Abstract
New neuronal recruitment in an adult animal’s brain is presumed to contribute to brain plasticity and increase the animal’s ability to contend with new and changing environments. During long-distance migration, birds migrating greater distances are exposed to more diverse spatial information. Thus, we hypothesized that greater migration distance in birds would correlate with the recruitment of new neurons into the brain regions involved with migratory navigation. We tested this hypothesis on two Palearctic migrants - reed warblers (Acrocephalus scirpaceus) and turtle doves (Streptopelia turtur), caught in Israel while returning from Africa in spring and summer. Birds were injected with a neuronal birth marker and later inspected for new neurons in brain regions known to play a role in navigation - the hippocampus and nidopallium caudolateral. We calculated the migration distance of each individual by matching feather isotopic values (δ2H and δ13C) to winter base-maps of these isotopes in Africa. Our findings suggest a positive correlation between migration distance and new neuronal recruitment in two brain regions - the hippocampus in reed warblers and nidopallium caudolateral in turtle doves. This multidisciplinary approach provides new insights into the ability of the avian brain to adapt to different migration challenges.
Exposure to new information in adult animals has been positively correlated with the magnitude of new neuronal recruitment in their brains1,2. This exposure, linked to new neuronal recruitment, could be manifested in many ways throughout an animal’s life history and the challenges it faces3,4,5. The recruitment of new neurons is considered to be promoted by an elevated need to acquire new information in relevant brain regions6. Such recruitment may serve this purpose by facilitating brain plasticity and has been suggested to play an important role in acquiring new memories in both invertebrates7,8 and vertebrates1,9. Birds have been used as model animals in studies linking new neuronal recruitment with various behaviors, such as social complexity10, food hoarding4,11, song learning12, the reproductive cycle13, and others.
Long-distance migration is highly demanding in many aspects of a bird’s life cycle. Beyond the immense physiological effort required in traveling large geographical distances, birds also have to avoid predation, minimize resource competition, overcome adverse weather, and accurately navigate and orient14. The mortality rate of young avian migrants is far higher than that of their adult counterparts which suggests that learned experience is of great importance for migratory birds15. Indeed, several studies have explored the importance of learning and experience for accurate navigation in migratory birds16,17. Learning could be key to improved navigation ability; identification of suitable stop-over sites en-route; avoidance of harsh weather or potential predators; and for outcompeting both fellow migrants and local birds. Such needs for increased learning ability during migration may be answered by a greater neuronal recruitment in the relevant brain regions of migratory birds. To date, only two studies of passerines18,19 have directly investigated the possible relationship between neuronal recruitment and migratory behavior in birds. Those studies revealed that, for passerines, migrant species or sub-species possessed more new neurons than resident species in the brain regions that play a role in spatial information processing.
During the past century, great advances have been made in tracking and monitoring migratory birds. In addition to the increased use of recovery-based ringing and tagging, researchers nowadays have access to radio transmitters and satellite or GPS tracking to monitor individual birds20. Recently, the use of stable isotopes has been shown to be effective in quantifying individual or population migratory connectivity21,22. The use of stable isotopes to track migratory movement is based on the predictable global spatial patterns for hydrogen (H) and carbon (C) isotopes. Wherever a bird grows its feathers (often at its natal or overwintering site), the local isotopic patterns are translated through diet along the trophic cascade, and are fixed into the growing feathers, thereby reflecting the region the feathers were grown23. When a bird is caught later, its feathers’ 2H/H and 13C/12C ratios can be non-lethally analyzed and compared to known H and C isotopic distribution base-maps (e.g. isoscapes). This tissue-to-isoscape comparison is used to obtain estimates of the region where a bird had molted or grown its feathers. One of the primary advantages of the isotope technique is that every bird captured provides intrinsic spatial information about its migration route, without the need for the mark-recapture used in ringing and marking techniques24.
In this study we hypothesized a possible link between an increase in new neuronal recruitment and the distances traveled by migrant birds. We explored this phenomenon in reed warblers (Acrocephalus scirpaceus) and turtle doves (Streptopelia turtur), which are summer visitors in Israel and winter and molt in Africa25. Migration distance estimates were determined using feather isotopic ratios as an indicator of molt locality in the wintering grounds. New neuronal recruitment was evaluated in two brain regions known to take part in navigation and spatial orientation tasks-the Hippocampal complex (HC)26,27, and the nidopallium caudolateral (NCL)27.
Results
Migration distance and recruitment of new neurons
When exploring links between migration distance and new neuronal recruitment in turtle doves we found marginally significant increase in recruitment into the NCL for birds flying longer distances (P = 0.07; ρ = 0.53; N = 12; Fig. 1), but not in the HC (P = 0.96; ρ = −0.01; N = 11). In the reed warbler we found a similar but opposite trend, with increased neuronal recruitment into the HC (P = 0.12; ρ = 0.69; N = 6; Fig. 2), but not into the NCL (P = 0.8; ρ = −0.13; N = 6) of birds flying longer distances. Notably, these marginally significant results, respectively for the turtle doves and the reed warblers, were based on small sample sizes. For the turtle doves the statistical power for the NCL analysis was 76% at the nominal α = 0.05 and 84.7% for α = 0.1. For the reed warbler analysis of the HC, the statistical power was low (35%), which meant that for our sample size (six birds), at the nominal rejection rate of 0.05 and the effect size we found, it was unlikely that we would obtain significant results. Upon setting our rejection rate to 0.1, the power of the test increased to 47.4%. In order to obtain a power value of 80% with the same effect size we would have needed about 10 birds in our sample at the 0.1 rejection rate or 12 birds for the 0.05 rejection rate.
Wintering molt location
Only a few of the individuals that were analyzed for stable isotopes also underwent neuronal recruitment analysis (see below). We used our larger sample size of individuals whose feathers were measured for stable isotopes (25 reed warblers, 14 turtle doves), to estimate probable wintering grounds in Africa for these two species. Figs 3 and 4 display kernel distributions of wintering grounds for reed warblers and turtle doves, respectively. The maps also show data from ringing returns (obtained from the Society for the Protection of Nature in Israel – www.natureisrael.org), as well as known wintering grounds based on expert drawn maps28,29.
Discussion
In this work we found a tentative link between the migration distance of two bird species and new neuronal requirement into the brain regions that play a role in spatial orientation and navigation. While our sample sizes are inevitably small (see also below), we found in two distinct regions – the HC for reed warblers and the NCL in the turtle doves, an increase in neuronal recruitment corresponding with increased migration distance. Our combined approach, though innovative, employed two commonly used and accepted isotope and neuronal analyses methods19,30.
As noted above, the limitation of our results is that they are based on small sample sizes. Furthermore, just a few data points were responsible for much of the observed effect, both in the HC of reed warblers and the NCL of turtle doves (Figs 1 and 2). However, these data points are not the outcome of any histological failure, because our brains were processed simultaneously in few batches, and these data points were in batches that included other brains, which yielded data points with different scores. Moreover, statistically, these data points did not depart from the central tendency of the other values and were not designated as an outlier when fitted to a Poisson distribution31. Therefore, despite the small sample sizes and the consequent low statistical power obtained, we suggest that our data might indicate a general phenomenon. This suggestion is manifested in the fairly high coefficients of determination – ρ (0.53 for the turtle doves in the NCL and 0.69 for the reed warblers in the HC), for the two separate tests. Nevertheless, further investigation using larger datasets in different regions and organisms will be critical to obtain additional support for our hypothesis.
While a larger sample size would have been desirable in order to confirm our hypothesis, the experimental design and field logistics made this impossible. Initially, we sampled only adult birds caught in the wild within a particular season. Furthermore, these birds had to survive 35 days of captivity, during which new neurons migrated and were incorporated into the designated brain regions.
The analysis we conducted targeted neuronal differences within populations of the same species. Such differences probably arose due to evolutionary adaptations to different migration distances. As such, this phenomenon somewhat differs from cases in which new neuronal recruitment is temporally tuned to answer a specific need10,13,32. Our results indicated a variance in the degree of plasticity in the brain, potentially derived from genetic differences between populations migrating longer or shorter distances. A similar mechanism of differences in new neuronal recruitment between populations of the same species has been recorded with respect to food hoarding in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla) experiencing different climatic conditions33,34.
The HC has been shown in previous works on passerines to be important for the processing of long-term memory, navigation, and spatial orientation1,26. Similarly, the NCL has been found to be involved in processing spatial information27,35 and working memory36,37 in pigeons. Thus, our findings that the HC and the NCL are important for navigation and orientation in reed warblers and turtle doves, respectively, are in line with previous results. However, to the best of our knowledge, passerine NCL has never been tested with respect to orientation and navigation. Our results show that, at least for reed warblers, the NCL plays a minor role in these brain functions relative to the HC. Doves were previously shown to depend on the HC for navigation across a familiar space within 10–20 km from the home loft, whereas long-distance navigation over unfamiliar spaces was HC independent26. Therefore, turtle dove migration between Africa and Israel is unlikely to be HC-dependent, and indeed we did not find any link between neuronal recruitment in the HC and migration distances traveled by this species (Fig. 1).
The differences in the brain regions found to be significantly correlated with migration distance in the two species might be explained by differences in their migratory behavior. Reed warblers are lone nocturnal migrants that rely on self-orientation for navigation. Conversely, turtle doves tend to migrate in large flocks, mostly at night but also during the day38. The evolutionary advantage of flocking lies in superior group decisions, known as the “many wrong” principle, in which pooling information from many inaccurate compasses yields a single more accurate compass39. In addition, group flight may also assist in locating limited or obscured landmarks while flying40, and allow inexperienced individuals to follow experienced partners41,42. Such information sharing between flock members may reduce the navigational investment per individual, but requires developed social interaction and communication among migrating individuals40,43. The NCL has been suggested to take part in the integration of visual and auditory pathways44,45 and may play a primary role in social interactions. Flock size is a plastic social structure which has been found to positively correlate with migration distance in many avian species46,47. Flocking migrants that fly longer distances could thus need to acquire more information on more complex social structures. This may explain the link we found in doves between neuronal recruitment in the NCL and migration distances. Beyond their behavioral differences, turtle doves and reed warblers belong to clades that are thought to have been separated from one another other ~85 million years ago48, that may have resulted in a parallel evolution manifested in similar functions of different brain regions.
Using information from two stable isotopes, as well as spatial averaging via use of kernel estimations, we were able to ascribe probable localities of the winter molt for each individual bird. Furthermore, our analysis and methodology enabled us to determine the wintering grounds of the populations of the two species under study - the reed warbler and the turtle dove. Our findings add information about the geographical extent of these species during winter in a region that is lacking in research and surveys. Only 60 returns for ringed reed warblers and six returns for turtle doves from Europe and the Middle East have been recorded in Israel over the past 20 years, with two ringing returns for these two species from all of Africa (Figs 3 and 4). Moreover, the maps we constructed based on isotope analysis have proven similar to the limited published information28,29 and ringing data, thereby supporting our methodology. Wintering ground maps are particularly important for turtle doves as there is a growing concern regarding the global decline of populations of this species49. Identifying the wintering grounds of this species could aid in their future conservation measures.
Here we present, for the first time, a preliminary indication of a linkage between distances traveled and new neuronal recruitment between different populations of migrating birds. Our evidence suggests a possible important adaptation of migrating birds – those that travel greater distances exhibit greater brain plasticity. Obviously, more work is needed in different species, habitats, and brain regions, in order to substantiate our findings. Nonetheless, our methodologies and results can serve as a first important step. This work is also of value in light of the ongoing climatic, enviromental and landuse changes, as an indication that pre-adaptation towards greater brain plasticity could prove advantagous for species or populations in novel enviroments or ecosystems. We hope that our work will promote additional research on the effects of migration on the avian brain.
Methods
Experimental design
Our neuronal analysis was carried out on six adult reed warblers and twelve adult turtle doves, collected under the Israel Nature and National Parks Protection Authority permit (2005/24706). The study was approved by the Tel Aviv University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (permit L-06-008) and was performed in accordance with its regulations and guidelines regarding the care and use of animals for experimental procedures. Birds were caught with mist nets in the Jordan Rift Valley, Israel (32°.41′N; 35°.53′ E) during spring (February to April) and summer (June to July) between the years 2005–2009. Birds were aged according to plumage, iris, and leg color25,50. We used only adult birds in order to avoid possible effects of age on new neuronal recruitment and to ensure that the sampled individuals had molted their feathers at least once in Africa (see below). Two tail feathers were collected from each individual and tested for stable isotopes (Carbon: δ13C and Deuterium: δ2Η). Isotopic data were compared to the respective isoscapes of Africa (see below). Brains of all birds were processed and analyzed for new neuronal recruitment.
Stable isotope analysis
Two tail feathers were sampled from each individual for the analysis of stable isotope remains (δ2H; δ13C). Feather H isotopic composition for non-exchangeable H was analyzed following the comparative equilibration method described in Wassenaar and Hobson51. All feathers were cleaned of surface oils using a 2:1 chloroform:methanol solution and then dried overnight in a hood. For the δ13C analysis, we used 1.2 ± 0.2 mg subsamples of the feathers (weighed by microbalance - Sartorius SE2; Gottingen, Germany) and placed in tin capsules (D1007; Elemental microanalysis; UK). For the δ2H analyses, subsamples weighing 0.35 ± 0.01 mg were placed in silver capsules (D1007; Elemental microanalysis; UK). Samples were then analyzed on a Europa 20:20 continuous-flow isotope-ratio mass spectrometer (CF-IRMS) interfaced with a Robo Prep elemental analyzer. δ13 C measurements were reported in δ-notation relative to the Pee Dee Belemnite standard (PDB) in parts per mil deviations (‰). Measurement error is estimated at 0.1‰ and 0.3‰ for the δ13C values. The δ2H measurements were reported as parts per mil deviations (‰) relative to Vienna Standard Mean Ocean Water–Standard Light Antarctic precipitation scale (VSMOW-SLAP). The H isotope analysis is more complex than that of δ13C due to the problem of uncontrolled isotopic exchange between feathers and ambient water vapor51. To correct this effect, accepted keratin standards were used so that the δ2H values reported here correspond to non-exchangeable feather hydrogen. This gave us δ13C and δ2H values for each individual, of the two species. Reed warblers and turtle doves are known to molt during winter in Africa52,53, and we therefore allocated their feathers’ isotope values to this continent. Feather isotope values were matched to precipitation isotope values, and a constant isotope fractionation factor of +25‰ was added for δ2H feathers values51 and +1‰ for δ13C54.
Spatial analysis
To define the wintering molting regions for all birds we used ‘map lookup’55 and spatial averaging approaches. This entailed comparing the two isotopic values measured in the feathers of each bird to their parallel values in the known isoscapes, in order to create a probable molt region for each isotope. Regional δ2H isoscapes for the relevant feathers molting months (October–December) were obtained from WaterIsotopes.org web page, while δ13C annual isoscape was provided by Dr. C. Still (Department of Geography & Institute for Computational Earth System Science UC Santa Barbara, USA).
Initially, we extracted for each bird and each of the two isotopes studied, those values of the isoscapes that corresponded to the measured values in the feathers up to the precision level of the isoscape layer. Thus, if our measured value was X we extracted from the isoscape layer only those values that X lay between – i.e. those values immediately preceding or following X. All of these extracted values for each bird were spatially compared between the two isotopes studies. In cases where there was a distance of 10 km or less between these extractions for the two isotopes a possible location was indicated. The natural spatial spread of both 2H and 13C in Africa does not show a clear east-west gradient. Consequently, for some individuals our approach found several possible locations on both the eastern and western sides of the continent that corresponded to their feathers’ isotopic values. However, previous studies have shown that reed warblers and turtle doves migrating across the eastern Mediterranean sea (where the birds were caught), spend the winter in eastern Africa28,29. Thus we excluded from our analysis those possible locations which were west of the 18 oE longitude. For each bird all possible locations eastern of 18 oE longitude, were spatially averaged using a kernel density smoothing method. This method ultimately averages out those locations further away from the majority of all matches and produces a single potential point (as the centroid of the 10% kernel volume contour) which was then used as the location of the wintering ground for that bird. We next measured the distance between this point and the locality where the bird was captured in Israel (using an equal distance global projection) in order to determine the minimum migration distance traveled by each bird. We later used all the centroid locations of all the birds for each species to produce a map of the wintering grounds of the two species. All spatial analysis was conducted in ArcGIS 10.156.
Neuronal recruitment analysis
Following capture, the birds were transferred to outdoor aviaries in the Botanical Gardens of Tel Aviv University and injected three times, at 24 hours apart, with the cell birth-date marker 5-bromo-2-deoxyuridine (BrdU). Five weeks post-BrdU treatment birds were killed with an overdose of anesthesia and their brains underwent through histological procedures in which they were embedded in polyethylene glycol, blocked and cut transversely at thickness of 6 μm, along the rosto-caudal axis. Then, brain sections went through immunohistochemistry procedures that stained all neurons with fluorescent green (with anti-HuC/HuD), and nuclei of new neurons with fluorescent red (with anti-BrdU). Therefore, cells with co-localization of green cytoplasm and a red fluorescent nucleus were identified as new neurons (Fig. 5). For full details of the histological and immunohistochemical protocols used for both species, see Barkan et al.19.
Brain mapping and quantification
In both species, the brain regions we examined were the HC and NCL. In turtle doves, for each brain region, we defined and examined the most rostral and caudal section, and examined five additional sections between them, separated by an average distance of 240 μm (Fig. 6). For the HC, the most rostral section was defined by the presence of the commissura anterior (CoA), and corresponded to level A7.75 in the atlas of the pigeon brain57, and the most caudal section of the HC corresponded to level A6.25 in that atlas (Fig. 6B). The ventral, dorsal, and medial boundaries in each section were defined according to previously defined criteria4. For the NCL, the most caudal section corresponded to level A3.0 in the atlas of the pigeon brain57. The most rostral section corresponded to level A4.5 in this atlas, and was defined according to the lateral ventricle, along the dorsal part of the brain (Fig. 6C). The ventral boundary was indistinguishable by our staining methods and was therefore determined according to the dopaminergic innervation recorded in pigeons58. Since brains of laughing doves and turtle doves differ in size from the pigeon brain, we followed the method we previously used19 to determine the ventral boundary relative to that in the pigeon brain. For the definition of HC and NCL in reed warblers, a similar procedure was performed, as described in our previous publication19.
In both species, we used a computerized brain-mapping system (Stereo Investigator; MicroBrightField Inc.) to draw the boundaries of the HC and NCL in each section sampled, mark the position the new neurons and quantify their number. Total neuronal density was quantified in one section in each brain region, by counting all neurons within 12–18 sampling squares (100 × 100 μm each), randomly chosen by the software, using the fractionator probe. Our measure of new neuronal recruitment was calculated as a percentage of new out of total neurons per mm3, in these two brain regions.
Statistical analysis
Non-parametric Spearman’s rank correlation was conducted between values of new neuronal recruitment and the calculated distances the birds had travelled. These regressions were conducted separately for the two species and each of the brain regions examined. We used the forward discovery rate correction for multiple testing59 to assign significance, using α = 0.05 as our target for rejection of the null. We then conducted power analyses on these four separate tests. All statistical analyses were conducted in R60.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Barkan, S. et al. Possible linkage between neuronal recruitment and flight distance in migratory birds. Sci. Rep. 6, 21983; doi: 10.1038/srep21983 (2016).
Acknowledgments
We thank Kobi Merom for assisting in field work and bird ringing. We also thank Prof. Jacob Garty, The Botanical Gardens of Tel-Aviv University for providing us facilities for this work; The Israeli Ornithology Center of the Society for the Protection of Nature in Israel, for providing ringing data; Dr. Christopher Still, Dept. of Geography & Institute for Computational Earth System Science UC Santa Barbara for providing the δ13C isoscape; and Naomi Paz for editing the manuscript. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable and constructive comments, which helped us to improve the manuscript. This study was supported by The National Institute for Psychobiology in Israel and The Open University Research Fund. UR was supported by the Adams Fellowship Program of the Israeli Academy of Sciences and the Humanities.
Footnotes
Author Contributions S.B., A.B. and Y.Y.T. conceived this work, S.B. conducted the experiments, L.W. conducted the stable isotope analyses, S.B. and U.R. analyzed the data, all authors wrote the paper and contributed to ideas.
References
- Barnea A. & Pravosudov V. Birds as a model to study adult neurogenesis: bridging evolutionary, comparative and neuroethological approaches. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 884–907 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao C. M., Deng W. & Gage F. H. Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell 132, 645–660 doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.033 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chancellor L. V., Roth T. C., LaDage L. D. & Pravosudov V. V. The effect of environmental harshness on neurogenesis: A large-scale comparison. Dev. Neurobiol. 71, 246–252 doi: 10.1002/Dneu.20847 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnea A. & Nottebohm F. Seasonal recruitment of hippocampal-neurons in adult free- ranging black-capped chickadees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 11217–11221 (1994). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel S. N., Clayton N. S. & Krebs J. R. Spatial learning induces neurogenesis in the avian brain. Behav. Brain Res. 89, 115–128 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F. Neuronal replacement in adult brain. Brain Res. Bull. 57, 737–749 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bridges R. S. & Grattan D. R. Prolactin-induced neurogenesis in the maternal brain. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 14, 199–201 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomassese S. S. et al. Influence of environmental stimulation on neurogenesis in the adult insect brain. J. Neurobiol. 45, 162–171 (2000). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonfanti L. & Peretto P. Adult neurogenesis in mammals - a theme with many variations. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 930–950 doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07832.x (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipkind D., Nottebohm F., Rado R. & Barnea A. Social change affects the survival of new neurons in the forebrain of adult songbirds. Behav. Brain Res. 133, 31–43 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherry D. F. & Hoshooley J. S. Seasonal hippocampal plasticity in food-storing birds. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365, 933–943 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirn J. R. The relationship of neurogenesis and growth of brain regions to song learning. Brain Lang. 115, 29–44 doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.09.006 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkan S., Ayali A., Nottebohm F. & Barnea A. Neuronal recruitment in adult zebra finch brain during a reproductive cycle. Dev. Neurobiol. 67, 687–701 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alerstam T., Hedenstrom A. & Akesson S. Long-distance migration: evolution and determinants. Oikos 103, 247–260 (2003). [Google Scholar]
- Newton I. The migration ecology of birds. (Academic Press, 2010). [Google Scholar]
- Perdeck A. C. Two types of orientation in migranting Sturnus vulgaris and fringilla coalebs as revealed by displacement experiments. Ardea 46, 1–37 (1958). [Google Scholar]
- Thorup K. et al. Evidence for a navigational map stretching across the continental US in a migratory songbird. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 18115–18119 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaDage L. D., Roth T. C. & Pravosudov V. V. Hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with migratory behaviour in adult but not juvenile sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys ssp.). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 278, 138–143 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkan S., Yom-Tov Y. & Barnea A. A possible relation between new neuronal recruitment and migratory behavior in Acrocephalus warblers. Dev. Neurobiol. 74, 1194–1209 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wikelski M. et al. Going wild: what a global small-animal tracking system could do for experimental biologists. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 181–186 doi: 10.1242/Jeb.02629 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reichlin T. S. et al. Migratory connectivity in a declining bird species: using feather isotopes to inform demographic modelling. Divers. Distrib. 16, 643–654 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Hobson K. A. & Wassenaar L. I. Linking brooding and wintering grounds of neotropical migrant songbirds using stable hydrogen isotopic analysis of feathers. Oecologia 109, 142–148 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wassenaar L. I. In Tracking animal migration with stable isotopes Vol. 1st Terrestrial ecology series (eds Wassenaar L. I. & Hobson K. A.) 21–44 (Academic Press, 2008). [Google Scholar]
- Hobson K. A. & Norris D. R. In Tracking animal migration with stable isotopes Vol. 1st (eds Hobson K. A. & Wassenaar L. I.) 1–20 (Elsevier 2008).
- Shirihai H., Dovrat E., Christie D. A., Harris A. & Cottridge D. The birds of Israel. 692 (Academic Press London, 1996). [Google Scholar]
- Herold C., Coppola V. J. & Bingman V. P. The maturation of research into the avian hippocampal formation: Recent discoveries from one of the nature’s foremost navigators. Hippocampus 00, 1–19 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bingman V. P., Riters L. V., Strasser R. & Gagliardo A. In Animal cognition in nature (eds Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg & Alan C. Kamil) 201–226 (Academic Press, 1998). [Google Scholar]
- Urban E. K., Fry C. H. & Stuart K. In The birds of Africa, Vol. 2 (eds Urban E. K., Fry C. H. & Stuart K.) 490–492 (Academic press, 1986). [Google Scholar]
- Urban E. K., Fry C. H. & Stuart K. In The birds of Africa, Vol. 5 (eds Urban E. K., Fry C. H. & Stuart K.) 104–107 (Academic press, 1997). [Google Scholar]
- Inger R. & Bearhop S. Applications of stable isotope analyses to avian ecology. Ibis 150, 447–461 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- Hoaglin D. C. A Poissonness plot. Am. Stat. 34, 146–149 (1980). [Google Scholar]
- Kirn J. R., O’Loughlin, Kasparian S. & Nottebohm F. Cell-death and neuronal recruitment in the high vocal center of adult male Canaries are temporally related to changes in song. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 7844–7848 (1994). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth T. C. & Pravosudov V. V. Hippocampal volumes and neuron numbers increase along a gradient of environmental harshness: a large-scale comparison. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 276, 401–405 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freas C. A., LaDage L. D., Roth T. C. & Pravosudov V. V. Elevation-related differences in memory and the hippocampus in mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli. Anim. Behav. 84, 121–127 (2012). [Google Scholar]
- Riters L. V. & Bingman V. P. The effects of lesions to the caudolateral neostriatum on sun compass based spatial learning in homing pigeons. Behav. Brain Res. 98, 1–15 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogensen J. & Divac I. Behavioral-effects of ablation of the pigeon-equivalent of the mammalian prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 55, 101–107 doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90012-F (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose J. & Colombo M. Neural correlates of executive control in the avian brain. PLoS Biol. 3, 1139–1146 doi: ARTN e190, 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030190 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreau R. E. Migration in the Mediterranean area. Ibis 95, 329–364 (1953). [Google Scholar]
- Simons A. M. Many wrongs: the advantage of group navigation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 453–455 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton W. J. Evidence concerning the function of nocturnal call notes of migratory birds. Condor, 390–401 doi: 10.2307/1365547 (1962). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Couzin I. D., Krause J., Franks N. R. & Levin S. A. Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433, 513–516 doi: 10.1038/Nature03236 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biro D., Sumpter D. J. T., Meade J. & Guilford T. From compromise to leadership in pigeon homing. Curr. Biol. 16, 2123–2128 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guttal V. & Couzin I. D. Social interactions, information use, and the evolution of collective migration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 16172–16177 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe S. & Masuda S. Integration of auditory and visual information in human face discrimination in pigeons: Behavioral and anatomical study. Behav. Brain Res. 207, 61–69 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunturkun O. The avian ‘prefrontal cortex’ and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 686–693 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder M. A. Behavioral-differences of female spruce grouse undertaking short and long migrations. Condor 87, 281–286 doi: 10.2307/1366896 (1985). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchamp G. Long-distance migrating species of birds travel in larger groups. Biol. Letters 7, 692–694 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0243 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jetz W., Thomas G. H., Joy J. B., Hartmann K. & Mooers A. O. The global diversity of birds in space and time. Nature 491, 444–448 doi: 10.1038/Nature11631 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vickery J. A. et al. The decline of Afro-Palaearctic migrants and an assessment of potential causes. Ibis 156, 1–22 doi: 10.1111/ibi.12118 (2014). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Merom K., McCleery R. & Yom-Tov Y. Age-related changes in wing-length and body mass in the Reed Warbler Acrocephalus scirpaceus and Clamorous Reed Warbler A-stentoreus. Bird Study 46, 249–255 (1999). [Google Scholar]
- Wassenaar L. I. & Hobson K. A. Comparative equilibration and online technique for determination of non-exchangeable hydrogen of keratins for use in animal migration studies. Isot. Environ. Health Stud. 39, 211–217 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson D. J. The wintering and migration of Palaearctic passerines at Kampala, southern Uganda. Ibis 114, 43–60 doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1972.tb02588.x (1972). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson D. J. Moult of some Palaearctic warblers wintering in Uganda. Bird Study 20, 24–36 (1973). [Google Scholar]
- Hobson K. A. & Clark R. G. Assessing avian diets using stable isotopes .2. Factors influencing diet-tissue fractionation. Condor 94, 189–197 (1992). [Google Scholar]
- Wunder M. B. & Norris D. R. In Tracking animal migration with stable isotopes. [Terrestrial Ecology Series 2.]. (eds Hobson Keith A. & Wassenaar Leonard I.) 107–128 (Academic Press, 2008). [Google Scholar]
- ArcGIS Desktop (2012). ESRI - Environmental Systems Research Institute, Redlands, CA. URL http:// http://www.esri.com/
- Karten H. J. & Hodos W. A Stereotaxic atlas of the brain of the pigeon (Columba livia). The Johns Hopkins Press (1967). [Google Scholar]
- Waldmann C. & Gunturkun O. The dopaminergic innervation of the pigeon caudolateral forebrain-immunocytochemical evidence for a prefrontal cortex in birds. Brain Res. 600, 225–234 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y. & Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate-a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. Roy. Stat. Soc. B Met. 57, 289–300 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL http://www.R-project.org/ (2013). [Google Scholar]