Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2016 Jan 23;171(0):131–136. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.01.021

Neurocognitive insight and objective cognitive functioning in schizophrenia

Cynthia Z Burton a,1, Philip D Harvey b,c, Thomas L Patterson d, Elizabeth W Twamley d,e
PMCID: PMC4762729  NIHMSID: NIHMS754581  PMID: 26811232

Abstract

Neurocognitive impairment is a core component of schizophrenia affecting everyday functioning; the extent to which individuals with schizophrenia show awareness of neurocognitive impairment (neurocognitive insight) is unclear. This study investigated neurocognitive insight and examined the cross-sectional relationships between neurocognitive insight and objective neurocognition and functional capacity performance in a large outpatient sample.

214 participants with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders completed measures of neurocognition, functional capacity, and self-reported neurocognitive problems. Latent profile analysis classified participants with regard to neuropsychological performance and self-report of neurocognitive problems. The resulting classes were then compared on executive functioning performance, functional capacity performance, and psychiatric symptom severity.

More than three quarters of the sample demonstrated objective neurocognitive impairment (global deficit score ≥ 0.50). Among the participants with neurocognitive impairment, 54% were classified as having “impaired” neurocognitive insight (i.e., reporting few neurocognitive problems despite having objective neurocognitive impairment). Participants with impaired vs. intact neurocognitive insight did not differ on executive functioning measures or measures of functional capacity or negative symptom severity, but those with intact neurocognitive insight reported higher levels of positive and depressive symptoms.

A substantial portion of individuals with schizophrenia and objectively measured neurocognitive dysfunction appear unaware of their deficits. Patient self-report of neurocognitive problems, therefore, is not likely to reliably assess neurocognition. Difficulty self-identifying neurocognitive impairment appears to be unrelated to executive functioning, negative symptoms, and functional capacity. For those with intact neurocognitive insight, improving depressive and psychotic symptoms may be a valuable target to reduce illness burden.

Keywords: Cognition, awareness, psychosis, functional capacity

1. Introduction

Little doubt remains regarding the significance of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Empirical evidence has consistently demonstrated stable, enduring deficits in attention, processing speed, working memory, learning, and executive function (Heaton et al., 2001; Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998), and that domain-specific deficits are relative and exist against a backdrop of generalized dysfunction (Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998). Furthermore, a critical link has been identified between cognitive impairment and functional outcome; that is, neuropsychological dysfunction affects performance of real-world everyday activities that are necessary to live independently in the community (Green et al., 2000).

Several types of insight, or awareness of dysfunction, have been described in schizophrenia. Clinical insight refers to awareness of psychotic illness (Amador et al., 1993). Metacognition is a broad term that generally refers to “thinking about thinking”; conceptually, it is related to but not synonymous with neurocognition (Lysaker et al., 2011a). Two subtypes of metacognition include cognitive insight, which refers to awareness of “mistakes in thinking” such as jumping to conclusions or catastrophizing (Beck et al., 2004), and neurocognitive insight, defined as awareness of neuropsychological dysfunction (e.g., impaired attention, memory, problem-solving) (Medalia and Thysen, 2008) expressed through subjective cognitive complaints (Stip et al., 2003).

The domains of clinical and cognitive insight in schizophrenia and their relationships with neurocognition have been extensively investigated (Nair et al., 2014). For example, several studies demonstrated that greater clinical and cognitive insight was associated with better executive functioning (Aleman et al., 2006; Burton et al., 2011; Larøi et al., 2000; Medalia and Thysen, 2010; Monteiro et al., 2008; Mysore et al., 2007; Orfei et al., 2010; Shad et al., 2004; Simon et al., 2009), lending support to the view that poor insight in schizophrenia may be a function of specific prefrontally-mediated neurocognitive deficits rather than a global deficit in neuropsychological functioning (Shad et al., 2004).

Clinical and cognitive insight have also been studied in relation to psychiatric symptomatology and functioning. Better clinical insight is modestly associated with less severe positive and negative symptoms but more severe depressive symptoms (Mintz et al., 2003; McEvoy et al., 2006; Sabbag et al., 2012; Wiffen et al., 2010). Some data suggest that good cognitive insight is related to less severe positive symptoms (Bora et al., 2007; but see also Greenberger and Serper, 2010). In terms of functioning, good clinical insight is associated with improved functional skills ratings (Schwartz et al., 1997).

There is less known about neurocognitive insight. Instruments to directly measure neurocognitive insight have been created to assess awareness of cognitive deficits in comparison to actual performance on cognitive tests, as well as to allow reliable measurement of patients’ or caregivers’ opinions about a patient’s degree of neurocognitive deficit (Keefe et al., 2006; Medalia and Thysen, 2010; Stip et al., 2003). Although the literature on neurocognitive insight is limited, there is some evidence that individuals with schizophrenia have poorer insight into their neurocognitive symptoms than their clinical symptoms, prompting researchers to encourage that they be addressed separately in treatment (Medalia and Thysen, 2010). For example, a 2011 review indicated that 14 of 26 published studies found no correlation between objective cognitive performance and subjective cognitive complaints (Homayoun et al., 2011). Another study reported that 95% of participants were cognitively impaired, though more than half of the sample had no awareness of cognitive dysfunction (Medalia and Thysen, 2008). Still other researchers have concluded that even when patients express cognitive difficulties, their specific complaints do not align with the cognitive domains tested (Prouteau et al., 2004). To date, no consistent evidence has emerged to suggest that neurocognitive insight or self-reported cognitive functioning converges with objective global or composite cognitive performance (Durand et al., 2015; Gould et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2011; Keefe et al., 2006; Keefe et al., 2015; Medalia et al., 2008; Medalia and Lim, 2004; Moritz et al., 2004; Poletti et al., 2012; Saperstein et al., 2012). Surprisingly few studies have examined the link between neurocognitive insight and executive functioning specifically; those that did reported negative findings from relatively small samples using one subtest from the same cognitive battery (Brief Assessment of Cognition Scale) (Medalia and Thysen, 2008; Poletti et al., 2012).

Similarly, the relationship between neurocognitive insight and everyday functioning has not been extensively examined; in a previous analysis of this database, greater discrepancies between self-reported and clinician-rated cognitive functioning were associated with poorer everyday outcomes as rated by clinicians (Gould et al., 2015). Another published study determined that metacognitive mastery was associated with functional competence in comprehension/planning (Lysaker et al., 2011b).

Despite the apparent lack of association between neurocognitive insight and objective global cognitive performance, numerous studies have demonstrated that greater self-report of cognitive problems is significantly related to increased depression and anxiety (Durand et al., 2015; Medalia et al., 2008; Moritz et al., 2004; Sabbag et al., 2012; Saperstein et al., 2012). The relationship between neurocognitive insight and negative symptoms has not been fully examined; one study suggested no significant relationship (Medalia & Thysen, 2010). Furthermore, a recent study showed that higher rates of self-reported cognitive complaints were associated with lower treatment utilization, suggesting that clinicians may need to target those at risk for drop out with more intensive follow-up care, compensatory strategies, and psychoeducation (Gooding et al., 2012).

In summary, despite the known cognitive dysfunction associated with schizophrenia, the extent to which affected individuals show awareness of such impairment is unclear as are its performance-based correlates. The aims of this study were to explore neurocognitive insight among a large, multi-site sample of individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia and demonstrating objective cognitive impairment, and to evaluate cross-sectional relationships between neurocognitive insight and objective cognitive and functional capacity performance. Given the equivocal evidence regarding the relationship between insight and executive functioning, the first hypothesis was that participants with impaired neurocognitive insight would demonstrate domain-specific impairment in executive functioning. We also hypothesized that participants with impaired neurocognitive insight would demonstrate poorer functional capacity, and that individuals with impaired neurocognitive insight would have more severe negative symptoms but less severe depressive symptoms (Sabbag et al., 2012).

2. Method

2.1 Participants

These analyses were conducted as part of the larger Validating Everyday Real-World Outcomes study (VALERO) Phase II, which aimed to identify the determinants of impaired self-assessment in schizophrenia. Participants included 214 individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder receiving outpatient care at one of three sites: UCSD Outpatient Psychiatric Services (n = 100), the University of Miami Miller School of Medicine (n = 79), and Skyland Trail Rehabilitation Services in Atlanta (n = 35). Participants were enrolled in the VALERO II parent study that was approved by each site’s institutional review board. On average, participants were 41 years old and had completed 12 years of education; the majority of the sample was male, Caucasian, diagnosed with schizophrenia, and prescribed antipsychotic medication (Table 1).

Table 1.

Demographic and Clinical Features of the Full Sample and the Neurocognitively Impaired Sample

Full Sample (n=214) Neurocognitively Impaired Sample (n=168)
Mean (sd) or % Mean (sd) or %
Age, years 41.2 (12.4) 42.7 (12.3)
Education, years 12.3 (2.2) 12.1 (2.1)
% Male 65.4 67.3
% Caucasian 54.7 51.2
% Hispanic ethnicity 23.4 21.4
% African American 36.0 40.5
% Schizophrenia (vs. Schizoaffective) 58.2 63.0
% Prescribed antipsychotic medication 98.1 98.2
% Living independently 73.3 72.6
% Employed 9.8 10.1
% Never married 53.2 50.3
PANSS positive symptoms total 15.7 (5.5) 16.1 (5.7)
PANSS negative symptoms total 15.7 (6.1) 16.2 (6.3)
BDI-II total 15.3 (11.7) 15.0 (11.6)

Note. BDI-II=Beck Depression Inventory, second edition; PANSS=Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale

2.2 Procedures

Potential participants were referred to the study by treating clinicians or self-response to recruitment flyers posted in psychiatric care centers; all participants provided written informed consent prior to any data collection. Participants’ diagnoses were confirmed via structured diagnostic interview (Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview) (Sheehan et al., 1998) administered by a trained research assistant. Inclusion criteria were: (a) age between 18 and 65, (b) DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, (c) English-speaking, and (d) minimum 8th grade reading level. Patients were excluded if they had (a) history of unconsciousness greater than 10 minutes, (b) a seizure disorder or other neurological condition affecting cognition, (c) current substance abuse or dependence, (d) for patients aged 55 or older, a score less than 27 on the Mini Mental State Exam (Folstein et al., 2001). Participants completed a one-time comprehensive neuropsychological, clinical, and functional battery.

2.3 Measures

Premorbid intellectual functioning was measured with the reading subtest of the Wide Range Achievement Test–Third Edition (Wilkinson, 1993). Psychiatric symptom severity was measured with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (Kay et al., 1987) and the Beck Depression Inventory–II (Beck et al., 1996). Self-reported frequency of cognitive problems was measured with the Measure of Insight into Cognition–Self-Rated (MIC-SR) (Medalia et al., 2008). The MIC-SR consists of twelve statements about attention, executive functioning, and memory (e.g., “I have trouble listening and paying attention”; “I have difficulty thinking through possible solutions to problems”) rated by respondents in terms of frequency: never, once a week or less, twice a week, or almost daily. The maximum score is 36; higher scores indicate greater frequency of cognitive problems.

Current cognitive functioning was measured with a modified version of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) (Nuechterlein et al., 2008); the social cognition measure was excluded from the VALERO trial due to concern that social cognition measures would differ from neurocognitive measures in terms of their relationship to everyday functioning. The other nine subtests of the MCCB measured six neurocognitive domains, as follows:

  1. Speed of processing (Trail Making Test, Part A; Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia Symbol Coding subtest; Category Fluency Test, animal naming)

  2. Attention/vigilance (Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs Version [CPT-IP])

  3. Working memory (Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edition, Spatial Span subtest; Letter-Number Span test)

  4. Verbal learning (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, immediate recall)

  5. Visual learning (Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised, immediate recall)

  6. Reasoning and problem solving (Neuropsychological Assessment Battery, Mazes subtest)

Because phase II of the VALERO trial explored impaired self-assessment in schizophrenia, a metacognitive version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was also included to measure executive functioning (Koren et al., 2004). In this task, the WCST is administered following the standardized instructions; however, prior to receiving feedback about the accuracy of the sort, participants are asked to (1) rate their confidence in the correctness of that sort on a “0” (guessing) to “100” (completely confident) scale, and (2) decide whether they do or do not want that sort to be “counted” toward their overall performance score on the test. 29 Several key metacognitive variables can be derived; for example, the accuracy score is calculated as the number of correct sorts out of the total number of volunteered sorts.

Everyday functional skills were evaluated with the UCSD Performance-based Skills Assessment, Brief version (UPSA-B) (Mausbach et al., 2007), in which participants perform everyday tasks related to finance (e.g., write a check to pay a utility bill), and communication (e.g., call a doctor to reschedule an appointment). The UPSA-B takes 10–15 minutes to administer and yields raw subscale scores as well as raw scores that are converted into a total score ranging from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating better functional capacity.

2.4 Analyses

All continuous variables were normally distributed. To first identify participants with objective neurocognitive impairment, a global deficit score (GDS) was calculated for each participant (Heaton et al., 2007). To accomplish this, each of the nine t-scores calculated by the MCCB scoring program was assigned a numerical degree of deficit on a scale of 0 (t-score ≥ 40; no deficit) to 5 (t-score ≤ 19; severe deficit) in five point decrements in the t-score. The GDS is the average deficit score across measures. In this sample, eight participants were missing at least one MCCB score (seven participants were missing one score, and one participant was missing three scores); for these participants, the GDS was calculated from all available data. The recommended cutoff for cognitive impairment is GDS ≥ 0.50, which roughly corresponds to mild impairment on half of the component measures (Heaton et al., 2007). For the remaining analyses, only participants with objective cognitive impairment were included, both because the intent of the study was to evaluate awareness of existing neuropsychological dysfunction rather than knowledge of neuropsychological status in general, and due to the clinical relevance of identifying and engaging individuals with such neuropsychological dysfunction in cognitive remediation treatment.

To examine participants’ level of neurocognitive insight, latent profile analysis (LPA) was used to group cases based on participants’ similarities on two continuous observed variables: the GDS (objective neurocognition) and the MIC-SR total score (subjective report of neurocognitive problems). Conceptually similar to cluster analysis, LPA is a multivariate approach that groups individuals based on shared characteristics that distinguish members of one group from members of another group. LPA determines class assignment through fit statistics and tests of significance, and uses probabilities to take uncertainty of membership (error) into account (Herman et al., 2007). For the descriptive fit indices, lower values are considered indicative of better fit for the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), and Adjusted BIC; higher values indicate better fit for Entropy. The Lo-Mendell-Rubin Test (LMRT) of significance indicates whether the model under consideration is statistically a superior fit to the lower-order model (e.g., if a 3-class model fits better statistically than a 2-class model). A cutoff of <.05 is used for significance. Similar to other data reduction techniques like factor analysis, LPA statistically indicates the optimal number of groups, then the values of relevant variables are examined to descriptively label the groups.

The three hypotheses were tested using the resulting groups from the LPA. The groups were compared via t-tests for independent samples on the following variables: executive functioning (Koren WCST accuracy score; Mazes t-score), functional capacity (UPSA-B total score), positive and negative symptom severity (PANSS positive total and negative total scores), and depressive symptom severity (BDI-II total score). To correct for multiple comparisons, alpha for significance was set at 0.008 (0.05/6).

Exploratory analyses examined domain-specific neurocognitive awareness using Pearson correlations between: MIC-SR attention score and CPT-IP t-score, MIC-SR executive function score and Mazes t-score/Koren WCST accuracy score, and MIC-SR memory score and MCCB HVLT total recall t-score. Data were analyzed using Mplus (Version 7.11) and SPSS (Version 21).

3. Results

Among all 214 participants, 168 (78.5%) were classified as neurocognitively impaired (GDS ≥0.5), whereas 46 (21.5%) were classified as neurocognitively intact. The remainder of the analyses included only the 168 cognitively impaired participants (Table 1).

The LPA demonstrated that on two of the four descriptive fit indices as well on the statistical test of model fit, a 2-class model was preferable to both a 1- and 3-class solution (Table 2). Because the LMRT for the 3-class model was not significant, further higher-order models were not analyzed. According to the class assignments yielded by this LPA, one group included 77 participants (46%) with a mean GDS score of 1.75 and mean MIC-SR of 27.89. This group was labeled “intact neurocognitive insight” owing to the high degree of cognitive impairment and high self-reported frequency of cognitive problems. The other group included 91 participants (54%) with a mean GDS of 1.61 and a mean MIC-SR of 9.49; this group was described as “impaired neurocognitive insight” due to high cognitive impairment but low self-reported frequency of cognitive problems. The groups did not statistically differ on any demographic variables (Table 3). The intact neurocognitive insight group were rated as having significantly more severe positive symptoms (mean PANSS positive total score 17.58 versus 14.89; t=−3.09; df=162; p=.002) and reported more depressive symptoms (mean BDI-II total score 20.25 versus 10.39; t=−5.87; df=144.08; p<.001). The groups did not differ significantly on Koren WCST accuracy score, Mazes t-score, UPSA-B, or PANSS negative symptom total score (Table 3). Follow-up item analysis of the PANSS positive symptoms subscale indicated that participants with intact neurocognitive insight were rated more highly on delusions, hallucinatory behavior, suspiciousness, and hostility (all ps<.016), though across items the mean ratings were “minimal” to “mild.”

Table 2.

Descriptive and Statistical Fit Indices

Cognitively Impaired (n=168) 1 class 2 classes 3 classes
AIC 1680.06 1663.97 1663.02
BIC 1692.55 1685.83 1694.26
Adjusted BIC 1679.89 1663.67 1662.60
Entropy N/A 0.705 0.611
LMRT N/A 0.001 0.8773

Note. Favorable values are indicated in bold font. AIC=Akaike Information Criterion; BIC=Bayesian Information Criterion; LMRT=Lo-Mendell-Rubin Test of significance

Table 3.

Demographic, clinical, and cognitive differences between participants with intact versus impaired neurocognitive insight (n=168)

Intact Impaired
n Mean (sd) or % n Mean (sd) or % t or χ2 df p Cohen’s d
Age, years 77 43.6 (12.0) 91 41.9 (12.5) −0.91 166 0.364
Education, years 77 11.9 (1.9) 90 12.3 (2.2) 1.33 165 0.185
% Male 77 59.7 91 73.6 3.65 1 0.056
% Caucasian 77 48.1 91 53.8 0.80 2 0.671
% Hispanic ethnicity 77 16.9 91 25.3 1.74 1 0.187
% Schizophrenia 73 57.5 89 67.4 1.68 1 0.195
% Prescribed antipsychotic medication 76 97.4 88 98.9 0.51 1 0.476
% Living independently 77 77.9 91 68.1 4.43 3 0.219
% Employed 77 9.1 91 11.0 1.73 3 0.630
% Never married 70 47.1 75 53.3 7.53 6 0.275
WRAT-III reading total 77 43.2 (6.9) 89 44.6 (6.9) 1.26 164 0.210
Koren accuracy score 73 0.5 (0.2) 86 0.5 (0.2) −1.26 157 0.208 0.00
Mazes score 77 37.3 (7.9) 90 39.9 (8.2) 2.11 165 0.036 0.32
PANSS positive symptoms total 76 17.6 (5.7) 88 14.9 (5.5) −3.09 162 0.002 0.49
PANSS negative symptoms total 76 16.9 (6.3) 89 15.6 (6.3) −1.30 163 0.195 0.21
BDI-II total 76 20.3 (11.7) 88 10.4 (9.5) −5.87 144.08 <0.001 0.94
UPSA-B total 75 67.9 (13.9) 89 68.0 (16.5) 0.04 162 0.966 0.01

Note. Significant differences are indicated in bold font. BDI-II=Beck Depression Inventory, second edition; PANSS=Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; UPSA-B=UCSD Performance Based Skills Assessment, Brief; WRAT-III=Wide Range Achievement Test, third edition

Exploratory analyses of domain-specific neurocognitive awareness among all participants yielded no statistically significant correlations between the attention and memory domains of the MIC-SR and their objective cognitive counterparts (MIC-SR attention and CPT-IP r=.069; p=.325; MIC-SR memory and HVLT t-score r=−.013; p=.854). There was no significant relationship between MIC-SR executive function and Mazes (r=−.123; p=.077), though participants who reported more problems with executive functioning scored significantly higher on Koren accuracy (r=.147; p=.038).

4. Discussion

These results are consistent with previous research showing that the majority of people with schizophrenia demonstrate neuropsychological impairment (Palmer et al., 1997), and that a large proportion of those individuals minimally endorse cognitive problems (Medalia and Thysen, 2008). Indeed, more than half of participants with objectively measured cognitive impairment demonstrated impaired neurocognitive insight. This finding is consistent with previous literature suggesting that many individuals with schizophrenia are poor raters of their own cognition (Johnson et al., 2011; Keefe et al., 2006; Medalia et al., 2008; Medalia and Lim, 2004; Moritz et al., 2004; Poletti et al., 2012; Saperstein et al., 2012) and everyday functioning (Durand et al., 2015; Sabbag et al., 2012).

The first and second hypotheses, that impaired neurocognitive insight would be related to poorer executive functioning and functional capacity performance, were not supported. Our measures of executive functioning were not comprehensive, and included only a reasoning task and a speeded planning task; thus, constructs such as switching and inhibition were not measured. It is possible that these latter tasks are more related to neurocognitive insight, but it is also possible that neurocognitive insight is unrelated to executive skills and the ability to carry out tasks of daily living. We generally found no evidence of domain-specific neurocognitive insight; cognitive symptom complaints in specific domains were mostly unrelated to actual neurocognitive performance, except that greater executive functioning symptoms were weakly related to better executive performance.

The third hypothesis had mixed support; as hypothesized, participants with intact neurocognitive insight reported more severe depressive symptoms, which is consistent with numerous previous studies examining insight and depression (Medalia et al., 2008; Moritz et al., 2004; Sellwood et al., 2013). These findings are also in line with previous research demonstrating that more severe self-rated depression was associated with underestimation of functional abilities (Bowie et al., 2007) as well as higher self-reported disability (Sabbag et al., 2012). In the current study, participants with intact insight did not differ from those with impaired insight, however, on negative symptom severity. Unexpectedly, participants with intact neurocognitive insight reported more severe positive symptoms of psychosis. Item analysis demonstrated that the groups significantly differed on delusions, hallucinatory behavior, suspiciousness, and hostility, though on average the ratings were no greater than “mild.” As voices and related delusional ideation are conceptualized in the cognitive-behavioral model as negative cognitions about oneself reflecting interpersonal vulnerability (Beck and Rector, 2005), perhaps those with intact awareness are more confronted with their cognitive problems via voices. Additional investigation is warranted to examine the relationship between neurocognitive insight and positive symptom severity.

This study is limited by characteristics of the sample (e.g., outpatient status, middle-age, chronic psychosis); these results may not generalize to inpatients and/or first-episode schizophrenia patients. The narrow measurement of executive functioning and functional capacity, and the exclusion of a social cognition measure, may have also limited our ability to detect significant differences between participants with impaired versus intact neurocognitive insight. In addition, although classification of participants into groups reflecting “intact” and “impaired” insight was conducted using a statistically sound technique and for the purpose of comparing groups on other relevant variables, we acknowledge that neurocognitive insight (like other types of insight) likely exists on a continuum and is not simply an all-or-none feature of schizophrenia. A similar limitation in characterizing neurocognitive insight is that the MIC-SR does not probe all of the cognitive domains represented in the MCCB. Therefore, participants’ subjective evaluation of abilities related to speed of processing, working memory, and visual learning were not specifically assessed, which could have affected the relationship between subjective and objective cognitive functioning and resulting classification into ‘impaired’ versus ‘intact’ neurocognitive insight. Finally, the MIC-SR is a relatively new measure and its psychometrics were not examined within this sample; given a construct as complex as insight, its reliable and valid measurement may prove challenging.

Looking ahead, as the concept of neurocognitive insight becomes better characterized and understood, investigation of its role in neurocognitive treatment adherence and outcome will be critical. Just as patients with low levels of clinical insight are less likely to adhere to their prescribed medication regimen (McEvoy et al., 2006), those with low levels of neurocognitive insight may be less likely to be interested in or adherent to cognitive training interventions. In fact, in a recent treatment study (Keefe et al., 2015), treatment with enceniclene, an alpha-7 nicotinic partial agonist, was found to lead to improvements in performance on the MCCB as well as informant-rated cognitive performance. Critically, self-reports of cognitive functioning were not sensitive to treatment with encenicline despite beneficial effects detected with both objective and informant-based rates of cognitive change. Because individuals tend to be poor raters of their own cognitive functioning, particularly when they show evidence of objective cognitive impairment, it may be helpful to provide education and feedback regarding cognitive performance to increase awareness of cognitive impairment (Medalia et al., 2012), as well as incorporate motivational interviewing techniques addressing impaired cognition and its impact to increase adherence to cognitive remediation treatment (Fiszdon et al., 2015). Future studies may emphasize clinical features of those lacking neurocognitive insight, as well as the relationship between neurocognitive insight at baseline and outcome following cognitive remediation treatment. As more treatments with efficacy for cognition are developed, it will become critical to assist patients in detecting their cognitive impairments at baseline and their treatment-related improvements in order to optimize treatment of cognitive deficits and to reduce cognition-related everyday disability.

Acknowledgments

Funding body agreements and policies: This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01MH078737 to T.L.P., R01MH078775 to P.D.H., and T32MH019934 to C.Z.B.)

We gratefully acknowledge the participants who volunteered for this study.

Footnotes

Contributors: Authors Harvey, Patterson, and Twamley wrote the VALERO protocol. Authors Burton and Twamley designed this study. Author Burton managed the literature searches and statistical analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest: Dr. Harvey has received consulting fees from Boehringer Ingelheim, Forum Pharmaceuticals (En Vivo), Lundbeck Pharma, Otsuka America Pharma, Sanofi Pharma, Sunovion Pharma, and Takeda Pharma during the past year. The other authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Cynthia Z. Burton, Email: czburton@ucsd.edu.

Philip D. Harvey, Email: philipdharvey1@cs.com.

Thomas L. Patterson, Email: tpatterson@ucsd.edu.

Elizabeth W. Twamley, Email: etwamley@ucsd.edu.

References

  1. Aleman A, Agrawal N, Morgan KD, David AS. Insight in psychosis and neuropsychological function: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:204–212. doi: 10.1192/bjp.189.3.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amador XF, Strauss DH, Yale SA, Flaum MM, Endicott J, Gorman JM. Assessment of insight in psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:873–879. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.6.873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beck AT, Baruch E, Balter JM, Steer RA, Warman DM. A new instrument for measuring insight: The Beck Cognitive Insight Scale. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:319–329. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00189-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck AT, Rector NA. Cognitive approaches to schizophrenia: theory and therapy. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:577–606. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bora E, Erkan A, Kayahan B, Veznedaroglu B. Cognitive insight and acute psychosis in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;61:634–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01731.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bowie CR, Twamley EW, Anderson H, Halpern B, Patterson TL, Harvey PD. Self-assessment of functional status in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2007;41:1012–1018. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burton CZ, Vella L, Twamley EW. Clinical and cognitive insight in a compensatory cognitive training intervention. Am J Psychiatr Rehabil. 2011;14:307–326. doi: 10.1080/15487768.2011.622159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Durand D, Strassnig M, Sabbag S, Gould F, Twamley EW, Patterson TL, Harvey PD. Factors influencing self-assessment of cognition and functioning in schizophrenia: implications for treatment studies. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25:185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fiszdon JM, Kurtz MM, Choi J, Bell MD, Martino S. Motivational interviewing to increase cognitive rehabilitation adherence in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2015 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv143. epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR, Fanjiang G. Mini-Mental State Examination user’s guide. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gooding AL, Saperstein A, Rivera Mindt M, Medalia A. Predictors of treatment utilisation at cognitive remediation groups for schizophrenia: The roles of neuropsychological, psychological and clinical variables. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2012;22:516–531. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2012.665583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gould F, McGuire LS, Durand D, Sabbag S, Larrauri C, Patterson TL, Twamley EW, Harvey PD. Self-assessment in schizophrenia: Accuracy of evaluation of cognition and everyday functioning. Neuropsychology. 2015;29:675–682. doi: 10.1037/neu0000175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Greenberger C, Serper MR. Examination of clinical and cognitive insight in acute schizophrenia patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010;198:465–469. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181e4f35d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, Kuck J, Marcotte TD, Jeste DV. Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:24–32. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Heaton RK, Miller WS, Taylor MJ, Grant I. Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults (HRB) Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  18. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychol. 1998;12:426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Herman KC, Ostrander R, Walkup JT, Silva SG, March JS. Empirically derived subtypes of adolescent depression: Latent profile analysis of co-occurring symptoms in the treatment for adolescents with depression study (TADS) J Consult Clin Psych. 2007;75:716–728. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Homayoun S, Nadeau-Marcotte F, Luck D, Stip E. Subjective and objective cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia – is there a link? Front Psychol. 2011;2:148. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Johnson I, Tabbane K, Dellagi L, Kebir O. Self-perceived cognitive functioning does not correlate with objective measures of cognition in schizophrenia. Comp Psychiatry. 2011;52:688–692. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Keefe RSE, Davis VG, Spagnola NB, Hilt D, Dgetluck N, Ruse S, Patterson TL, Narasimhan M, Harvey PD. Reliability, validity and treatment sensitivity of the Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale. Eur Neuropsychoparmacol. 2015;25:176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Keefe RSE, Poe M, Walker TM, Kang JW, Harvey PD. The Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale: An interview-based assessment and its relationship to cognition, real-world functioning, and functional capacity. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:426–432. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Koren D, Seidman LJ, Poyurovsky M, Goldsmith M, Viksman P, Zichel S, Klein E. The neuropsychological basis of insight in first-episode schizophrenia: a pilot metacognitive study. Schizophr Res. 2004;70:195–202. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Larøi F, Fannemel M, Rønneberg U, Flekkøy K, Opjordsmoen S, Dullerud R, Haakonsen M. Unawareness of illness in chronic schizophrenia and its relationship to structural brain measures and neuropsychological tests. Psychiatry Res. 2000;100:49–58. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(00)00063-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G, Buck KD, Callaway SS, Salvatore G, Carcione A, Nicolò G, Stanghellini G. Poor insight in schizophrenia: links between different forms of metacognition with awareness of symptoms, treatment need, and consequences of illness. Comp Psychiatry. 2011a;52:253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lysaker PH, McCormick BP, Snethen G, Buck KD, Hamm JA, Grant M, Nicolò G, Dimaggio G. Metacognition and social function in schizophrenia: associations of mastery with functional skills competence. Schizophr Res. 2011b;131:214–218. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mausbach BT, Harvey PD, Goldman SR, Jeste DV, Patterson TL. Development of a brief scale of everyday functioning in persons with serious mental illness. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:1364–1372. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. McEvoy JP, Johnson J, Perkins D, Lieberman JA, Hamer RM, Keefe RSE, Tohen M, Glick ID, Sharma T. Insight in first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med. 2006;36:1385–1393. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Medalia A, Lim RW. Self-awareness of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;71:331–338. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Medalia A, Saperstein A, Choi KH, Choi J. The efficacy of a brief psycho-educational intervention to improve awareness of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2012;199:164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Medalia A, Thysen J. Insight into neurocognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1221–1230. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Medalia A, Thysen J. A comparison of insight into clinical symptoms versus insight into neuro-cognitive symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;118:134–139. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Medalia A, Thysen J, Freilich B. Do people with schizophrenia who have objective cognitive impairment identify cognitive deficits on a self report measure? Schizophr Res. 2008;105:156–164. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mintz AR, Dobson KS, Romney DM. Insight in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2003;61:75–88. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00316-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Monteiro LC, Silva VA, Louzã MR. Insight, cognitive dysfunction and symptomatology in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008;258:402–405. doi: 10.1007/s00406-008-0809-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Moritz S, Ferahli S, Naber D. Memory and attention performance in psychiatric patients: Lack of correspondence between clinician-rated and patient-rated functioning with neuropsychological test results. J Int Neuropsych Soc. 2004;10:623–633. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mysore A, Parks RW, Lee KH, Bhaker RS, Birkett P, Woodruff PW. Neurocognitive basis of insight in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:529–530. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.029181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nair A, Palmer EC, Aleman A, David AS. Relationship between cognition, clinical and cognitive insight in psychotic disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2014;152:191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, Essock S, Fenton WS, Frese FJ, Gold JM, Goldberg T, Heaton RK, Keefe RSE, Kraemer H, Mesholam-Gately R, Seidman LJ, Stover E, Weinberger DR, Young AS, Zalcman S, Marder SR. The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:203–213. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Orfei MD, Spoletini I, Banfi G, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Neuropsychological correlates of cognitive insight in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178:51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Palmer BW, Heaton RK, Paulsen JS, Kuck J, Braff D, Harris MJ, Zisook S, Jeste DV. Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal? Neuropsychology. 1997;11:437–446. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.11.3.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Poletti S, Anselmetti S, Riccaboni R, Bosia M, Buonocore M, Smeraldi E, Cavallaro R. Self-awareness of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia: Patients and their relatives. Psychiatry Res. 2012;198:207–211. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Prouteau A, Verdoux H, Briand C, Lesage A, Lalone P, Nicole L, Reinharz D, Stip E. Self-assessed cognitive dysfunction and objective performance in outpatients with schizophrenia participating in a rehabilitation program. Schizophr Res. 2004;69:85–91. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sabbag S, Twamley EW, Vella L, Heaton RK, Patterson TL, Harvey PD. Predictors of the accuracy of self assessment of everyday functioning in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;137:190–195. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Saperstein AM, Thysen J, Medalia A. The Measure of Insight into Cognition: Reliability and validity of clinician-rated and self-report scales of neurocognitive insight for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;134:54–58. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schwartz RC, Cohen BN, Grubaugh A. Does insight affect long-term inpatient treatment outcome in chronic schizophrenia? Compr Psychiatry. 1997;38:283–288. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(97)90061-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sellwood W, Morrison AP, Beck R, Heffernan S, Law H, Bentall RP. Subjective cognitive complaints in schizophrenia: relation to antipsychotic medication dose, actual cognitive performance, insight and symptoms. PloS ONE. 2013;8(12):e83774. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Shad MU, Muddasani S, Prasad K, Sweeney JA, Keshavan MS. Insight and prefrontal cortex in first-episode schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1315–1320. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Simon V, De Hert M, Wampers M, Peuskens J, van Winkel R. The relation between neurocognitive dysfunction and impaired insight in patients with schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry. 2009;24:239–243. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Stip E, Caron J, Renaud S, Pampoulova T, Lecomte Y. Exploring cognitive complaints in schizophrenia: The Subjective Scale to Investigate Cognition in Schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. 2003;44:331–340. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(03)00086-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wiffen BD, Rabinowitz J, Lex A, David AS. Correlates, change and ‘state or trait’ properties of insight in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2010;122:94–103. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wilkinson GS. Wide-Range Achievement Test 3: Administration Manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES