Abstract
Memories are encoded within sparsely distributed neuronal ensembles. However, the defining cellular properties of neurons within a memory trace remain incompletely understood. Using a fluorescence-based Arc reporter, we were able to visually identify the distinct subset of lateral amygdala (LA) neurons activated during auditory fear conditioning. We found that Arc-expressing neurons have enhanced intrinsic excitability and are preferentially recruited into newly encoded memory traces. Furthermore, synaptic potentiation of thalamic inputs to the LA during fear conditioning is learning-specific, postsynaptically mediated and highly localized to Arc-expressing neurons. Taken together, our findings validate the immediate-early gene Arc as a molecular marker for the LA neuronal ensemble recruited during fear learning. Moreover, these results establish a model of fear memory formation in which intrinsic excitability determines neuronal selection, whereas learning-related encoding is governed by synaptic plasticity.
Introduction
Fear conditioning is a robust form of associative learning in which a previously neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) comes to predict an aversive unconditioned event, eliciting defensive behaviors and fearful emotions.1 The neurobiological circuitry underlying auditory fear learning has been extensively investigated, wherein there is overwhelming evidence that the lateral amygdala (LA) is a critical site of plasticity.2, 3 In particular, long-term N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-dependent synaptic potentiation of glutamatergic inputs onto LA principal neurons remains the leading candidate mechanism for fear memory encoding.4 Accordingly, both genetic and pharmacological blockade of synaptic plasticity in the LA prevent the formation of long-term fear memories,5, 6, 7, 8, 9 whereas potentiation of glutamatergic synaptic transmission onto LA pyramidal neurons is induced by fear conditioning.5, 6, 10, 11, 12
Intriguingly, only a limited subset of neurons appears to be recruited during fear memory encoding. In particular, recent studies have implicated the cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) as a critical factor guiding LA neuron recruitment into a fear memory network. Targeted restoration of CREB expression selectively into the LA of CREB-deficient mice is sufficient to fully restore auditory fear conditioning.13 Furthermore, optogenetic activation of neurons with elevated CREB levels at the time of training is sufficient to induce fear memory retrieval.14 Moreover, studies using virus-mediated mosaic overexpression of CREB in wild-type mice have shown that recruitment of LA neurons during fear learning is not merely a cell autonomous process,13 but rather is dependent upon relative neuronal excitability at the time of learning.15, 16 Importantly, however, the hypothesis that the recruitment of LA neurons into fear memory networks is determined by their relative excitability has never been evaluated under endogenous physiological conditions.
Recent computational modeling has proposed that the encoding of fear memories in the LA is constrained to a limited subset of neurons by the local microcircuitry through a combination of intrinsic excitability and synaptic plasticity.17 Consistent with this model, in vivo extracellular single-unit recordings have demonstrated that only a minority of LA neurons undergo significant changes in tone-evoked firing during auditory fear conditioning.18, 19 Furthermore, ex vivo whole-cell patch-clamp recordings also found that learning-induced plasticity was restricted to a limited subset of LA neurons.5
Recent studies have provided strong experimental support that immediate-early genes (IEGs), including the proto-oncogene c-Fos and the activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc), represent time-limited molecular tags of these sparsely encoded neurons in both sensory representations20, 21, 22 and memory networks.13, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27 Inhibition or ablation of IEG-tagged neurons disrupts the recall and maintenance of fear-conditioning memories, respectively.15, 28 Conversely, artificial activation of this sparse IEG-tagged population is sufficient to induce fear memory recall29 or falsely modify contextual memories.30, 31 Importantly, however, no previous studies have performed targeted electrophysiological recordings from a defined memory trace, a crucial step towards achieving a comprehensive understanding of how the brain encodes learned associations.
Therefore, to investigate the neurophysiological properties of individual LA neurons recruited during fear conditioning, we utilized a fluorescence-based reporter of Arc as a time-limited molecular tag of these sparsely encoded neurons. We found that neurons with elevated baseline intrinsic excitability were preferentially recruited into the fear memory network. Furthermore, synaptic potentiation of thalamic inputs to the LA during fear conditioning was learning-specific and highly localized to Arc-expressing neurons. Taken together, our findings establish a model of fear memory formation in which intrinsic excitability determines neuronal selection, whereas learning-related encoding is governed by synaptic plasticity.
Materials and methods
Animals
Arc::dVenus mice were backcrossed more than 10 generations into C57BL/6J.32 Mice were maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. All experiments were performed during the light phase, using adult mice (postnatal weeks 8–11). Mice were individually housed for 5 days prior to the start of experiments. Randomization was assigned based on the outcome of the littermate genotyping, and experimenter blinding was performed whenever possible. All experiments were approved by the Dutch Ethical Committee and in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) guidelines.
Auditory fear conditioning
Fear conditioning was performed using a Med Associates Standard Fear Conditioning chamber (30.5 cm × 24.1 cm × 21.0 cm) with a stainless steel electrifiable grid floor, and enclosed within a larger sound-attenuating box. Video images were recorded using a progressive scan CCD video camera with a visible light filter suitable for near-infrared imaging. Mice in the naïve group received no handling or exposure to the training context. Naïve mice remained in their standard housing conditions until immediately prior to behavioral testing for CS-evoked freezing, perfusion for confocal imaging or killing for electrophysiology. In contrast, mice in the unpaired and paired training groups were habituated to the conditioning chamber, 24 h prior the training session. Habituation sessions consisted of a 30 min exposure to the training context without any tone or shock presentations. On the day of conditioning, mice receiving paired training were placed in the conditioning chamber for 180 s, followed by a series of three co-terminating presentations of a tone CS (30 s, 5 KHz, 85 dB) and scrambled footshock unconditioned stimulus (US) (2 s, 0.75 mA). The intertrial interval between tone-shock presentations was 210 s. The experiments shown in Supplementary Figure 2 comparing the strength of conditioning and Arc-dVenus activation following 1, 3 or 9 CS-US pairings used independent groups of mice. Training was implemented using the same parameters (180 s placement-to-shock interval, 210 s interstimulus interval) and CS/US stimuli as the paired condition. Mice in the unpaired group received the identical CS and US stimuli but in an explicitly unpaired sequence. The unpaired protocol consisted of 3 US presentations (10 s interstimulus interval) in which the first shock was delivered immediately upon placement in the chamber, and followed by 3 CS presentations initiated 400 s after the last US presentation (90 s interstimulus interval). Previous studies using similar explicitly unpaired controls have demonstrated that subjects acquire minimal or no associative fear of the CS.1, 33 Tone-evoked freezing was tested 24 h after conditioning in a novel context (120 s baseline, 180 s tone). Freezing was defined as the cessation of all movement except for respiration and scored using an automated algorithm.34
Immunofluorescence
After deep anesthesia induced by intra-peritoneal injection of pentobarbital (50 mg kg−1), mice were transcardially perfused with saline, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were dissected and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 2 h at 4 °C. After post-fixation, the brains were transferred into 10% sucrose phosphate buffer (PB 0.1 M, pH 7.3) and stored overnight at 4 °C. Embedding was performed in a 10% gelatin+10% sucrose block, with fixation in 10% paraformaldehyde+30% sucrose solution for 2 h at room temperature and immersed in 30% sucrose at 4 °C. Forty micrometer coronal sections were collected serially (rostral to caudal) using a freezing microtome (Leica, Wetzlar, Germany; SM 2000R) and stored in 0.1 M PB. Free-floating sections were incubated in sodium citrate (10 mM) at 80 °C for 1 h and rinsed with tris-buffered saline (TBS, pH 7.6). Sections were pre-incubated with a blocking TBS buffer containing 0.5% Triton X-100 and 10% normal horse serum (NHS; Invitrogen, Bleiswijk, The Netherlands) for 1 h at room temperature. Sections were incubated in a mixture of primary antibodies, in TBS buffer containing 0.4% Triton X-100 and 2% NHS for 72 h at 4 °C.
The following primary antibodies were used: mouse anti-NeuN (1:2000, Millipore, Hertfordshire, UK; MAB377), goat anti-choline acetyltransferase (1:200, Millipore AB144P), rabbit anti-Tbr1 (1:2000, Millipore AB10554), mouse anti-Arc (C-7, 1:200, Santa Cruz sc-17839, Heidelberg, Germany), rabbit anti-c-Fos (ab-5, 1:10000, Millipore PC38), mouse anti-GAD67 (1:1000, Millipore MAB5406). Sections were washed with TBS, and incubated with corresponding Alexa-conjugated secondary antibodies (1:200, Invitrogen) and cyanine dyes (1:200, Sanbio, Uden, The Netherlands) in TBS buffer containing 0.4% Triton X-100, 2% NHS for 2 h at room temperature. For some experiments, nuclear staining was performed using DAPI (1:100, Invitrogen). Sections were washed with PB 0.1 M and mounted on slides, cover slipped with Vectashield H1000 fluorescent mounting medium (Vector Labs, Peterborough, UK), and sealed.
Fluorescent in situ hybridization
Fluorescent in situ hybridization was performed using mice perfused at 5 min post training, to optimally visualize nuclear foci of dVenus and Arc transcription.23 Coronal brain sections (40 μm) were collected in RNAse-free 0.1 M PB as described in the immunofluorescence section. The cDNA templates encoding the following mRNAs were used for single-stranded RNA probe synthesis: Arc/Arg3.1 (3.5 kb, full length probe, GeneID: 11838; Image Clone number: 349057; generously provided by J. Holstege and M. Hosseini); Venus fluorescent protein (720 kb probe from pISH-Venus Addgene plasmid 15865, kindly deposited by P. Mombaerts). The riboprobes were obtained by linearizing the recombinant plasmids with the appropriate restriction enzymes (Fermentas, Bleiswijk, The Netherlands; New England BioLabs, Hitchin, UK) and RNA polymerases. Transcription was performed in the presence of digoxigenin or fluorescein-labeled 11-UTP (Roche, Almere, The Netherlands), for Venus or Arc riboprobes, respectively, using a commercial RNA labeling kit (Roche). Riboprobes were purified by standard LiCl precipitation protocol. Integrity and yield of riboprobes was confirmed by gel electrophoresis and Nanodrop spectrophotometry (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). All solutions used until the completion of hybridization were treated with Diethylpyrocarbonat to ensure optimal RNAse-free working conditions.
The protocol used for fluorescent in situ hybridization was adapted from Hossaini et al.35 Free-floating sections were first washed in 0.1 M PB, treated for 5 min with 0.2% glycine in PBS, rinsed in PBS and fixed for 10 min in 4% paraformaldehyde. After another rinse in PBS, sections were treated (10 min) in PBS containing 0.1 M triethanolamine (Merck, Hertfordshire, UK) pH 8.0 and 0.0025% acetic anhydride (Sigma-Aldrich, Munich, Germany). Sections were then washed in 4 × standard saline citrate (pH 4.5) and prehybridized for 1 h at 65 °C in hybridization solution consisting of 5 × standard saline citrate (pH 4.5), 50% formamide (Sigma-Aldrich), 2% Blocking Reagent (Roche), 0.05% 3-[(3-cholamidopropyl)dimethylammonio]-1-propanesulfonate (Sigma-Aldrich), 1 μg ml−1 yeast tRNA (tRNA brewer's yeast, Sigma-Aldrich), 50 μg ml−1 Heparin (Sigma-Aldrich) and 5 mM ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (pH 8.0, Sigma-Aldrich). Sections were hybridized for 18–24 h at 65 °C in hybridization solution containing 1.2 μg ml−1 of each anti-sense riboprobe, Arc/Arg3.1 and Venus. After hybridization, sections were washed in 2 × standard saline citrate (pH 4.5), followed by three washes of 15 min at 65 °C in 2 × standard saline citrate (pH 4.5) and 50% formamide, and a final wash in PBS. The sections were then pre-incubated for 90 min at room temperature in blocking buffer, consisting of 0.5% Triton X-100 and 10% NHS in TBS. For detection of the digoxigenin and fluorescein tags in riboprobes, sections were incubated in 0.4% Triton X-100 and 2% NHS in TBS (pH 7.6), with primary sheep polyclonal anti-digoxigenin (1:500, Thermo Scientific) and mouse monoclonal anti-fluorescein (1:500, Roche) antibodies, for 72 h at 4 °C. Subsequently, sections were washed with TBS and detection of anti-digoxigenin and anti-fluorescein primary antibodies was carried out using anti-sheep Cy3 from donkey (1:200, Jackson Laboratories, Bar Harbor, ME, USA) and anti-mouse Alexa647 from donkey (1:200, Jackson Laboratories), respectively, in 0.4% Triton X-100 and 2% NHS in TBS (pH 7.6) at room temperature, for 2 h. Sections were then washed in 0.1 M PB and stained using DAPI (1:100, Invitrogen) as a nuclear marker. Sections were then mounted on slides, cover slipped with Vectashield H1000 fluorescent mounting medium (Vector Labs) and sealed.
Confocal imaging
Stained LA images were acquired using a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope (Carl Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) equipped with Zeiss Plan-Apochromat × 10/0.45, × 20/0.8 and × 40/1.3 (oil immersion) objectives. Native dVenus, Cy3, Alexa647 and DAPI were imaged using the excitation wavelengths of 488, 555, 639 and 405 nm, respectively. Native dVenus fluorescence intensity was quantified using ImageJ (NIH, 1.42q) with the Multi Measure plug-in. The mean fluorescence intensity of each Arc-dVenus+ neuron was determined by drawing a region of interest around the cell soma.
Stereology
Coronal brain sections were collected serially through the entire extent of the LA of each mouse, with a section thickness of 40 μm and interval distance of 160 μm (Supplementary Figure 1). Sections were immunofluorescently labeled with anti-NeuN and anti-choline acetyltransferase to identify mature neurons and to define the border between the lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala,36 respectively. Furthermore, to optimally standardize the stereological analysis and in light of recent findings demonstrating hemispheric lateralization of Arc expression within the insular cortex following taste learning,37 all stereological and fluorescence intensity data were collected exclusively from the left hemisphere.
Stereological estimation of the total population (NeuN+) and Arc-dVenus+ subset of neurons was performed using the Optical Fractionator probe within Stereo Investigator (version 10, MBF Bioscience, Williston, VT, USA). Stacks of confocal images (156 × 156 × 1 μm) across the thickness of the sections (with a separation level of 1 μm) of Arc-dVenus+ and NeuN+ neurons were systematically collected. A counting frame size of 100 μm × 100 μm was used to mark Arc-dVenus+ neurons throughout the entire grid, using an exhaustive sampling configuration. NeuN+ cells were counted using 35 μm × 35 μm counting frames, which were selected in a systematic random procedure by the analysis software. The grid size of both exhaustive and random sampling configurations was set to 100 μm × 100 μm.
The section thickness was assessed empirically at every sampling site to precisely calculate any potential thickness variation across the sections as a result of post-processing of the tissue. Guard zones (2 μm) were used at the top and bottom of each section with a dissector height of 15 μm. Accuracy in the estimation of the total number of quantified cells per subject was estimated using the coefficient of error equations.38, 39, 40 Coefficient of error values were <0.1 in all mice analyzed.
Brain slice electrophysiology
Mice were anesthetized using isoflurane, decapitated and the brain dissected in ice-cold modified artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing the following (in mM): 110 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 1 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 10 glucose, 0.2 ascorbate, 0.2 thiourea. Acute coronal slices (300 μm) containing the LA were cut using a vibratome (Microm 650 V, Thermo Scientific) and transferred to a storage chamber in ACSF, saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2 and maintained at 32–34 °C. After at least 1 h of recovery time, slices were transferred to the recording chamber where they were continuously perfused with oxygenated ACSF at a perfusion rate of 1.5–2 ml min−1.
Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings of LA neurons were performed at 32–34 °C under infrared differential interference contrast visual guidance using an upright microscope (Nikon, Tokyo, Japan; Eclipse E600FN). Arc-dVenus+ fluorescence cells were detected via illumination of a mercury lamp using a YFP filter (Semrock, Rochester, NY, USA). Borosilicate glass pipettes (4–7 MOhm) were connected to an Axon Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA, USA) and data were acquired at 20 KHz, filtered at 3 KHz, stored and analyzed using the pClamp software (pClamp 10, Molecular Devices). Pipettes were filled with the following medium (in mM): 130 KMeSO3, 11 KCl, 10 HEPES, 5 NaCl, 0.1 EGTA, 1 MgCl2, 2 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 5 phosphocreatine, 50 U ml−1 creatin phosphokinase, the pH was adjusted to 7.2 and osmolarity to 290 mOsm. Slices were continuously superfused with ACSF, saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2 and maintained at 32–34 °C. Liquid junction potential was left uncorrected. Except for measurements of intrinsic properties, the GABAA receptor blocker picrotoxin (00 μM) was added to the ACSF. Large, pyramidal-like somata were visualized targeted for recordings, and readily distinguished from fast-spiking neurons, characteristic of LA interneurons.41, 42, 43 No fast-spiking neurons were found in the Arc-dVenus+ population, consistent with Arc expression in the LA being limited to glutamatergic principal neurons.44
Passive membrane properties were analyzed using a 10 mV hyperpolarizing voltage step in voltage-clamp mode. Resting membrane potential was measured immediately after establishing the whole-cell configuration. Single action potentials (APs) were evoked by a 10 ms current injection whose amplitude was minimally sufficient to reach the threshold from a potential of −75 mV. The threshold was defined as the inflection point at the foot of the regenerative upstroke. AP amplitude and after-hyperpolarizing potential were measured from the threshold to the peak and to the maximal hyperpolarizing value, respectively. AP duration was measured at half of the maximal amplitude.
For evoked postsynaptic currents, thalamic fibers of the ventral part of the striatum were stimulated using a bipolar Platinum-Iridium electrode (FHC, Bowdoin, ME, USA). Postsynaptic responses were recorded from Arc-dVenus− and Arc-dVenus+ neighboring cells, thereby reducing interslice variability. Input-output curves were constructed by varying the stimulus intensity from 0 to 200 μA (in 25 μA increments) at 0.1 Hz. Excitatory postsynaptic current (EPSC) amplitude was normalized by the cell capacitance. Paired-pulse ratio was analyzed as the ratio of the second to the first EPSC resulting from two consecutive stimulations, in which the interstimulus interval ranged from 25 to 100 ms (in 15 ms increments) and from 100 to 300 ms (in 25 ms increments). For α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA)/NMDA recordings, the intracellular solution was modified by substituting KMeSO3 and KCl with CsMeSO3 and CsCl, respectively. The AMPA component was measured as the peak current recorded at −70 mV. The NMDA component was recorded at +40 mV (measured 100 ms after stimulus onset), and entirely blocked in the presence of 1-amino-phosphovaleric acid (50 μM).
Statistical analysis
Significance of observations was established by unpaired Student's t test or analysis of variance followed by Tukey's post hoc test. Cumulative probability distributions of fluorescence intensity were compared using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Data are expressed as mean±s.e.m. Significance threshold was set at P<0.05 for all statistical comparisons.
Results
Arc-dVenus expression accurately reflects endogenous Arc transcription
To visualize LA neurons recruited during fear learning, we utilized a recently engineered mouse line expressing destabilized Venus fluorescent protein (dVenus) under the control of a transgenic Arc promoter (Arc::dVenus mice), thereby leaving the endogenous Arc genes unmodified.32 Hence, these mice function as a fluorescence-based reporter of Arc transcription without interfering with the function of endogenous Arc itself. Using compartmental analysis of temporal gene transcription by fluorescence in situ hybridization,23 we confirmed the high co-localization of Arc-dVenus and endogenous Arc nuclear RNA in the LA after auditory fear conditioning, thereby demonstrating the validity of Arc::dVenus reporter mice for visualizing LA cells with recent endogenous Arc activation (Figure 1b).
To examine the specificity of Arc-dVenus activation during fear learning, we used three independent groups: naïve (homecage) controls, explicitly unpaired presentations of tone and shock, or paired tone-shock conditioning (Figure 1a). Mice were killed 5 h after fear conditioning, consistent with previous reports demonstrating that maximal experience-driven Arc-dVenus expression occurs within 4–6 h.32, 45 Arc-dVenus fluorescence was robustly increased in the LA of mice receiving paired training (Figure 1e). In contrast, naïve mice (Figure 1c) and those receiving unpaired training (Figures 1d) showed relatively weaker fluorescence, confirming the specificity of Arc-dVenus activation in the LA to fear learning.
Previous studies have demonstrated that endogenous Arc expression is localized to principal neurons within the forebrain.44 Therefore, to confirm the cell-type specificity of the Arc-dVenus reporter in the LA, we performed immunohistochemical labeling with antibodies against Tbr1 or GAD67, markers for glutamatergic projection neurons or GABAergic interneurons, respectively.46 Indeed, we found that Arc-dVenus+ neurons in the LA were always NeuN+ (Figures 1c–e) and Tbr1+ (Figure 1f). Conversely, we never observed an Arc-dVenus+ neuron that was GAD67+ (Figure 1g), thereby confirming that Arc-dVenus expression is exclusively limited to glutamatergic neurons in the LA, consistent with the cell-type specificity of endogenous Arc.
Fear learning robustly and selectively induces Arc-dVenus expression
Using confocal stereology, we quantified the percentage and fluorescence intensity of Arc-dVenus+ neurons in the LA following fear conditioning (Figure 2; Supplementary Figure 1). In naïve mice, only weak levels of Arc-dVenus fluorescence were detectable in LA neurons, consistent with the low baseline expression of endogenous Arc.47 Unpaired conditioning did not influence the percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons. In contrast, the percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons observed in mice receiving paired conditioning was significantly increased compared with naïve (P<0.01) and unpaired (P<0.05) groups (Figure 2b). Moreover, paired training induced a strong right-shift of the cumulative probability distribution of Arc-dVenus fluorescence, compared with naïve (P<0.0001) and unpaired (P<0.001) conditions (Figure 2c). Together, our findings indicate that the induction of Arc-dVenus expression in the LA during fear conditioning is highly specific for associative learning, compared with non-associative sensory stimulation.
To further explore the relationship between the strength of learning, percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons and dVenus fluorescence intensity, we used independent groups of mice trained with 1, 3 or 9 CS-US pairings. Stereological analysis demonstrated an asymptotic percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons beyond 3 CS-US pairings, which closely paralleled the CS-evoked freezing curve (Supplementary Figures 2A and B). Notably, however, despite a similar strength of conditioning and percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons, mice receiving 9 CS-US pairings had a significantly increased dVenus fluorescence intensity compared with mice receiving only 3 CS-US pairings (Supplementary Figures 2C and E). Therefore, successive CS-US pairings do not recruit cells randomly within the LA, but instead result in a highly overlapping re-activation of a similar neuronal subpopulation.
Recent models of memory formation have hypothesized that at any given time, a limited subset of neurons exist in an a priori primed state, which could serve to preferentially bias their allocation into a newly encoded associative memory trace.13, 15, 16, 17, 31, 48, 49 Therefore, we considered the possible mechanisms by which cellular activation in the LA could transform the Arc-dVenus fluorescence intensity curve from the baseline (naïve) state to the distribution observed after fear conditioning (Figure 2c). In particular, the rightward shift in the Arc-dVenus fluorescence intensity distribution could have resulted from two non-mutually exclusive possibilities: (i) In Figure 2b, we observed a ~50% increase in the number of Arc-dVenus+ neurons in the LA following paired training. Accordingly, if these newly Arc-dVenus+ neurons are predominantly of high fluorescence intensity, the resulting cumulative probability curve would shift to the right. (ii) A second possibility is that baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons are preferentially recruited during fear conditioning. Prior to conditioning, ~10% of LA neurons are Arc-dVenus+, and thereby represent the fluorescence intensity distribution of the baseline (naïve) group. During fear conditioning, activation of these baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons would necessarily increase their fluorescence level and consequently shift the overall population distribution to the right. Therefore, to distinguish between these possibilities, we examined the absolute frequency histograms of fluorescence intensity, which fully account for the difference in the overall percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons (Figure 2d). Notably, the fluorescence intensity distribution remained significantly right-shifted despite having fully accounted for the increased percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons, and consistent with a model of neuronal selection during fear conditioning in which baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons are preferentially recruited into the memory trace.
Preferential recruitment of neurons with enhanced intrinsic excitability
To further examine the hypothesis that baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons are preferentially recruited during fear learning, we made use of the differential half-life of endogenous Arc50 compared with dVenus.32 Endogenous Arc is nearly undetectable in naïve mice,47 and peaks in the LA at 1 h after fear conditioning, specifically marking neurons that were activated during conditioning (Supplementary Figure 3). In contrast, baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons remain easily detectable over a 1 h period given that the in vivo half-life of Arc-dVenus fluorescence is 3 h.32 Therefore, at 1 h after fear conditioning, a high percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons with co-localized expression of endogenous Arc would confirm that baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons are preferentially recruited during fear conditioning. In contrast, a low rate of co-localized expression would suggest that the baseline Arc-dVenus+ population has no a priori bias towards activation. Indeed, consistent with a model of preferential recruitment, 92.6% of Arc-dVenus+ neurons from mice undergoing paired training were co-localized with endogenous Arc, compared with only 10.2% in naïve mice (Figure 3); P<0.0001. Mice receiving unpaired training also showed recruitment of baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons, although the co-localization was significantly lower than in mice receiving paired training (P<0.05). Lastly, c-Fos activation was also highly co-localized with Arc-dVenus at 1 h post-training (Supplementary Figure 4), demonstrating that this finding is not simply restricted to Arc. Together, these data indicate that baseline Arc-dVenus expression represents a unique molecular marker for LA neurons that are preferentially recruited during fear memory encoding.
Given that Arc-dVenus+ neurons are preferentially recruited during fear conditioning, their defining electrophysiological properties might offer unique insights into the mechanisms underlying associative memory encoding. Passive membrane properties and single AP characteristics of Arc-dVenus+ and neighboring Arc-dVenus− neurons demonstrated no two-way interactions of Arc-dVenus status and training condition (Supplementary Table 1). Furthermore, there were no overall main effects of Arc-dVenus status. However, three parameters demonstrated overall main effects of training condition: membrane resistance (F2,104=5.52, P<0.01), AP threshold (F2,95=8.37, P<0.001), and AP half-width (F2,95=6.03, P<0.01) (Supplementary Table 1). Post hoc pairwise comparisons across training conditions demonstrated that membrane resistance was significantly lower in mice receiving paired training compared with naïve mice (P<0.01), with no significant differences of either condition in comparison with mice receiving unpaired training. AP threshold was significantly more depolarized in mice from the paired (P<0.01) and unpaired (P<0.01) condition, compared with naïve mice. Lastly, AP half-width was significantly narrower in mice receiving paired training, compared with naïve (P<0.05) or unpaired (P<0.01). Importantly, these main effects of training condition are independent of whether the recorded neurons were Arc-dVenus+ or Arc-dVenus−, and therefore reflect global experience-dependent changes observed broadly throughout the LA.
Intrinsic excitability has been widely hypothesized as a candidate mechanism for neuronal recruitment during associative learning.15, 16, 17 However, no previous studies have been able to directly address this hypothesis under entirely physiological conditions. Therefore, we performed targeted whole-cell recordings from Arc-dVenus+ neurons and their non-activated Arc-dVenus− neighbors. Consistent with the hypothesis that increased excitability might support their preferential recruitment into the fear memory trace, baseline Arc-dVenus+ neurons had significantly higher intrinsic excitability than their non-activated neighbors (Figure 4). Moreover, Arc-dVenus+ neurons from both the paired and unpaired conditions displayed a similar increase in excitability, the magnitude of which was independent of learning or sensory stimulation. Accordingly, Arc-dVenus+ neurons also displayed higher instantaneous AP frequencies than neighboring Arc-dVenus− neurons (Supplementary Figure 5). Notably however, no differences were observed in AP amplitude or duration across spike trains (Supplementary Figures 6 and 7). Taken together, these findings suggest that enhanced excitability cannot account for the encoding of a fear memory, but rather is highly consistent with a model for neuronal selection during learning regulated by intrinsic excitability.
Synaptic plasticity is highly localized to Arc-dVenus+ neurons during fear conditioning, and postsynaptically mediated
The encoding of auditory fear memories is thought to occur through selective potentiation of glutamatergic synaptic inputs to the LA.5, 6, 10, 11, 12 However, previous studies investigating auditory fear-conditioning-induced synaptic modifications have been performed without knowledge of whether recorded neurons were part of the memory trace. Therefore, the Arc::dVenus mice represented a unique opportunity to examine directly whether learning-induced synaptic potentiation is preferentially localized to Arc-dVenus+ neurons, as predicted by a model of sparse memory encoding. We recorded EPSCs evoked by stimulation of thalamic afferents to LA neurons. In both naïve and unpaired conditions, similar EPSC amplitudes were observed in Arc-dVenus+ and neighboring Arc-dVenus− neurons (Figures 5a and b). In contrast, paired conditioning induced a robust and highly specific potentiation of thalamic afferent synapses, selectively in Arc-dVenus+ neurons (Figures 5a and b). Therefore, Arc expression defines the LA neuronal ensemble onto which synaptic plasticity is highly localized during fear conditioning.
We next sought to determine whether the site of plasticity for the enhancement in glutamatergic synaptic transmission during auditory fear conditioning was pre- or postsynaptic. If fear conditioning differentially modifies the neurotransmitter release probability onto Arc-dVenus+ versus Arc-dVenus− neurons, such a change should be evident by a decrease in the paired-pulse ratio for glutamatergic inputs onto Arc-dVenus+ neurons compared with neighboring Arc-dVenus− neurons. Therefore, we performed paired-pulse stimulation of thalamic afferents across a range of interstimulus intervals from 25 to 300 ms (Figures 5c and d). Notably, Arc-dVenus+ and Arc-dVenus− neurons showed similar paired-pulse ratios across all interstimulus intervals examined, making it unlikely that the learning-induced synaptic potentiation of Arc-dVenus+ neurons was presynaptic in origin.
Alternatively, we measured the ratio of AMPA to NMDA currents, a widely used measure that is highly sensitive to postsynaptically mediated plasticity of glutamatergic transmission, including long-term potentiation.6, 51 Indeed, consistent with a postsynaptic locus of plasticity, fear conditioning induced a significant increase in the AMPA/NMDA current ratio in Arc-dVenus+ neurons compared with their Arc-dVenus− neighbors (Figures 5e and f). Taken together, our findings demonstrate that learning-induced synaptic potentiation is postsynaptically mediated and selectively localized onto the sparse population of Arc-expressing neurons.
Discussion
The elucidation of the physiological mechanisms underlying memory encoding remains a considerable technical challenge, owing to the sparseness of neuronal representations. Therefore, we used a novel Arc reporter mouse32, 45, 52 to permit visual identification and neurophysiological interrogation of neurons with recent activation. Using this powerful approach for exploring learning-specific alterations in neuronal physiology, we now demonstrate that fear-conditioning-induced glutamatergic synaptic potentiation in the LA is preferentially localized to Arc+ neurons, thereby confirming the sparse encoding hypothesis and identifying Arc as a bona fide molecular marker of the LA fear memory trace. Furthermore, we show that baseline differences in neuronal excitability are highly predictive of the ensemble of neurons selectively recruited into the fear memory trace.
We found that the potentiation of glutamatergic synaptic transmission from the thalamic input pathway was postsynaptically mediated, given the highly significant enhancement in AMPA/NMDA ratio from mice receiving paired training, in the absence of changes in the presynaptically mediated paired-pulse ratio. These findings are consistent with the comprehensive series of previous studies reporting a postsynaptically mediated plasticity of the thalamic input pathway,5, 6, 9, 53 although a minority of reports have also suggested the contribution of a presynaptic mechanism.54 Nonaka et al.52 recently used the Arc::dVenus mice to examine neuronal recruitment and synaptic plasticity following contextual conditioning in the basolateral amygdala. Similar to our findings, they observed a preferential recruitment of Arc-dVenus+ neurons evident in both the learning and non-associative conditions. Moreover, a presynaptically mediated potentiation of cortical-basolateral amygdala synaptic transmission was observed selectively in Arc-dVenus+ neurons, as evidenced by an increase in mEPSC frequency and a decrease in paired-pulse ratio. Taken together, these findings demonstrate that the induction of Arc IEG activation is a highly reliable marker for identifying the limited subset of neurons recruited to the fear memory trace and defined by pathway-specific alterations in synaptic transmission.
Previous studies demonstrating postsynaptically mediated plasticity of the thalamic input pathway to the LA using whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed in randomly chosen LA neurons without knowledge of their Arc expression.5, 6 Our findings now extend these results by demonstrating that potentiation of glutamatergic synaptic transmission occurs disproportionately onto Arc+ neurons. However, this also raises an important question regarding the Arc− population, which presumably constitute a substantial proportion of the recorded neurons. Notably, although not statistically significant, there was a strong trend for increased synaptic transmission within the Arc− population in mice receiving paired training, compared with the unpaired and naïve groups (Figure 5b). Moreover, there are two notable aspects of our experimental design that may also be important to consider. First, we performed the electrophysiological recordings directly following training without intervening memory testing, given the increasing literature demonstrating that retrieval of newly learned associations modifies synaptic physiology.12, 55, 56, 57, 58 Given the highly divergent CS-evoked freezing responses between the paired and unpaired groups, electrophysiological recordings would have always been confounded by the impact of their differential fear responses during the intervening test session.
Second, we chose a behavioral training protocol that did not result in overtraining (Supplementary Figure 2). In contrast, previous studies of thalamic-LA synaptic transmission demonstrating postsynaptically mediated plasticity have used stronger conditioning protocols. With more robust training, it is possible that changes in thalamic-LA synaptic transmission might have occurred outside the Arc+ population. Alternatively, in the setting of more robust training the Arc+ population might have constituted a significantly larger proportion of LA neurons than we have observed, for which random sampling would have yielded the previously reported effects yet consistent with learning-specific changes in thalamic-LA synaptic transmission being largely restricted to the Arc+ population. Importantly however, this latter possibility is inconsistent with our finding of an asymptotic percentage of Arc+ neurons beyond 3 CS-US pairings.
In addition to learning-induced plasticity, we also observed that neurons with baseline elevation of intrinsic excitability are preferentially recruited into the fear memory trace. Previous studies using viral-mediated overexpression of CREB have proposed intrinsic excitability as a candidate cellular mechanism for neuronal selection during fear learning in the LA.15–17,59 However, it remained unknown to what extent these findings recapitulated the endogenous physiological mechanism. Our present results add compelling evidence that intrinsic excitability is indeed a highly influential cellular mechanism underlying recruitment of individual LA neurons during fear memory encoding. Interestingly, studies in both vertebrate and invertebrate species have demonstrated robust and enduring learning-induced alterations in neuronal excitability.60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67 Our findings provide strong support for the hypothesis that neurons with baseline elevation of intrinsic excitability are preferentially recruited into the memory trace, and may serve to bind together experiences acquired closely together in time. Moreover, we speculate that the recent elegant studies using targeted manipulations of IEG-defined memory traces are influencing neuronal selection during learning precisely through modulation of neuronal excitability.30, 31
We observed an asymptotic percentage of Arc-dVenus+ neurons beyond 3 CS-US pairings, despite a further increase in dVenus fluorescence intensity in mice receiving 9 CS-US pairings. Therefore, neuronal selection during fear learning appears to be constrained by the intrinsic microcircuitry of the LA, leading to re-activation of a similar neuronal subpopulation upon successive CS-US pairings throughout the training session. Given the extensive inhibitory network within the LA, a feed-forward inhibitory microcircuitry is a well-suited candidate for mediating this outcome.68, 69 Previous studies have demonstrated unique mechanisms of inhibitory interneuron plasticity within the LA that are likely to function critically in both neuronal selection and fear memory encoding.17, 68, 69, 70, 71, 72 Future studies using multicellular recordings will be required to more precisely define the local microcircuit connectivity and cell-type-specific mechanisms of plasticity.
Our experiments utilized two independent control groups: (i) Unpaired: mice receiving explicitly unpaired CS and US presentations but matched with the paired condition regarding the number and specifications of the CS and US, context exposure and handling; and (ii) Naïve: mice that were truly naïve to any experimental manipulations in that they had no context exposures, nor any handling beyond their standard housing conditions. Our rationale for this design was that the unpaired condition would provide the ideal control for handling, context exposure and the influence of CS and US stimuli independent of auditory fear conditioning. However, any differences observed between the naïve and unpaired groups remain difficult to precisely attribute etiologically, as these effects could be due to the handling, context exposure, CS and/or US stimuli. Moreover, mice receiving unpaired training undergo contextual conditioning. Differences in the naïve and unpaired groups were observed exclusively in experiments examining dVenus fluorescence intensity (Figures 2c and d) and neuronal recruitment (Figure 3), in which the effect size was, in both cases, smaller than observed in the paired condition. No differences were observed in electrophysiological recordings comparing dVenus+ and dVenus− neurons between the naïve and unpaired conditions. Rather, the only electrophysiological difference observed between naïve and unpaired mice regarded AP threshold (Supplementary Table 1), an effect that was independent of dVenus status. Therefore, learning-specific changes in synaptic plasticity cannot be accounted for by handling, context exposure or the unpaired presentation of CS and US stimuli. In contrast, neuronal recruitment in the LA appears to occur independently of auditory fear learning, and mediated by one or more of the stimuli distinguishing the unpaired and naïve groups. Given the function of the LA in assigning emotional valence, we would hypothesize that the strong recruitment in the unpaired condition likely results from the stress sensitization of the US stimuli, but future studies will be required to examine this in further detail.
Taken together with previous findings, we propose a model of fear learning in which non-associative neuronal selection and Hebbian synaptic encoding of the learned association are distinct physiological processes: intrinsic excitability determines neuronal selection, whereas learning-related encoding is governed by synaptic plasticity.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by ZonMw Vidi 017.106.384 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, NeuroBasic-PharmaPhenomics consortium, Dutch Technology Foundation STW which is the applied science division of NWO and the Technology Programme of the Ministry of Economic Affairs (Project 12197), and FP7 EU Marie Curie (254711) to SAK; by Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia to IMM. We thank Gerard Borst for insightful comments on the manuscript; Katie Achterberg for assistance with fear conditioning; Hans Rüdiger Geis and Elize Haasdijk for assistance with immunohistochemistry; Joan Holstege and Aram Hossaini for providing advice and reagents for in situ hybridizations; and Masha Stern, Ben Haydock and Susan Hendricks for advice regarding stereological analysis.
Author Contributions
LAG-C, BH and SAK designed the experiments. LAG-C performed the electrophysiological recordings. BH performed the behavioral, confocal stereology and fluorescence intensity measurements. IMM performed the in situ hybridizations. JS, DJ and DES assisted with the electrophysiological, immunofluorescence and behavioral experiments, respectively. LAG-C, BH, IMM, ARH, DJ, YE and SAK analyzed the data. SY provided the Arc-dVenus mice. LAG-C, BH and SAK wrote the manuscript. The manuscript was reviewed, edited and approved by all authors. LAG-C and BH contributed equally to the work.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Molecular Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/mp)
Supplementary Material
References
- 1Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychol Rev 1967; 74: 71–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2LeDoux JE, Cicchetti P, Xagoraris A, Romanski RM. The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. J Neurosci 1990; 10: 1062–1069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3Quirk GJ, Repa JC, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron 1995; 15: 1029–1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4Johansen JP, Cain CK, Ostroff LE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and memory. Cell 2011; 147: 509–524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science 2005; 308: 83–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6Clem RL, Huganir RL. Calcium-permeable AMPA receptor dynamics mediate fear memory erasure. Science 2010; 330: 1108–1112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7Rodrigues SM, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Intra-amygdala blockade of the NR2B subunit of the NMDA receptor disrupts the acquisition but not the expression of fear conditioning. J Neurosci 2001; 21: 6889–6896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8Bauer EP, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. NMDA receptors and L-type voltage-gated calcium channels contribute to long-term potentiation and different components of fear memory formation in the lateral amygdala. J Neurosci 2002; 22: 5239–5249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9Humeau Y, Reisel D, Johnson AW, Borchardt T, Jensen V, Gebhardt C et al. A pathway-specific function for different AMPA receptor subunits in amygdala long-term potentiation and fear conditioning. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 10947–10956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10Rogan MT, Stäubli U, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature 1997; 390: 604–607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11McKernan MG, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear conditioning induces a lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature 1997; 390: 607–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12Li Y, Meloni EG, Carlezon WA Jr, Milad MR, Pitman RK, Nader K et al. Learning and reconsolidation implicate different synaptic mechanisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110: 4798–4803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13Han J-H, Kushner SA, Yiu AP, Cole CJ, Matynia A, Brown RA et al. Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation. Science 2007; 316: 457–460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14Kim J, Kwon J-T, Kim H-S, Josselyn SA, Han J-H. Memory recall and modifications by activating neurons with elevated CREB. Nat Neurosci 2013; 17: 65–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15Zhou Y, Won J, Karlsson MG, Zhou M, Rogerson T, Balaji J et al. CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nat Neurosci 2009; 12: 1438–1443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16Silva AJ, Zhou Y, Rogerson T, Shobe J, Balaji J. Molecular and cellular approaches to memory allocation in neural circuits. Science 2009; 326: 391–395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17Kim D, Paré D, Nair SS. Assignment of model amygdala neurons to the fear memory trace depends on competitive synaptic interactions. J Neurosci 2013; 33: 14354–14358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18Repa JC, Muller J, Apergis J, Desrochers TM, Zhou Y, LeDoux JE. Two different lateral amygdala cell populations contribute to the initiation and storage of memory. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4: 724–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19An B, Hong I, Choi S. Long-term neural correlates of reversible fear learning in the lateral amygdala. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 16845–16856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20Kawashima T, Kitamura K, Suzuki K, Nonaka M, Kamijo S, Takemoto-Kimura S et al. Functional labeling of neurons and their projections using the synthetic activity-dependent promoter E-SARE. Nat Methods 2013; 10: 889–895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21Benedetti BL, Takashima Y, Wen JA, Urban-Ciecko J, Barth AL. Differential wiring of layer 2/3 neurons drives sparse and reliable firing during neocortical development. Cereb Cortex 2013; 23: 2690–2699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22Wang KH, Majewska A, Schummers J, Farley B, Hu C, Sur M et al. In vivo two-photon imaging reveals a role of arc in enhancing orientation specificity in visual cortex. Cell 2006; 126: 389–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23Guzowski JF, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Worley PF. Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2: 1120–1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24Guzowski JF, Miyashita T, Chawla MK, Sanderson J, Maes LI, Houston FP et al. Recent behavioral history modifies coupling between cell activity and Arc gene transcription in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; 103: 1077–1082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25Reijmers LG, Perkins BL, Matsuo N, Mayford M. Localization of a stable neural correlate of associative memory. Science 2007; 317: 1230–1233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26Tayler KK, Tanaka KZ, Reijmers LG, Wiltgen BJ. Reactivation of neural ensembles during the retrieval of recent and remote memory. Curr Biol 2013; 23: 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27Trouche S, Sasaki JM, Tu T, Reijmers LG. Fear extinction causes target-specific remodeling of perisomatic inhibitory synapses. Neuron 2013; 80: 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28Han J-H, Kushner SA, Yiu AP, Hsiang HL, Buch T, Waisman A et al. Selective erasure of a fear memory. Science 2009; 323: 1492–1496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K et al. Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature 2012; 484: 381–385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30Garner AR, Rowland DC, Hwang SY, Baumgaertel K, Roth BL, Kentros C et al. Generation of a synthetic memory trace. Science 2012; 335: 1513–1516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31Ramirez S, Liu X, Lin PA, Suh J, Pignatelli M, Redondo RL et al. Creating a false memory in the hippocampus. Science 2013; 341: 387–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32Eguchi M, Yamaguchi S. In vivo and in vitro visualization of gene expression dynamics over extensive areas of the brain. Neuroimage 2009; 44: 1274–1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33Cain CK, LeDoux JE. Escape from fear: a detailed behavioral analysis of two atypical responses reinforced by CS termination. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2007; 33: 451–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34Anagnostaras SG, Wood SC, Shuman T, Cai DJ, Leduc AD, Zurn KR et al. Automated assessment of pavlovian conditioned freezing and shock reactivity in mice using the video freeze system. Front Behav Neurosci 2010; 4: 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35Hossaini M, Jongen JLM, Biesheuvel K, Kuhl D, Holstege JC. Nociceptive stimulation induces expression of Arc/Arg3.1 in the spinal cord with a preference for neurons containing enkephalin. Mol Pain 2010; 6: 43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36Ben-Ari Y, Zigmond RE, Shute CC, Lewis PR. Regional distribution of choline acetyltransferase and acetylcholinesterase within the amygdaloid complex and stria terminalis system. Brain Res 1977; 120: 435–444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37Inberg S, Elkobi A, Edri E, Rosenblum K. Taste familiarity is inversely correlated with Arc/Arg3.1 hemispheric lateralization. J Neurosci 2013; 33: 11734–11743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38Gundersen HJ. Stereology of arbitrary particles. A review of unbiased number and size estimators and the presentation of some new ones, in memory of William R. Thompson. J Microsc 1986; 143: 3–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39Gundersen HJ, Jensen EB. The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology and its prediction. J Microsc 1987; 147: 229–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40Schmitz C. Variation of fractionator estimates and its prediction. Anat Embryol 1998; 198: 371–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41Lang EJ, Paré D. Synaptic responsiveness of interneurons of the cat lateral amygdaloid nucleus. Neuroscience 1998; 83: 877–889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42Mahanty NK, Sah P. Calcium-permeable AMPA receptors mediate long-term potentiation in interneurons in the amygdala. Nature 1998; 394: 683–687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43Faber ES, Callister RJ, Sah P. Morphological and electrophysiological properties of principal neurons in the rat lateral amygdala in vitro. J Neurophysiol 2001; 85: 714–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44Vazdarjanova A, Ramirez-Amaya V, Insel N, Plummer TK, Rosi S, Chowdhury S et al. Spatial exploration induces ARC, a plasticity-related immediate-early gene, only in calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II-positive principal excitatory and inhibitory neurons of the rat forebrain. J Comp Neurol 2006; 498: 317–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45Rudinskiy N, Hawkes JM, Betensky RA, Eguchi M, Yamaguchi S, Spires-Jones TL et al. Orchestrated experience-driven Arc responses are disrupted in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15: 1422–1429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46Waclaw RR, Ehrman LA, Pierani A, Campbell K. Developmental origin of the neuronal subtypes that comprise the amygdalar fear circuit in the mouse. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 6944–6953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47Lyford GL, Yamagata K, Kaufmann WE, Barnes CA, Sanders LK, Copeland NG et al. Arc, a growth factor and activity-regulated gene, encodes a novel cytoskeleton-associated protein that is enriched in neuronal dendrites. Neuron 1995; 14: 433–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48Parsons RG, Davis M. A metaplasticity-like mechanism supports the selection of fear memories: role of protein kinase a in the amygdala. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 7843–7851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49Cowansage KK, Bush DEA, Josselyn SA, Klann E, Ledoux JE. Basal variability in CREB phosphorylation predicts trait-like differences in amygdala-dependent memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110: 16645–16650 doi:10.1073/pnas.1304665110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50Soulé J, Alme M, Myrum C, Schubert M, Kanhema T, Bramham CR. Balancing Arc synthesis, mRNA decay, and proteasomal degradation: maximal protein expression triggered by rapid eye movement sleep-like bursts of muscarinic cholinergic receptor stimulation. J Biol Chem 2012; 287: 22354–22366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51Liao D, Hessler NA, Malinow R. Activation of postsynaptically silent synapses during pairing-induced LTP in CA1 region of hippocampal slice. Nature 1995; 375: 400–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52Nonaka A, Toyoda T, Miura Y, Hitora-Imamura N, Naka M, Eguchi M et al. Synaptic plasticity associated with a memory engram in the basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci 2014; 34: 9305–9309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53Humeau Y, Herry C, Kemp N, Shaban H, Fourcaudot E et al. Dendritic spine heterogeneity determines afferent-specific Hebbian plasticity in the amygdala. Neuron 2005; 45: 119–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54Li Y, Meloni EG, Carlezon WA Jr, Milad MR, Pitman RK, Nader K et al. Learning and reconsolidation implicate different synaptic mechanisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110: 4798–4803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55Ben Mamou C, Gamache K, Nader K. NMDA receptors are critical for unleashing consolidated auditory fear memories. Nat Neurosci 2006; 9: 1237–1239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56Rao-Ruiz P, Rotaru DC, van der Loo RJ, Mansvelder HD, Stiedl O, Smit AB et al. Retrieval-specific endocytosis of GluA2-AMPARs underlies adaptive reconsolidation of contextual fear. Nat Neurosci 2011; 14: 1302–1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57Hong I, Kim J, Kim J, Lee S, Ko HG, Nader K et al. AMPA receptor exchange underlies transient memory destabilization on retrieval. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110: 8218–8223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58Fukushima H, Zhang Y, Archbold G, Ishikawa R, Nader K, Kida S. Enhancement of fear memory by retrieval through reconsolidation. Elife 2014; 3: e02736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59Yiu AP, Mercaldo V, Yan C, Richards B, Rashid AJ, Hsiang HL et al. Neurons are recruited to a memory trace based on relative neuronal excitability immediately before training. Neuron 2014; 83: 722–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60Brons JF, Woody CD. Long-term changes in excitability of cortical neurons after Pavlovian conditioning and extinction. J Neurophysiol 1980; 44: 605–615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61Alkon DL. Calcium-mediated reduction of ionic currents: a biophysical memory trace. Science 1984; 226: 1037–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62Moyer JR, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning increases CA1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. J Neurosci 1996; 16: 5536–5546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63Antonov I, Antonova I, Kandel ER, Hawkins RD. The contribution of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity to classical conditioning in Aplysia. J Neurosci 2001; 21: 5536–5546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64Saar D, Barkai E. Long-term modifications in intrinsic neuronal properties and rule learning in rats. Eur J Neurosci 2003; 17: 2727–2734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65Santini E, Quirk GJ, Porter JT. Fear conditioning and extinction differentially modify the intrinsic excitability of infralimbic neurons. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 4028–4036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66McKay BM, Matthews EA, Oliveira FA, Disterhoft JF. Intrinsic neuronal excitability is reversibly altered by a single experience in fear conditioning. J Neurophysiol 2009; 102: 2763–2770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67Sehgal M, Ehlers VL, Moyer JR. Learning enhances intrinsic excitability in a subset of lateral amygdala neurons. Learn Mem 2014; 21: 161–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68Bissière S, Humeau Y, Lüthi A. Dopamine gates LTP induction in lateral amygdala by suppressing feedforward inhibition. Nat Neurosci 2003; 6: 587–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69Polepalli JS, Sullivan RKP, Yanagawa Y, Sah P. A specific class of interneuron mediates inhibitory plasticity in the lateral amygdala. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 14619–14629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70Bauer EP, LeDoux JE. Heterosynaptic long-term potentiation of inhibitory interneurons in the lateral amygdala. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 9507–9512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71Szinyei C, Narayanan RT, Pape H-C. Plasticity of inhibitory synaptic network interactions in the lateral amygdala upon fear conditioning in mice. Eur J Neurosci 2007; 25: 1205–1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72Wolff SBE, Gründemann J, Tovote P, Krabbe S, Jacobson GA, Müller C et al. Amygdala interneuron subtypes control fear learning through disinhibition. Nature 2014; 509: 453–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.