correlation between mother and offspring phenotypes as it may increase or decrease the correlation reported in the manuscript.

Example of such a correlation between IDE and IEE: differences in noise across cages may lead to B6 mothers in noisy cages showing reduced maternal behaviour and BXD offspring in those same cages soliciting less (or more). To account for this covariance, you would need to focus on the genetic component of the correlation and use the replicate structure of the data (3 replicates per genotype) to calculate something analogous to a genetic correlation but where phenotypes are measured in B6 mothers and BXD offspring (instead of in the same individual as is traditional) and the genotypes would be those of the BXD offspring.

The example given that a correlation between B6 maternal behaviour and BXD offspring solicitation could arise because all B6 shared the same environment (e.g. noise) is a very unlikely scenario as all animals are kept in the same small room (exclusively used by us), which is sound proofed, and in IVC cages, each with the same conditions. We note that the conditions experienced by B6 females are identical except the genotypes of the adoptive BXD litters, and that correlations are calculated per maternal genotype, not cage; and have added some additional information about housing to the Methods.

Although a sentence was added in the Discussion with regard to in-utero effects, you still need to put the results of part 1 in perspective and address interpretation of the results of part 2 (co-adaptation).

Yes, agreed. We have added potential implications of in-utero effects to that section in the Discussion.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing the B6 maternal nestbuilding behaviour on day 6 per pup for the BXD lines.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.005

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.005
    Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing the B6 maternal behaviour on day 14 per pup for the BXD lines.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.007

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.007
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing the B6 offspring growth in the second postnatal week per pup for the BXD lines.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.009

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.009
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing BXD offspring short-term weight change on day 10 per pup, BXD maternal provisioning on day 10, BXD offspring solicitation on day 14 per pup and BXD maternal provisioning on day 14 for the BXD lines.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.012

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.012
    Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5 as uploaded to GeneNetwork, showing BXD offspring solicitation on day 10 per pup, BXD offspring weight on day 10 per pup, BXD offspring solicitation on day 14 per pup and BXD offspring weight on day 14 per pup for the BXD lines.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.014

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.014
    Supplementary file 1:

    A) Functional and further details about the genes within the OspIge5.1 QTL for B6 maternal behaviour on day 14, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. Potential candidate genes in this region are Cyp3a16, Cyp3a44, Cyp3a11, Cyp3a25 and Cyp3a41a because of their involvement in steroid hormone biosynthesis. B) Functional and further details about the genes within the MatDge1.1 QTL for BXD nestbuilding on day 6, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. Potential candidate genes in this region include: Hsd17b7, as it is involved in steroid hormone synthesis, and hormonal regulation is needed to initiate nestbuilding behaviour (Gammie et al., 2007; Keisala et al., 2007; Bester-Meredith and Marler, 2012); Pou2f1, Lmx1a and Rgs4 as they are linked to activity related phenotypes; finally, Pbx1 and Ddr2 have been linked to craniofacial morphology, which may affect the ability to make nests (Schneider et al., 2012). C) Functional and further details about the genes within the MatDge10.1 QTL for BXD maternal behaviour on day 6, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. A potential candidate gene is Ifngr1 as it is related to depression-like behaviour, which in turn has been linked to reduced maternal care (Smith et al., 2004; Malkesman et al., 2008). D) Functional and further details about the genes within the OspDge5.1 QTL for BXD offspring solicitation on day 6, obtained from GeneNetwork, Entrez genes, and Mouse Genome Informatics. Cdk5 might be a good candidate as it is involved in several neuronal annotations (e.g. axonogenesis and synaptic transmission), and mutants have no suckling reflex (Ohshima et al., 1996).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.015

    elife-11814-supp1.xlsx (101.5KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.015
    Supplementary file 2: Details of correlation analyses.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.11814.016

    elife-11814-supp2.docx (765.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.11814.016

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES